Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp
Fostering creativity across countries: The moderating effect of cultural bundles on creativity
T
⁎
Kevyn Yonga, , Pier Vittorio Mannuccib, Michel W. Landerc a
Singapore Institute of Management, 461 Clementi Road, Singapore 599491, Singapore London Business School, 1 Sussex Place, London NW1 4SA, United Kingdom c HEC Paris, 1 rue de la Libération, 78351 Jouy-en-Josas, France b
A R T I C LE I N FO
A B S T R A C T
Keywords: Creativity Culture Meta-analysis fs/QCA
Research has traditionally focused on the moderating role of single cultural dimensions to capture differences in how individual creativity is fostered across cultures. Culture, however, is a multidimensional construct, with cultural dimensions operating interdependently. Building on this reasoning, we propose that the moderating effect of culture is better understood by focusing on the configuration of cultural bundles. We define a cultural bundle as the set including the cultural value dimensions that characterize a given country, and the strength of the norms enforcing these values. We find support for this view in a mixed-methods study that combines a metaanalysis of 584 effect sizes from 205 studies set in 38 different countries with fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis (fs/QCA). We discuss the theoretical and practical implications of these findings, arguing for the importance of focusing on cultural bundles, rather than cultural dimensions in isolation, to understand the moderating effect of culture on creativity.
1. Introduction Creativity – the generation of novel and useful outcomes – is a source of competitive advantage for organizations all over the world (Florida & Goodnight, 2005; Morris & Leung, 2010; Zhou & Su, 2010). While the importance of creativity is global, how creativity is achieved in organizations varies significantly across countries (Erez & Nouri, 2010; Loewenstein & Mueller, 2016; Zhou & Su, 2010). Extant research has explored how the antecedents fostering creativity differ in efficacy across cultures, focusing on the moderating role of one cultural value dimension in isolation – mainly individualism – to explain these differences (e.g., Erez & Nouri, 2010; Goncalo & Staw, 2006; Nouri et al., 2015). More recently, scholars looked at how a specific cultural value interacts with the strength of norms that enforce cultural values – i.e., cultural tightness (Gelfand, Nishii, & Raver, 2006; Triandis, 1989) – to obtain a more fine-grained understanding of the moderating role of culture (Liu, Jiang, Shalley, Keem, & Zhou, 2016). Culture, however, is a multidimensional construct, and there is no reason to believe that cultural dimensions operate independently from each other (Kirkman, Lowe, & Gibson, 2006; Leung, Bhagat, Buchan, Erez, & Gibson, 2005). Some scholars have tried to address this issue by focusing on the broader East versus West distinction (e.g., Morris & Leung, 2010; Ng, 2003; Simonton & Ting, 2010). However, this
⁎
approach may overlook some nuances in the effect of cultural differences since “East” and “West” display differences within themselves. For example, the Chinese culture does not comprise the same set of dimensions as the Indian culture; and the same is true for the American culture compared to the French one (Hofstede, 1980; Hofstede, Hofstede, & Minkov, 2010). Moreover, this approach does not allow for a precise identification of the cultural dimensions driving intercultural differences in fostering creativity. We propose that we need to take into account the multidimensional nature of culture to understand the moderating effect of culture on creativity. To do this, we need to focus not on single cultural dimensions in isolation, but on cultural bundles. We define a cultural bundle as the set including (a) the conceptually distinct yet interconnected cultural value dimensions that characterize a given country, and (b) the strength of the norms enforcing these values. In this paper, we theorize and operationalize a cultural bundle to include (a) the four cultural value dimensions of individualism, power distance, masculinity, and uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede et al., 2010; Hofstede, 1980) and (b) the cultural tightness of each country. Building on the componential theory of creativity to classify creativity antecedents (Amabile & Pratt, 2016; Amabile, 1988, 1996, 2013) and on the toolkit view of culture (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986), we suggest that the configuration of a cultural bundle influences the
Corresponding author. E-mail addresses:
[email protected] (K. Yong),
[email protected] (P.V. Mannucci),
[email protected] (M.W. Lander).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.12.004 Received 7 February 2018; Received in revised form 4 December 2019; Accepted 15 December 2019 0749-5978/ © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
focus from a single cultural value dimension in isolation to the notion of cultural bundles providing a more fine-grained theory and analysis of the moderating effect of culture on creativity. Second, we contribute to research on culture and creativity by demonstrating how cultures achieve creativity in different ways (e.g. De Dreu, 2010; Simonton & Ting, 2010). Third, our research contributes to “further quantitative integrations” on the effects of antecedents on individual creativity (Anderson, Potočnik, & Zhou, 2014: p. 1323) by showing that the effect of the components of creativity on individual creativity is contingent on culture.
effectiveness of each of the components of creativity (i.e. domain-relevant skills, creativity-relevant skills, and task motivation) in fostering creativity in organizations. Specifically, we focus on the degree to which a bundle includes values that promote the use of a given component. We predict that within bundles that include cultural tightness the relationship between a given component and creativity will become stronger (weaker) as the number of cultural values that promote the use of that component increases (decreases). Within bundles that include cultural tightness, the development and use of a given component “benefits from a culturally consistent rationale to defend its legitimacy” (Peterson, 2016, p. 36). Since individuals in tight cultures incur sanctions for deviating from culturally valued behaviors, they need a larger number of cultural values promoting the use and development of a specific component in order to apply it to creative endeavors. In contrast, in cultural bundles that include cultural looseness, individuals will less likely be sanctioned should they deviate from what the cultural value dimensions mandate. Thus, these bundles do not necessarily need to have a high number of cultural value dimensions that promote the use of that component in order to encourage the application of that component to creative endeavors. We test our theory using a multi-method approach integrating metaanalytic methodology and qualitative comparative analysis. We first conduct a meta-analysis of 584 effect sizes from 205 studies conducted in 38 different countries across more than 60 years of research to identify the effect of creativity components within and across cultures. We then use meta-analytic regressions to test our moderation hypotheses on the interaction between the number of cultural values that promote the use of a given component and the presence of cultural tightness for each component-creativity relationship. We further test our hypotheses by using fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis (fs/ QCA; Fiss, 2011; Ragin, 2008). Fs/QCA allows us to identify the precise configurations of values and tightness that are fostering each component-creativity relationship, something that cannot be done in the metaanalytic regression. Moreover, it allows us to identify which bundles that include cultural looseness lead to a stronger/weaker componentcreativity relationship – something that could not be predicted ex ante. Finally, we show the predictive strength of cultural bundles in moderating the components-creativity relationship by running a set of supplementary meta-analytic regressions. Our analyses supported our theory and hypotheses. Within bundles that include cultural tightness, the presence of a larger number of cultural values emphasizing the use of a given component is necessary to strengthen the effect of that component. In contrast, within bundles characterized by cultural looseness, we find that the presence of some or even all cultural values that discourage the use of a component actually results in a stronger relationship between that component and creativity. Our study stands to make three main contributions. First, we shift
2. Theoretical background Early cross-cultural research on creativity suggested that individuals in some cultures are inherently more or less creative based on how much their culture emphasizes creativity as an end goal (see Morris & Leung, 2010, and Zhou & Shalley, 2010, for reviews). More recently, however, the observation that individuals display creativity in cultures that do not see creativity as a valued goal has prompted scholars to suggest a different perspective. This perspective shifts the focus from whether a culture enables individuals to achieve creativity or not, to how a culture shapes the way individuals achieve creativity – i.e., to the moderating role of culture (e.g., Erez & Nouri, 2010; Zhou & Shalley, 2010). We adopt this perspective to study how culture moderates the link between each of the three components of creativity – domain-relevant skills, creativity-relevant skills, and task motivation – and creativity. We draw on the toolkit view of culture to develop our theory. 2.1. The toolkit view of culture and creativity components The toolkit view suggests that culture shapes individual action by providing different “components that are used to construct strategies of action” (Swidler, 1986: p. 273). Translating this to creative endeavors, culture determines the set of resources that individuals use to guide and build their creative actions (Becker, 1982; Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986). According to the componential theory of creativity (Amabile & Pratt, 2016; Amabile, 1988, 1996, 2013) – one of the most prominent theoretical frameworks on individual creativity in organizational contexts (Anderson et al., 2014; George, 2007) – the set of resources that individuals draw on to construct creative actions can be classified into three components: domain-relevant skills, creativity-relevant skills, and task motivation (see Table 1). Domain-relevant skills refer to the knowledge, expertise, techniques, and skills associated with the particular domain where the problem-solver is working (Amabile, 1988, 2013). This includes personal attributes such as education, employment, and personal experience (e.g. Madjar, Oldham, & Pratt, 2002; Shin & Zhou, 2007). Creativityrelevant skills refer to the cognitive styles and personality characteristics that are conducive to taking new perspectives on problems
Table 1 Components and sub-components. Component
Definition
Sub-components and examples
Domain-relevant skills
The knowledge, expertise, techniques, and skills associated with a particular domain where the problem-solver is working.
Creativity-relevant skills
The cognitive styles and personality characteristics that are conducive to independence, risktaking, and taking new perspectives on problems.
Task motivation
The internal drive to solve a problem or undertake a task because it is interesting in itself, rather than driven by extrinsic motives such as rewards and surveillance.
2
- Education (e.g., Burt, 2004; Madjar & OrtizWalters, 2008) - Expertise (e.g., Liao et al., 2010; Obstfeld, 2005) - Knowledge diversity (e.g., Perry-Smith, 2006; Shin & Zhou, 2007) - Affect (Baron & Tang, 2011; George & Zhou, 2007) - Cognitive style (Ford & Gioia, 2000; Tierney et al., 1999) - Personality characteristics (e.g., Baer, 2010; George & Zhou, 2001) - Intrinsic motivation (e.g., Grant & Barry, 2011; Sauermann & Cohen, 2010). - Self-efficacy (e.g., Tierney & Farmer, 2002, 2011)
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
(Amabile, 1988, 2013), such as flexible thinking and openness to experience (Barron & Harrington, 1981; Campbell, 1960; George & Zhou, 2001; Smith, Ward, & Finke, 1995). Task motivation refers to the internal drive to solve a problem or undertake a creative task because it is interesting in itself, rather than being driven by extrinsic motives such as rewards and surveillance (Amabile, 1988, 2013). It is not restricted to intrinsic motivation, but also encompasses other types of task motivation like self-efficacy – i.e. a belief in one’s ability to perform a specific task (Bandura, 1986; Tierney & Farmer, 2002). The toolkit view of culture suggests that different cultures will emphasize with different degrees the use of any given component. As culture imprints a preference for certain resources rather than others (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986), individuals from the same culture should rely on the same component(s) of creativity to construct their creative actions (Hofstede, 1980; Swidler, 1986). In particular, they should rely more strongly on the component(s) emphasized by the culture, using others to a lesser degree (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986).
avoidance do not. Overall, this example illustrates how deriving predictions from cultural values in isolation can lead to partial and even misleading conclusions. Cross-cultural scholars have long been aware of these issues: culture is conceptualized as a set of values and norms (Gelfand et al., 2006; Hofstede, 1980; Morris & Leung, 2010), and is thus by definition a multi-dimensional concept (Taras, Kirkman, & Steel, 2010). Consequently, “there are no compelling theoretical reasons to suspect that cultural values operate independently to influence outcomes” (Kirkman et al., 2006: p. 311). However, the vast majority of studies on culture and creativity has focused on examining the moderating effects of one cultural dimension (e.g., Erez & Nouri, 2010; Goncalo & Staw, 2006; Hu, Erdogan, Jiang, Bauer, & Liu, 2018), with only a handful examining the joint effects of two cultural dimensions (e.g., Liu et al., 2016; Nouri et al., 2015). This was most likely due to methodological limitations, because interactions beyond two-way effects become increasingly difficult to theorize, model, and interpret (Fiss, 2011). In order to get closer to a comprehensive theory of culture and creativity, there is thus a need to “broaden our analysis of culture” (Leung et al., 2005: p. 373). To this end, we introduce the concept of cultural bundles, and we argue that looking at cultural bundle characteristics will allow us to identify which component-creativity relationship will be strengthened or weakened in a given country. We define a cultural bundle as a set including the (a) conceptually distinct yet interconnected value dimensions that characterize a given country, and (b) the strength of the norms enforcing these values. In this paper, we conceptualize cultural bundles as composed of (a) individualismcollectivism, power distance, masculinity-femininity, and uncertainty avoidance, and (b) cultural tightness.
2.2. Culture as a bundle of cultural dimensions Consistent with the above, research has shown that cultural values can promote or discourage the development and use of domain-relevant skills (e.g., Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010), creativity-relevant skills (e.g., Gelfand, 2011; Henrich et al., 2010; Markus & Kitayama, 1991), and motivation sources (e.g., Elliot, Chirkov, Kim, & Sheldon, 2001). This research has primarily focused on the role played by the cultural value dimensions of individualism-collectivism, power distance, masculinity-femininity, and uncertainty avoidance (Hofstede, 1980). Individualism-collectivism reflects the degree to which a culture emphasizes the “I” relative to the “we” (Brewer & Chen, 2007; Hofstede, 1994; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Power distance is defined as the extent to which a society accepts that power in institutions and organizations is distributed unequally (Hofstede, 1980). Masculinityfemininity is defined as the degree to which a culture values assertiveness and the acquisition of things, e.g. money, expertise, etc. Uncertainty avoidance refers to the degree to which an individual feels uncomfortable in uncertain and ambiguous situations (Hofstede, 1980). Table 2 summarizes how extant research would predict the extent to which each cultural value dimension promotes or discourages the development and use of different creativity components.1 We can see from the table that considering the effect of one cultural value dimension in isolation produces conflicting expectations about the degree to which a country promotes the use of each component. As an example, consider domain-relevant skills within China and the United States. China is characterized by collectivism, high power distance, moderate masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance; whereas the United States is characterized by individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. By focusing only on individualism, we would expect the use of domain-relevant skills to be promoted in China but discouraged in the United States. However, considering only power distance we would draw a different conclusion: the use of domain-relevant skills should be discouraged in China but promoted in the United States. Taking into account all the cultural dimensions in conjunction presents yet another picture, with both countries being equally likely to promote (or discourage) the relationship between domain-relevant skills and creativity. China’s culture includes two value dimensions emphasizing the use of domain-relevant skills (i.e., collectivism and masculinity), and two value dimensions that do not (i.e., high power distance and low uncertainty avoidance). The same is true for the United States: low power distance and masculinity emphasize the use of domain-relevant skills and individualism and low uncertainty
2.3. The moderating effect of cultural bundles configurations The toolkit view suggests that individuals from the same culture will rely more on the component(s) normatively emphasized by that culture, ignoring or using the other components to a lesser degree (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986). However, what “normatively emphasized” means varies significantly across countries depending on the strength and the stability of the norms enforcing cultural values – i.e., on the country’s cultural tightness (Gelfand et al., 2006). According to the toolkit view, in fact, individuals within tight cultures internalize “what is accepted and acceptable” and would not consider developing and using resources that they do not “experience as being consistent with what is societally acceptable” (Peterson, 2016: p. 36). On the contrary, within loose cultures – characterized by less stable norms and lower accountability (Gelfand et al., 2006) – individuals have much more discretion in their choice of resources. Consequently, they can choose to rely on a given component without the need for a strong cultural rationale defending its legitimacy (Peterson, 2016). Following this logic, within cultural bundles that include cultural tightness the relationship between a given component and creativity should become stronger as the number of cultural values that emphasize the use of that component increases. In tight cultures, individuals will be afraid of incurring sanctions, and will thus need strong signals in order to be able to make sense of the situation (Leonardi, 2011; Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986) and apply a given component to creative endeavors. Mixed signals on whether or not to use a specific component will likely result in individuals’ inability or unwillingness to use that component, and consequently a weaker effect. Conversely, cultural bundles that include cultural looseness will engender less concerns of being sanctioned (Gelfand et al., 2006). While individuals will probably still need some type of signal, the higher sense of freedom and autonomy experienced in loose cultures (Chua, Roth, & Lemoine, 2015; Gelfand et al., 2006) means that this signal can be significantly weaker (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986). Individuals could apply a given component to creative endeavors, thereby strengthening its relationship with creativity, even if only some cultural values
1 We will elaborate on the content of Table 2 and on the specific relationship between each value dimension and component in our hypothesis development below.
3
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 2 Cultural dimensions and emphasis on creativity components. Domain-relevant Skills
Creativity-relevant Skills
Task Motivation
Individualism
Development and use Less emphasized Reason Preference for knowledge transferable across contexts, rather than for domain-specific knowledge and information. Supporting research Cousins, 1989; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1984
Development and use More emphasized Reason Preference for independent thinking, risk taking, and divergent thinking in order to distinguish themselves from others. Supporting research Erez & Nouri, 2010; Eylon & Au, 1999; Hofstede et al., 2010
Development and use More emphasized Reason Emphasis on intrinsic drivers. Moreover, preference for drawing on self-efficacy, especially when performing tasks individually. Supporting research Earley, 1994; Eaton & Dembo, 1997; Elliot et al., 2001; Lam et al., 2002; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Schwarzer et al., 1997
Power distance
Development and use Less emphasized Reason Lower likelihood to activate a learning orientation, to seek feedback, and to actively look for information. Supporting research Taras et al., 2010; Tett & Burnett, 2003
Development and use Less emphasized Reason Preference for avoiding both standing out and displays of openness to experience/independent thinking. Supporting research Becker, 2012; Eylon & Au, 1999; Harzing & Hofstede, 1996; Hofstede & McCrae, 2004; Westwood & Low, 2003
Development and use More emphasized Reason Displays of high level of intrinsic motivation in response to goals set by organizations. Higher likelihood to develop self-efficacy. Supporting research Erez & Earley, 1987; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002
Masculinity
Development and use More emphasized Reason Preference for displays of expertise to signal competence and distinguish oneself from others.
Development and use More emphasized Reason Preference for displays of openness to experience and divergent thinking.
Supporting research Hofstede, 1980
Supporting research Hofstede & McCrae, 2004
Development and use More emphasized Reason Enhanced intrinsic desire to compete and distinguish themselves from others by mastering a task. Supporting research Hofstede, 1980, 1994; Taras et al., 2010
Development and use More emphasized Reason Preference for the attainment of expertise and for accumulating sufficient and appropriate knowledge to generate clearly defined solutions. Supporting research Hofstede, 1980; Kruglanski & Freund, 1983; Kruglanski & Webster, 1996
Development and use Less emphasized Reason Preference for well-defined problem procedures and restraint from exhibiting divergent thinking and risk-taking. Supporting research Erez & Nouri, 2010; Eylon & Au, 1999; Harzing & Hofstede, 1996; Hofstede et al., 2010; Rank et al., 2004; Rietzschel et al., 2007; Westwood & Low, 2003
Uncertainty Avoidance
Development and use Less emphasized Reason Higher motivation to avoid negative outcomes rather than to approach a task for its intrinsic interest or challenge. Supporting research Hofstede, 1980
through the collection of feedback and information (Taras et al., 2010). Third, masculinity encourages distinctiveness, and the display of expertise and knowledge variety acts as a signal of competence that helps individuals stand out from the crowd (Hofstede, 1980). Conversely, femininity encourages humility, and therefore discourages self-interested displays of knowledge (Hofstede, 1980). Finally, high uncertainty avoidance promotes the attainment of expertise (Hofstede, 1980) and imprints a preference for accumulating sufficient and appropriate knowledge to generate clearly defined solutions (Kruglanski & Freund, 1983; Kruglanski & Webster, 1996). Conversely, people in low uncertainty avoidant countries are less dependent on existing knowledge to take action and make decisions (Hofstede, 1980). Considered together, this evidence leads us to predict2:
promote its development and use. Consequently, while extant research does not allow us to identify ex ante a specific configuration for these bundles, we expect that only a few cultural values promoting the use of a given component could be sufficient to strengthen the componentscreativity relationships when the cultural bundle includes cultural looseness. In the next sections, we build on this reasoning and on extant research on how each cultural value promotes or discourages the use of each component (summarized in Table 2) to develop our hypotheses. 2.3.1. Domain-relevant skills Extant research suggests that the use of domain-relevant skills, and thus its effect on creativity, is fostered by the cultural value dimensions of collectivism, low power distance, masculinity, and high uncertainty avoidance. First, collectivism promotes the development and use of knowledge that is “specific to the focal context” (Markus & Kitayama, 1991: p. 231), and the development of a rich and elaborated knowledge store (Shweder & Bourne, 1984). Conversely, individualism promotes the acquisition of knowledge that is transferable across contexts, rather than domain-specific descriptions and information (Cousins, 1989; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1984). Second, low power distance promotes learning because the lack of strong, steep hierarchies encourages individuals’ exploration and acquisition of knowledge. On the contrary, high power distance is less likely to activate a learning orientation because high hierarchical distance and centralized decision-making are likely to stifle curiosity (Tett & Burnett, 2003). Moreover, high power distance discourages knowledge search
Hypothesis 1. Within bundles that include cultural tightness, the
2 Developing configurational hypotheses requires developing hypotheses about "how multiple theoretical attributes will combine (conjunctural causality), what different combinations will comprise multiple pathways to the outcome (equifinality), and/or how both the presence and absence of particular attributes may lead to the outcome (causal asymmetry)" (Misangyi et al., 2017: p. 269). Our hypotheses follow this logic by indicating that different configurations of cultural dimensions, characterized by the presence of a larger vs. lower number of cultural values emphasizing a given component, can result in that component having a stronger effect (conjunctural causality); and that a given component can have stronger effects through different cultural bundle configurations (equifinality).
4
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
positive effect of domain-relevant skills on creativity will become stronger as the number of cultural value dimensions in the bundle that promote the development and use of domain-relevant skills (i.e., collectivism, low power distance, masculinity, and high uncertainty avoidance) increases.
actions. Finally, low uncertainty avoidance promotes the motivation to approach a task for intrinsic interest or challenge because it reduces the fear of failure. In contrast, high uncertainty avoidance promotes the motivation to avoid negative outcomes, thus discouraging the motivation to approach a task for its intrinsic challenges (Hofstede, 1980). Overall, this evidence leads us to predict:
2.3.2. Creativity-relevant skills Extant research suggests that the cultural value dimensions emphasizing the development and use of creativity-relevant skills, and thus strengthening the effect of this component on creativity, are individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. First, individualistic cultures promote the use of creativityrelevant skills such as independent thinking (Eylon & Au, 1999), risk taking (Hofstede et al., 2010), and divergent thinking (Erez & Nouri, 2010) to gain distinction from others. Conversely, collectivistic cultures are less likely to value the display of such skills to avoid incurring social sanctions for “standing out” (Goncalo & Staw, 2006). Second, low power distance encourages the expression of deviant opinions, curiosity, and independent thinking. In contrast, high power-distant cultures discourage these expressions to avoid standing out (Becker, 2012; Eylon & Au, 1999; Harzing & Hofstede, 1996; Hofstede & McCrae, 2004; Westwood & Low, 2003). Third, masculine cultures promote the display of creativity-relevant skills such as openness to experience and divergent thinking. In contrast, feminine cultures discourage those (Hofstede & McCrae, 2004). Finally, low uncertainty avoidance encourages the development and exhibition of divergent thinking and risk-taking (Erez & Nouri, 2010; Eylon & Au, 1999; Harzing & Hofstede, 1996; Westwood & Low, 2003). In contrast, high uncertainty avoidance imprints a strong preference for well-defined, low-risk problem procedures (Rank, Pace, & Frese, 2004; Rietzschel, De Dreu, & Nijstad, 2007). Considered together, this evidence leads us to predict:
Hypothesis 3. Within bundles that include cultural tightness, the positive effect of task motivation on creativity will become stronger as the number of cultural value dimensions in the bundle that promote the development and use of task motivation (i.e. individualism, high power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance) increases.
3. Methods We adopted a multi-method approach to test our hypotheses on the moderating effects of the configuration of cultural bundles on the components-creativity relationships. We first used artifact-corrected meta-analyses (ACMA, Hunter & Schmidt, 2004) to identify the main effect of each creativity component on creativity, as well as differences in these effects across different countries. We then used Hierarchical Meta-Analytical Regression Analysis (HMARA) in the form of a threelevel, variance-known meta-analytical regression (see Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002, and Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, & Congdon, 2004), to test our hypotheses on the interaction between whether the bundle included cultural tightness-looseness and the number of cultural values that promote the use of that component included in the bundle. This analysis represents a first, simplified test of our configurational hypotheses, in that it treats all variables as binaries and separates tightness from the other cultural dimensions.3 We then used fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fs/QCA) to test more precisely our hypotheses and to identify the specific configurations of values and tightness-looseness that are associated with stronger effects for each component-creativity relationship – something that cannot be done in meta-analytic regression. While fs/QCA methods have traditionally been used for inductive theorizing, they are increasingly used to develop and test configurational hypotheses (e.g., Bell, Filatotchev, & Aguilera, 2014; see Misangyi et al., 2017, and Greckhamer, Furnari, Fiss, & Aguilera, 2018, for reviews). This method, in fact, “lends itself both to an a priori model specification and an inductive exploration” (Misangyi et al., 2017: p. 268). Thus, it is particularly well-suited for addressing theoretical puzzles characterized by complex relationships between predictors and outcomes (Fiss, Sharapov, & Cronqvist, 2013; Greckhamer, Misangyi, Elms, & Lacey, 2008). We deductively hypothesized the configurations that would strengthen the effect of each component on creativity within bundles that include cultural tightness. On the other hand, extant theory does not provide adequate evidence to hypothesize ex ante the optimal configuration of cultural values for bundles that include cultural looseness. fs/QCA enables us to both test our hypotheses and inductively explore the configurations leading to stronger (and weaker) components-creativity relationships.
Hypothesis 2. Within bundles that include cultural tightness, the positive effect of creativity-relevant skills on creativity will become stronger as the number of cultural value dimensions in the bundle that promote the development and use of creativity-relevant skills (i.e. individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance) increases.
2.3.3. Task motivation Extant findings suggest that the cultural value dimensions emphasizing the use of task motivation, and thus strengthening the effect of this component on creativity, are individualism, high power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. First, individualistic cultures promote self-efficacy to a greater extent than collectivistic cultures (Earley, 1994; Lam, Chen, & Schaubroeck, 2002; Schwarzer, Bäßler, Kwiatek, Schröder, & Zhang, 1997), especially when performing tasks individually, as is the case with idea generation (PerrySmith & Mannucci, 2017). Conversely, collectivistic cultures discourage individual initiative in order to prevent negative outcomes (Eaton & Dembo, 1997; Elliot et al., 2001; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Second, high power distance cultures encourage self-efficacy when executing tasks (Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002), as well as the display of higher levels of intrinsic motivation in response to goals set by organizations or superiors (Erez & Earley, 1987; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999) because they provide normative expectations for task execution and goal attainment coming from an authoritative source. In contrast, low power distance cultures are less likely to promote self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation when assigned a certain goal or task (Erez & Earley, 1987; Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002). Third, masculine cultures promote competition and distinction from others, whereas feminine cultures discourage competition because it undermines social harmony (Hofstede, 1980, 1994; Taras et al., 2010). Thus, individuals in feminine cultures are less likely to be motivated by the mastery of the task itself and are less likely to develop task motivation and to draw on it when constructing their creative
3.1. Meta-analytic approach Meta-analysis is a quantitative method used to integrate research evidence from prior studies (Hedges & Olkin, 1985), but it is also wellsuited for extending theory. It allows for building on existing empirical work to model differences across-studies that would otherwise be too complex to model in primary studies. 3 A more appropriate test would require a five-way interaction, which would be virtually impossible to interpret. This is one of the reasons why fs/QCA provides a superior test when it comes to testing configurational theory and hypotheses (see Fiss, 2007, and Misangyi et al., 2017, for more details).
5
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
qualitative studies, and papers focusing on the team or organization levels of analysis. Second, a study had to contain an effect size estimate either in the form of a bivariate correlation, information that would allow for the calculation of a bivariate correlation, or any effect size (e.g. t-values) that allows for the calculation of a partial correlation (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004; Rosenthal, 1991). Third, the effect size had to refer to creativity within an organizational setting, as the componential theory of creativity was specifically developed to understand creativity in organizations (Amabile & Pratt, 2016; Amabile, 1988). Fourth, we focused only on variables that were theoretically expected to have a positive effect on creativity according to the componential theory of creativity5 (see Table 1 for the different operationalizations of each component of creativity). Fifth, we excluded studies with a sample drawn from multiple countries, or where the country was not specified. Our approach led to a final sample of 584 effect sizes from 205 studies (see Appendix A for the list of included studies and Appendix B for excluded studies). 566 of these effect sizes were bivariate correlations (96.92% of our sample). As each study can contain more than one test of the focal relationships, the total number of effect sizes exceeds the number of studies.
We started our analysis by using ACMA to uncover the effect of each component on creativity in organizational settings. We chose ACMA because it is particularly suitable for primary data collected from survey methods – which was the case for the majority of our sample – as it allows us to correct statistical artifacts such as measurement errors in the independent and dependent variables. We corrected for measurement errors by using the Cronbach’s alphas provided in primary studies. We calculated effect sizes using bivariate correlations drawn from correlation matrices and partial correlations drawn from regression models. We used partial correlations to calculate the effect sizes only when the article did not include a bivariate correlation matrix. We obtained 584 effect sizes from 205 studies and 215 independent samples (N = 656,254) drawn from 38 countries. 566 effect sizes came from bivariate correlations. As the test for heterogeneity in the retrieved mean effect size distribution was significant, we modeled the heterogeneity in effect sizes through meta-analytical regression analyses. Given that our moderators and some of our control variables are at the country or industry level, whereas our dependent variable is at the sample level, we used a random effects three-level, variance-known hierarchical meta-analytical regression analysis (HMARA) using HLM software (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Raudenbush et al., 2004). This method is more robust than traditional meta-analytic regressions in the presence of multilevel data structures (Fischer & Mansell, 2009; Konstantopoulos, 2011).
3.2.1. Coding and data set We followed a two-step coding procedure. First, the second and third author coded the effect sizes into the three individual-level creativity components6 based on the definitions provided by Amabile (2013). They coded each effect size into each component of creativity, bearing in mind that each component has been measured through different constructs (the sub-components) that reflect its theoretical description. For example, domain-relevant skills have been measured as education (e.g., Burt, 2004), expertise (e.g., Mannucci & Yong, 2018), and knowledge diversity (e.g., Shin & Zhou, 2007). We measured interrater agreement by computing the Cohen’s kappa (Cohen, 1960) and obtained a coefficient of 0.88, which is considered almost perfect agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977). In the few cases where disagreement arose, the authors resolved it through discussion.
3.2. Artifact-corrected meta-analysis Literature search. We used two complementary literature retrieval procedures to uncover as many studies as possible that fell within our scope of inquiry (White, 1994). We examined numerous electronic databases, including EBSCOHost, Emerald, Factiva, Google Scholar, JSTOR, ProQuest, PsycINFO, ScienceDirect, Sage Full-Text Collections, and Wiley InterScience to uncover studies published between 1950 (year of the first seminal publication on creativity in American Psychologist by J. P. Guilford(Guilford, 1950) and 2018 by using the keywords “creative” and “creativity”. As a first selection criterion, we decided to focus on studies measuring creativity, excluding studies measuring innovation. Creativity and innovation are two different constructs: creativity refers to the generation of novel and useful ideas or solutions, whereas innovation refers to the implementation of creative ideas or solutions (Amabile, 1996; Fleming, Mingo, & Chen, 2007). Creativity and innovation, however, are often used interchangeably. Moreover, some objective measures of creativity, for example patents, are also used to measure innovation. We therefore included studies that, while using the label “innovation” in theory building, were actually using scales that measure creativity. As a second step in the search, we used a snowballing approach to backward-trace all references reported in the studies identified in the first step and in review articles on creativity in order to check for any studies not yet included in our sample. This approach resulted in an initial sample of 668 published studies.4 Finally, we used five heuristics to determine which studies to include in our final sample (cf. Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). First, a study had to report at least one relationship between an operationalization of a component of creativity and an operationalization of creativity at the individual level of analysis. Thus, we excluded theory papers, reviews,
3.3. Hierarchical meta-analytic regression analysis We used hierarchical meta-analytic regression analysis (HMARA) for two purposes. First, we tested the moderating effect of each cultural value dimension as a basis for discussing the potential effects of isolated cultural values vis-à-vis our cultural bundles. Second, we tested our hypotheses on the moderating effect of cultural bundles. Three-level HMARA allows for testing of variance at the third level while taking into account the between-study variance of the study-specific estimates of effect sizes (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Konstantopoulos, 2011; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). In our case, effect sizes drawn from the same sample within a study are nested at level 2. Studies conducted in the same country are nested together at level 3. Three-level HMARA models assume that the variance of each effect size is known and are thus referred to as “variance known” hierarchical linear models (Fischer & Mansell, 2009). This method uses full maximum likelihood estimation in order to test for the average effect size across studies while trying to explain and estimate their variance using 5 The componential theory of creativity is theorized from a Western perspective. Given our research objectives, choosing only factors that the componential theory of creativity theorizes to have a positive effect was warranted in order to check (a) whether the theorized effects are indeed present in all Western cultures and (b) if and how their effects are generalizable to other cultures. 6 While included in the componential model, Amabile conceptualizes affect as a separate, “non-componential” antecedent (Amabile, 2013; see also Amabile & Mueller, 2008). For the purpose of this paper, we categorized affect as a creativity-relevant skill, following Amabile et al. (2005), as it “broadens a person’s repertoire of cognitions and action” (Amabile et al., 2005, p. 395).
4 Following past meta-analyses (e.g., Crook, Ketchen, Combs, & Todd, 2008; Dalton & Dalton, 2005), we excluded non-published studies because the quality of non-peer-reviewed studies is questionable, and a call for non-published papers cannot guarantee a representative sample of the population of non-published studies. Moreover, evidence suggests that excluding non-published studies does not influence the consistency and efficiency of meta-analytic findings (Dalton et al., 2012). However, we report robustness tests on publication bias below.
6
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
measured as only novelty (coded 1) or as a combination of novelty and usefulness (coded 0). Second, we included a dummy variable for whether creativity was measured subjectively (coded 1) or objectively (coded 0). Following a recent review of methods used by contemporary organizational creativity researchers (Amabile & Mueller, 2008), we coded as subjective the self-assessments of creativity and all those measures in which experts, supervisors, or peers use rating-scales to make subjective judgments of the creativity of a person, process, or outcome. Third, we created a same source variable to check whether both the independent variable (i.e., the creativity component) and the dependent variable were assessed by the same source (coded 1) or by different sources (coded 0). We also controlled whether the study was based solely on primary data sources. For organizational characteristics, we coded for whether the study sample was drawn from a private or public organization, with mixed samples as the reference category; and whether the sample was drawn from a small or large organization, with large organization as the reference category.7
the independent variables (for a more in-depth description of the procedure, see Fischer & Mansell, 2009, and Konstantopoulos, 2011). We grand-mean centered all continuous variables, while leaving dummy variables uncentered. We ran analyses on separate sub-samples for each of the creativity components in order to understand the effect of cultural bundles on each component separately. 3.3.1. Level 3 – country level variables At level 3, we included the cultural value dimensions of individualism, uncertainty avoidance, power distance, and masculinity (Hofstede, 1980, 2001). We also included cultural tightness as a measure of the strength of these values (Gelfand, 2011). Using Hofstede’s updated cultural value dimensions scores (Hofstede et al., 2010) and cultural tightness-looseness scores from Gelfand (2011), we assigned each study its relative score on each dimension based on the country from which primary data was drawn. For countries where the cultural tightness score was unknown (e.g., Taiwan), we assigned the mean score of its relative country cluster as identified by Ronen and Shenkar (2013). To test our hypotheses, we also created a variable that represents the number of cultural value dimensions within each country’s bundle that theoretically expected to promote the use of a given component which we label number of “promoting” cultural values. We created this variable following a two-step procedure. First, we classified the cultural values of each country based on whether the country score on each cultural value dimension was above or below the commonly used cutoff point of 50 (Hofstede, 1980, 2001). For example, if a country scored 25 on the individualism-collectivism scale, it would be classified as collectivist. Second, we classified cultural values as “promoting” based on our predictions in Table 2. For example, collectivism would be classified as “promoting” for domain-relevant skills, and “discouraging” for creativity-relevant skills. As a result of this procedure, the variable ranges from zero (none of the cultural values in the bundle promotes the use of that component) to four (all the cultural values in the bundle promote the use of that component). We also created a dummy variable, tight culture, that took the value of 1 when the cultural tightness score of the country was higher than 6.5 (the average value reported by Gelfand, 2011), and 0 when it was lower. We then included the interaction between these two variables to test our hypotheses. Finally, we use the fs/QCA-derived cultural bundles that showed the strongest mean effect sizes for each componentcreativity relationship to calculate the configurational minimum fit scores (Fiss, 2011; Meuer, Rupietta, & Backes-Gellner, 2015 – see Appendix C for more details on their calculation). We added these scores as predictors – cultural bundle fit – to the HMARA to further test our theory that the cultural bundles configurations significantly moderate the components-creativity relationships.
3.4. Fuzzy-set qualitative comparative analysis To further understand the moderating effect of cultural bundles on the components–creativity relationships, we supplemented our metaanalysis with a configurational approach (Fiss, 2011; Ragin, 2008; see Joshi, Son, & Roh, 2015, and Miron-Spektor, Erez, & Naveh, 2011, for similar combinations of meta-analysis/“classic” statistical methods with fs/QCA). While multivariate regression models “treat variables as competing in explaining variation in outcomes rather than showing how variables combine to create outcomes” (Fiss, 2007: 1181), a configurational approach allows for the possibility that combinations of variables (referred to as conditions in fs/QCA), rather than any condition in isolation, lead to a certain outcome, and that many different paths can lead to the same outcome (i.e., equifinality). It is thus ideal for testing how multiple variables jointly influence a phenomenon of interest (Fiss, 2011; Greckhamer et al., 2018), and for testing our theory on the multidimensional nature of culture, allowing us to identify which cultural bundle configurations strengthen or weaken the components-creativity relationships. 3.4.1. Calibrating the data A crucial step in fs/QCA studies is the calibration of the causal conditions (Ragin & Rihoux, 2009). Calibration is the process of determining each case’s membership in the sets representing the outcome and conditions (Greckhamer et al., 2018: p. 488). For our data, this means classifying which countries have similar cultures (and thus belong to the same membership set) and how this relates to the strength of the components-creativity relationships. During calibration one identifies thresholds that meaningfully represent differences in membership or non-membership. This identification is based on theory and criteria external to the study’s sample (Greckhamer et al., 2018; Misangyi et al., 2017), as sample-based calibration “should be avoided whenever possible” (Greckhamer et al., 2018: p. 489). Following this reasoning, we calibrated Hofstede’s cultural value dimensions and cultural tightness and their midpoints, high points, and low points based on the most recent scales of each dimension (Gelfand, 2011; Hofstede et al., 2010). More details on this procedure are available in Appendix C. We calibrated our outcome (i.e. the effect size of each component in each country) following the recommended best practice to set the mid-point as the median score, adding one standard deviation for the high-point and subtracting one standard deviation for the low-point (Greckhamer et al., 2008).
3.3.2. Level 2 – study level variables For publication characteristics, we included the publication year and whether the journal where the study was published is included in the Financial Times list (1 yes, 0 no). 3.3.3. Level 1 – effect size level variables We included dummy variables for the different operationalizations of each creativity component to identify differences in effect sizes across sub-components. We followed the taxonomy presented in Table 1. For domain-relevant skills, we included dummy variables for knowledge diversity and expertise, with education as the reference category. For creativity-relevant skills, we included dummy variables for cognitive style and personality characteristics, with affect as the reference category. Finally, for task motivation, we included a dummy variable for intrinsic motivation, with self-efficacy as the reference category. We also included a set of moderators to control for non-hypothesized differences in effect sizes due to methodological specifications. First, we included a dummy variable measuring whether creativity was
7 These variables (i.e., primary data source, private/public organization, small or large organization) are at the effect size level. A study can in fact contain multiple samples, and hence differ on these variables at the effect size level rather than at the study level.
7
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
4.1.1. Robustness checks While meta-analysis best practices support the appropriateness of including only published papers in meta-analytic samples (Dalton, Aguinis, Dalton, Bosco, & Pierce, 2012), we conducted further analyses to rule out the possibility that publication bias was affecting our estimates (Suurmond, van Rhee, & Hak, 2017). We used Duval and Tweedie (2000) trim-and-fill method to test for the potential effect of publication bias. For domain-relevant skills, we find 13 imputed studies following the trim-and-fill method, resulting in a drop of the mean rho from 0.14 to 0.11. The failsafe-Ns (the number of studies necessary to turn a mean rho insignificant) following Rosenthal’s, Orwin’s and Fisher’s calculations were respectively equal to 20446, 349, and 1397. For creativity-relevant skills, no studies were imputed, resulting in a non-adjusted mean rho. The failsafe-Ns were 43151, 1207, and 1539, respectively. Finally, for task motivation we again find no imputed studies, with failsafe-Ns being 7244, 538, and 600. Overall, our results do not seem to be affected by publication bias.
3.4.2. Analysis We followed the three-step procedure proposed by Fiss (2011) to conduct our fs/QCA analyses. The first step is to construct a truth matrix that displays all the potential combinations of the conditions. The second step requires the researcher to indicate (a) the minimum number of cases that ought to be present for a configuration to be considered, and (b) the minimum consistency levels8 of those configurations (Ragin & Rihoux, 2009). As we only have a limited number of cases (i.e., countries), we set to 1 the minimum number of observable cases for including a configuration (Ragin & Rihoux, 2009). In the third step, Boolean algebra is used to reduce the number of truth table rows to simplified combinations. The analysis then produces configurations (i.e. cultural bundles) that show stronger and weaker componentscreativity relationships. We used fs/QCA 3.0 software to analyze our data.
4. Results 4.2. Hierarchical meta-analytic regressions results We structured our results section as follows. First, we present the results of our ACMA analysis, showing the average effect size of each component across and within countries. Second, we present our HMARA results, where we show the predictive strength of cultural value dimensions in isolation, the main effects of the number of promoting cultural values and tight cultures, and we look at the interaction between the latter two to test our hypotheses. Third, we present the results of our fs/QCA analysis to further test our hypotheses. We also inductively discuss the theoretical implications of the observed patterns of complementarity among cultural value dimensions, as it is customary when using configurational approaches such as fs/QCA, even when adopting a deductive hypothesis-testing approach (e.g., Bell et al., 2014; Fiss, 2011; Misangyi et al., 2017). Finally, we go back to the HMARA results where we show the relative predictive strength of the cultural bundles variable in predicting a stronger relationship between each component and creativity.
Tables 5–7 report HMARA results for each component. For each set of analyses, Model 1 includes the control variables, Model 2 adds the cultural value dimensions and cultural tightness, and Model 3 introduces the number of promoting cultural value dimensions and the dummy variable of cultural tightness. In Model 4 we add the interaction term.9 Model 2 (Table 5) shows that the effect of domain-relevant skills on creativity is not significantly influenced by cultural value dimensions or cultural tightness. Model 2 (Table 6) shows non-significant effects of cultural value dimensions on the creativity-relevant skills-creativity relationship, while cultural tightness has a significant negative effect (p < .05). Finally, Model 2 (Table 7) shows that the effect of task motivation is not affected by cultural value dimensions nor cultural tightness. Model 3 (Tables 5–7) shows that the coefficients of the number of promoting cultural value dimensions and of the cultural tightness dummy are not significant for any component. This suggests that the two variables do not have independent effects on the components-creativity relationships. Model 4 (Tables 5–7) shows the results of the interaction between the number of promoting cultural value dimensions and the cultural tightness dummy. The interaction effect is positive and significant for domain-relevant skills (Table 5; p < 0.01) and task motivation (Table 7; p < 0.05) while it is positive but not significant for creativity-relevant skills (Table 6). Hence, our HMARA supports Hypotheses 1 and 3, but not Hypothesis 2. Given the complexity of the relationships characterizing cultural bundles, we conduct a more precise test of our hypotheses using fs/QCA.
4.1. Artifact-corrected meta-analysis results Although our study does not focus on the differences in the relative effect size for each of the components of creativity, the observed pattern of ACMA results is interesting. The effect of each of the components is substantially different (see Table 3). The task motivation component has the largest effect size (ρ = 0.32), followed by the creativity-relevant skills component (ρ = 0.25). The domain-relevant skills component has the smallest effect size on creativity with a mean rho of 0.14. The mean rhos for each sub-component are reported in Table 3 and support the appropriateness of our theory-driven aggregations: the effect sizes of each sub-component are not significantly different from the others belonging to the same component, as indicated by the overlap in the confidence intervals. Table 4 reports the mean effect sizes of each component within the countries included in the sample. We see significant differences in the strength of each component both within and across cultures.
4.3. Results of the fs/QCA analyses 4.3.1. Truth tables and bundles included in our sample Figs. 1–3 present Venn diagrams that are the graphical representation of the truth tables for each component. A truth table presents all possible cultural bundles configurations. Each cell in the Figures describes a specific bundle characterized by the presence (score above the cutoff point – see Appendix C for more details; indicated by 1) or absence (score below the cutoff point; indicated by 0) of each of the cultural dimensions in our analysis. In our case, “absent” means that the opposite end of the cultural value continuum is present: for example, the absence of individualism implies the presence of collectivism. The cultural dimensions are presented in the following order (as indicated in the top row of the diagram): power distance, individualism-collectivism, masculinity-femininity, uncertainty avoidance, tightness-
8 Consistency is the proportion of cases that display a particular configuration that leads to a specific outcome (in our case, a stronger/weaker componentcreativity relationship), divided by the number of cases that have the same configuration but do not achieve the same outcome. It represents “how closely a perfect subset relation is approximated” (Ragin, 2008: p. 44), and is analogous to the assessment of significance in regression analysis (Misangyi et al., 2017). In fs/QCA it is also important to consider PRI (proportional reduction in inconsistency) to avoid simultaneously having subset relations of configurations in both the outcome and its absence (Greckhamer et al., 2018). Following established best practices, we set the consistency threshold at 0.80 and PRI to 0.70 (Fiss, 2011; Greckhamer et al., 2018).
9
8
Model 5 includes a supplementary analysis that we illustrate on p. 34.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 3 ACMA results – average effect of creativity components.a K
N
Mean ρ
wsd ρ
CI ρ 95%
Q
p
I2
Full
584
1,277,294
0.23
0.11
0.22:0.24
15683.45
0.00
96.28%
Domain skills Education Expertise Knowledge diversity
185 103 50 32
1,150,325 30,108 540,378 579,839
0.14 0.10 0.21 0.15
0.07 0.14 0.08 0.03
0.13:0.16 0.08:0.13 0.13:0.28 0.12:0.18
5478.46 565.78 3380.42 361.91
0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
96.64% 81.97% 98.55% 91.43%
Creativity skills Cognitive style Personality Affect
300 129 130 41
90,205 51,248 31,256 7701
0.25 0.22 0.27 0.27
0.21 0.16 0.22 0.31
0.23:0.28 0.19:0.25 0.23:0.31 0.17:0.37
3753.37 1218.00 1544.85 719.61
0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
92.03% 89.49% 91.65% 94.44%
Task motivation Intrinsic motivation Self-efficacy
99 71 28
36,764 28,007 8757
0.32 0.32 0.33
0.22 0.22 0.22
0.28:0.37 0.26:0.37 0.25:0.41
1759.18 1350.42 406.23
0.00 0.00 0.00
94.43% 94.82% 93.35%
a K = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; Mean ρ = average effect size; wsd ρ = weighted standard deviation of mean ρ; CI mean ρ 95% = 95% confidence interval for mean ρ; Q = Cochran’s homogeneity test statistic; p = probability of Q; I2 = scale-free index of heterogeneity.
looseness. For example, the cell described in the Figures by “10010” represents a bundle that includes high power distance, collectivism, femininity, high uncertainty avoidance, and cultural looseness. Each of the cultural bundles in the Figures represent one or more countries. The countries that are present in our dataset are represented in their respective cells. For example, for domain-relevant skills (Fig. 1) the bundle described above (10010) is represented by Bulgaria, Romania, Slovenia, and Chile. When the cell does not include any country, it means that the cultural bundle identified by that configuration is not present in our dataset. We highlighted those cultural bundles empirically found to strengthen (light grey) and weaken (dark grey) the relationship between each component and individual creativity. We also highlighted the theoretically optimal configurations with four values promoting the use of each component (light grey grid). It is important to note that for creativity-relevant skills our dataset includes only one country (the UK – see Fig. 2) whose cultural bundle includes tightness and all the four values expected to promote the use of that component. We thus expect the UK to have the strongest creativity-relevant skills – creativity relationship. However, for both domain-relevant skills and task motivation our dataset does not include any countries whose bundles include tightness and the four “promoting” values (see Figs. 1 and 3). The dataset did include countries whose bundle includes tightness and three “promoting” values (e.g., Mexico for domain-relevant skills, China for task motivation). For these two components, we would thus expect to find these bundles among those with the strongest component-creativity relationship. Finally, it is worth noting that our dataset comprises countries whose bundles include looseness and different numbers of “promoting” values. For example, for creativity-relevant skills we have bundles that include no promoting value (e.g., Bulgaria), one promoting value (e.g., Spain), two (e.g., Israel), three (e.g., Australia) and four (e.g., the US). This diversity in bundles configurations provides us with the opportunity to meaningfully conduct an exploratory analysis on the cultural bundles that include looseness.
denote core features, whereas small circles represent peripheral features.10 When the circles are filled (•) the condition is present; when they are blank with a cross (⊗) the condition is absent. As mentioned above, “absent” in our analysis means that the opposite end of the cultural value continuum is present. The solution for each component may consist of multiple configurations – i.e., there are different configurations of cultural bundles that can lead to a stronger or weaker component-creativity relationship. The “coverage” of each configuration shows the proportion of cases that display that particular configuration. Specifically, unique coverage is the proportion of cases that display only that particular configuration leading to either a stronger or weaker relationship between a given component and creativity. It reflects the relative importance of each configuration in explaining the outcome: the higher the unique coverage, the more important the configuration relative to the others in explaining the stronger/weaker effect of the component on creativity (Ragin, 2008). At the big picture level, Tables 8a and 8b show that there is no single cultural dimension that is always present in bundles that strengthen the components-creativity relationships. The same dimension can be simultaneously included in a bundle that strengthens the component-creativity relationship and another bundle that weakens the component-creativity relationship: its effect depends on the other dimensions included in the bundle. This evidence once more supports our overarching theory that the effect of culture is determined by cultural bundle configurations, rather than cultural dimensions in isolation. 4.3.3. fs/QCA results of bundles that include tightness Panel 1 in Table 8a presents s the three cultural bundles associated with a stronger relationship between the domain-relevant skills component and creativity. Solution 3 is the only one that includes cultural tightness, coupled with collectivism, high power distance, masculinity, and high uncertainty avoidance. Panel 1 in Table 8b presents the one cultural bundle associated with a weaker effect of domain-relevant skills on creativity: this bundle includes tightness, along with individualism, low power distance, femininity, and low uncertainty avoidance. Overall, these results provide further support for Hypothesis 1: the cultural bundle that includes tightness and strengthens the effect of domain-relevant skills on creativity includes three cultural values
4.3.2. fs/QCA analysis Tables 8a and 8b present the findings of our fs/QCA analysis. Each solution reported in these tables represents an alternative configuration of a cultural bundle that strengthens (Table 8a) or weakens (Table 8b) the relationship between a component and creativity. We performed various robustness analyses to ensure the stability of our configurations. These tests confirmed the results presented below, and are reported in Appendix C. We follow the notation introduced by Ragin and Fiss (2008). Blank spaces represent ‘do not care’ situations where the cultural dimension does not play a key role within the configuration in predicting a stronger or weaker component-creativity relationship. Large circles
10 Fs/QCA distinguishes between core and peripheral causal conditions, which allows us to draw conclusions on first-order equifinality (configurations producing similar outcomes in spite of having different core causal conditions) and second-order equifinality (configurations producing similar outcomes with identical core conditions, but dissimilar peripheral conditions; Fiss, 2011). For more details on the distinction between core and peripheral conditions, please see Appendix C.
9
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 4 ACMA results –differences in the effects of components within and across cultures.a K
N
Mean rho
Wsdr
CI rho 95%
Q
P
I2
Full Australia Domain skills Australia Creativity skills Australia Task motivation Australia
3 – 3 –
228 – 228 –
0.55 – 0.55 –
0.18 – 0.18 –
0.30:0.81 – 0.30:0.81 –
7.10 – 7.10 –
0.03 – 0.03 –
71.83% – 71.83% –
Full Austria Domain skills Austria Creativity skills Austria Task motivation Austria
2 – 1 1
512 – 256 256
0.28 – 0.32 0.24
0.04 – – –
0.19:0.37 – – –
0.81 – – –
0.37 – – –
0% – – –
Full Belgium Domain skills Belgium Creativity skills Belgium Task motivation Belgium
2 – – 2
1854 – – 1.854
0.26 – – 0.26
0.04 – – 0.04
0.19:0.33 – – 0.19:0.33
2.60 – – 2.60
0.11 – – 0.11
61.54% – – 61.54%
Full Bhutan Domain skills Bhutan Creativity skills Bhutan Task motivation Bhutan
2 – 2 –
500 – 500 –
0.39 – 0.39 –
0.04 – 0.04 –
0.30:0.47 – 0.30:0.47 –
0.61 – 0.61 –
0.44 – 0.44 –
0% – 0% –
Full Bulgaria Domain skills Bulgaria Creativity skills Bulgaria Task motivation Bulgaria
6 2 3 1
1251 417 682 152
0.21 0.02 0.28 0.40
0.16 0.04 0.12 –
0.07:0.35 −0.08:0.11 0.11:0.44 –
31.50 0.51 9.21 –
0.00 0.48 0.01 –
84.13% 0% 78.28% –
Full Canada Domain skills Canada Creativity skills Canada Task motivation Canada
5 – 5 –
881 – 881 –
0.27 – 0.27 –
0.05 – 0.05 –
0.21:0.34 – 0.21:0.34 –
2.39 – 2.39 –
0.66 – 0.66 –
0% – 0% –
Full Chile Domain skills Chile Creativity skills Chile Task motivation Chile
4 1 3 –
880 220 660 –
0.37 0.17 0.44 –
0.18 – 0.15 –
0.17:0.57 – 0.23:0.64 –
26.75 – 14.29 –
0.00 – 0.00 –
88.79% – 86.00% –
Full China Domain skills China Creativity skills China Task motivation China
73 30 24 19
27,320 10,517 9096 7707
0.25 0.08 0.29 0.44
0.26 0.13 0.27 0.22
0.19:0.31 0.03:0.13 0.18:0.40 0.34:0.55
1853.03 184.32 645.84 362.07
0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
96.11% 84.27% 96.44% 95.03%
Full Colombia Domain skills Colombia Creativity skills Colombia Task motivation Colombia
5 – 5 –
1329 – 1329 –
0.18 – 0.18 –
0.33 – 0.33 –
−0.14:0.50 – −0.14:0.50 –
138.45 – 138.45 –
0.00 – 0.00 –
97.11% – 97.11% –
Full Denmark Domain skills Denmark Creativity skills Denmark Task motivation Denmark
9 – 8 1
1602 – 1528 74
0.19 – 0.17 0.55
0.11 – 0.07 –
0.12:0.27 – 0.12:0.22 –
17.24 – 7.39 –
0.03 – 0.39 –
53.60% – 5.28% –
Full Egypt Domain skills Egypt Creativity skills Egypt Task motivation Egypt
2 1 – 1
404 202 – 202
0.43 0.52 – 0.33
0.10 – – –
0.24:0.61 – – –
3.59 – – –
0.06 – – –
72.14% – – –
Full Finland Domain skills Finland Creativity skills Finland Task motivation Finland
1 – – 1
103 – – 103
0.76 – – 0.76
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
Full France Domain skills France Creativity skills France Task motivation France
2 2 – –
212 212 – –
0.33 0.33 – –
0.07 0.07 – –
0.19:0.47 0.19:0.47 – –
1.01 1.01 – –
0.32 0.32 – –
0.99% 0.99% – –
Full Germany Domain skills Germany Creativity skills Germany Task motivation Germany
17 7 9 1
8827 3667 5024 136
0.20 0.09 0.23 0.54
0.15 0.07 0.18 –
0.12:0.28 0.03:0.16 0.10:0.37 –
203.74 17.36 158.98 –
0.00 0.01 0.00 –
92.15% 65.44% 94.97% –
Full Hong Kong Domain skills Hong Kong Creativity skills Hong Kong Task motivation Hong Kong
5 1 3 1
1599 162 454 983
0.31 0.05 0.35 0.40
0.11 – 0.09 –
0.17:0.44 – 0.23:0.47 –
20.26 – 3.50 –
0.00 – 0.17 –
80.26% – 42.86% –
Full India Domain skills India Creativity skills India Task motivation India
8 4 2 2
2591 1362 514 715
0.35 0.26 0.16 0.70
0.34 0.24 0.05 0.30
0.09:0.60 −0.01:0.53 0.07:0.25 0.10:1.00
292.78 75.52 1.01 63.01
0.00 0.00 0.31 0.00
97.61% 96.03% 0.99% 98.41%
Full Iran Domain skills Iran
5 2
761 170
0.55 0.68
0.24 0.03
0.30:0.79 0.52:0.83
41.28 0.10
0.00 0.75
90.31% 0%
(continued on next page) 10
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 4 (continued) K
N
Mean rho
Wsdr
CI rho 95%
Q
P
I2
Creativity skills Iran Task motivation Iran
2 1
289 302
0.29 0.82
0.09 –
0.10:0.48 –
2.33 –
0.13 –
57.08% –
Full Israel Domain skills Israel Creativity skills Israel Task motivation Israel
16 8 7 1
2924 1515 1207 202
0.21 0.12 0.27 0.62
0.23 0.06 0.27 –
0.10:0.33 0.07:0.17 0.05:0.49 –
148.27 5.82 87.12 –
0.00 0.56 0.00 –
89.88% 0% 93.11% –
Full Italy Domain skills Italy Creativity skills Italy Task motivation Italy
9 5 2 2
3796 2311 893 592
0.19 0.10 0.12 0.45
0.22 0.19 0.04 0.05
0.03:0.35 −0.10:0.30 0.03:0.21 0.33:0.57
180.64 82.05 1.16 1.72
0.00 0.00 0.28 0.19
95.57% 95.12% 13.79% 41.86%
Full Lithuania Domain skills Lithuania Creativity skills Lithuania Task motivation Lithuania
2 – 1 1
244 – 122 122
0.47 – 0.38 0.56
0.09 – – –
0.29:0.65 – – –
1.93 – – –
0.17 – – –
48.19% – – –
Full Malaysia Domain skills Malaysia Creativity skills Malaysia Task motivation Malaysia
4 – 4 –
668 – 668 –
0.22 – 0.22 –
0.01 – 0.01 –
0.14:0.30 – 0.14:0.30 –
0.13 – 0.13 –
0.99 – 0.99 –
0% – 0% –
Full Mexico Domain skills Mexico Creativity skills Mexico Task motivation Mexico
5 4 1 –
7523 6816 707 –
0.49 0.59 0.05 –
0.17 0.06 – –
0.32:0.65 0.53:0.66 – –
211.03 24.55 – –
0.00 0.00 – –
98.10% 87.78% – –
Full Netherlands Domain skills Netherlands Creativity skills Netherlands Task motivation Netherlands
1 – 1 –
157 – 157 –
0.37 – 0.37 –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
Full Pakistan Domain skills Pakistan Creativity skills Pakistan Task motivation Pakistan
2 1 – 1
868 180 – 688
0.22 0.08 – 0.34
0.11 – – –
−0.04:0.47 – – –
9.51 – – –
0.00 – – –
89.48% – – –
Full Portugal Domain skills Portugal Creativity skills Portugal Task motivation Portugal
12 3 7 2
1831 373 875 583
0.24 0.12 0.27 0.29
0.11 0.10 0.07 0.09
0.17:0.31 −0.01:0.26 0.20:0.33 0.08:0.49
21.77 3.41 3.80 4.39
0.03 0.18 0.70 0.04
49.47% 41.35% 0% 77.22%
Full Romania Domain skills Romania Creativity skills Romania Task motivation Romania
3 1 2 –
603 201 402 –
0.22 0.13 0.39 –
0.25 – 0.00 –
−0.12:0.56 – 0.29:0.49 –
35.69 – 0.00 –
0.00 – 1.00 –
94.40% – 0% –
Full Saudi Arabia Domain skills Saudi Arabia Creativity skills Saudi Arabia Task motivation Saudi Arabia
1 1 – –
175 175 – –
0.29 0.29 – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
– – – –
Full Singapore Domain skills Singapore Creativity skills Singapore Task motivation Singapore
4 – 4 –
2115 – 2115 –
0.23 – 0.23 –
0.03 – 0.03 –
0.18:0.27 – 0.18:0.27 –
1.29 – 1.29 –
0.73 – 0.73 –
0% – 0% –
Full Slovenia Domain skills Slovenia Creativity skills Slovenia Task motivation Slovenia
2 1 – 1
480 240 – 240
0.23 0.06 – 0.39
0.17 – – –
−0.10:0.55 – – –
12.90 – – –
0.00 – – –
92.25% – – –
Full South Korea Domain skills South Korea Creativity skills South Korea Task motivation South Korea
6 2 1 3
1403 320 157 926
0.18 −0.09 0.22 0.35
0.30 0.05 – 0.28
−0.08:0.45 −0.20:0.02 – −0.05:0.74
122.60 0.64 – 72.71
0.00 0.43 – 0.00
95.92% 0% – 97.25%
Full Spain Domain skills Spain Creativity skills Spain Task motivation Spain
6 2 3 1
801 267 320 214
0.31 0.31 0.26 0.35
0.07 0.08 0.05 –
0.24:0.38 0.11:0.51 0.15:0.37 –
3.91 1.82 0.65 –
0.58 0.18 0.72 –
0% 45.05% 0% –
Full Sweden Domain skills Sweden Creativity skills Sweden Task motivation Sweden
8 4 – 4
2444 983 – 1461
0.18 0.05 – 0.29
0.15 0.11 – 0.06
0.07:0.29 −0.08:0.18 – 0.22:0.37
52.42 12.52 – 5.86
0.00 0.01 – 0.12
86.65% 76.04% – 48.81%
Full Taiwan Domain skills Taiwan Creativity skills Taiwan Task motivation Taiwan
38 13 19 6
9929 4649 3775 1505
0.27 0.12 0.36 0.29
0.21 0.16 0.22 0.13
0.20:0.34 0.02:0.22 0.26:0.47 0.17:0.40
432.95 114.36 182.41 23.82
0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
91.45% 89.51% 90.13% 79.01%
Full Thailand
2
290
0.15
0.27
−0.38:0.68
20.70
0.00
95.17%
(continued on next page) 11
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 4 (continued) K
N
Mean rho
Wsdr
CI rho 95%
Q
P
I2
Domain skills Thailand Creativity skills Thailand Task motivation Thailand
1 1
145 145
−0.12 0.42
– –
– –
– –
– –
– –
Full Turkey Domain skills Turkey Creativity skills Turkey Task motivation Turkey
6 1 1 4
1032 163 163 706
0.28 0.13 0.27 0.31
0.11 – – 0.11
0.18:0.37 – – 0.19:0.43
12.24 – – 7.61
0.03 – – 0.05
59.15% – – 60.58%
Full UK Domain skills UK Creativity skills UK Task motivation UK
9 – 4 5
3048 – 1171 1877
0.22 – 0.35 0.13
0.18 – 0.11 0.16
0.09:0.36 – 0.21:0.49 −0.04:0.29
99.30 – 14.58 47.63
0.00 – 0.00 0.00
91.94% – 79.42% 91.60%
Full USA Domain skills USA Creativity skills USA Task motivation USA
295 88 170 37
1,185,493 1,115,058 55,271 15,164
0.19 0.14 0.22 0.21
0.07 0.05 0.19 0.16
0.18:0.20 0.12:0.16 0.18:0.25 0.16:0.27
6242.61 2459.57 1914.63 392.96
0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
95.29% 96.46% 91.17% 90.84%
Full Vietnam Domain skills Vietnam Creativity skills Vietnam Task motivation Vietnam
2 – 2 –
618 – 618 –
0.68 – 0.68 –
0.07 – 0.07 –
0.54:0.82 – 0.54:0.82 –
3.00 – 3.00 –
0.08 – 0.08 –
66.67% – 66.67% –
K = number of effect sizes; N = total sample size; Mean ρ = average effect size; wsd ρ = weighted standard deviation of mean ρ; CI mean ρ 95% = 95% confidence interval for mean ρ; Q = Cochran’s homogeneity test statistic; p = probability of Q; I2 = scale-free index of heterogeneity.
a
Table 5 HMARA results – the effect of culture on the domain-relevant skills-creativity relationship.
Intercept Level 3 – Country level variables Cultural bundles score N of promoting values × Tight culture Tight culture (dummy) Number of promoting values Tightness (scale) Uncertainty avoidance Masculinity Individualism Power Distance Level 2 – Study level variables Publication year FT list journal Level 1 – Effect size level variables Expertise Knowledge diversity Novelty only Measured subjectively Same source IV and DV Private firms Public firms Small firms Primary data source Deviance Number of parameters N at level 3 N at level 2 N at level 1
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
0.17** (0.05)
0.18* (0.07)
Model 4
Model 5 0.16* (0.08) 0.21* (0.09)
0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
0.02 0.03 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
(0.01) (0.00) (0.00) (0.00) (0.00)
(0.04) (0.02) (0.01) (0.00) (0.00) (0.00) (0.00)
0.27** (0.08) −0.22*** (0.04) 0.03 (0.02)
−0.00*** (0.00) −0.03 (0.02)
−0.00** (0.00) −0.03 (0.03)
−0.00** (0.00) −0.03 (0.03)
−0.01*** (0.00) −0.03 (0.02)
−0.00*** (0.00) −0.03 (0.02)
−0.01*** (0.02) 0.03* (0.01) −0.02 (0.04) −0.01 (0.03) 0.18*** (0.04) −0.00 (0.02) −0.04 (0.05) 0.00 (0.03) −0.01 (0.05) 964.53 14 24 118 185
−0.04 (0.02) 0.03 (0.02) −0.02 (0.03) −0.01 (0.03) 0.17*** (0.05) −0.01 (0.05) −0.06 (0.07) −0.01 (0.03) 0.00 (0.05) 961.19 19
−0.04*** (0.01) 0.03* (0.01) −0.02 (0.03) −0.01 (0.03) 0.17*** (0.05) −0.01 (0.02) −0.05 (0.06) 0.00 (0.03) −0.01 (0.05) 963.20 16
−0.04** (0.01) 0.03* (0.01) −0.02 (0.04) −0.01 (0.03) 0.16*** (0.04) −0.01 (0.02) −0.09 (0.05) 0.00 (0.03) −0.01 (0.04) 959.76 17
−0.04*** (0.01) 0.02** (0.01) −0.02 (0.04) −0.01 (0.03) 0.17*** (0.04) −0.01 (0.05) −0.04 (0.08) 0.00 (0.03) 0.00 (0.05) 963.10 15
*** p < 0.01. ** p < .01. * p < .05.
masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. Panel 2 in Table 8b presents the two cultural bundles associated with a weaker effect of creativity-relevant skills on creativity. Solution 2 is the only one that includes cultural tightness, coupled with individualism, low powerdistance, femininity, and low uncertainty avoidance. Overall, these findings provide modest support for Hypothesis 2: the cultural bundle that includes tightness and strengthens the effect of creativity-relevant skills on creativity includes all four cultural values that promote the use of this component, while the bundle that includes tightness and
(collectivism, masculinity, and high uncertainty avoidance) that promote the use of this component, while the bundle that includes tightness and weakens the effect of this component on creativity includes only one value (low power-distance) that promotes the use of the domain-relevant skills component. Panel 2 in Table 8a presents the three cultural bundles associated with a stronger relationship between the creativity-relevant skills component and creativity. Solution 3 is the only one that includes cultural tightness, coupled with individualism, low power distance, 12
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 6 HMARA results–the effect of culture on the creativity-relevant skills-creativity relationship. Model 1 Intercept Level 3 – Country level variables Cultural bundles score N of promoting values × Tight culture Tight culture (dummy) Number of promoting values Tightness (scale) Uncertainty avoidance Masculinity Individualism Power Distance Level 2 – Study level variables Publication year FT list journal Level 1 – Effect size level variables Cognitive skills Personality Novelty only Measured subjectively Same source IV and DV Private firms Public firms Small firms Primary data source Deviance Number of parameters N at level 3 N at level 2 N at level 1
**
0.26
(0.09)
Model 2
Model 3
0.26* (0.10)
0.27
**
(0.10)
Model 4 **
0.30
(0.10)
Model 5 0.25* (0.10) 0.13* (0.06)
−0.00 (0.01) −0.02 (0.04)
0.04 (0.02) −0.02 (0.01) −0.06 (0.04)
−0.03* (0.01) −0.00 (0.00) −0.00 (0.00) −0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) −0.00 (0.00) −0.03 (0.06)
−0.00 (0.00) −0.04 (0.05)
−0.00 (0.00) −0.03 (0.06)
−0.00 (0.00) −0.03 (0.06)
−0.00 (0.00) −0.02 (0.06)
−0.19*** (0.04) −0.04 (0.03) −0.04*** (0.01) −0.00 (0.05) 0.28* (0.14) 0.00 (0.03) −0.12*** (0.04) 0.15** (0.05) 0.02 (0.07) 2059.65 14 30 126 300
−0.19*** (0.04) −0.03 (0.03) −0.04*** (0.01) −0.00 (0.05) 0.29* (0.14) 0.01 (0.03) −0.10* (0.04) 0.15** (0.05) 0.00 (0.08) 2054.11 19
−0.19*** (0.04) −0.04 (0.03) −0.04*** (0.01) −0.00 (0.05) 0.28* (0.14) 0.00 (0.03) −0.11** (0.04) 0.15** (0.05) 0.02 (0.08) 2059.38 16
−0.19*** (0.04) −0.04 (0.03) −0.04*** (0.01) 0.00 (0.05) 0.27* (0.14) 0.01 (0.04) −0.12** (0.03) 0.15** (0.05) 0.02 (0.07) 2057.88 17
−0.19*** (0.04) −0.04 (0.03) −0.04*** (0.01) −0.00 (0.05) 0.27* (0.14) 0.01 (0.03) −0.12*** (0.04) 0.15*** (0.05) 0.02 (0.08) 2058.02 15
*** p < 0.01. ** p < .01. * p < .05. Table 7 HMARA results – the effect of culture on the task motivation-creativity relationship.
Intercept Level 3 – Country level variables Cultural bundles score N of promoting values X Tight culture Tight culture (dummy) Number of promoting values Tightness (scale) Uncertainty avoidance Masculinity Individualism Power Distance Level 2 – Study level variables Publication year FT list journal Level 1 – Effect size level variables Intrinsic motivation Novelty only Measured subjectively Same source IV and DV Private firms Public firms Small firms Primary data source Deviance Number of parameters N at level 3 N at level 2 N at level 1
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Model 5
0.30** (0.09)
0.28 (0.16)
0.29** (0.10)
0.39*** (0.10)
0.34*** (0.09)
0.01 (0.03) 0.01 (0.05)
0.11* (0.05) −0.07** (0.02) −0.05 (0.04)
0.24*** (0.05)
−0.00 (0.02) −0.00 (0.00) −0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.01*** (0.00) 0.01 (0.05)
0.01 (0.00) −0.00 (0.06)
0.01* (0.00) 0.01 (0.05)
0.01* (0.00) 0.00 (0.04)
0.01* (0.00) −0.03 (0.04)
0.01 (0.05) −0.06** (0.02) 0.19*** (0.02) 0.04 (0.05) −0.07 (0.04) −0.16* (0.07) 0.14*** (0.02) −0.11* (0.05) 516.65 13 24 68 99
0.01 (0.05) −0.07 (0.05) 0.19** (0.07) 0.07 (0.07) −0.08 (0.11) −0.15 (0.13) 0.14*** (0.04) −0.10 (0.11) 512.63 18
0.01 (0.04) −0.06** (0.02) 0.19*** (0.02) 0.04 (0.06) −0.08* (0.04) −0.16* (0.07) 0.14*** (0.02) −0.10* (0.05) 516.56 15
−0.00 (0.05) −0.07*** (0.02) 0.18*** (0.02) 0.04 (0.05) −0.11** (0.04) −0.18** (0.07) 0.13*** (0.03) −0.10* (0.05) 514.18 16
0.02 (0.04) −0.06* (0.03) 0.19*** (0.01) 0.06 (0.05) −0.11** (0.03) −0.18** (0.06) 0.13*** (0.03) −0.12*** (0.03) 509.62 14
*** p < 0.01. ** p < .01. * p < .05.
13
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Fig. 1. Venn diagram representing a truth table – domain-relevant skillsa. a: PD = power distance, IDV = individualism, MAS = masculinity, UA = uncertainty avoidance, TIGHT = tightness. 0 = presence, 1 = absence. The numbering order in each cel follows the same order (PD, IDV MAS, UA, TIGHT). Countries included in sample: Bulgaria (BG), Chile (CL), China (CN), Egypt (EG), France (FR), Germany (DE), Hong Kong (HK), Israel (IL), India (IN), Iran (IR), Italy (IT), South Korea (KR), Mexico (MX), Pakistan (PK), Portugal (PT), Romania (RO), Saudi Arabia (SA), Slovenia (SI), Spain (ES), Sweden (SE), Taiwan (TW), Thailand (TH), Turkey (TR), United States (US).
characteristics of the bundles appearing in Table 8a that do not include cultural tightness (i.e., they either include cultural looseness or have tightness as a “do not care” condition). We further inductively explore the patterns of tradeoffs and substitution patterns between specific cultural dimensions in Appendix C.
weakens the effect of this component on creativity includes three values (individualism, low power distance, and low uncertainty avoidance) that promote the use of the creativity-relevant skills component. Panel 3 in Table 8a presents the two cultural bundles associated with a stronger relationship between the task motivation component and creativity. Solution 2 is the only one that includes cultural tightness, coupled with collectivism, high power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. Panel 3 in Table 8b presents the two cultural bundles associated with a weaker effect of task motivation on creativity; none of these bundles includes cultural tightness. Overall, these findings provide further support for Hypothesis 3: the cultural bundle that includes tightness and strengthens the effect of task motivation on creativity includes three cultural value dimensions (high power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance) that promote the use of the task motivation component. Overall, the fs/QCA results provide further support for our hypotheses: the moderating effect of cultural bundles that include cultural tightness becomes stronger as the number of cultural values promoting the use of a given component increases. Since we did not have a priori expectations on the precise configurations for cultural bundles that include looseness, we follow fs/QCA best practices (Bell et al., 2014; Fiss, 2011; Greckhamer et al., 2018) and explore inductively the
4.3.4. Exploratory fs/QCA results of bundles that do not include tightness For domain-relevant skills, we find two bundles that do not include tightness and lead to a stronger effect of this component (Table 8a, Panel 1). Solution 1 includes individualism, high power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance, with cultural tightness as a ‘do not care’ condition. This means that, with this combination of cultural values included in the cultural bundle, the strength of the norms enforcing these cultural values becomes irrelevant (i.e., within bundles that include these values, domain-relevant skills will have a stronger effect on creativity regardless of whether the bundle includes tightness or looseness). Solution 2 includes individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance, coupled with cultural looseness. For creativity-relevant skills, we find two bundles that do not include tightness and lead to a stronger effect of this component (Table 8a, Panel 2). Solution 1 includes collectivism, high power 14
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Fig. 2. Venn diagram representing a truth table – creativity-relevant skillsa. a: PD = power distance, IDV = individualism, MAS = masculinity, UA = uncertainty avoidance, TIGHT = tightness. 0 = presence, 1 = absence. The numbering order in each cel follows the same order (PD, IDV MAS, UA, TIGHT). Countries included in sample: Australia (AU), Austria (AT), Bhutan (BT), Bulgaria (BG), Chile (CL), Canada (CA), China (CN), Colombia (CO), Denmark (DK), Germany (DE), Great Britain (UK), Hong Kong (HK), Israel (IL), India (IN), Iran (IR), Italy (IT), South Korea (KR), Lithuania (LT), Malaysia (MY), Mexico (MX), Netherlands (NL), Portugal (PT), Romania (RO), Singapore (SP), Spain (ES), Taiwan (TW), Thailand (TH), Turkey (TR), United States (US), Vietnam (VN).
do not include tightness and strengthen the effect of the component include either (a) individualism, high power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance; or (b) individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. These two solutions include respectively three (individualism, high power distance, and femininity) and two (individualism, low uncertainty avoidance) values that were theoretically expected to discourage, rather than promote, the use of this component (e.g., Hofstede, 1980; Kruglanski & Freund, 1983; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Tett & Burnett, 2003; see Table 2) and thus weaken its effect on creativity. Similarly, for creativity-relevant skills, our findings show that the two bundles that include cultural looseness and strengthen the effect of creativity-relevant skills on creativity (Table 8a, Panel 2, solutions 1 and 2) include respectively four (collectivism, high power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance) and two (femininity and high uncertainty avoidance) values that were theoretically expected to discourage the use of this component (Erez & Nouri, 2010; Eylon & Au, 1999; Harzing & Hofstede, 1996; Hofstede & McCrae, 2004; see Table 2).
distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance, coupled with cultural looseness. Solution 2 includes individualism, low power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance, coupled with cultural looseness. For task motivation, we find one bundle that does not include tightness and leads to a stronger effect of this component (Table 8a, Panel 3). Solution 1 includes individualism, low power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance, with cultural tightness as a ‘do not care’ condition. This means that, with this combination of cultural values included in the cultural bundle, task motivation will have a stronger effect on creativity regardless of whether the bundle includes tightness or looseness. Inductively looking at these findings reveals some regularities in terms of the characteristics of bundles that (a) do not include tightness and (b) strengthen the components-creativity relationships. At the big picture level, we observe that bundles that do not include tightness do not seem to need a high number of promoting values in order to strengthen the components-creativity relationships. For domain-relevant skills, the two bundles (Table 8a, Panel 1, solutions 1 and 2) that 15
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Fig. 3. Venn diagram representing a truth table – task motivationa. a: PD = power distance, IDV = individualism, MAS = masculinity, UA = uncertainty avoidance, TIGHT = tightness. 0 = presence, 1 = absence. The numbering order in each cel follows the same order (PD, IDV MAS, UA, TIGHT). Countries included in sample: Austria (AT), Belgium (BE), Bulgaria (BG), China (CN), Denmark (DK), Egypt (EG), Finland (FI), Germany (DE), Great Britain (UK), Hong Kong (HK), Israel (IL), India (IN), Iran (IR), Italy (IT), South Korea (KR), Lithuania (LT), Pakistan (PK), Portugal (PT), Slovenia (SI), Sweden (SE), Taiwan (TW), Turkey (TR), United States (US).
bundle fit variable. The results for each component can be seen in Model 5 of Tables 5–7. The variable has a positive and significant moderating effect for all three components (domain-relevant skills: p < .05; creativity-relevant skills: p < .05; task motivation: p < .001). This finding – coupled with the non-significant moderating effects of single cultural dimensions – supports our overarching argument that the moderating effect of culture on creativity is better understood by focusing on cultural bundles rather than on single cultural dimensions.
Finally, our findings show that the one bundle where tightness is a “do not care” condition and that strengthens the effect of task motivation on creativity (Table 8a, Panel 3, solution 1) includes three (low power distance, femininity, and high uncertainty avoidance )values that were theoretically expected to discourage the use of this component (Erez & Earley, 1987; Hofstede, 1980; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Taras et al., 2010; see Table 2). Table 8b, Panel 3 (solutions 1 and 2) further corroborates this evidence. The two cultural bundles that include cultural looseness and lead to a weaker task motivation–creativity relationship include three values (individualism, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance in Solution 1; individualism, high power distance, and masculinity in Solution 2) that were theoretically expected to promote the use of task motivation, and thus to strengthen the effect of task motivation on creativity, rather than weakening it. Overall, these results show that within bundles that do not include cultural tightness (i.e., they either include cultural looseness or have tightness as a “do not care” condition) individuals can develop and use a component to achieve creativity even when the cultural values in the bundle do not encourage it or even discourage it.
5. Discussion Research on the moderating effect of culture on creativity has mostly focused on the effect of cultural dimensions in isolation. However, culture is made of multiple cultural dimensions that mutually influence each other in shaping outcomes. Consequently, understanding the moderating effect of culture on creativity requires taking into account the multidimensional nature of culture. In this paper, we took on this challenge to develop theory on cultural bundles – consisting of multiple cultural value dimensions and of the strength of the norms enforcing these values – and how their configurations moderate the components-creativity relationships. We hypothesized that, in order to strengthen the effect of a given component on creativity, cultural
4.3.5. Cultural bundles fit analysis Finally, we returned to the HMARA to test the overall predictive strength of cultural bundles configurations by introducing the cultural 16
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Table 8a Results of fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis: stronger effects of components on creativity.a Panel 1 Domain-relevant Skills Solutions
Panel 2 Creativity-relevant Skills Solutions
Panel 3 Task Motivation Solutions
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
N of hypothesized promoting values included Consistency Raw coverage Unique coverage Countries described by this cultural bundle
1 0.99 0.24 0.09 France Spain
2 0.96 0.09 0.03 USA
3 0.88 0.35 0.21 Mexico Saudi Arabia
2 0.95 0.22 0.10 Lithuania Netherlands
4 0.95 0.14 0.06 UK
Mean rho of the configuration Overall solution consistency Overall solution coverage
0.32 0.91 0.49
0.14
0.44
0 0.87 0.20 0.13 Bulgaria Romania Chile 0.37 0.90 0.41
0.38
0.35
1 0.94 0.29 0.23 Finland Lithuania Israel 0.65 0.94 0.43
3 0.96 0.21 0.15 China Hong Kong India 0.51
Individualism Power Distance Masculinity Uncertainty Avoidance Cultural Tightness
a Black circles indicate the presence of a condition, and circles with “X” indicate its absence. Large circles indicate core conditions; small ones, peripheral conditions (see Appendix C for more details). Blank spaces indicate “don’t care”
5.1.1. Cultural bundles and creativity Our meta-analytic and fs/QCA findings converge to suggest that the moderating effect of culture cannot be fully understood by focusing on cultural dimensions in isolation. In so doing, we corroborate and extend cross-cultural research that has stressed the importance of accounting for the multidimensional nature of culture (Kirkman et al., 2006; Leung et al., 2005; Taras et al., 2010) by theorizing and empirically testing how cultural dimensions interact to shape outcomes. By introducing the concept of cultural bundles, we explain both the apparently incompatible predictions stemming from conflicting cultural dimensions, and the overlaying effects of cultural dimensions that are theorized to have similar moderating effects. Specifically, we show that extant predictions about the moderating effect of cultural dimensions might hold when these values are considered in isolation or in sets of two, as extant literature has done, but not when these cultural dimensions are considered all together. Focusing on one cultural dimension in isolation or on the interactive
bundles that include cultural tightness will need a high number of cultural value dimensions that promote the use of that component. We found empirical support for our theory and hypotheses through a mix of meta-analytic methods and fs/QCA analyses. Moreover, our fs/QCA analyses allowed us to inductively identify the cultural bundle configurations including cultural looseness that strengthen the componentscreativity relationships. Results show that cultural bundles that include looseness not only need a lower number of “promoting” values to strengthen a given component-creativity relationship, but they actually benefit from the presence of a large number of cultural values that were theoretically thought to discourage the use of that component. 5.1. Theoretical contributions Our study presents a strong case for a more nuanced understanding of the moderating effect of culture on creativity. In so doing, our study advances research on culture and creativity in several ways.
Table 8b Results of fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis: weaker effects of components on creativity.a Panel 1 ~Domain-relevant Skills Solution
Panel 2 ~Creativity-relevant Skills Solutions
Panel 3 ~Task Motivation Solutions
1
1
2
1
2
N of hypothesized promoting values included Consistency Raw Coverage Unique Coverage Country Example
0.81 0.13 0.13 Sweden
0.92 0.18 0.13 Denmark
3 0.88 0.12 0.06 USA
3 0.87 0.17 0.11 Belgium
Mean rho of the configuration Overall solution consistency Overall solution coverage
0.05 0.81 0.13
0.86 0.32 0.26 Mexico Colombia 0.12 0.87 0.44
0.21 0.90 0.23
0.26
Individualism Power Distance Masculinity Uncertainty Avoidance Cultural Tightness
0.17
a Black circles indicate the presence of a condition, and circles with “X” indicate its absence. Large circles indicate core conditions; small ones, peripheral conditions (see Appendix C for more details). Blank spaces indicate “don’t care.”
17
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
discipline” (Creative France, 2015). In contrast, the second bundle would prompt individuals to acquire domain-relevant skills because of an increased learning orientation and higher emphasis and encouragement of personal initiative (Taras et al., 2010), and to apply and display these skills in creative and deviant ways as a way to distinguish oneself from the crowd. The United States exemplifies this cultural bundle, where the acquisition of specialized knowledge is highly regarded, and its application to the creation of new ideas and ventures is considered the epitome of the “American dream.” For creativity-relevant skills and task motivation, our findings suggest that developing components that are not emphasized by cultural values could yield creative benefits in loose cultures. While using a discouraged component would be sanctioned in a tight culture, deviance in the use of a component can actually be conducive to a stronger component-creativity relationship in a loose culture or in a culture where tightness is not central. Within bundles that do not include tightness, the creative advantage of possessing creativity-relevant skills and task motivation does not seem to come from the fact that they are culturally-approved: our results suggest it might come from the fact that they are a scarce resource that provides those who possess it with a competitive advantage. While explorative in nature, these fs/QCA findings and our explanations open up opportunities for future research. Scholars could develop and test theories on the different patterns of interaction through which cultural dimensions within cultural bundles shape the relationship between a specific antecedent and creativity.
effect of a limited number of dimensions could lead to partial and even misleading conclusions about the moderating role of culture. For example, our findings on task motivation extend and contrast those of a recent meta-analytic study that conducted an exploratory analysis on the moderating effect of individualism and tightness on the task motivation-creativity relationship, without controlling for other cultural dimensions (Liu et al., 2016). In their study, Liu et al. (2016) find that individualism has a negative direct moderating effect, which turns positive when cultural tightness is high. This finding is consistent with our idea that cultural dimensions do not operate in isolation. However, accounting for the multilevel nature of the data and considering four cultural values, rather than just one, reveals a completely different picture. First, our HMARA shows that neither individualism nor tightness significantly moderate the task motivation-creativity relationship, while cultural bundle configurations do. Second, and most importantly, our fs/QCA results show that cultural bundles that include tightness and strengthen the effect of task motivation include collectivism, and not individualism. Individualism is included in another cultural bundle that strengthens the task motivation-creativity relationship, but that bundle does not include cultural tightness. Overall, our findings for task motivation is the perfect illustration of how focusing on one or two values, rather than on cultural bundles, could lead to partial and even misleading conclusions. More broadly, this suggests that making accurate and meaningful predictions on the moderating effect of culture requires focusing on the overall configuration of cultural bundles rather than on one or two cultural dimensions in isolation.
5.1.3. Culture as a moderator We corroborate and extend recent research that has moved away from the direct or mediated effects of culture on creativity to emphasize the moderating role of culture (e.g., Erez & Nouri, 2010; Liu et al., 2016; Nouri et al., 2015; Zhou & Su, 2010). This implies a shift from an outcome-focused view of the culture-creativity relationship to a process-focused one. We believe that this shift not only opens up new avenues for research, but can also yield relevant insights for research focusing on the direct effect of culture. Our findings on cultural tightness provide a clear example. Early theory on cultural tightness predicts a negative direct effect on creativity, based on the premise that the greater freedom allowed by loose cultures should encourage experimentation and divergent thinking (Gelfand et al., 2006). Subsequent studies focusing on direct effects have revealed a more complex relationship (e.g. Chua et al., 2015; Chua, Huang, & Jin, 2019), suggesting that individuals in tight cultures can also achieve creativity under certain conditions. For example, in a study of creative contests on a global online crowdsourcing platform, Chua et al. (2015) found that, while individuals in culturally loose cultures were more likely than individuals in culturally tight cultures to succeed in creative tasks in foreign settings, individuals in culturally tight cultures were more likely than individuals in culturally loose cultures to succeed in creative tasks in local settings. By shifting the focus from the direct effect of cultural tightness to its moderating effect within cultural bundles, we reframe the role of cultural sanctioning versus tolerance for deviance as something that affects the cultural preferences on how to achieve creativity, rather than creativity itself. We also offer a potential alternative explanation for existing findings: the “local advantage” of creators within tight cultures could be due to the fact that the tools they apply to creative tasks foster their creativity locally, where the culture emphasizes their development, but do not work as well when they move to a different location. This suggests a potential transferability issue of the components: components that foster creativity in a given culture do not work when transferred to another culture – something that could be explored by future research.
5.1.2. Cultural bundles and patterns to creativity A second contribution of our study is that we show that there is no universal cultural bundle leading to stronger components-creativity relationships. Instead, we provide evidence for equifinality through our fs/QCA analysis (Fiss, 2011; Ragin, 2008) of different combinations of cultural dimensions in fostering the components-creativity relationships. These findings corroborate the “culture as toolkit” framework (Peterson, 2016; Swidler, 1986): different cultures emphasize the use of different sets of resources, and thus promote different paths towards the achievement of creativity. Broadly speaking, our findings suggest the presence of two macropatterns towards creativity. Our meta-analytic results and deductive fs/ QCA analyses show that individuals in countries whose bundles include cultural tightness are more likely to achieve creativity if they develop and use the components promoted by the largest possible number of cultural values included in the bundle. On the contrary, our inductive, exploratory fs/QCA analysis shows that individuals embedded in countries whose bundles include looseness – or where tightness is a “do not care” condition – can achieve creativity by developing components that are not promoted or that are even discouraged by cultural values included in the bundle. Our inductive exploratory analysis shows multiple micro-patterns through which a component can have a stronger effect on creativity when the cultural bundle does not include tightness. For domain-relevant skills, it seems that the presence of just a limited set of cultural values emphasizing the development of this component is enough to strengthen its effect on creativity. However, two very different bundles lead to this effect: one that includes individualism, high power distance, femininity and uncertainty avoidance, and the other including individualism, low power distance, masculinity, and low uncertainty avoidance. Considering each of these two bundles suggests different paths through which they each strengthen the effect of domain-relevant skills on creativity. The first bundle would prompt individuals to apply domain-relevant skills to the generation of creative ideas that are wellgrounded and societally relevant in order to minimize risk and failures. Consistent with this reasoning, France, a country whose cultural bundle exhibits this configuration, is known for “unparalleled creativity paired with a very Gallic, serious-minded approach synonymous with
5.1.4. Culture and the componential theory Finally, our research extends the componential theory of creativity by showing that the effect of the components of creativity is contingent 18
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
theory does not consider. While these bundles exhibited the same macro-pattern as those that include looseness, this apparent similarity could be driven by entirely different mechanisms. Future research could use our analysis as a starting point to develop theory on the moderating effect of cultural bundle configurations that include looseness, or of cultural bundle configurations regardless of cultural tightness-looseness. These theories could be tested either with fs/QCA techniques or in laboratory settings. Third, we combined similar variables into broader constructs when examining the unique effects of the three components. This approach is a common practice in meta-analytic reviews (e.g., Heugens & Lander, 2009; Lander & Heugens, 2017; Liu et al., 2016) and is consistent with the idea that, while heterogeneous in nature, the antecedents included in each component affect creativity through the same mechanisms and rationale (see Amabile, 1988, 1996, and Amabile & Pratt, 2016). Our findings suggest that this aggregation was appropriate and should not significantly affect our estimates: we did not find significant differences between the effect size of each subcomponent and the overall effect size of that component. However, we cannot rule out that there are unobserved intercultural differences in how sub-components affect creativity. Future research could explore the specific relationship between certain sub-components and creativity in different countries. Fourth, our theory focused on between-country differences in the effectiveness of each component. However, our ACMA analysis on each country revealed that differences also exist within each country, leading to a “hierarchy” of components in their effectiveness in fostering creativity. While this hierarchy is driven by the between-country differences we observed, exploring in-depth within-country differences was beyond the focus of our theory, and for some countries we did not have effect sizes for all the components, thus limiting our ability to draw meaningful conclusions. Future research could explore this issue by looking at each country separately and by cross-comparing different countries. Fifth, our fs/QCA truth tables point out the need to conduct research on all the components in those countries that have not yet been studied, particularly those that would be theoretically expected to lead to stronger or weaker effects. This would not only improve our understanding of how cultural bundles moderate the components-creativity relationships, but shed light on the within-country differences between components. Moreover, it would allow us to understand whether some cultural values are more or less relevant to foster or hamper the components-creativity relationships within each specific bundle. Finally, while our theorizing focused on the moderating effect of cultural bundles on creativity, we believe that the notion of cultural bundles could be extended to cross-cultural research more broadly. For example, studies have shown that work environment and HR practices tend to be more effective in promoting desired behaviors when they are consistent with the cultural values of the country (e.g., Aycan, Kanungo, & Sinha, 1999; Newman & Nollen, 1996). However, these studies have focused on the effect of cultural dimensions in isolation, rather than in conjunction: our results suggest that this “consistency argument” might not necessarily hold when multiple dimensions are considered together, and that cultural tightness could play a particularly relevant role. Future research could adopt a cultural bundles approach to the study of other outcomes of interest to gain a more finegrained understanding of how different cultural dimensions collectively shape the outcome.
on national culture. So far, the componential theory has mostly been used to predict what fosters creativity within an organization. However, as Amabile herself points out, “its failure to include outside forces […] limits the comprehensiveness of the theory in its current form” (Amabile, 2013: p. 138). Our findings show that taking into account how national culture – as an outside force – influences the effect of domain-relevant skills, creativity-relevant skills, and task motivation on creativity is warranted to build a more comprehensive componential theory of creativity. Both our meta-analytic and fs/QCA results show that, while all components are needed in order to achieve creativity (Amabile & Pratt, 2016), which component is more conducive to creativity is dependent on the culture the creator is embedded in. Our study suggests that environmental factors not only affect creativity through the components, as extant research suggests (Amabile & Pratt, 2016), but also act as key boundary conditions for the effectiveness of components in fostering creativity. Moreover, our meta-analytic effort and ACMA analyses extend recent studies (Liu et al., 2016) and join them in answering the call for “further quantitative integrations” on the antecedents of creativity (Anderson et al., 2014, p. 1323). 5.2. Limitations and directions for future research Our study has limitations that could inform future research. First, while we find supporting evidence for our hypotheses on bundles that include cultural tightness, our research design does not allow us to definitively rule out alternative explanations that may also be at work. For instance, research has suggested that individualism, low power distance, and low uncertainty avoidance foster novelty, whereas collectivism, high power distance and high uncertainty avoidance foster usefulness (Erez & Nouri, 2010; Morris & Leung, 2010). It might be that each component of creativity is more or less effective in promoting creativity based on the different aspect of creativity – novelty versus usefulness – that they emphasize. On one side, it could be that components that foster usefulness are more needed, and thus have a more positive effect, in cultures that foster novelty, and vice-versa, following a complementary-fit logic (Cable & Edwards, 2004). However, it could also be that components that foster novelty (usefulness) have a more positive effect in cultures whose values foster novelty (usefulness), following a supplementary-fit logic (Cable & Edwards, 2004). Another potential alternative explanation lies in intercultural differences in the definition of creativity. Research suggests that people in the US have narrower views of what creativity is compared to China, where individuals use a wider set of cues to determine whether an idea is creative (Loewenstein & Mueller, 2016). It might be that each creativity component is more or less conducive to creativity depending on how cultural bundles shape the definition of “creativity”. For example, our ACMA finding that two components have stronger effects in China compared to the US could be explained by the fact that Chinese people have a broader view of what creativity is: the fact that they need a wider set of cues in order to recognize an idea as creative might prompt them to emphasize a wider set of components to be applied to creative endeavors. Future research could explore and clearly pinpoint the mechanisms underlying the moderating effect of culture through the use of experimental designs or cross-cultural surveys. Second, while we hypothesized and found that having a high number of “promoting” cultural values is less needed in bundles that include cultural looseness, we did not have any a priori prediction on the number of “promoting” values that would be needed in order for these bundles to strengthen the components-creativity relationships. While our fs/QCA analysis provides some compelling insights, our interpretation of these findings can only be speculative. Moreover, we also unexpectedly observed a number of bundles where the tightnesslooseness dimension was a “do not care” condition. These findings raise the interesting possibility that cultural bundle configurations could lead to either stronger orweaker components-creativity relationships regardless of the strength of values enforcement – something that our
5.3. Conclusion Limitations notwithstanding, our study presents useful contributions to both theory and organizational practice. Our main conclusion is that the moderating effect of culture on creativity is better understood by looking at cultural bundles of cultural dimensions, rather than each cultural dimension in isolation. We hope that our study shifts current research on culture and creativity towards the adoption of a more 19
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
comprehensive view of culture that takes into account its multidimensional nature and the mutual influence of cultural value dimensions, further exploring the effects of cultural bundles. This will require adopting more holistic approaches to theory development and new empirical methods such as fs/QCA or combining multiple methods to fully capture the moderating effect of culture on creativity.
Kevyn Yong: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft, Writing review & editing. Pier Vittorio Mannucci: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing. Michel W. Lander: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing.
Appendix A. Studies Included in the Analysisa
References
Country
N
K
αCRE
Correlates (r/α)
Ahmetoglu, Harding, Akhtar, and Chamorro-Premuzic (2015) Al Shehri, Al Harthi, and Al-Khatib (2013) Alge, Ballinger, Tangirala, and Oakley (2006) Amabile, Barsade, Mueller, and Staw (2005) Amabile, Conti, Coon, Lazenby, and Herron (1996) Andrews and Smith (1996)
UK
210
3
0.85
IM (0.31/0.79); IM (0.14/0.85); PERS (0.44/0.87)
Saudi Arabia 175
1
0.97
EDU (0.29/1)
USA
489
4
0.88
EXP (0.18/0.79); CS (0.21/0.90); CS (0.05/0.75); PERS (0.24/0.89)
USA
222
7
1;0.81
USA
141
2
1
EDU (0.03/1); EDU (0.12/1); CS (0.16/1); AFF (0.02/1); AFF (0.02/1); AFF (0.03/0.79); AFF (0.18/0.79) PERS (0.55/0.84); IM (0.45/0.66)
USA
168
7
0.91
Baer (2010) Baer and Oldham (2006) Baer, Oldham, and Cummings (2003) Baron and Tang (2011) Basadur, Graen, and Green (1982) Bergendhal et al. (2015) Bergendhal & Magnusson (2015) Binnewies and Gromer (2012) Binnewies, Ohly, and Niessen (2008) Binyamin and Carmeli (2010) Birdi (2007) Blauth, Mauer, and Brettel (2014) Bolda & Naeem (2014) Bryant, Stone, and Wier (2011)
USA USA USA
216 170 142
3 2 2
0.93 0.98 0.90
KDIV (−0.13/1); KDIV (−0.03/1); KDIV (0.30/1); EXP (0.13/1); EXP (0.04/1); PERS (0.33/0.69); IM (0.29/0.60) EDU (0.27/1); KDIV (0.18/1); PERS (0.24/0.85) EDU (−0.05/1); PERS (0.00/0.72) EDU (0.29/1); CS (0.24/0.84)
USA USA
99 45
3 4
1; 0.74 1
PERS (0.27/0.74); AFF (0.38/0.82); AFF (0.21/0.82) CS (0.35/1); CS (0.21/1); CS (0.33/1); CS (0.04/1)
Sweden Sweden
234 234
1 3
1 1
IM (0.26/1) EDU (0.11/1); EDU (−0.11/1); EDU (0.01/1)
Germany Germany
122 117
1 1
0.82 0.89
PERS (0.63/0.84) EXP (0.08/1)
USA UK Germany
188 191 219
1 2 1
0.93 0.86 0.7
PERS (0.25/73) CS (0.21/0.79); IM (0.08/0.69) PERS (0.02/0.7)
Pakistan USA
688 419
1 6
0.74 0.72; 1
Buel (1965) Buel, Albright, and Glennon (1966) Buel and Bachner (1961)
USA USA
52 132
2 3
1 1
IM (0.25/0.74) EDU (0.04/1); EDU (0.16/1); PERS (0.09/0.79); PERS (0.05/0.83); PERS (−0.18/79); PERS (0.03/0.83) EXP (0.38/1); EXP (0.10/1) EXP (0.52/1); EXP (0.29/1); EXP (0.32/1)
USA
54
6
1
Burt (2004) Carmeli, Cohen-Meitar, and Elizur (2007) Carmeli, Gelbard, and ReiterPalmon (2013) Sample 1 Carmeli et al. (2013) Sample 2 Carmeli, Reiter-Palmon, and Ziv (2010) Carmeli, Sheaffer, Binyamin, Reiter-Palmon, and Shimoni (2014) Carmeli et al. (2014) Carmeli and Spreitzer (2009) Carnabuci & Dioszegi (2015) Cattani and Ferriani (2008) Çekmecelioğlu and Özbağ (2016) Černe, Hernaus, Dysvik, and Škerlavaj (2017) Chandler, Keller, and Lyon (2000) Chang, Jia, Takeuchi, and Cai (2014) Chen, Chang, and Chang (2015) Chen and Gable (2013)
USA Israel
455 175
2 1
1 0.95
PERS (0.30/1); PERS (0.23/1); PERS (0.14/1); CS (0.29/1); CS (0.29/1); CS (0.24/1) EDU (0.07/1); EDU (0.03/1) EDU (0.09/1)
Israel
274
1
0.94
EDU (0.19/1)
Israel Israel
130 150
3 1
0.96;1;1 0.94
EDU (0.13/1); EDU (0.01/1); EDU (−0.01/1) PERS (0.34/0.74)
Israel
202
3
0.89
EDU (0.15/1); CS (0.32/0.86); IM (0.56/0.91)
Israel Israel Italy USA Turkey
302 172 67 22,115 181
2 2 2 1 3
0.86 0.92 1 1 0.91
EDU (0.11/1); CS (0.26/7) EDU (0.14/1); CS (0.66/0.94) EDU (0.24/1); CS (0.24/1) CS (0.15/1) IM (0.26/0.83); IM (0.17/0.9); IM (0.42/0.87)
Slovenia
240
2
0.93
EDU (0.06/1); IM (0.34/0.83)
USA
429
1
0.88
EDU (0.10/1)
China
1059
2
0.9
EDU (0.06/1); PERS (0.04/0.81)
Taiwan
337
4
0.87
EXP (−0.06/1); PERS (0.46/0.66); PERS (0.18/0.61); PERS (0.49/0.7)
China
93
1
1
EDU (0.00/1)
20
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al. Chen and Hou (2016) Cheng (2014) Chiang, Hsu, and Shih (2017) Choi, Anderson, and Veillette (2009) Chua (2018) Chua, Morris, and Mor (2012) Sample 1 Chua et al. (2012) Sample 2 Coelho, Augusto, and Lages (2011) Cohen-Meitar, Carmeli, and Waldman (2009) Dahl and Moreau (2002) De Clerq et al. (2017) De Spiegelaere, Van Gyes, De Witte, Niesen, and Van Hootegem (2014) De Stobbleleir et al. (2011) Dewett (2006) Ding, Levin, Stephan, and Winkler (2010) Dong, Liao, Chuang, Zhou, and Campbell (2015) Dougan, Schiff, and Welch (1949) Eisenberger and Aselage (2009) Eisenberger and Rhoades (2001) Sample 1 Eisenberger and Rhoades (2001) Sample 2 Ettlie and O'Keefe (1982)
Taiwan Taiwan Taiwan Canada
291 353 224 123
1 2 4 1
1 0.81 0.79 0.95
EDU (0.08/1) CS (0.03/0.90); IM (0.14/0.83) EXP (0.27/0.92); PERS (0.66/0.9); PERS (0.47/0.87); SEF (0.41/0.9) CS (0.18/0.83)
USA USA
72 43
1 3
0.98 0.78
EXP (−0.22/1) EXP (−0.11/1); KDIV (−0.07/1); CS (0.09/0.88)
USA Portugal
1127 460
3 1
1 0.84
KDIV (0.01/1); AFF (0.58/0.81); CS (−0.03/0.78) IM (0.32/0.85)
Romania
201
3
0.94
EDU (−0.13/1); PERS (0.36/0.92); PERS (0.36/0.90)
USA Mexico Belgium
19 707 927
1 1 2
0.70 0.94 0.91
CS (0.42 /1) CS (0.05/0.78) IM (0.19/0.81); IM (0.26/0.85)
USA USA USA
456 287 3114
1 3 2
0.84 0.96 1
CS (0.23/0.83) PERS (0.17/0.86); PERS (−0.02/70); PERS (0.21/90) EXP (0.05/1); EXP (0.04/1)
Taiwan
380
1
0.94
IM (0.19/0.84)
USA
33
1
1
CS (0.21/1)
USA USA
180 239
2 1
0.94 0.88
PERS (0.22/0.72); IM (0.20/0.85) PERS (0.15/77)
USA
313
1
0.88
IM (0.13/0.78)
USA
123
5
0.84
Fagan (2004) Farmer, Tierney, and Kung-McIntyre (2003) Fleming et al. (2007) Ford and Gioia (2000) Ganjali and Rezaee (2016) George and Zhou (2001) George and Zhou (2002)
USA Taiwan
60 166
1 4
0.80 0.92
EDU (0.13/1); PERS (0.20/0.74); PERS (0.66/0.98); PERS (0.40/0.98); CS (0.31/0.85) CS (0.28/0.88) EDU (0.43/1); PERS (0.29/0.80); PERS (0.30/0.91); CS (0.47/0.95)
USA USA Iran USA USA
53,570 153 302 149 67
1 1 1 2 5
1 1 0.81 0.96 0.98
George and Zhou (2007) Gilson and Shalley (2004) Goldenberg, Lehmann, and Mazursky (2001) Gong, Huang, and Farh (2009) González et al. (2013) González-Gómez and Richter (2015) Sample 1 González-Gómez and Richter (2015) Sample 2 Grant and Berry (2011) Sample 1 Grant and Berry (2011) Sample 2 Grosser, Venkataramani, and Labianca (2017) Groza, Locander, and Howlett (2016) Gu, Tang, and Jiang (2015) Gu, Song, and Wu (2016) Gumusluoglu and Ilsev (2009) Guo, Su, and Zhang (2017) Hall and MacKinnin (1969)
USA UK Israel
161 137 127
3 1 3
0.94 0.80 1
KDIV (0.04/1) CS (0.24/0.61); IM (0.7/0.89) PERS (−0.03; 0.81); PERS (−0.02/69) CS (0.20/0.80); AFF (−0.03/0.80); AFF (0.23/0.91) PERS (0.16/0.93); PERS (0.14/0.83) EDU (−0.12/1); AFF (0.11/0.82); AFF (0.25/0.87) CS (0.24/0.87) CS (−0.29/1); CS (−0.01/1); CS (0.36/1)
Taiwan Mexico Colombia
178 1704 286
3 4 4
0.93 1 0.94;0.96
EDU (−0.10/1); CS (0.20/0.87); SEF (0.24/0.91) EXP (0.65/1); EXP (0.55/1); EXP (0.52/1); EXP (0.65/1) AFF (0.55/0.94); AFF (0.53/0.95); AFF (−0.04/0.94); AFF (−0.09/0.95)
Colombia
185
1
0.95
AFF (−0.11/0.89)
South Korea
90
4
0.97
PERS (0.07/0.97); PERS (0.06/0.75); PERS (0.07/0.77); IM (0.32/0.97)
South Korea
111
8
0.97
USA
144
3
0.71
KDIV (0.15/0.91); PERS (0.19/0.87); PERS (0.17/0.79); PERS (0.13/0.78); CS (0.02/0.80); IM (0.16/0.80); IM (0.21/0.91); IM (−0.04/0.90) EDU (0.09/1); EXP (0.01/1); SEF (0.25/0.73)
USA
262
4
0.87
EXP (0.38/0.86); EXP (0.59/0.83); CS (0.61/0.83); CS (0.52/0.81)
China China Turkey China USA
160 207 163 142 124
1 1 3 2 6
0.97 0.84 0.95 0.90 1
Singapore China
108 345
1 8
0.91 0.87;0.94
Taiwan
330
2
0.84
EDU (0.41/1); EDU (0.01/1) EDU (0.12/0.88); PERS (0.24/0.82); IM (0.24/0.77) PERS (0.62/0.79); PERS (0.41/0.81) PERS (0.36/1); PERS (0.24/1); PERS (0.24/1); PERS (0.43/1); PERS (0.19/1); CS (0.45/1) PERS (0.15/0.87) EDU (−0.01/1); EDU (0.21/1); CS (0.00/0.76); CS (0.14/76); CS (−0.04/0.76); CS (0.1/0.76); PERS (0.19/0.82); PERS (0.16/0.82); EDU (0.09/1); CS (0.11/0.89)
China China USA Singapore USA Taiwan Taiwan Taiwan
452 250 122 669 693 1703 306 120
2 2 2 3 1 1 1 5
0.94 0.94 1 0.94 0.84 0.80 0.86 0.91
Harrison and Wagner (2016) He, Yao, Wang, and Caughron (2016) Hirst, Van Knippenberg, Chen, and Sacramento (2011) Hon, Bloom, and Crant (2014) Hon and Leung (2011) Hood and Koberg (1991) Horng and Hu (2009) Houghton and DiLiello (2010) Hsu and Fan (2010) Sample 1 Hsu and Fan (2010) Sample 2 Hsu, Hou, and Fan (2011)
EDU (0.06/1); PERS (0.34/0.85) EDU (0.06/1); IM (0.40/0.79) AFF (0.00/0.79); IM (0.00/0.76) CS (0.24/0.94); CS (0.19/0.85); CS (0.21/0.99) SEF (0.12/0.84) EDU (0.06/1) EDU (0.02/1) PERS (0.37/0.71); PERS (0.25/0.70); PERS (0.45/0.89); PERS (0.18/0.70) SEF (0.42/0.80)
21
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al. Huang, Hsieh, and He (2014) Sample 1 Huang et al. (2014) Sample 2 Ibarra (1993) Isaksen and Lauer (2002) Jafri, Dem, and Choden (2016) Jaiswal and Dhar (2016) Janssen and Huang (2008) Jaskyte and Kisieliene (2006) Jaussi, Knights, and Gupta (2017) Jaussi, Randel, and Dionne (2007) Jaussi and Randel (2014)
China
200
1
0.77
KDIV (0.09/1)
China USA USA Bhutan India Netherlands Lithuania USA
82 79 154 250 424 157 122 129
1 4 2 2 3 1 2 6
0.95 1 1 1 0.81 0.94 0.95 0.71; 0.87
USA
179
4
0.92
KDIV (−0.13/1) EXP (0.30/1); EDU (0.07/1); EXP (0.31/1); EDU (0.33/1) AFF (−0.93/1); PERS (0.65/1) CS (0.35/1); PERS (0.42/1) EDU (0.20/1); EXP (0.15/1); SEF (0.79/72) CS (0.34/0.90) CS (0.34/0.85); IM (0.48/0.78) EXP (−0.08/0.8); EXP (−0.13/0.08); AFF (0.24/0.79); AFF (0.27/0.9); AFF (0.26/0.79); AFF (0.31/0.9) CS (0.17/0.71); PERS (0.26/0.89); PERS (0.07/65); SEF (0.15/0.62)
USA
368
8
0.74;0.8
Jiang, Gu, and Tang (2017) Jo and Lee (2012) Jones (1964) Kalyar (2011) Kang, Solomon, and Choi (2015) Keller and Holland (1978)
China South Korea USA Pakistan USA
253 365 88 180 105
2 1 2 1 1
0.86 0.95 1 0.81 0.81
USA
256
6
1
Khazanchi and Masterson (2011) Kim, Hon, and Crant (2009) Kim, Hon, and Lee (2010) Lapierre and Giroux (2003) Li, Deng, Leung, and Zhao (2017) Li, Li, Guo, Li, and Harris (2018) Liao, Liu, and Loi (2010) Lindskog, Hemphälä, and Eriksson (2017) Litchfield, Ford, and Gentry (2015) Liu, Chiu, and Chiu (2010) Liu, Chen, and Yao (2011) Study 1 Liu et al. (2011) Study 2 Liu, Gong, Zhou, and Huang (2017) Liu and Lin (2012) Lomberg, Kollmann, and Stöckmann (2017) Lu et al. (2017) Madjar et al. (2002) Madjar, Greenberg, and Chen (2011) Madjar and Ortiz-Walters (2008) Madrid and Patterson (2016) Mannucci and Yong (2018) McDermid (1965)
India
223
2
0.80
EDU (0.22/1); EDU (0.06/1); PERS (−0.18/80); PERS (−0.13/0.80); PERS (0.17/86); PERS (−0.04/0.86) CS (0.58/0.35); PERS (0.16/0.72)
Hong Kong South Korea Canada China
146 157 122 196
2 2 1 2
0.94 0.94 1 0.90
PERS (0.36/0.81); IM (0.38/0.88) EDU (−0.04; 1); PERS (0.28/0.88) CS (0.18/1) EDU (0.14/1); IM (0.13/0.83)
China
200
3
0.76; 0.81; 0.87
EDU (0.08/1); EDU (0.02/1); EDU (0.14/1)
China Sweden
828 281
4 1
1 1
EXP (−0.20/1); EDU (0.03/1); SEF (0.31/0.96); SEF (0.32/0.90) EDU (0.19/1)
USA
146
1
0.95
CS (0.17/0.85)
China China
551 525
1 4
1 0.91
KDIV (0.36/1) EDU (0.01/1); PERS (0.23/0.84); IM (0.36/0.85); IM (0.16/0.88)
China China
856 352
3 4
0.90 0.90
EDU (−0.02/1); PERS (0.44/0.86); IM (0.40/0.91) EDU (0.12/1); EXP (0.39/0.80); IM (0.24/0.84); SEF (0.15/0.77)
Taiwan Denmark
77 191
2 8
1 0.85/0.89
USA USA Taiwan
141 265 152
3 3 3
1 1 0.92
EXP (−0.11/1); EXP (−0.11/1) CS (0.22/1); CS (0.07/1); CS (0.13/1); CS (0.21/1); CS (0.16/1); CS (0.08/1); CS (0.12/1); CS (0.26/1) EDU (0.06/1); PERS (−0.01/1); PERS (0.04/1) EDU (−0.01/1); PERS (0.14/0.82); AFF (0.20/0.71) EDU (0.06/1); PERS (0.40/0.84); IM (0.36/0.88)
USA
136
3
0.71; 0.88; 0.94
EDU (−0.04/1); EDU (−0.06/1); EDU (0.01/1)
Chile USA Bulgaria
220 2070 58
4 2 9
0.83 1 1
USA USA
1038 212
1 2
1 1
EDU (0.15/1); PERS (0.23/0.85); PERS (0.49/0.73); PERS (0.32/0.72) EXP (0.14/1); KDIV (0.09/1) EXP (0.19/1); PERS (0.37/1); PERS (0.01/1); PERS (0.19/1); PERS (0.06/1); PERS (0.27/1); CS (0.14/1); CS (0.23/1); CS (0.10/1) EDU (0.20/1) CS (0.25/1); CS (0.29/1)
USA
135
3
0.87
EDU (−0.03/1); EXP (−0.08/1); CS (0.33/0.80)
Iran
204
1
1
PERS (0.37/1)
Italy
442
2
0.92
EDU (0.15/1); IM (0.46/0.94)
India USA Spain
291 258 53
3 5 3
0.91 0.71 0.80
EDU (−0.06/1); PERS (0.11/1); IM (0.35/0.87) CS (0.30/0.91); CS (0.26/0.91); CS (0.28/0.91); CS (0.36/0.91); IM (0.31/0.91) EXP (0.17/0.89); PERS (0.16/0.92); PERS (0.31/0.93)
Austria
256
2
0.77
PERS (0.25/0.79); IM (0.19/0.81)
Spain
250
3
0.93
CS (0.00/0.92); AFF (0.52/1); PERS (0.51/0.91)
Finland Hong Kong Germany
103 162 81
1 2 1
0.81 0.93 0.93
SEF (0.60/0.78) EDU (0.05;1); AFF (0.21/0.87) CS (0.03/1)
USA Germany USA
152 136 171
3 2 3
1 0.96 0.90;1;1
EDU (0.05/1); EXP (0.36/0.88); EXP (0.23/1) CS (0.51/0.88); IM (0.48/0.84)ì PERS (0.12/0.70); PERS (0.27/0.70); PERS (0.00/0.70)
McFadyen and Cannella (2004) Menon, Bharadwaj, Adidam, and Edison (1999) Miron-Spektor, Ingram, Keller, Smith, and Lewis (2018) Moghadam, Beheshtifar, and Mazrae-Sefidi (2012) Montani, Battistelli, and Odoardi (2017) Mueller and Kamdar (2011) Mumford et al. (2010) Muñoz-Doyague, González-Álvarez, and Nieto (2008) Mussner, Strobl, Veider, and Matzler (2017) Neubert, Kacmar, Carlson, Chonko, and Roberts (2008) Nisula and Kianto (2016) Ng and Feldman (2009) Noefer, Stegmaier, Molter, and Sonntag (2009) Obstfeld (2005) Ohly and Fritz (2010) Oldham and Cummings (1996)
EXP (0.04/1); EXP (0.05/1); KDIV (0.12/0.71); KDIV (0.06/0.81); KDIV (0.15/ 0.71); KDIV (0.15/0.81); SEF (0.06/0.77); SEF (0.17/0.77) EDU (−0.04/1); SEF (0.31/0.83) IM (0.66/0.87) CS (0.35/1); PERS (0.24/1) EDU (0.76/1) EDU (0.16/1)
22
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al. Othman (2011) Parke, Seo, and Sherf (2015)
Malaysia USA
167 129
4 5
0.91 0.96
Peng, Lin, and Baum (2013) Perry-Smith (2006) Radaelli, Lettieri, Mura, and Spiller (2014) Rahimi & Allameh (2011) Rego, Machado, Leal, and Cunha (2009) Rice (2006) Rodan (2002) Rotolo et al. (2013) Sacramento, Fay, and West (2013) Sample 1 Sauermann and Cohen (2010) Schemmann, Chappin, and Herrmann (2017) Schultz and Schreyogg (2013) Schweisfurth (2017)
Taiwan USA Italy
34 97 150
2 4 2
1 0.91; 1 0.90
Iran Portugal
85 125
3 9
1 0.92
Egypt France Italy Portugal
202 106 826 123
2 2 3 2
0.71 1 1 0.90
KDIV (0.65/1); EXP (0.70/1); CS (0.17/1) EDU (0.21/1); EDU (0.15/1); AFF (0.26/1); AFF (0.19/1); AFF (0.26/1); AFF (0.20/1); AFF (0.14/1); AFF (0.31/1); AFF (0.25/1) KDIV (0.44/1); IM (0.27/1) KDIV (0.26/1); KDIV (0.40/1) EXP (0.13/1); EXP (−0.24/1); CS (0.10/1) EDU (−0.01/1); PERS (0.14/1)
USA Germany
1707 72
3 1
1 1
IM (0.03/1); IM (0.06/1); IM (0.05/1) EXP (0.19/1)
USA Germany
287 1103
1 6
1 0.75; 0.1
Germany
54
2
0.91
EXP (0.14/0.78) EDU (0.04/1); EDU (0.1/1); CS (0.06/0.80); CS (−0.01/0.80); PERS (−0.03/0.63); PERS (0.21/0.63) EXP (0.54/1); CS (0.58/0.88)
USA Canada USA USA
172 212 141 2200
3 3 1 1
0.89 0.86 0.80 1
EDU (0.26/1); CS (0.18/0.91); CS (−0.29/0.90) CS (0.26/0.83); CS (0.26/0.85); PERS (0.26/0.81) KDIV (0.08/1) EDU (0.44/1)
USA
1430
4
0.78
CS (−0.23; 0.83); PERS (0.16/0.79); IM (0.28/0.88); IM (0.30.70)
China South Korea USA
68 290 128
3 1 2
0.91 0.95 0.75
CS (0.13/1); PERS (0.21/1); SEF (0.16/0.86) IM (0.19/0.84) EDU (0.17/1); SEF (0.57/0.89)
USA USA
515,816 331
2 2
1 1
KDIV (0.06/1); EXP (−0.01/1) EXP (0.52/1); EXP (0.52/1)
Germany South Korea USA Australia Denmark Sweden UK Vietnam USA USA
156 163 609 90 74 409 633 309 172 194
1 1 2 1 1 3 3 2 2 4
1 0.98 1 0.95 0.83 1 1 1 1 0.95
PERS (0.17/1) EDU (−0.13/1) KDIV (0.07/1); KDIV (0.01/1) AFF (0.71/0.85) IM (0.47/0.88) IM (0.26/1); IM (0.26/1); IM (0.40/1) PERS (0.23/1); IM (−0.12/1); IM (0.14/1) PERS (0.61/1); PERS (0.75/1) KDIV (0.40/1); PERS (0.38/1) EDU (0.03/1); EDU (0.12/1); SEF (0.24/0.87); SEF (0.01/0.85)
USA
502
2
0.96
SEF (0.17/0.83); SEF (0.13/0.77)
USA USA
140 237
3 8
0.96 0.93
USA
191
9
0.95;1;1
Australia
68
2
0.93
EDU (0.97/1); EXP (−0.02/1); SEF (0.23/0.76) PERS (. 21/0.80); PERS (0.20/0.86); PERS (0.12/0.80); PERS (0.13/0.86); SEF (0.26/0.74); SEF (0.23/0.81); SEF (0.11/0.74); SEF (0.29/0.81) EDU (0.40/1); EDU (0.17/1); EDU (0.39/1); CS (0.35/0.86); CS (0.01/0.86); CS (0.17/0.86); IM (0.28/0.74); IM (0.13/0.74); IM (0.10/0.74) AFF (0.42/0.88); CS (0.32/0.74)
Thailand
145
2
0.94
EDU (−0.12/1); AFF (0.38/0.88)
China USA
240 78
2 10
0.95 1
Spain
214
3
0.8
EDU (0.02/1); PERS (0.06/0.7) CS (0.33/1); CS (0.59/1); CS (0.71/1); CS (−0.05;1); CS (0.07/1); CS (0.45/1); CS (0.26/1); CS (0.71/1); CS (0.05/1); CS (0.06/1) EDU (0.34/1); PERS (0.21/0.76); IM (0.29/0.88)
USA
844
1
1
KDIV (0.22/1)
China Taiwan USA
219 159 208
1 2 24
1 0.97 0.78;0.80;0.89;0.75;0.81;0.89;.
Hong Kong USA China
983 41 70
1 3 1
0.81 1 0.78
EDU (0.01/1) EDU (0.05/1); PERS (0.20/0.81) PERS (0.33/0.82); CS (0.29/0.75); CS (0.08/0.82); CS (0.23/0.93); PERS (0.27/ 0.82); CS (0.26/0.75); CS (0.09/0.82); CS (0.13/0.93); PERS (0.23/0.82); CS (0.19/0.75); CS (0.05/0.82); CS (0.09/0.93); PERS (0.25/0.82); CS (0.24/0.75); CS (0.10/0.82); CS (0.17/0.93); PERS (0.17/0.82); CS (0.19/0.75); CS (0.02/ 0.82); CS (0.13/0.93); PERS (0.11/0.82); CS (0.15/0.75); CS (−0.03/0.82); CS (0.12/0.93) IM (0.31/0.81) AFF (0.53/0.86); AFF (0.61/0.92); PERS (0.48/0.07) PERS (0.04/0.56)
Taiwan
250
4
0.89
EXP (0.37/0.91); KDIV (−0.03/0.93); CS (0.09/0.89); SEF (0.14/0.91)
Schweisfurth and Herstatt (2015) Scott and Bruce (1994) Scratchley and Hakstian (2001) Sethi, Smith, and Park (2001) Shalley, Gilson, and Blum (2000) Shalley, Gilson, and Blum (2009) Shin, Kim, Lee, and Bian (2012) Shin and Zhou (2003) Simmons, Payne, and Pariyothorn (2014) Singh and Fleming (2010) Smith, Albright, and Glennon (1961) Smith and White (1965) Son, Cho, and Kang (2017) Sosa (2011) Spanjol, Tam, and Tam (2015) Stea and Pedersen (2017) Stetler and Magnusson (2015) Swailes (2000) Tai and Mai (2016) Thatcher and Brown (2010) Tierney and Farmer (2002) Sample 1 Tierney and Farmer (2002) Sample 2 Tierney and Farmer (2004) Tierney and Farmer (2011) Tierney, Farmer, and Graen (1999) To, Fisher, and Ashkanasy (2015) Tongchaiprasit & Ariyabuddhiphongs, 2016) Tse, To, and Chiu (2018) Tucker, Cline, and Schmitt (1967) Venkataramani, Richter, and Clarke (2014) Wang, Rodan, Fruin, and Xu (2014) Wang and Casimir (2007) Wang and Cheng (2010) Williams (2004)
Wong & Latkin (2008) Wright and Walton (2003) Wu, McMullen, Neubert, and Yi (2008) Wu, Lee, and Tsai (2012)
AFF (0.18/0.90); AFF (0.22/0.89); AFF (0.20/0.92); AFF (0.20/0.89) AFF (0.1/0.78); CS (−0.01/0.74); CS (−0.03/0.59); PERS (0.04/0.88); PERS (−0.08/0.79) PERS (0.28/1); CS (0.40/1) KDIV (0.31/1); EDU (0.21/1); KDIV (0.10/1); IM (0.20/0.71) KDIV (0.22 /0.84); IM (0.32/0.83)
23
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al. Yoon, Sung, Choi, Lee, and Kim South Korea (2015) Zacher, Robinson, and Rosing USA (2016) Zhang and Bartol (2010a) China Zhang and Bartol (2010b) Zhang and Zhou (2014) Sample 1 Zhang and Zhou (2014) Sample 2 Zhang, Chan, Zhong, and Yu (2016) Zhao, Kessel, and Kratzer (2014) Zhou (2003) Zhou and George (2001) Zhou, Shin, Brass, Choi, and Zhang (2009) Zou and Ingram (2013)
271
1
0.93
IM (0.09/0.94)
388
3
0.87
AFF (0.42/0.88); CS (0.6/0.85); PERS (0.44/0.85)
367
9
0.88;0.91
China China
367 322
3 3
0.91 0.94
EDU (0.05/1); EDU (0.04/1); IM (70/0.88); PERS (0.24/0.77); PERS (0.23/ 0.77); PERS (0.37/0.82); PERS (0.37/0.82); IM (0.65/0.82); IM (0.66/0.82) IM (0.70/0.88); PERS (0.64/0.86); IM (0.66/0.82) EDU (0.19/1); PERS (−0.20/0.80); SEF (0.61/0.79)
China
199
3
0.96
EDU (0.27/1); PERS (−0.06/0.84); SEF (0.39/0.87)
China
311
3
0.90
EDU (0.17/1); AFF (0.66/0.87); PERS (0.92/1)
China
358
1
0.90
EDU (0.14/1)
USA USA China
123 149 151
2 2 1
0.97 0.96 0.95
EDU (0.30/1); PERS (0.07/0.97) IM (−0.24/0.78); IM (−0.11/0.86) EDU (0.05/1)
USA
318
4
1
KDIV (0.10/1); KDIV (−0.13/1); CS (0.10/1); CS (0.44/1)
a
EDU = Education, EXP = Expertise, KDIV = Knowledge diversity, CS = Cognitve style, PERS = Personality, AFF = Affect, IM = Intrinsic Motivation, SEF = Selfefficacy.
Ahmetoglu, G., Harding, X., Akhtar, R., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2015). Predictors of creative achievement: Assessing the impact of entrepreneurial potential, perfectionism, and employee engagement. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 198–205. Al Shehri, O. A. M., Al Harthi, A. S., & Al-Khatib, A. J. (2013). Patterns of creativity trends among health managers in health sector of Saudi Arabia, Riyadh region. European Scientific Journal, 9(6), 111–128. Alge, B. J., Ballinger, G. A., Tangirala, S., & Oakley, J. L. (2006). Information privacy in organizations: empowering creative and extra role performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(1), 221–232. Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50 (3), 367–403. Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., &Herron, M. (1996). Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 39(5), 1154–1184. Andrews, J., & Smith, D. C. (1996). In search of the marketing imagination: Factors affecting the creativity of marketing programs for mature products. Journal of Marketing Research, 33 (2), 174–187. Baer, M. (2010). The strength-of-weak-ties perspective on creativity: a comprehensive examination and extension. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(3), 592–601. Baer, M., & Oldham, G. R. (2006). The curvilinear relation between experienced creative time pressure and creativity: moderating effects of openness to experience and support for creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 963–970. Baer, M., Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (2003). Rewarding creativity: when does it really matter?. The Leadership Quarterly, 14(4), 569–586. Baron, R. A., &Tang, J. (2011). The role of entrepreneurs in firm-level innovation: Joint effects of positive affect, creativity, and environmental dynamism. Journal of Business Venturig, 26(1), 49–60. Basadur, M., Graen, G. B., & Green, S. G. (1982). Training in creative problem solving: Effects on ideation and problem finding and solving in an industrial research organization. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 30(1), 41–70. Bergendahl, M., & Magnusson, M. (2015). Creating ideas for innovation: Effects of organizational distance on knowledge creation processes. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 87–101. Bergendahl, M., Magnusson, M., & Björk, J. (2015). Ideation high performers: A study of motivational factors. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 361–368. Binnewies, C., & Gromer, M. (2012). Creativity and innovation at work: the role of work characteristics and personal initiative. Psicothema, 24(1), 100–105. Binnewies, C., Ohly, S., & Niessen, C. (2008). Age and creativity at work: The interplay between job resources, age and idea creativity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 23(4), 438–457. Binyamin, G., & Carmeli, A. (2010). Does structuring of human resource management processes enhance employee creativity? The mediating role of psychological availability. Human Resource Management, 49 (6), 999–1024. Birdi, K. (2007). A lighthouse in the desert? Evaluating the effects of creativity training on employee innovation. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 41(4), 249–270. Blauth, M., Mauer, R., & Brettel, M. (2014). Fostering creativity in new product development through entrepreneurial decision making. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(4), 495–509. Bodla, M. A., & Naeem, B. (2014). Creativity as mediator for intrinsic motivation and sales performance. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 468–473. Bryant, S. M., Stone, D., & Wier, B. (2011). An exploration of accountants, accounting work, and creativity. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 23(1), 45–64. Buel, W. D. (1965). Biographical data and the identification of creative research personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49(5), 318–321. Buel, W. D., Albright, L. E., & Glennon, J. R. (1966). A note on the generality and cross-validity of personal history for identifying creative research scientists. Journal of Applied Psychology, 50(3), 217–219. Buel, W. D., & Bachner, V. M. (1961). The assessment of creativity in a research setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45(6), 353–358. Burt, R. S. (2004). Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology, 110(2), 349–399. Carmeli, A., Cohen-Meitar, R., & Elizur, D. (2007). The role of job challenge and organizational identification in enhancing creative behavior among employees in the workplace. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 41(2), 75–90. 24
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Carmeli, A., Gelbard, R., & Reiter‐Palmon, R. (2013). Leadership, Creative Problem‐Solving Capacity, and Creative Performance: The Importance of Knowledge Sharing. Human Resource Management, 52(1), 95–121. Carmeli, A., McKay, A. S., & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). Emotional intelligence and creativity: The mediating role of generosity and vigor. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(4), 290–309. Carmeli, A., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Ziv, E. (2010). Inclusive leadership and employee involvement in creative tasks in the workplace: The mediating role of psychological safety. Creativity Research Journal, 22(3), 250–260. Carmeli, A., Sheaffer, Z., Binyamin, G., Reiter‐Palmon, R., & Shimoni, T. (2014). Transformational leadership and creative problem‐solving: The mediating role of psychological safety and reflexivity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(2), 115–135. Carmeli, A., &Spreitzer, G. M. (2009). Trust, connectivity, and thriving: Implications for innovative behaviors at work. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 43(3), 169–191. Carnabuci, G., & Diószegi, B. (2015). Social networks, cognitive style, and innovative performance: A contingency perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 58(3), 881–905. Cattani, G., & Ferriani, S. (2008). A core/periphery perspective on individual creative performance: Social networks and cinematic achievements in the Hollywood film industry. Organization Science, 19(6), 824–844. Çekmecelioğlu, H. G., & Özbağ, G. K. (2016). Psychological empowerment and support for innovation in Turkish manufacturing industry: Relations with individual creativity and firm innovativeness. Journal of East European Management Studies, 10–34. Černe, M., Hernaus, T., Dysvik, A., & Škerlavaj, M. (2017). The role of multilevel synergistic interplay among team mastery climate, knowledge hiding, and job characteristics in stimulating innovative work behavior. Human Resource Management Journal, 27(2), 281–299. Chandler, G. N., Keller, C., & Lyon, D. W. (2000). Unraveling the determinants and consequences of an innovation-supportive organizational culture. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 25(1), 59–76. Chang, S., Jia, L., Takeuchi, R., & Cai, Y. (2014). Do high-commitment work systems affect creativity? A multilevel combinational approach to employee creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(4), 665. Chen, A. S. Y., & Hou, Y. H. (2016). The effects of ethical leadership, voice behavior and climates for innovation on creativity: A moderated mediation examination. The Leadership Quarterly, 27(1), 1–13. Chen, L., & Gable, G. G. (2013). Larger or broader: Performance implications of size and diversity of the knowledge worker's egocentric network. Management and Organization Review, 9(01), 139–165. Chen, M. H., Chang, Y. Y., & Chang, Y. C. (2015). Entrepreneurial orientation, social networks, and creative performance: middle managers as corporate entrepreneurs. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 493–507. Cheng, H. H. (2014). The effect of social embeddedness on bloggers’ creativity. Online Information Review, 38(3), 399–416. Chiang, Y. H., Hsu, C. C., & Shih, H. A. (2017). Extroversion Personality, Domain Knowledge, and the Creativity of New Product Development Engineers. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 387–39 Choi, J. N., Anderson, T. A., & Veillette, A. (2009). Contextual inhibitors of employee creativity in organizations the insulating role of creative ability. Group & Organization Management, 34(3), 330–357. Chua, R. Y. (2018). Innovating at cultural crossroads: How multicultural social networks promote idea flow and creativity. Journal of Management, 44(3), 1119–1146. Chua, R. Y., Morris, M. W., & Mor, S. (2012). Collaborating across cultures: Cultural metacognition and affect-based trust in creative collaboration. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(2), 116–131. Coelho, F., Augusto, M., & Lages, L. F. (2011). Contextual factors and the creativity of frontline employees: The mediating effects of role stress and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Retailing, 87(1), 31–45. Cohen-Meitar, R., Carmeli, A., &Waldman, D. A. (2009). Linking meaningfulness in the workplace to employee creativity: The intervening role of organizational identification and positive psychological experiences. Creativity Research Journal, 21(4), 361–375. Dahl, D. W., & Moreau, P. (2002). The influence and value of analogical thinking during new product ideation. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(1), 47–60. De Clercq, D., Mohammad Rahman, Z., & Belausteguigoitia, I. (2017). Task conflict and employee creativity: The critical roles of learning orientation and goal congruence. Human Resource Management, 56(1), 93–109. De Spiegelaere, S., Van Gyes, G., De Witte, H., Niesen, W., & Van Hootegem, G. (2014). On the relation of job insecurity, job autonomy, innovative work behaviour and the mediating effect of work engagement. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 318–330. De Stobbeleir, K. E., Ashford, S. J., & Buyens, D. (2011). Self-regulation of creativity at work: The role of feedback-seeking behavior in creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 54(4), 811–831. Dewett, T. (2006). Exploring the role of risk in employee creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 40(1), 27–45. Ding, W. W., Levin, S. G., Stephan, P. E., & Winkler, A. E. (2010). The impact of information technology on academic scientists' productivity and collaboration patterns. Management Science, 56(9), 1439–1461. Dong, Y., Liao, H., Chuang, A., Zhou, J., & Campbell, E. M. (2015). Fostering employee service creativity: Joint effects of customer empowering behaviors and supervisory empowering leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(5), 1364. Dougan, C. P., Schiff, E., &Welch, L. (1949). Originality ratings of department store display department personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 33(1), 31–35. Eisenberger, R., &Aselage, J. (2009). Incremental effects of reward on experienced performance pressure: Positive outcomes for intrinsic interest and creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(1), 95–117. Eisenberger, R., & Rhoades, L. (2001). Incremental effects of reward on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 728–741. Ettlie, J. E., &O'Keefe, R. D. (1982). Innovative attitudes, values, and intentions in organizations. Journal of Management Studies, 19(2), 163–182. Fagan, M. H. (2004). The influence of creative style and climate on software development team creativity: An exploratory study. The Journal of Computer Information Systems, 44(3), 73. Farmer, S. M., Tierney, P., & Kung-McIntyre, K. (2003). Employee creativity in Taiwan: An application of role identity theory. Academy of Management Journal, 46(5), 618–630. Fleming, L., Mingo, S., & Chen, D. (2007). Collaborative brokerage, generative creativity, and creative success. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52(3), 443–475. 25
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Ford, C. M., &Gioia, D. A. (2000). Factors influencing creativity in the domain of managerial decision making. Journal of Management, 26(4), 705–732. Ganjali, A., & Rezaee, S. (2016). Linking perceived employee voice and creativity. Iranian Journal of Management Studies, 9(1), 175–191. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: an interactional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 513. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones don't: the role of context and clarity of feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 687. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2007). Dual tuning in a supportive context: Joint contributions of positive mood, negative mood, and supervisory behaviors to employee creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 50(3), 605–622. Gilson, L. L., & Shalley, C. E. (2004). A little creativity goes a long way: An examination of teams’ engagement in creative processes. Journal of Management, 30(4), 453–470. Goldenberg, J., Lehmann, D. R., & Mazursky, D. (2001). The idea itself and the circumstances of its emergence as predictors of new product success. Management Science, 47(1), 69–84. Gong, Y., Huang, J. C., &Farh, J. L. (2009). Employee learning orientation, transformational leadership, and employee creativity: The mediating role of employee creative self-efficacy. Academy of Management Journal, 52(4), 765–778. Gonzalez-Brambila, C. N., Veloso, F. M., & Krackhardt, D. (2013). The impact of network embeddedness on research output. Research Policy, 42(9), 1555–1567. González-Gómez, H. V., & Richter, A. W. (2015). Turning shame into creativity: the importance of exposure to creative team environments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 126, 142–161. Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. W. (2011). The necessity of others is the mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 73–96. Grosser, T. J., Venkataramani, V., & Labianca, G. J. (2017). An alter-centric perspective on employee innovation: The importance of alters’ creative self-efficacy and network structure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(9), 1360. Groza, M. D., Locander, D. A., & Howlett, C. H. (2016). Linking thinking styles to sales performance: The importance of creativity and subjective knowledge. Journal of Business Research, 69(10), 4185–4193. Gu, J., Song, J., & Wu, J. (2016). Abusive supervision and employee creativity in china: Departmental identification as mediator and face as moderator. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 37(8), 1187–1204. Gu, Q., Tang, T. L. P., & Jiang, W. (2015). Does moral leadership enhance employee creativity? Employee identification with leader and leadermember exchange (LMX) in the Chinese context. Journal of Business Ethics, 126(3), 513–529. Guo, J., Su, Q., & Zhang, Q. (2017). Individual creativity during the ideation phase of product innovation: An interactional perspective. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(1), 31–48. Gumusluoglu, L., &Ilsev, A. (2009). Transformational leadership, creativity, and organizational innovation. Journal of Business Research, 62(4), 461–473. Hall, W. B., &MacKinnin, D. W. (1969). Personality inventory correlates of creativity among architects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(4), 322–326. Harrison, S. H., & Wagner, D. T. (2016). Spilling outside the box: The effects of individuals’ creative behaviors at work on time spent with their spouses at home. Academy of Management Journal, 59(3), 841–859. He, Y., Yao, X., Wang, S., & Caughron, J. (2016). Linking failure feedback to individual creativity: The moderation role of goal orientation. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 52–59. Hirst, G., Van Knippenberg, D., Chen, C. H., & Sacramento, C. A. (2011). How does bureaucracy impact individual creativity? A cross-level investigation of team contextual influences on goal orientation-creativity relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 54(3), 624–641. Hon, A. H., Bloom, M., & Crant, J. M. (2014). Overcoming resistance to change and enhancing creative performance. Journal of Management, 40(3), 919–941. Hon, A. H., & Leung A. S. M. (2011). Employee achievement, power, affiliation motives and creativity: The moderating roles of organizational culture in the Chinese context. Cornell Hospitality Quarterly, 52,125–34. Hood, J. N., & Koberg, C. S. (1991). Accounting firm cultures and creativity among accountants. Accounting Horizons, 5(3), 12–19. Horng, J. S., & Hu, M. L. (2009). The creative culinary process: constructing and extending a four-component model. Creativity Research Journal, 21(4), 376–383. Houghton, J. D., &DiLiello, T. C. (2010). Leadership development: the key to unlocking individual creativity in organizations. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 31(3), 230–245. Hsu, M. L., & Fan, H. L. (2010). Organizational innovation climate and creative outcomes: Exploring the moderating effect of time pressure. Creativity Research Journal, 22(4), 378–386. Hsu, M. L., Hou, S. T., & Fan, H. L. (2011). Creative self‐efficacy and innovative behavior in a service setting: Optimism as a moderator. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 45(4), 258–272. Huang, X., Hsieh, J. J., & He, W. (2014). Expertise dissimilarity and creativity: The contingent roles of tacit and explicit knowledge sharing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(5), 816. Ibarra, H. (1993). Network centrality, power, and innovation involvement: Determinants of technical and administrative roles. Academy of Management Journal, 36(3), 471–501. Isaksen, S. G., &Lauer, K. J. (2002). The climate for creativity and change in teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 11(1), 74–86. Jafri, M. H., Dem, C., & Choden, S. (2016). Emotional intelligence and employee creativity: Moderating role of proactive personality and organizational climate. Business Perspectives and Research, 4(1), 54–66. Jaiswal, N. K., & Dhar, R. L. (2016). Fostering employee creativity through transformational leadership: Moderating role of creative self-efficacy. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 367–371. Janssen, O., & Huang, X. (2008). Us and me: Team identification and individual differentiation as complementary drivers of team members' citizenship and creative behaviors. Journal of Management, 34(1), 69–88. Jaskyte, K., & Kisieliene, A. (2006). Determinants of employee creativity: A survey of Lithuanian nonprofit organizations. Voluntas: International 26
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Journal of Voluntary and Nonprofit Organizations, 17(2), 128–136. Jaussi, K. S., Knights, A. R., & Gupta, A. (2017). Feeling Good, Being Intentional, and Their Relationship to Two Types of Creativity at Work. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 377–386. Jaussi, K. S., & Randel, A. E. (2014). Where to look? Creative self-efficacy, knowledge retrieval, and incremental and radical creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 400–410. Jaussi, K. S., Randel, A. E., &Dionne, S. D. (2007). I am, I think I can, and I do: The role of personal identity, self-efficacy, and cross-application of experiences in creativity at work. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 247–258. Jiang, W., Gu, Q., & Tang, T. L. P. (2017). Do victims of supervisor bullying suffer from poor creativity? Social cognitive and social comparison perspectives. Journal of Business Ethics, 1–20. Jo, N. Y., & Lee, K. C. (2012). The effect of organizational trust, task complexity and intrinsic motivation on employee creativity: Emphasis on moderating effect of stress. In J.J. Park (Ed), Human centric technology and service in smart space (pp. 199–206). Springer, Netherlands. Jones, F. E. (1964). Predictor variables for creativity in industrial science. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48(2), 134–136. Kalyar, M. N. (2011). Creativity, self-leadership and individual innovation. The Journal of Commerce, 3(3), 20–28. Kang, J. H., Solomon, G. T., & Choi, D. Y. (2015). CEOs' leadership styles and managers' innovative behaviour: Investigation of intervening effects in an entrepreneurial context. Journal of Management Studies, 52(4), 531–554. Keller, R. T., & Holland, W. E. (1978). Individual characteristics of innovativeness and communication in research and development organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(6), 759–762. Khazanchi, S., &Masterson, S. S. (2011). Who and what is fair matters: A multi‐foci social exchange model of creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(1), 86–106. Kim, T. Y., Hon, A. H., & Crant, J. M. (2009). Proactive personality, employee creativity, and newcomer outcomes: A longitudinal study. Journal of Business and Psychology, 24(1), 93–103. Kim, T. Y., Hon, A. H., & Lee, D. R. (2010). Proactive personality and employee creativity: The effects of job creativity requirement and supervisor support for creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 37–45. Lapierre, J., &Giroux, V. P. (2003). Creativity and work environment in a high‐tech context. Creativity and Innovation Management, 12(1), 11–23. Lee, H. H., & Yang, T. T. (2015). Employee goal orientation, work unit goal orientation and employee creativity. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(4), 659–674. Li, F., Deng, H., Leung, K., & Zhao, Y. (2017). Is perceived creativity‐reward contingency good for creativity? The role of challenge and threat appraisals. Human Resource Management, 56(4), 693–709. Li, Y., Li, N., Guo, J., Li, J., & Harris, B. (2018). A Network View of Advice-Giving and Individual Creativity in Teams: A Brokerage-Driven, Socially Perpetuated Phenomenon. Academy of Management Journal, (ja). Liao, H., Liu, D., & Loi, R. (2010). Looking at both sides of the social exchange coin: A social cognitive perspective on the joint effects of relationship quality and differentiation on creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 53(5), 1090–1109. Lindskog, P., Hemphälä, J., & Eriksson, A. (2017). Lean tools promoting individual innovation in healthcare. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(2), 175–188. Litchfield, R. C., Ford, C. M., & Gentry, R. J. (2015). Linking individual creativity to organizational innovation. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(4), 279–294. Liu, D., Chen, X. P., & Yao, X. (2011). From autonomy to creativity: a multilevel investigation of the mediating role of harmonious passion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(2), 294. Liu, D., Gong, Y., Zhou, J., & Huang, J. C. (2017). Human resource systems, employee creativity, and firm innovation: The moderating role of firm ownership. Academy of Management Journal, 60(3), 1164–1188. Liu, C.H., Chiu, S. C., &Chiu, C. L. (2010). Intranetwork relationships, creativity, knowledge diversification, and network position. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal, 38(9), 1173–1190. Liu, C.H., & Lin, J. Y. (2012). Social relationships and knowledge creation: the mediate of critical network position. The Service Industries Journal, 32(9), 1469–1488. Lomberg, C., Kollmann, T., & Stöckmann, C. (2017). Different styles for different needs-The effect of cognitive styles on idea generation. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(1), 49–59. Lu, J. G., Hafenbrack, A. C., Eastwick, P. W., Wang, D. J., Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2017). “Going out” of the box: Close intercultural friendships and romantic relationships spark creativity, workplace innovation, and entrepreneurship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(7), 1091. Madjar, N., Greenberg, E., & Chen, Z. (2011). Factors for radical creativity, incremental creativity, and routine, noncreative performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 730–743. Madjar, N., Oldham, G. R., &Pratt, M. G. (2002). There's no place like home? The contributions of work and nonwork creativity support to employees' creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45(4), 757–767. Madjar, N., & Ortiz‐Walters, R. (2008). Customers as contributors and reliable evaluators of creativity in the service industry. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29(7), 949–966. Madrid, H. P., & Patterson, M. G. (2016). Creativity at work as a joint function between openness to experience, need for cognition and organizational fairness. Learning and Individual Differences, 51, 409–416. Mannucci, P. V., & Yong, K. (2018). The differential impact of knowledge depth and knowledge breadth on creativity over individual careers. Academy of Management Journal, 61(5), 1741–1763. McDermid, C. D. (1965). Some correlates of creativity in engineering personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49(1), 14. McFadyen, M. A., & Cannella, A. A. (2004). Social capital and knowledge creation: Diminishing returns of the number and strength of exchange relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 47(5), 735–746. Menon, A., Bharadwaj, S. G., Adidam, P. T., & Edison, S. W. (1999). Antecedents and consequences of marketing strategy making: a model and a test. Journal of Marketing, 63(2), 18–40. Miron-Spektor, E., Ingram, A., Keller, J., Smith, W. K., & Lewis, M. W. (2018). Microfoundations of organizational paradox: The problem is how we think about the problem. Academy of Management Journal, 61(1), 26–45. Moghadam, M. N., Beheshtifar, M., & Mazrae-Sefidi, F. (2012). relationship between employees’ perfectionism and their creativity. African 27
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Journal of Business Management, 6(12), 4659–4665. Montani, F., Battistelli, A., & Odoardi, C. (2017). Proactive goal generation and innovative work behavior: The moderating role of affective commitment, production ownership and leader support for innovation. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(2), 107–127. Mueller, J. S., & Kamdar, D. (2011). Why seeking help from teammates is a blessing and a curse: a theory of help seeking and individual creativity in team contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(2), 263. Mumford, M. D., Waples, E. P., Antes, A. L., Brown, R. P., Connelly, S., Murphy, S. T., &Devenport, L. D. (2010). Creativity and ethics: The relationship of creative and ethical problem-solving. Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 74–89. Muñoz-Doyague, M. F., González-Álvarez, N., & Nieto, M. (2008). An examination of individual factors and employees' creativity: the case of Spain. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1), 21–33. Mussner, T., Strobl, A., Veider, V., & Matzler, K. (2017). The effect of work ethic on employees' individual innovation behavior. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(4), 391–406. Neubert, M. J., Kacmar, K. M., Carlson, D. S., Chonko, L. B., & Roberts, J. A. (2008). Regulatory focus as a mediator of the influence of initiating structure and servant leadership on employee behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1220–1233. Nisula, A. M., & Kianto, A. (2016). The antecedents of individual innovative behaviour in temporary group innovation. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(4), 431–444. Ng, T. W., & Feldman, D. C. (2009). Occupational embeddedness and job performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(7), 863–891. Noefer, K., Stegmaier, R., Molter, B., & Sonntag, K. (2009). A great many things to do and not a minute to spare: Can feedback from supervisors moderate the relationship between skill variety, time pressure, and employees' innovative behavior?. Creativity Research Journal, 21(4), 384–393. Obstfeld, D. (2005). Social networks, the tertius iungens orientation, and involvement in innovation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50(1), 100–130. Ohly, S., & Fritz, C. (2010). Work characteristics, challenge appraisal, creativity, and proactive behavior: A multi‐level study. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(4), 543–565. Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (1996). Employee creativity: Personal and contextual factors at work. Academy of Management Journal, 39(3), 607–634. Othman, A. K. (2011). Emotional intelligence and employees’ innovator role: The moderating effect of service types. Asian Social Science, 7(10), 151–160. Parke, M. R., Seo, M. G., & Sherf, E. N. (2015). Regulating and facilitating: The role of emotional intelligence in maintaining and using positive affect for creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(3), 917. Peng, K. L., Lin, M. C., & Baum, T. (2013). The constructing model of culinary creativity: an approach of mixed methods. Quality & Quantity, 47(5), 2687–2707. Perry-Smith, J. E. (2006). Social yet creative: The role of social relationships in facilitating individual creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 49(1), 85–101. Radaelli, G., Lettieri, E., Mura, M., & Spiller, N. (2014). Knowledge sharing and innovative work behaviour in healthcare: A micro‐level investigation of direct and indirect effects. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(4), 400–414. Rahimi, H., Arbabisarjou, A., Allameh, S. M., & Aghababaei, R. (2011). Relationship between knowledge management process and creativity among faculty members in the University. Interdisciplinary Journal of Information, Knowledge, and Management, 6, 17–33. Rego, A., Machado, F., Leal, S., & Cunha, M. P. E. (2009). Are hopeful employees more creative? An empirical study. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 223–231. Rice, G. (2006). Individual values, organizational context, and self-perceptions of employee creativity: Evidence from Egyptian organizations. Journal of Business Research, 59(2), 233–241. Rodan, S. (2002). Innovation and heterogeneous knowledge in managerial contact networks. Journal of Knowledge Management, 6(2), 152–163. Rotolo, D., & Messeni Petruzzelli, A. (2013). When does centrality matter? Scientific productivity and the moderating role of research specialization and cross‐community ties. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34(5), 648–670. Sacramento, C. A., Fay, D., & West, M. A. (2013). Workplace duties or opportunities? Challenge stressors, regulatory focus, and creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(2), 141–157. Sauermann, H., & Cohen, W. M. (2010). What makes them tick? Employee motives and firm innovation. Management Science, 56(12), 2134–2153. Schemmann, B., Chappin, M. M., & Herrmann, A. M. (2017). The right kind of people: Characteristics of successful ideators' online behaviour. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(3), 277–290. Schultz, C., &Schreyogg, J. (2013). The impact of network ties and resource input on research performance: An empirical investigation among surgeons in academic medical centers. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 60(3), 457–467. Schweisfurth, T. G. (2017). Comparing internal and external lead users as sources of innovation. Research Policy, 46(1), 238–248. Schweisfurth, T. G., & Herstatt, C. (2015). Embedded (lead) users as catalysts to product diffusion. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 151–168. Scott, S. G., & Bruce, R. A. (1994). Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37(3), 580–607. Scratchley, L. S., &Hakstian, A. R. (2001). The measurement and prediction of managerial creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 367–384. Sethi, R., Smith, D. C., & Park, C. W. (2001). Cross-functional product development teams, creativity, and the innovativeness of new consumer products. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(1), 73–85. Shalley, C. E., Gilson, L. L., & Blum, T. C. (2000). Matching creativity requirements and the work environment: Effects on satisfaction and intentions to leave. Academy of Management Journal, 43(2), 215–223. Shalley, C. E., Gilson, L. L., & Blum, T. C. (2009). Interactive effects of growth need strength, work context, and job complexity on self-reported creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 52(3), 489–505. Shin, S. J., & Zhou, J. (2003). Transformational leadership, conservation, and creativity: Evidence from Korea. Academy of Management Journal, 46(6), 703–714. Shin, S. J., Kim, T. Y., Lee, J. Y., & Bian, L. (2012). Cognitive team diversity and individual team member creativity: A cross-level interaction. 28
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Academy of Management Journal, 55(1), 197–212. Simmons, A. L., Payne, S. C., & Pariyothorn, M. M. (2014). The role of means efficacy when predicting creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 53–61. Singh, J., & Fleming, L. (2010). Lone inventors as sources of breakthroughs: Myth or reality?. Management Science, 56(1), 41–56. Smith, W. J., Albright, L. E., &Glennon, J. R. (1961). The prediction of research competence and creativity from personal history. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45(1), 59–62. Smith, E. E., & White, H. L. (1965). Wit, creativity, and sarcasm. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49(2), 131–134. Son, S. Y., Cho, D. H., & Kang, S. W. (2017). The impact of close monitoring on creativity and knowledge sharing: The mediating role of leader‐member exchange. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(3), 256–265. Sosa, M. E. (2011). Where do creative interactions come from? The role of tie content and social networks. Organization Science, 22(1), 1–21. Spanjol, J., Tam, L., & Tam, V. (2015). Employer-employee congruence in environmental values: An exploration of effects on job satisfaction and creativity. Journal of Business Ethics, 130(1), 117–130. Stea, D., & Pedersen, T. (2017). Not all brokers are alike: Creative implications of brokering networks in different work functions. Human Relations, 70(6), 668–693. Stetler, K. L., & Magnusson, M. (2015). Exploring the tension between clarity and ambiguity in goal setting for innovation. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 231–246. Swailes, S. (2000). Goals, creativity and achievement: commitment in contemporary organizations. Creativity and Innovation Management, 9(3), 185–194. Tai, H. T., & Mai, N. Q. (2016). Proactive personality, organizational context, employee creativity and innovative capability: Evidence from MNCs and domestic corporations. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 24(3), 370–389. Thatcher, S. M., & Brown, S. A. (2010). Individual creativity in teams: The importance of communication media mix. Decision Support Systems, 49(3), 290–300. Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its potential antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45(6), 1137–1148. Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2004). The Pygmalion process and employee creativity. Journal of Management, 30(3), 413–432. Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2011). Creative self-efficacy development and creative performance over time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(2), 277–293. Tierney, P., Farmer, S. M., & Graen, G. B. (1999). An examination of leadership and employee creativity: The relevance of traits and relationships. Personnel Psychology, 52(3), 591–620. To, M. L., Fisher, C. D., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2015). Unleashing angst: Negative mood, learning goal orientation, psychological empowerment and creative behaviour. Human relations, 68(10), 1601–1622. Tongchaiprasit, P., & Ariyabuddhiphongs, V. (2016). Creativity and turnover intention among hotel chefs: The mediating effects of job satisfaction and job stress. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 55, 33–40. Tse, H. H., To, M. L., & Chiu, W. C. (2018). When and why does transformational leadership influence employee creativity? The roles of personal control and creative personality. Human Resource Management, 57(1), 145–157. Tucker, M. F., Cline, V. B., & Schmitt, J. R. (1967). Prediction of creativity and other performance measures from biographical information among pharmaceutical scientists. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(2), 131–138. Venkataramani, V., Richter, A. W., & Clarke, R. (2014). Creative benefits from well-connected leaders: Leader social network ties as facilitators of employee radical creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(5), 966. Wang, A. C., & Cheng, B. S. (2010). When does benevolent leadership lead to creativity? The moderating role of creative role identity and job autonomy. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(1), 106–121. Wang, C., Rodan, S., Fruin, M., & Xu, X. (2014). Knowledge networks, collaboration networks, and exploratory innovation. Academy of Management Journal, 57(2), 484–514. Wang, K. Y., & Casimir, G. (2007). How attitudes of leaders may enhance organizational creativity: Evidence from a Chinese study. Creativity and Innovation Management, 16(3), 229–238. Williams, S. D. (2004). Personality, attitude, and leader influences on divergent thinking and creativity in organizations. European Journal of Innovation Management, 7(3), 187–204. Wong, S. C. K., & Ladkin, A. (2008). Exploring the relationship between employee creativity and job-related motivators in the Hong Kong hotel industry. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 27(3), 426–437. Wright, T. A., & Walton, A. P. (2003). Affect, psychological well-being and creativity: Results of a field study. Journal of Business and Management, 9(1), 21. Wu, C. S., Lee, C. J., &Tsai, L. F. (2012). Influence of creativity and knowledge sharing on performance. Journal of Technology Management in China, 7(1), 64–77. Wu, C., McMullen, J. S., Neubert, M. J., &Yi, X. (2008). The influence of leader regulatory focus on employee creativity. Journal of Business Venturing, 23(5), 587–602. Yoon, H. J., Sung, S. Y., Choi, J. N., Lee, K., & Kim, S. (2015). Tangible and intangible rewards and employee creativity: The mediating role of situational extrinsic motivation. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 383–393. Zacher, H., Robinson, A. J., & Rosing, K. (2016). Ambidextrous leadership and employees' self‐reported innovative performance: The role of exploration and exploitation behaviors. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(1), 24–46. Zhang, G., Chan, A., Zhong, J., & Yu, X. (2016). Creativity and social alienation: The costs of being creative. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 27(12), 1252–1276. Zhang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010a). Linking empowering leadership and employee creativity: The influence of psychological empowerment, intrinsic motivation, and creative process engagement. Academy of Management Journal, 53(1), 107–128. Zhang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010b). The influence of creative process engagement on employee creative performance and overall job performance: a curvilinear assessment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(5), 862–873. Zhang, X., & Zhou, J. (2014). Empowering leadership, uncertainty avoidance, trust, and employee creativity: Interaction effects and a mediating 29
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
mechanism. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 124(2), 150–164. Zhao, H., Kessel, M., & Kratzer, J. (2014). Supervisor-Subordinate Relationship, Differentiation, and Employee Creativity: A Self‐Categorization Perspective. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(3), 165–184. Zhou, J., & George, J. M. (2001). When job dissatisfaction leads to creativity: Encouraging the expression of voice. Academy of Management Journal, 44(4), 682–696. Zhou, J., Shin, S. J., Brass, D. J., Choi, J., & Zhang, Z. X. (2009). Social networks, personal values, and creativity: evidence for curvilinear and interaction effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1544–1552. Zou, X., & Ingram, P. (2013). Bonds and boundaries: Network structure, organizational boundaries, and job performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120(1), 98–109. Appendix B. Studies considered but excluded from the meta-analyses Excluded Because Country Was Not Specified / Multiple Countries Were Included (Twenty-nine) Azoulay, P., Graff Zivin, J. S., &Manso, G. (2011). Incentives and creativity: evidence from the academic life sciences. The RAND Journal of Economics, 42(3), 527–554. Barrett, J. D., Vessey, W. B., Griffith, J. A., Mracek, D., & Mumford, M. D. (2014). Predicting scientific creativity: the role of adversity, collaborations, and work strategies. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 39–52. Beuk, F., & Basadur, T. (2016). Regulatory focus, task engagement and divergent thinking. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(2), 199–210. Bockstedt, J., Druehl, C., & Mishra, A. (2015). Problem-solving effort and success in innovation contests: The role of national wealth and national culture. Journal of Operations Management, 36, 187–200. Chiu, S. C. (2013). Position, experience, and knowledge creation: A longitudinal case study of Chinese academic networks. Social Behavior and Personality, 41(6), 1009–1018. Christensen, B. T., Hartmann, P. V., & Rasmussen, T. H. (2017). Threshold Theory Tested in an Organizational Setting: The Relation Between Perceived Innovativeness and Intelligence in a Large Sample of Leaders. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 188–193. Chua, R. Y., Roth, Y., & Lemoine, J. F. (2015). The impact of culture on creativity: How cultural tightness and cultural distance affect global innovation crowdsourcing work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60(2), 189–227. Criscuolo, P., Salter, A., & Ter Wal, A. L. (2014). Going underground: Bootlegging and individual innovative performance. Organization Science, 25(5), 1287–1305. Eubanks, D. L., Palanski, M. E., Swart, J., Hammond, M. M., & Oguntebi, J. (2016). Creativity in Early and Established Career: Insights into Multi‐Level Drivers from Nobel Prize Winners. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(4), 229–251. Furnham, A. (2015). The bright and dark side correlates of creativity: Demographic, ability, personality traits and personality disorders associated with divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 39–46. Godart, F. C., Maddux, W. W., Shipilov, A. V., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Fashion with a foreign flair: Professional experiences abroad facilitate the creative innovations of organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 58(1), 195–220. Hirst, G., Van Dick, R., & Van Knippenberg, D. (2009). A social identity perspective on leadership and employee creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(7), 963–982. Hirst, G., Van Knippenberg, D., & Zhou, J. (2009). A cross-level perspective on employee creativity: Goal orientation, team learning behavior, and individual creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 52(2), 280–293. Hirst, G., Van Knippenberg, D., Zhou, Q., Zhu, C. J., & Tsai, P. C. F. (2018). Exploitation and exploration climates’ influence on performance and creativity: Diminishing returns as function of self-efficacy. Journal of Management, 44(3), 870–891. Huang, C. E., & Liu, C. H. S. (2015). Employees and creativity: Social ties and access to heterogeneous knowledge. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 206–213. Kim, K., & Choi, S. B. (2017). Influences of creative personality and working environment on the research productivity of business school faculty. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 10–20. Kyvik, O., Zhang, Y., & Romero-Martinez, A. M. (2012). Value dimensions and creativity: an international comparative study. International Journal of Manpower, 33(4), 349–366. Li, E. Y., Liao, C. H., & Yen, H. R. (2013). Co-authorship networks and research impact: A social capital perspective. Research Policy, 42(9), 1515–1530. Liu, C. H. (2013). The processes of social capital and employee creativity: empirical evidence from intraorganizational networks. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 24(20), 3886–3902. Martinez, M. G. (2015). Solver engagement in knowledge sharing in crowdsourcing communities: Exploring the link to creativity. Research Policy, 44(8), 1419–1430. Pollick, M. F., & Kumar, V. K. (1997). Creativity styles of supervising managers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 31(4), 260–270. Rego, A., Sousa, F., Pina e Cunha, M., Correia, A., & Saur‐Amaral, I. (2007). Leader Self‐Reported Emotional Intelligence and Perceived Employee Creativity: An Exploratory Study. Creativity and Innovation Management, 16(3), 250–264. Rodan, S., & Galunic, C. (2004). More than network structure: how knowledge heterogeneity influences managerial performance and innovativeness. Strategic Management Journal, 25(6), 541–562. Saether, E. A., & Sætre, A. S. (2017). The impact of individual motivations on idea submission and future motivation to participate in an organization's virtual idea campaign. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(4), 379–390. Schweitzer, F., Gassmann, O., & Rau, C. (2014). Lessons from ideation: Where does user involvement lead us?. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(2), 155–167. Teigland, R., Chen, Y., & Fey, C. F. (2009). Contingency effects of national culture and institusions on how social networks influence creativity. Academy of Management Proceedings (Vol. 1), 1–6. Tortoriello, M., & Krackhardt, D. (2010). Activating cross-boundary knowledge: The role of Simmelian ties in the generation of innovations. Academy of Management Journal, 53(1), 167–181. 30
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Wong, S. S., & Boh, W. F. (2014). The contingent effects of social network sparseness and centrality on managerial innovativeness. Journal of Management Studies, 51(7), 1180–1203. Yunlu, D. G., Clapp-Smith, R., & Shaffer, M. (2017). Understanding the role of cultural intelligence in individual creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 236–243. Excluded Because of Non-Organizational Setting (Two Hundred and Thirty-three) Acar, S., & Runco, M. A. (2014). Assessing associative distance among ideas elicited by tests of divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 229–238. Agnoli, S., Franchin, L., Rubaltelli, E., & Corazza, G. E. (2015). An eye-tracking analysis of irrelevance processing as moderator of openness and creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 125–132. Agogué, M., Kazakçi, A., Hatchuel, A., Le Masson, P., Weil, B., Poirel, N., & Cassotti, M. (2014). The impact of type of examples on originality: Explaining fixation and stimulation effects. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(1), 1–12. Amado, D., Del Villar, F., Sánchez‐Miguel, P. A., Leo, F. M., & García‐Calvo, T. (2016). Analysis of the impact of creative technique on the motivation of Physical Education students in dance content: gender differences. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(1), 64–79. Anderson, L. R., & Fiedler, F. E. (1964). The effect of participatory and supervisory leadership on group creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48(4), 227–236. Anderson, R. C., Pitts, C., & Smolkowski, K. (2017). Creative Ideation Meets Relational Support: Measuring Links Between these Factors in Early Adolescence. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 244–256. Andreas, S. F., Zech, S., Coyle, T. R., & Rindermann, H. (2016). Unconventionality and Originality: Does Self-Assessed Unconventionality Increase Original Achievement?. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 198–206. Antes, A. L., & Mumford, M. D. (2009). Effects of time frame on creative thought: Process versus problem-solving effects. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 166–182. Baer, M., Leenders, R. T. A., Oldham, G. R., & Vadera, A. K. (2010). Win or lose the battle for creativity: The power and perils of intergroup competition. Academy of Management Journal, 53(4), 827–845. Baer, M., Oldham, G. R., Hollingshead, A. B., & Jacobsohn, G. C. (2008). The personality composition of teams and creativity: The moderating role of team creative confidence. Journal of Creative Behavior, 42, 255–282. Barczak, G., Lassk, F., & Mulki, J. (2010). Antecedents of team creativity: An examination of team emotional intelligence, team trust and collaborative culture. Creativity and Innovation Management, 19(4), 332–345. Basadur, M., & Head, M. (2001). Team performance and satisfaction: A link to cognitive style within a process framework. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 35(4), 227–248. Batey, M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Individual differences in ideational behavior: can the big five and psychometric intelligence predict creativity scores?. Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 90–97. Bateson, P., & Nettle, D. (2014). Playfulness, ideas, and creativity: A Survey. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 219–222. Baughman, W. A., & Mumford, M. D. (1995). Process-analytic models of creative capacities: Operations influencing the combination-and-reorganization process. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 37–62. Blazhenkova, O., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2016). Types of creativity and visualization in teams of different educational specialization. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 123–135. Bledow, R., Rosing, K., & Frese, M. (2013). A dynamic perspective on affect and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 56(2), 432–450. Botella, M., Zenasni, F., & Lubart, T. (2015). Alexithymia and affect intensity of fine artists. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(1), 1–12. Bouchard Jr, T. J., & Hare, M. (1970). Size, performance, and potential in brainstorming groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54(1p1), 51–55. Bowers, M. T., Green, B. C., Hemme, F., & Chalip, L. (2014). Assessing the relationship between youth sport participation settings and creativity in adulthood. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 314–327. Brass, J., & Niu, W. (2016). The Impact of Implicit Self-Theory on Rated Creativity in Third-and Sixth-Graders. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 318–327. Brown, G., & Baer, M. (2015). Protecting the turf: The effect of territorial marking on others’ creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(6), 1785. Burroughs, J. E., Dahl, D. W., Moreau, C. P., Chattopadhyay, A., & Gorn, G. J. (2011). Facilitating and rewarding creativity during new product development. Journal of Marketing, 75(4), 53–67. Burroughs, J. E., & Mick, D. G. (2004). Exploring antecedents and consequences of consumer creativity in a problem-solving context. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 402–411. Cantero, M. J., Alfonso-Benlliure, V., & Melero, R. (2016). Creativity in middle childhood: Influence of perceived maternal sensitivity, selfesteem, and shyness. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 105–113. Chen, B., Hu, W., & Plucker, J. A. (2016). The effect of mood on problem finding in scientific creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(4), 308–320. Chiang, N. C., & Chen, M. L. (2017). Benefits of Incubation on Divergent Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 282–291. Choi, H. S., & Thompson, L. (2005). Old wine in a new bottle: Impact of membership change on group creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 98(2), 121–132. Chou, C., Yang, K. P., & Jhan, J. (2015). Empowerment strategies for ideation through online communities. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 169–181. Clark, K., & James, K. (1999). Justice and positive and negative creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 12(4), 311–320. Cline, V. B., Richards Jr, J. M., & Needham, W. E. (1963). Creativity tests and achievement in high school science. Journal of Applied Psychology, 47(3), 184–189. Cohen, J. R., & Ferrari, J. R. (2010). Take some time to think this over: The relation between rumination, indecision, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 68–73. Collaros, P. A., & Anderson, L. R. (1969). Effect of perceived expertness upon creativity of members of brainstorming groups. Journal of Applied 31
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Psychology, 53(2p1), 159–163. Connolly, T., Jessup, L. M., & Valacich, J. S. (1990). Effects of anonymity and evaluative tone on idea generation in computer-mediated groups. Management Science, 36(6), 689–703. Cooper, R. B., & Jayatilaka, B. (2006). Group creativity: The effects of extrinsic, intrinsic, and obligation motivations. Creativity Research Journal, 18(2), 153–172. Copeland, C. T. (2016). Take some time to feel this over: Relations between mood responses, indecision, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 11–15. Corgnet, B., Espín, A. M., & Hernán-González, R. (2016). Creativity and cognitive skills among millennials: thinking too much and creating too little. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 1626. Cummings, L. L., Hinton, B. L., & Gobdel, B. C. (1975). Creative behavior as a function of task environment: Impact of objectives, procedures, and controls. Academy of Management Journal, 18(3), 489–499. Curşeu, P. L. (2010). Team creativity in web site design: An empirical test of a systemic model. Creativity Research Journal, 22(1), 98–107. Dahl, D. W., & Moreau, C. P. (2007). Thinking inside the box: Why consumers enjoy constrained creative experiences. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(3), 357–369. Dahmen-Wassenberg, P., Kämmerle, M., Unterrainer, H. F., & Fink, A. (2016). The relation between different facets of creativity and the dark side of personality. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 60–66. Dailey, A., Martindale, C., & Borkum, J. (1997). Creativity, synesthesia, and physiognomic perception. Creativity Research Journal, 10(1), 1–8. Deng, L., Wang, L., & Zhao, Y. (2016). How creativity was affected by environmental factors and Individual characteristics: A cross-cultural comparison perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 357–366. Dennis, A. R., & Valacich, J. S. (1994). Group, sub-group, and nominal group idea generation: New rules for a new media?. Journal of Management, 20(4), 723–736. De Pauw, A. S., Venter, D., & Neethling, K. (2011). The effect of negotiator creativity on negotiation outcomes in a bilateral negotiation. Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 42–50. Diener, M. L., Wright, C. A., Brehl, B., & Black, T. (2016). Socioemotional Correlates of Creative Potential in Preschool Age Children: Thinking Beyond Student Academic Assessments. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 450–457. Diener, M. L., Wright, C. A., Smith, K. N., & Wright, S. D. (2014). Assessing visual-spatial creativity in youth on the autism spectrum. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 328–337. Dollinger, S. J., Burke, P. A., & Gump, N. W. (2007). Creativity and values. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 91–103. Doron, E. (2016). Short Term Intervention Model for Enhancing Divergent Thinking among School Aged Children. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 372–378. Dostál, D., Plháková, A., & Záškodná, T. (2017). Domain‐specific creativity in relation to the level of empathy and systemizing. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(3), 225–239. Dow, G. T. (2015). Do cheaters never prosper? The impact of examples, expertise, and cognitive load on cryptomnesia and inadvertent selfplagiarism of creative tasks. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 47–57. Drus, M., Kozbelt, A., & Hughes, R. R. (2014). Creativity, psychopathology, and emotion processing: A liberal response bias for remembering negative information is associated with higher creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 251–262. Ederer, F., & Manso, G. (2013). Is Pay for Performance Detrimental to Innovation?. Management Science, 59(7), 1496–1513. Eisenman, R. (1969). Creativity and academic major: Business versus English majors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(5), 392–395. Ellwood, S., Pallier, G., Snyder, A., & Gallate, J. (2009). The incubation effect: Hatching a solution?. Creativity Research Journal, 21(1), 6–14. Erbas, A. K., & Bas, S. (2015). The contribution of personality traits, motivation, academic risk-taking and metacognition to the creative ability in mathematics. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 299–307. Fern, E. F. (1982). The use of focus groups for idea generation: the effects of group size, acquaintanceship, and moderator on response quantity and quality. Journal of Marketing Research, 19, 1–13. Firth, L., Alderson-Day, B., Woods, N., & Fernyhough, C. (2015). Imaginary Companions in Childhood: Relations to Imagination Skills and Autobiographical Memory in Adults. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 308–313. Fong, C. T. (2006). The effects of emotional ambivalence on creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 49(5), 1016–1030. Ford, C. M. (1999). Interpretive style, motivation, ability and context as predictors of executives’ creative performance. Creativity and Innovation Management, 8(3), 188–196. Friedrich, T. L., & Mumford, M. D. (2009). The effects of conflicting information on creative thought: A source of performance improvements or decrements?. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 265–281. Galati, F. (2015). Complexity of judgment: What makes possible the convergence of expert and nonexpert ratings in assessing creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 24–30. Garfield, M. J., Taylor, N. J., Dennis, A. R., & Satzinger, J. W. (2001). Research report: modifying paradigms—individual differences, creativity techniques, and exposure to ideas in group idea generation. Information Systems Research, 12(3), 322–333. Gasper, K. (2004). Permission to seek freely? The effect of happy and sad moods on generating old and new ideas. Creativity Research Journal, 16 (2–3), 215–229. Giambatista, R. C., & Bhappu, A. D. (2010). Diversity’s harvest: Interactions of diversity sources and communication technology on creative group performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111(2), 116–126. Gino, F., Argote, L., Miron-Spektor, E., & Todorova, G. (2010). First, get your feet wet: The effects of learning from direct and indirect experience on team creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111(2), 102–115. Girotra, K., Terwiesch, C., & Ulrich, K. T. (2010). Idea generation and the quality of the best idea. Management Science, 56(4), 591–605. Goncalo, J. A., Chatman, J. A., Duguid, M. M., & Kennedy, J. A. (2015). Creativity from constraint? How the political correctness norm influences creativity in mixed-sex work groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60(1), 1–30. Goncalo, J. A., & Duguid, M. M. (2012). Follow the crowd in a new direction: When conformity pressure facilitates group creativity (and when it does not). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(1), 14–23. Goncalo, J. A., & Staw, B. M. (2006). Individualism–collectivism and group creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 32
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
100(1), 96–109. Gostoli, S., Cerini, V., Piolanti, A., & Rafanelli, C. (2017). Creativity, Bipolar Disorder Vulnerability and Psychological Well-Being: A Preliminary Study. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 63–70. Gu, C., Hu, B. Y., Ngwira, F. F., Jing, Z., & Zhou, Z. (2016). The effect of general creative personality and freedom of task choice on adolescents' social creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(2), 132–149. Guo, J., & McLeod, P. L. (2014). The Impact of Semantic Relevance and Heterogeneity of Pictorial Stimuli on Individual Brainstorming: An Extension of the SIAM Model. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 361–367. Han, J., Long, H., & Pang, W. (2017). Putting Raters in Ratees’ Shoes: Perspective Taking and Assessment of Creative Products. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 270–281. Hannam, K., & Narayan, A. (2015). Intrinsic motivation, organizational justice, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 214–224. Hao, N., Ku, Y., Liu, M., Hu, Y., Grabner, R. H., & Fink, A. (2014). Enhancing verbal creativity via brief interventions during an incubation interval. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 30–38. Harrington, D. M., & Chin-Newman, C. S. (2017). Conscious Motivations of Adolescent Visual Artists and Creative Writers: Similarities and Differences. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 442–451. Hartzell, S. A., & Hong, E. (2016). Exploring the relationship between adolescent activities and choice of graduate school discipline: Implications for creativity development. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(2), 109–131. Hasirci, D., & Demirkan, H. (2007). Understanding the effects of cognition in creative decision making: A creativity model for enhancing the design studio process. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 259–271. Haught, C. (2015). The role of constraints in creative sentence production. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 160–166. Hinton, B. L. (1968). Environmental frustration and creative problem solving. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52(3), 211–217. Hoffmann, J., Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2016). Creativity in the Age of Technology: Measuring the Digital Creativity of Millennials. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 149–153. Hong, E., O’Neil, H. F., & Peng, Y. (2016). Effects of explicit instructions, metacognition, and motivation on creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 33–45. James, K., & Asmus, C. (2001). Personality, cognitive skills, and creativity in different life domains. Creativity Research Journal, 13(2), 149–159. James, K., & Eisenberg, J. (2004). Personal identity and group identity influences on algorithmic and original task performance. Creativity Research Journal, 16(1), 91–103. Jung, D. I. (2001). Transformational and transactional leadership and their effects on creativity in groups. Creativity Research Journal, 13(2), 185–195. Kabanoff, B., & Bottger, P. (1991). Effectiveness of creativity training and its relation to selected personality factors. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12(3), 235–248. Kachelmeier, S. J., Reichert, B. E., & Williamson, M. G. (2008). Measuring and motivating quantity, creativity, or both. Journal of Accounting Research, 46(2), 341–373. Kachelmeier, S. J., & Williamson, M. G. (2010). Attracting creativity: The initial and aggregate effects of contract selection on creativity-weighted productivity. The Accounting Review, 85(5), 1669–1691. Kandler, C., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., Spinath, F. M., Borkenau, P., & Penke, L. (2016). The nature of creativity: The roles of genetic factors, personality traits, cognitive abilities, and environmental sources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(2), 230. Kapoor, H. (2015). The creative side of the dark triad. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 58–67. Karwowski, M. (2015). Peer effect on students’ creative self‐concept. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(3), 211–225. Karwowski, M. (2016). The dynamics of creative self-concept: Changes and reciprocal relations between creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 99–104. Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D. M., Gajda, A., Marczak, M., Groyecka, A., & Sorokowski, P. (2016). Greater male variability in creativity outside the WEIRD world. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 467–470. Kasof, J. (1997). Creativity and breadth of attention. Creativity Research Journal, 10(4), 303–315. Kaufmann, G., & Vosburg, S. K. (2002). The effects of mood on early and late idea production. Creativity Research Journal, 14(3–4), 317–330. Kaufman, S. B., Kozbelt, A., Silvia, P., Kaufman, J. C., Ramesh, S., & Feist, G. J. (2016). Who finds Bill Gates sexy? Creative mate preferences as a function of cognitive ability, personality, and creative achievement. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(4), 294–307. Kemmelmeier, M., & Walton, A. P. (2016). Creativity in Men and Women: Threat, Other-Interest, and Self-Assessment. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 78–88. Kéri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: Latent inhibition, general intellectual functions and social network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5(3), 215–221. Khedhaouria, A., & Ribiere, V. (2013). The influence of team knowledge sourcing on team creativity: Evidences from information system development. The Learning Organization, 20(4/5), 308–321. Kim, J. (2015). Physical activity benefits creativity: squeezing a ball for enhancing creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 328–333. Kirkham, J. A., & Kidd, E. (2017). The effect of Steiner, Montessori, and national curriculum education upon children's pretence and creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(1), 20–34. Klein, S. P., & Owens Jr, W. A. (1965). Faking of a scored life history blank as a function of criterion objectivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49(6), 452–454. Kopcsó, K., & Láng, A. (2017). Regulated divergence: Textual patterns, creativity and cognitive emotion regulation. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 218–223. Kratzer, J., & Lettl, C. (2008). A social network perspective of lead users and creativity: An empirical study among children. Creativity and Innovation Management, 17(1), 26–36. Kristensson, P., Magnusson, P. R., & Matthing, J. (2002). Users as a hidden resource for creativity: Findings from an experimental study on user involvement. Creativity and Innovation Management, 11(1), 55–61. Kristensson, P., & Norlander, T. (2003). The creative product and the creative processes in virtual environments. Creativity and Innovation Management, 12(1), 32–40. 33
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Krumm, G., Lemos, V., & Filippetti, V. A. (2014). Factor structure of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Figural Form B in Spanish-speaking children: Measurement invariance across gender. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 72–81. Kurtzberg, T. R. (1998). Creative thinking, a cognitive aptitude, and integrative joint gain: A study of negotiator creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11(4), 283–293. Kuscevic, D., Kardum, G., & Brajcic, M. (2014). Visual preferences of young school children for paintings from the 20th century. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 297–304. Leahy, K. (2016). Winning the Future: An Investigation into the Creativity Capacity Across the Levels of Education in Ireland. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 188–197. LeBoutillier, N., Barry, R., & Westley, D. (2014). The role of schizotypy in predicting performance on figural and verbal imagery-based measures of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 461–467. Lee, Y. S., Chang, J. Y., & Choi, J. N. (2017). Why Reject Creative Ideas? Fear as a Driver of Implicit Bias Against Creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 225–235. Leikin, M., & Tovli, E. (2014). Bilingualism and creativity in early childhood. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 411–417. Lennartsson, A. K., Horwitz, E. B., Theorell, T., & Ullén, F. (2017). Creative Artistic Achievement Is Related to Lower Levels of Alexithymia. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 29–36. Li, Y. H., Tseng, C. Y., Tsai, A. C. H., Huang, A. C. W., & Lin, W. L. (2016). Different brain wave patterns and cortical control abilities in relation to different creative potentials. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 89–98. Liang, C., Chang, C. C., & Hsu, Y. (2014). Differential effects of personality traits and environmental predictors on reproductive and creative imagination. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(4), 237–253. Lin, W. L., & Shih, Y. L. (2016). Designing EEG Neurofeedback Procedures to Enhance Open-ended versus Closed-ended Creative Potentials. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 458–466. Lonergan, D. C., Scott, G. M., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). Evaluative aspects of creative thought: Effects of appraisal and revision standards. Creativity Research Journal, 16 (2–3), 231–246. Ma, C. C. E., & Rapee, R. M. (2015). Differences in mathematical performance, creativity potential, and need for cognitive closure between Chinese and Australian students. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(4), 295–310. MacCrimmon, K. R., & Wagner, C. (1994). Stimulating ideas through creative software. Management Science, 40(11), 1514–1532. Madjar, N., & Shalley, C. E. (2008). Multiple tasks' and multiple goals' effect on creativity: Forced incubation or just a distraction?. Journal of Management, 34(4), 786–805. Maier, N. R., & Hoffman, L. R. (1961). Organization and creative problem solving. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45(4), 277–280. Malycha, C. P., & Maier, G. W. (2017). The Random-Map Technique: Enhancing Mind-Mapping with a Conceptual Combination Technique to Foster Creative Potential. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 114–124. Mann, S., & Cadman, R. (2014). Does being bored make us more creative?. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 165–173. Marakas, G. M., & Elam, J. J. (1997). Creativity enhancement in problem solving: through software or process?. Management Science, 43(8), 1136–1146. Marguc, J., Van Kleef, G. A., & Förster, J. (2015). Welcome interferences: dealing with obstacles promotes creative thought in goal pursuit. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 207–216. Martín-Brufau, R., & Corbalán, J. (2016). Creativity and Psychopathology: sex matters. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 222–228. Mashal, N., & Coblentz, S. (2014). Creative Interpretations of Novel Conceptual Combinations in Aging. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 158–164. Maslej, M. M., Rain, M., Fong, K., Oatley, K., & Mar, R. A. (2014). The hierarchical personality structure of aspiring creative writers. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 192–202. Massetti, B. (1996). An empirical examination of the value of creativity support systems on idea generation. MIS Quarterly, 20(1), 83–97. McGlynn, R. P., McGurk, D., Effland, V. S., Johll, N. L., & Harding, D. J. (2004). Brainstorming and task performance in groups constrained by evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 93(1), 75–87. McLeod, P. L., Lobel, S. A., & Cox, T. H. (1996). Ethnic diversity and creativity in small groups. Small Group Research, 27(2), 248–264. Meadow, A., & Parnes, S. J. (1959). Evaluation of training in creative problem solving. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43(3), 189–194. Mecca, J. T., & Mumford, M. D. (2014). Imitation and creativity: Beneficial effects of propulsion strategies and specificity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(3), 209–236. Mihelič, K. K., & Aleksić, D. (2017). “Dear Employer, Let Me Introduce Myself”–Flow, Satisfaction with Work–Life Balance and Millennials’ Creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 397–408. Miller, A. L. (2014). A self-report measure of cognitive processes associated with creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 203–218. Miron-Spektor, E., & Beenen, G. (2015). Motivating creativity: The effects of sequential and simultaneous learning and performance achievement goals on product novelty and usefulness. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 127, 53–65. Miron-Spektor, E., Efrat-Treister, D., Rafaeli, A., & Schwarz-Cohen, O. (2011). Others' anger makes people work harder not smarter: The effect of observing anger and sarcasm on creative and analytic thinking. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(5), 1065–1075. Miron-Spektor, E., Gino, F., & Argote, L. (2011). Paradoxical frames and creative sparks: Enhancing individual creativity through conflict and integration. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(2), 229–240. Moreau, C. P., & Dahl, D. W. (2005). Designing the solution: The impact of constraints on consumers' creativity. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 13–22. Morrison, R. G., McCarthy, S. W., & Molony, J. M. (2017). The Experience of Insight Follows Incubation in the Compound Remote Associates Task. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(2), 180–187. Navaresse, D. O., Yauch, C. A., Goff, K., & Fonseca, D. J. (2014). Assessing the effects of organizational culture, rewards, and individual creativity on technical workgroup performance. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 439–455. Nemeržitski, S. (2017). Implicit Theories of Creativity of Secondary School Students from Estonia and Russia: Effects of Collectivism, Individualism, and A Bilingual Educational Environment. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 56–62. Nemiro, J., Larriva, C., & Jawaharlal, M. (2017). Developing creative behavior in elementary school students with robotics. The Journal of Creative 34
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Behavior, 51(1), 70–90. Osburn, H. K., & Mumford, M. D. (2006). Creativity and planning: Training interventions to develop creative problem-solving skills. Creativity Research Journal, 18(2), 173–190. Ottemiller, D. D., Elliott, C. S., & Giovannetti, T. (2014). Creativity, overinclusion, and everyday tasks. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 289–296. Owens, W. A. (1969). Cognitive, noncognitive, and environmental correlates of mechanical ingenuity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(3p1), 199–208. Paek, S. H., Park, H., Runco, M. A., & Choe, H. S. (2016). The contribution of ideational behavior to creative extracurricular activities. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 144–148. Paletz, S. B., & Peng, K. (2009). Problem finding and contradiction: Examining the relationship between naive dialectical thinking, ethnicity, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 139–151. Palmiero, M., Di Giacomo, D., & Passafiume, D. (2014). Divergent thinking and age-related changes. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 456–460. Palmiero, M., Nakatani, C., & van Leeuwen, C. (2017). Visual Creativity Across Cultures: A Comparison Between Italians and Japanese. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 86–90. Paulus, P. (2000). Groups, teams, and creativity: The creative potential of idea‐generating groups. Applied Psychology, 49(2), 237–262. Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82(1), 76–87. Pannells, T.C., & Claxton, A.F. 2008. Happiness, creative ideation, and locus of control. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1): 67–71 Pearsall, M. J., Ellis, A. P., & Evans, J. M. (2008). Unlocking the effects of gender faultlines on team creativity: Is activation the key?. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(1), 225–234. Peilloux, A., & Botella, M. (2016). Ecological and dynamical study of the creative process and affects of scientific students working in groups. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 165–170. Pérez-Fabello, M. J., Campos, A., & Meana, J. C. (2014). Vividness and control of mental imagery and the components of in-depth drawing. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 244–247. Perry-Smith, J. E. (2014). Social network ties beyond nonredundancy: An experimental investigation of the effect of knowledge content and tie strength on creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(5), 831. Pfundmair, M., Frey, D., & Hodgson, T. L. (2017). Taking the focus away from the self: Low individualism mediates the effect of oxytocin on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 91–96. Phipps, S. T., & Prieto, L. C. (2012). Knowledge is power? An inquiry into knowledge management, its effects on individual creativity, and the moderating role of an entrepreneurial mindset. Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 11(1), 43–57. Piffer, D., & Hur, Y. M. (2014). Heritability of creative achievement. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 151–157. Polzer, J. T., Milton, L. P., & Swarm, W. B. (2002). Capitalizing on diversity: Interpersonal congruence in small work groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47(2), 296–324. Porath, C. L., & Erez, A. (2009). Overlooked but not untouched: How rudeness reduces onlookers’ performance on routine and creative tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(1), 29–44. Prabhu, V., Sutton, C., & Sauser, W. (2008). Creativity and certain personality traits: Understanding the mediating effect of intrinsic motivation. Creativity Research Journal, 20(1), 53–66. Puccio, G. J., Treffinger, D. J., & Talbot, R. J. (1995). Exploratory examination of relationships between creativity styles and creative products. Creativity Research Journal, 8(2), 157–172. Puente-Díaz, R., & Cavazos-Arroyo, J. (2017). Creative self-efficacy: The influence of affective states and social persuasion as antecedents and imagination and divergent thinking as consequences. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 304–312. Puryear, J. S. (2015). Metacognition as a Moderator of Creative Ideation and Creative Production. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 334–341. Rastogi, D., & Sharma, N. K. (2010). Creativity under concurrent and sequential task conditions. Creativity Research Journal, 22(2), 139–150. Razumnikova, O. M., & Volf, N. V. (2015). Creativity-Related Hemispheric Selective Processing: Correlations on Global and Local Levels of Attentional Set. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 394–399. Razumnikova, O. M., Volf, N. V., & Tarasova, I. V. (2009). Strategy and results: Sex differences in electrographic correlates of verbal and figural creativity. Human Physiology, 35(3), 285–294. Reiter-Palmon, R., & Arreola, N. J. (2015). Does generating multiple ideas lead to increased creativity? A comparison of generating one idea vs. many. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 369–374. Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., O'Connor Boes, J., & Runco, M. A. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: The role of ability, cue consistency, and active processing. Creativity Research Journal, 10(1), 9–23. Reppa, G. P., & Theodorakou, K. (2015). The effect of creative gymnastics intervention on physical activity self-efficacy in primary school students. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 96–101. Riley, M. (1993). Organization and creativity: a replication of the KAI test. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 8(6), i-iv. Riquelme, H. (2000). How to develop more creative strategic plans: results from an empirical study. Creativity and Innovation Management, 9(1), 14–20. Ritter, S. M., Kühn, S., Müller, B. C., Van Baaren, R. B., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). The creative brain: Corepresenting schema violations enhances TPJ activity and boosts cognitive flexibility. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 144–150. Rogaten, J., & Moneta, G. B. (2015). Use of creative cognition and positive affect in studying: Evidence of a reciprocal relationship. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 225–231. Rook, L. (2014). Exposure to the color red enhances creative thinking depending on appetitive-aversive cues. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 124–130. Sagiv, L., Arieli, S., Goldenberg, J., & Goldschmidt, A. (2010). Structure and freedom in creativity: The interplay between externally imposed structure and personal cognitive style. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(8), 1086–1110. Sellier, A. L., & Dahl, D. W. (2011). Focus! Creative success is enjoyed through restricted choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(6), 996–1007. Sen, S. (2016). Applying the Mixed Rasch Model to the Runco Ideational Behavior Scale. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 426–434. 35
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Shalley, C. E. (1991). Effects of productivity goals, creativity goals, and personal discretion on individual creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(2), 179–185. Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on creativity and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 38(2), 483–503. Shalley, C. E., & Oldham, G. R. (1997). Competition and creative performance: Effects of competitor presence and visibility. Creativity Research Journal, 10(4), 337–345. Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-psychological factors on creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected evaluation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84(1), 1–22. Sharma, S., & Babu, N. (2017). Interplay between creativity, executive function and working memory in middle-aged and older adults. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 71–77. Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and self-determination in personality. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 25–36. Silva, J. E., Ferreira, P., Coimbra, J. L., & Menezes, I. (2017). Theater and Psychological Development: Assessing Socio-Cognitive Complexity in the Domain of Theater. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 157–166. Simmons, A. L. (2011). The influence of openness to experience and organizational justice on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 9–23. Simmons, A. L., & Ren, R. (2009). The influence of goal orientation and risk on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 21(4), 400–408. Simper, N., Reeve, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2016). Effects of concept mapping on creativity in photo stories. Creativity Research Journal, 28(1), 46–51. Sitar, A. S., Černe, M., Aleksić, D., & Mihelič, K. K. (2016). Individual learning styles and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 334–341. Smith, R. E., MacKenzie, S. B., Yang, X., Buchholz, L. M., & Darley, W. K. (2007). Modeling the determinants and effects of creativity in advertising. Marketing Science, 26(6), 819–833. Sosik, J. J., Kahai, S. S., & Avolio, B. J. (1998). Transformational leadership and dimensions of creativity: Motivating idea generation in computer-mediated groups. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 111–121. Sousa, F. C., Monteiro, I. P., Walton, A. P., & Pissarra, J. (2014). Adapting creative problem solving to an organizational context: A study of its effectiveness with a student population. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(2), 111–120. Stephan, E. (2017). The influence of a foreign versus native language on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 426–432. Stone, T. H. (1971). Effects of mode of organization and feedback level on creative task groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 55(4), 324–330. Storme, M., Lubart, T., Myszkowski, N., Cheung, P. C., Tong, T., & Lau, S. (2017). A Cross‐Cultural Study of Task Specificity in Creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(3), 263–274. Street, W. R. (1974). Brainstorming by individuals, coacting and interacting groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59(4), 433–436. Sun, L., Xiang, W., Yang, C., Yang, Z., & Lou, Y. (2014). The Role of Sketching States in the Stimulation of Idea Generation: An Eye Movement Study. Creativity Research Journal, 26(3), 305–313. Taggar, S. (2001). Group composition, creative synergy, and group performance. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 35(4), 261–286. Taggar, S. (2002). Individual creativity and group ability to utilize individual creative resources: A multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal, 45(2), 315–330. Tan, C. S., & Qu, L. (2015). Stability of the positive mood effect on creativity when task switching, practice effect, and test item differences are taken into consideration. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(2), 94–110. Tan, J., Zhang, H., & Wang, L. (2015). Network closure or structural hole? The conditioning effects of network‐level social capital on innovation performance. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 39(5), 1189–1212. Taylor, D. W., Berry, P. C., & Block, C. H. (1958). Does group participation when using brainstorming facilitate or inhibit creative thinking?. Administrative Science Quarterly, 3 (1), 23–47. Tidikis, V., Ash, I. K., & Collier, A. F. (2017). The Interaction of Emotional Valence and Arousal on Attentional Breadth and Creative Task Performance. Creativity Research Journal, 29(3), 313–330. Toubia, O. (2006). Idea generation, creativity, and incentives. Marketing Science, 25(5), 411–425. Trench, M., & Minervino, R. (2015). Training to generate creative metaphors by reviving dormant analogies. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 188–197. Tyagi, T. K. (2016). Mathematical creativity and mathematical aptitude: A cross-lagged Panel analysis. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 328–333. Tyagi, T. K. (2017). Mathematical Intelligence and Mathematical Creativity: A Causal Relationship. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 212–217. Ulger, K. (2015). The structure of creative thinking: Visual and verbal areas. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 102–106. Van Leeuwen, W., & Baas, M. (2017). Creativity Under Attack: How People’s Role in Competitive Conflict Shapes Their Creative Idea Generation. Creativity Research Journal, 29(4), 354–369. Verhaeghen, P., Trani, A. N., & Aikman, S. N. (2017). On Being Found: How Habitual Patterns of Thought Influence Creative Interest, Behavior, and Ability. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 1–9. Vuculescu, O., & Bergenholtz, C. (2014). How to solve problems with crowds: a computer‐based simulation model. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(2), 121–136. Wang, L., Xu, X., Wang, Q., Healey, G., Su, L., & Pang, W. (2017). Are Individuals with Schizophrenia or Schizotypy More Creative? Evidence from Multiple Tests of Creative Potential. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 145–156. Weinstein, E. C., Clark, Z., DiBartolomeo, D. J., & Davis, K. (2014). A decline in creativity? It depends on the domain. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 174–184. Welter, M. M., Jaarsveld, S., & Lachmann, T. (2017). Problem Space Matters: The Development of Creativity and Intelligence in Primary School Children. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 125–132. Welter, M. M., Jaarsveld, S., van Leeuwen, C., & Lachmann, T. (2016). Intelligence and creativity: over the threshold together?. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 212–218. Weisskopf-Joelson, E., & Eliseo, T. S. (1961). An experimental study of the effectiveness of brainstorming. Journal of applied psychology, 45(1), 45–49. Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the classroom?. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 1–10. White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2016). Scope of semantic activation and innovative thinking in college students with ADHD. Creativity Research Journal, 36
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
28(3), 275–282. Wong, M. K. Y., & So, W. C. (2016). Spoken narrative assessment: A supplementary measure of children’s creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 471–477. Xiao, F., Wang, L., Chen, Y., Zheng, Z., & Chen, W. (2015). Dispositional and situational autonomy as moderators of mood and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 76–86. Yang, J. S., & Hung, H. V. (2015). Emotions as constraining and facilitating factors for creativity: Companionate love and anger. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 217–230. Yu, Q., Zhang, S., & Zhang, J. H. (2017). Association of Dopamine D2 Receptor Gene with Creative Ideation. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 108–113. Yuan, F., & Zhou, J. (2008). Differential effects of expected external evaluation on different parts of the creative idea production process and on final product creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 20(4), 391–403. Zhou, J. (1998). Feedback valence, feedback style, task autonomy, and achievement orientation: Interactive effects on creative performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(2), 261–276. Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2001). Enhancing creative performance: Effects of expected developmental assessment strategies and creative personality. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 35(3), 151–167. Ziller, R. C., Behringer, R. D., & Goodchilds, J. D. (1962). Group creativity under conditions of success or failure and variations in group stability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 46(1), 43–49. Zmigrod, S., Colzato, L. S., & Hommel, B. (2015). Stimulating creativity: modulation of convergent and divergent thinking by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 353–360. Excluded Because Creativity Was Measured at the Team Level of Analysis (Sixty-six) Açıkgöz, A., Günsel, A., Kuzey, C., & Seçgin, G. (2016). Functional diversity, absorptive capability and product success: The moderating role of project complexity in new product development teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(1), 90–109. Agostini, L., Nosella, A., & Filippini, R. (2016). Towards an Integrated View of the Ambidextrous Organization: A Second‐Order Factor Model. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(1), 129–141. Amabile, T. M., & Conti, R. (1999). Changes in the work environment for creativity during downsizing. Academy of Management Journal, 42(6), 630–640. Ancona, D. G., &Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Demography and design: Predictors of new product team performance. Organization Science, 3(3), 321–341. Andries, P., & Thorwarth, S. (2014). Should Firms Outsource their Basic Research? The Impact of Firm Size on In‐House versus Outsourced R&D Productivity. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 303–317. Blau, J. R., & McKinley, W. (1979). Ideas, complexity, and innovation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24 (2), 200–219. Büchel, B., Nieminen, L., Armbruster-Domeyer, H., & Denison, D. (2013). Managing stakeholders in team-based innovation: The dynamics of knowledge and trust networks. European Journal of Innovation Management, 16(1), 22–49. Carmeli, A., & Paulus, P. B. (2015). CEO ideational facilitation leadership and team creativity: The mediating role of knowledge sharing. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(1), 53–75. Chandrasekaran, A., Linderman, K., & Schroeder, R. (2015). The role of project and organizational context in managing high‐tech R&D projects. Production and Operations Management, 24(4), 560–586. Chang, S. C., Tein, S. W., & Lee, H. M. (2010). Social capital, creativity, and new product advantage: An empirical study. International Journal of Electronic Business Management, 8(1), 43. Chen, J., Neubaum, D. O., Reilly, R. R., & Lynn, G. S. (2015). The relationship between team autonomy and new product development performance under different levels of technological turbulence. Journal of Operations Management, 33, 83–96. Chen, M. H. (2006). Understanding the benefits and detriments of conflict on team creativity process. Creativity and Innovation Management, 15(1), 105–116. Chen, M. H. (2007). Entrepreneurial leadership and new ventures: Creativity in entrepreneurial teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 16(3), 239–249. Chen, M. H. (2009). Guanxi networks and creativity in Taiwanese project teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 18(4), 269–277. Curşeu, P. L., & ten Brink, T. (2016). Minority dissent as teamwork related mental model: implications for willingness to dissent and group creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 86–96. Dahl, D. W., & Moreau, P. (2002). The influence and value of analogical thinking during new product ideation. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(1), 47–60. De Dreu, C. K., & West, M. A. (2001). Minority dissent and team innovation: the importance of participation in decision making. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(6), 1191–1201. Dokko, G., Kane, A. A., & Tortoriello, M. (2014). One of us or one of my friends: How social identity and tie strength shape the creative generativity of boundary-spanning ties. Organization Studies, 35(5), 703–726. Eggers, F., Lovelace, K. J., & Kraft, F. (2017). Fostering creativity through critical thinking: The case of business start‐up simulations. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(3), 266–276. Fairchild, J., & Hunter, S. T. (2014). “We've Got Creative Differences”: The Effects of Task Conflict and Participative Safety on Team Creative Performance. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(1), 64–87. Ford, C. M., Sharfman, M. P., & Dean, J. W. (2008). Factors associated with creative strategic decisions. Creativity and Innovation Management, 17(3), 171–185. Gundry, L. K., Ofstein, L. F., & Monllor, J. (2016). Entrepreneurial team creativity: Driving innovation from ideation to implementation. Journal of Enterprising Culture, 24(01), 55–77. Hoegl, M., &Parboteeah, K. P. (2007). Creativity in innovative projects: How teamwork matters. Journal of Engineering and Technology Management, 24(1), 148–166. 37
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Hoever, I. J. J., Zhou, J., & van Knippenberg, D. (2018). Different strokes for different teams: The contingent effects of positive and negative feedback on the creativity of informationally homogeneous and diverse teams. Academy of Management Journal, (ja). Homan, A. C., Buengeler, C., Eckhoff, R. A., van Ginkel, W. P., & Voelpel, S. C. (2015). The interplay of diversity training and diversity beliefs on team creativity in nationality diverse teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(5), 1456. Hu, J., Erdogan, B., Jiang, K., Bauer, T. N., & Liu, S. (2018). Leader humility and team creativity: The role of team information sharing, psychological safety, and power distance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(3), 313. Huang, L., Gibson, C. B., Kirkman, B. L., & Shapiro, D. L. (2017). When is traditionalism an asset and when is it a liability for team innovation? A two-study empirical examination. Journal of International Business Studies, 48(6), 693–715. Hurley, R. F., &Hult, G. T. M. (1998). Innovation, market orientation, and organizational learning: an integration and empirical examination. Journal of Marketing, 62(3), 42–54. Jang, S. (2017). Cultural brokerage and creative performance in multicultural teams. Organization Science, 28(6), 993–1009. Jia, L., Shaw, J. D., Tsui, A. S., & Park, T. Y. (2014). A social–structural perspective on employee–organization relationships and team creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 57(3), 869–891. Jiang, H., & Zhang, Q. P. (2014). Development and validation of team creativity measures: A complex systems perspective. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 264–275. Khan, M. S., Breitenecker, R. J., Gustafsson, V., & Schwarz, E. J. (2015). Innovative entrepreneurial teams: The give and take of trust and conflict. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(4), 558–573. Kratzer, J., Leenders, R. T. A., & Van Engelen, J. M. (2004). Stimulating the potential: Creative performance and communication in innovation teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 13(1), 63–71. Kratzer, J., Leenders, R. T. A., & Van Engelen, J. M. (2006a). Team polarity and creative performance in innovation teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 15(1), 96–104. Kratzer, J., Leenders, R. T. A., & Van Engelen, J. M. (2006b). Managing creative team performance in virtual environments: an empirical study in 44 R&D teams. Technovation, 26(1), 42–49. Kurtzberg, T. R. (2005). Feeling creative, being creative: An empirical study of diversity and creativity in teams. Creativity Research Journal, 17(1), 51–65. Lee, Y. N., Walsh, J. P., & Wang, J. (2015). Creativity in scientific teams: Unpacking novelty and impact. Research Policy, 44(3), 684–697. Leenders, R. T. A., Van Engelen, J. M., & Kratzer, J. (2003). Virtuality, communication, and new product team creativity: a social network perspective. Journal of Engineering and Technology Management, 20(1), 69–92. Mannucci, P. V. (2017). Drawing Snow White and animating Buzz Lightyear: Technological toolkit characteristics and creativity in cross-disciplinary teams. Organization Science, 28(4), 711–728. McMahon, K., Ruggeri, A., Kämmer, J. E., & Katsikopoulos, K. V. (2016). Beyond idea generation: the power of groups in developing ideas. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 247–257. Merlo, O., Bell, S. J., Mengüç, B., & Whitwell, G. J. (2006). Social capital, customer service orientation and creativity in retail stores. Journal of Business Research, 59(12), 1214–1221. Mo, S., Ling, C. D., & Xie, X. Y. (2017). The curvilinear relationship between ethical leadership and team creativity: The moderating role of team faultlines. Journal of Business Ethics, 1–14. Nisula, A. M., & Kianto, A. (2016). Group Climate and Creativity in Temporary Innovation Camp Settings. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(1), 157–171. Peng, J., Wang, Z., & Chen, X. (2018). Does Self-Serving Leadership Hinder Team Creativity? A Moderated Dual-Path Model. Journal of Business Ethics, 1–15. Perry-Smith, J. E., & Shalley, C. E. (2014). A social composition view of team creativity: The role of member nationality-heterogeneous ties outside of the team. Organization Science, 25(5), 1434–1452. Pirola‐Merlo, A., & Mann, L. (2004). The relationship between individual creativity and team creativity: Aggregating across people and time. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(2), 235–257. Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2014). Effects of problem scope and creativity instructions on idea generation and selection. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 185–191. Rost, K. (2011). The strength of strong ties in the creation of innovation. Research Policy, 40(4), 588–604. Santos, C. M., Uitdewilligen, S., & Passos, A. M. (2015). Why is Your Team More Creative Than Mine? The Influence of Shared Mental Models on Intra‐group Conflict, Team Creativity and Effectiveness. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(4), 645–658. Shin, S. J., & Zhou, J. (2007). When is educational specialization heterogeneity related to creativity in research and development teams? Transformational leadership as a moderator. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(6), 1709–1721. Schippers, M. C., West, M. A., & Dawson, J. F. (2015). Team reflexivity and innovation: The moderating role of team context. Journal of Management, 41(3), 769–788. Shin, Y., & Eom, C. (2014). Team proactivity as a linking mechanism between team creative efficacy, transformational leadership, and risk‐taking norms and team creative performance. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(2), 89–114. Shin, N., & Jang, Y. J. (2017). Group Creativity Training for Children: Lessons Learned from Two Award‐Winning Teams. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(1), 5–19. Siegel, S. M., &Kaemmerer, W. F. (1978). Measuring the perceived support for innovation in organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(5), 553–562. Sung, S. Y., & Choi, J. N. (2012). Effects of team knowledge management on the creativity and financial performance of organizational teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(1), 4–13. Taylor, A., & Greve, H. R. (2006). Superman or the fantastic four? Knowledge combination and experience in innovative teams. Academy of Management Journal, 49(4), 723–740. Tang, C. (2010). An empirical study on firm R&D team's creativity: implications from China's hi-tech industries. Journal of Science and Technology Policy in China, 1(3), 275–284. Tang, C., Shang, J., Naumann, S. E., & Zedtwitz, M. (2014). How team identification and expertise identification affect R&D employees' creativity. 38
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 276–289. Tang, C., & Ye, L. (2015). Diversified knowledge, R&D team centrality and radical creativity. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 123–135. Tu, Y., Lu, X., Choi, J. N., & Guo, W. (2018). Ethical Leadership and Team-Level Creativity: Mediation of Psychological Safety Climate and Moderation of Supervisor Support for Creativity. Journal of Business Ethics, 1–15. Uzzi, B., & Spiro, J. (2005). Collaboration and creativity: The small world problem. American Journal of Sociology, 111(2), 447–504. Valentine, S., Godkin, L., Fleischman, G. M., &Kidwell, R. (2011). Corporate ethical values, group creativity, job satisfaction and turnover intention: The impact of work context on work response. Journal of Business Ethics, 98(3), 353–372. Vera, D., & Crossan, M. (2005). Improvisation and innovative performance in teams. Organization Science, 16(3), 203–224. Wang, X. H. F., Kim, T. Y., & Lee, D. R. (2016). Cognitive diversity and team creativity: Effects of team intrinsic motivation and transformational leadership. Journal of Business Research, 69(9), 3231–3239. West, M. A., & Anderson, N. R. (1996). Innovation in top management teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(6), 680–693. Zhang, W., Zhang, Q., & Song, M. (2015). How do Individual‐Level Factors Affect the Creative Solution Formation Process of Teams?. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 508–524. Excluded Because Creativity Was Measured at the Organizational Level of Analysis (Fifty-one) Badiola-Sánchez, A., &Coto-Millán, P. (2013). Creativity, Innovation and Technical Efficiency: An Analysis from the Nordic Countries and the Atlantic ARC. IUP Journal of Knowledge Management, 11(1), 58–71. Bellini, E., Dell'Era, C., Frattini, F., & Verganti, R. (2017). Design‐Driven Innovation in Retailing: An Empirical Examination of New Services in Car Dealership. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(1), 91–107. Bengtsson, L., Lakemond, N., Lazzarotti, V., Manzini, R., Pellegrini, L., & Tell, F. (2015). Open to a select few? Matching partners and knowledge content for open innovation performance. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 72–86. Beugelsdijk, S. (2008). Strategic human resource practices and product innovation. Organization Studies, 29(6), 821–847. Bogers, M., Foss, N. J., & Lyngsie, J. (2018). The “human side” of open innovation: The role of employee diversity in firm-level openness. Research Policy, 47(1), 218–231. Craig, J. B., Pohjola, M., Kraus, S., & Jensen, S. H. (2014). Exploring relationships among proactiveness, risk‐taking and innovation output in family and non‐family firms. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(2), 199–210. Crema, M., & Verbano, C. (2016). Managing intellectual capital in Italian manufacturing SMEs. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(3), 408–421. Crossland, C., Zyung, J., Hiller, N. J., & Hambrick, D. C. (2014). CEO career variety: Effects on firm-level strategic and social novelty. Academy of Management Journal, 57(3), 652–674. Custódio, C., Ferreira, M. A., & Matos, P. (2017). Do general managerial skills spur innovation?. Management Science. De Visser, M., & Faems, D. (2015). Exploration and Exploitation within Firms: The Impact of CEO s' Cognitive Style on Incremental and Radical Innovation Performance. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 359–372. Fang, G., Ma, X., Ren, L., & Zhou, Q. (2014). Antecedents of network capability and their effects on innovation performance: an empirical test of hi‐tech firms in China. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(4), 436–452. Fay, D., Shipton, H., West, M. A., & Patterson, M. (2015). Teamwork and organizational innovation: The moderating role of the HRM context. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 261–277. Fleming, L., King III, C., & Juda, A. I. (2007). Small worlds and regional innovation. Organization Science, 18(6), 938–954. Gao, G. Y., Xie, E., & Zhou, K. Z. (2015). How does technological diversity in supplier network drive buyer innovation? Relational process and contingencies. Journal of Operations Management, 36, 165–177. Giustiniano, L., Lombardi, S., & Cavaliere, V. (2016). How knowledge collecting fosters organizational creativity. Management Decision, 54(6), 1464–1496. Godart, F. C., Shipilov, A. V., & Claes, K. (2014). Making the most of the revolving door: The impact of outward personnel mobility networks on organizational creativity. Organization Science, 25(2), 377–400. Goduscheit, R. C., & Knudsen, M. P. (2015). How Barriers to Collaboration Prevent Progress in Demand for Knowledge: A Dyadic Study of Small and Medium‐Sized Firms, Research and Technology Organizations and Universities. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 29–54. Govendo, J. A. (2005). Workforce, diversity and corporate creativity. Handbook of Business Strategy, 6(1), 213–218. Grabner, I. (2014). Incentive system design in creativity-dependent firms. The Accounting Review, 89(5), 1729–1750. Gundry, L. K., Muñoz‐Fernandez, A., Ofstein, L. F., & Ortega‐Egea, T. (2016). Innovating in organizations: a model of climate components facilitating the creation of New value. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(2), 223–238. Haneda, S., & Ito, K. (2018). Organizational and human resource management and innovation: Which management practices are linked to product and/or process innovation?. Research Policy, 47(1), 194–208. Hong, J. C., Hwang, I. Y., & Lin, C. L. (2003). Chi and organizational creativity: A case study of three Taiwanese computer firms. Creativity and Innovation Management, 12(4), 202–210. Huang, S., Ding, D., & Chen, Z. (2014). Entrepreneurial leadership and performance in Chinese new ventures: A moderated mediation model of exploratory innovation, exploitative innovation and environmental dynamism. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(4), 453–471. Ingram, T. (2016). Relationships between talent management and organizational performance: The role of climate for creativity. Entrepreneurial Business and Economics Review, 4(3), 195. Jeong, I., & Shin, S. J. (2017). High-performance work practices and organizational creativity during organizational change: A collective learning perspective. Journal of Management, 0149206316685156. Labitzke, G., Svoboda, S., & Schultz, C. (2014). The role of dedicated innovation functions for innovation process control and performance–an empirical study among hospitals. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 235–251. Lazzarotti, V., Manzini, R., Nosella, A., & Pellegrini, L. (2016). Collaborations with scientific partners: The mediating role of the social context in fostering innovation performance. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(1), 142–156. 39
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Lefebvre, V. M., Raggi, M., Viaggi, D., Sia‐Ljungström, C., Minarelli, F., Kühne, B., & Gellynck, X. (2014). SMEs' preference for innovation networks: A choice experimental approach. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(4), 415–435. Liu, W., Zhang, P., Liao, J., Hao, P., & Mao, J. (2016). Abusive supervision and employee creativity: The mediating role of psychological safety and organizational identification. Management Decision, 54(1), 130–147. Magazzini, L., Pammolli, F., & Riccaboni, M. (2016). Real Options and Incremental Search in Pharmaceutical R&D Project Portfolio Management. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(2), 292–302. Manohar, S. S., & Pandit, S. R. (2014). Core values and beliefs: A study of leading innovative organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 125(4), 667–680. Matzler, K., Uzelac, B., & Bauer, F. (2014). The role of intuition and deliberation for exploration and exploitation success. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 252–263. Michelino, F., Lamberti, E., Cammarano, A., & Caputo, M. (2015). Measuring open innovation in the Bio‐Pharmaceutical industry. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 4–28. Mina, A., Bascavusoglu-Moreau, E., & Hughes, A. (2014). Open service innovation and the firm's search for external knowledge. Research Policy, 43(5), 853–866. Mostafa, M. (2005). Factors affecting organisational creativity and innovativeness in Egyptian business organisations: an empirical investigation. Journal of Management Development, 24(1), 7–33. Nadkarni, S., & Chen, J. (2014). Bridging yesterday, today, and tomorrow: CEO temporal focus, environmental dynamism, and rate of new product introduction. Academy of Management Journal, 57(6), 1810–1833. Naranjo‐Valencia, J. C., Jimenez‐Jimenez, D., & Sanz‐Valle, R. (2017). Organizational culture and radical innovation: Does innovative behavior mediate this relationship?. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(4), 407–417. Ozer, M., Demirkan, I., &Gokalp, O. N. (2013). Collaboration networks and innovation: does corporate lobbying matter?.Journal of Strategy and Management, 6(3), 286–308. Pahnke, E. C., Katila, R., & Eisenhardt, K. M. (2015). Who takes you to the dance? How partners’ institutional logics influence innovation in young firms. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60(4), 596–633. Park, R., & Kruse, D. (2014). Group incentives and financial performance: the moderating role of innovation. Human Resource Management Journal, 24(1), 77–94. Riivari, E., & Lämsä, A. M. (2014). Does it pay to be ethical? Examining the relationship between organisations’ ethical culture and innovativeness. Journal of Business Ethics, 124(1), 1–17. Schmid, T., Achleitner, A. K., Ampenberger, M., & Kaserer, C. (2014). Family firms and R&D behavior–New evidence from a large-scale survey. Research Policy, 43(1), 233–244. Scuotto, V., Santoro, G., Bresciani, S., & Del Giudice, M. (2017). Shifting intra‐and inter‐organizational innovation processes towards digital business: An empirical analysis of SMEs. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(3), 247–255. Sierzchula, W., Bakker, S., Maat, K., & Wee, B. (2015). Alliance formation in the automobile sector during an era of ferment. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(1), 109–122. Slater, S. F., Hult, G. T. M., & Olson, E. M. (2010). Factors influencing the relative importance of marketing strategy creativity and marketing strategy implementation effectiveness. Industrial Marketing Management, 39(4), 551–559. Tang, Y., Li, J., & Yang, H. (2015). What I see, what I do: How executive hubris affects firm innovation. Journal of Management, 41(6), 1698–1723. Tzabbar, D., & Margolis, J. (2017). Beyond the Startup Stage: The Founding Team’s Human Capital, New Venture’s Stage of Life, Founder–CEO Duality, and Breakthrough Innovation. Organization Science, 28(5), 857–872. Von Nordenflycht, A. (2007). Is public ownership bad for professional service firms? Ad agency ownership, performance, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 50(2), 429–445. Yeh-Yun Lin, C., & Liu, F. C. (2012). A cross-level analysis of organizational creativity climate and perceived innovation: The mediating effect of work motivation. European Journal of Innovation Management, 15(1), 55–76. Zampetakis, L. A., Gruys, M. L., & Dewett, T. (2014). Ideas and Implementation: The Effective Implementation of Novel Marketing Programmes in Small‐to Medium‐Sized Greek Firms. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(2), 168–182. Zdunczyk, K., & Blenkinsopp, J. (2007). Do organisational factors support creativity and innovation in Polish firms?. European Journal of Innovation Management, 10(1), 25–40. Excluded Because Creativity Was Measured at the Country Level of Analysis (Three) Colovic, A., & Lamotte, O. (2015). Technological Environment and Technology Entrepreneurship: A Cross‐Country Analysis. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(4), 617–628. Fang, Z., Xu, X., Grant, L. W., Stronge, J. H., & Ward, T. J. (2016). National Culture, Creativity, and Productivity: What’s the Relationship with Student Achievement?. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 395–406. Rossberger, R. J. (2014). National personality profiles and innovation: The role of cultural practices. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(3), 331–348. Excluded Because The Study Did Not Measure Creativity (Forty-five) Andersen, K. V. (2013). The problem of embeddedness revisited: Collaboration and market types. Research Policy, 42(1), 139–148. Behrens, J. (2016). A Lack of insight: An experimental analysis of R&D managers’ decision making in innovation portfolio management. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(2), 239–250. Ceylan, C., Dul, J., & Aytac, S. (2008). Can the office environment stimulate a manager's creativity?.Human Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing & Service Industries, 18(6), 589–602. Chang, C. C., Yao, S. N., Chen, S. A., King, J. T., & Liang, C. (2016). Imagining Garage Start-Ups: Interactive Effects of Imaginative Capacities on Entrepreneurial Intention. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 289–297. 40
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Christensen, K., Nørskov, S., Frederiksen, L., & Scholderer, J. (2017). In search of new product ideas: Identifying ideas in online communities by machine learning and text mining. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(1), 17–30. Cirella, S., Canterino, F., Guerci, M., & Shani, A. B. R. (2016). Organizational learning mechanisms and creative climate: Insights from an Italian fashion design company. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(2), 211–222. Damanpour, F., & Schneider, M. (2006). Phases of the adoption of innovation in organizations: Effects of environment, organization and top managers. British Journal of Management, 17(3), 215–236. de Buisonjé, D. R., Ritter, S. M., de Bruin, S., ter Horst, J. M. L., & Meeldijk, A. (2017). Facilitating creative idea selection: The combined effects of self-affirmation, promotion focus and positive affect. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 174–181. Delmestri, G., Montanari, F., & Usai, A. (2005). Reputation and strength of ties in predicting commercial success and artistic merit of independents in the italian feature film industry. Journal of Management Studies, 42(5), 975–1002. Eling, K., Langerak, F., & Griffin, A. (2015). The Performance effects of combining rationality and intuition in making early new product idea evaluation decisions. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 464–477. Eschleman, K. J., Mathieu, M., & Cooper, J. (2017). Creating a Recovery Filled Weekend: The Moderating Effect of Occupation Type on the Relationship between Non-work Creative Activity and State of Feeling Recovered at Work. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 97–107. Frederiksen, M. H., & Knudsen, M. P. (2017). From Creative Ideas to Innovation Performance: The Role of Assessment Criteria. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(1), 60–74. Gambardella, A., Ganco, M., & Honoré, F. (2014). Using what you know: Patented knowledge in incumbent firms and employee entrepreneurship. Organization Science, 26(2), 456–474. Gilson, L. L., Mathieu, J. E., Shalley, C. E., & Ruddy, T. M. (2005). Creativity and standardization: complementary or conflicting drivers of team effectiveness?.Academy of Management Journal, 48(3), 521–531. Groves, K. S., & Vance, C. M. (2015). Linear and nonlinear thinking: A multidimensional model and measure. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(2), 111–136. Harrison, S. H., & Dossinger, K. (2017). Pliable Guidance: A Multilevel Model of Curiosity, Feedback Seeking, and Feedback Giving in Creative Work. Academy of Management Journal, 60(6), 2051–2072. Hartley, K. A., & Plucker, J. A. (2014). Teacher use of creativity-enhancing activities in Chinese and American elementary classrooms. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 389–399. Hong, S. W., & Lee, J. S. (2015). Nonexpert evaluations on architectural design creativity across cultures. Creativity Research Journal, 27(4), 314–321. Ibérico Nogueira, S., Almeida, L., Garcês, S., Pocinho, M., & Wechsler, S. (2016). The style troika model: A structural model of the thinking and creating styles scale. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(4), 333–346. Kawakami, T., & Parry, M. E. (2015). The Adoption of Electronic Innovations with Indirect Network Externalities that Compete with Standalone Physical Products. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 430–448. Keem, S., Shalley, C. E., Kim, E., & Jeong, I. (2018). Are creative individuals bad apples? A dual pathway model of unethical behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(4), 416–431. Kim, B. H., & Yu, J. (2015). Level of creativity and attitudes toward an advertisement. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 133–138. Kozbelt, A., Dexter, S., Dolese, M., Meredith, D., & Ostrofsky, J. (2015). Regressive imagery in creative Problem‐Solving: Comparing verbal protocols of expert and novice visual artists and computer programmers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(4), 263–278. Lee, H., Kim, J., Ryu, Y., & Song, S. (2015). Do people use their implicit theories of creativity as general theories?. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(2), 77–93. Lee, S. Y., & Min, J. (2016). The profiles of creative potential and personality characteristics of adult professionals. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 298–309. Leso, G., Dias, G., Ferreira, J. P., Gama, J., & Couceiro, M. S. (2017). Perception of creativity and game intelligence in soccer. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 182–187. Livingstone, L. P., Nelson, D. L., & Barr, S. H. (1997). Person-environment fit and creativity: an examination of supply-value and demand-ability versions of fit. Journal of Management, 23(2), 119–146. Ma, R., Huang, Y. C., &Shenkar, O. (2011). Social networks and opportunity recognition: A cultural comparison between Taiwan and the United States. Strategic Management Journal, 32(11), 1183–1205. Maritz, A. (2010). Networking, entrepreneurship and productivity in universities. Innovation: Management, Policy & Practice, 12(1), 18–25. Mehra, A., Kilduff, M., & Brass, D. J. (2001). The social networks of high and low self-monitors: Implications for workplace performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46(1), 121–146. Middlewood, B. L., Gallegos, J., & Gasper, K. (2016). Embracing the Unusual: Feeling Tired and Happy is Associated With Greater Acceptance of Atypical Ideas. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 310–317. Moneta, G. B. (2012). Opportunity for creativity in the job as a moderator of the relation between trait intrinsic motivation and flow in work. Motivation and Emotion, 36(4), 491–503. Puech, L., & Durand, T. (2017). Classification of time spent in the intrapreneurial process. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(2), 142–151. Ramos, S. J., & Puccio, G. J. (2014). Cross-cultural studies of implicit theories of creativity: A comparative analysis between the United States and the main ethnic groups in Singapore. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 223–228. Sacchetti, S., & Tortia, E. C. (2013). Satisfaction with creativity: A study of organizational characteristics and individual motivation. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(6), 1789–1811. Sarooghi, H., Libaers, D., & Burkemper, A. (2015). Examining the relationship between creativity and innovation: A meta-analysis of organizational, cultural, and environmental factors. Journal of Business Venturing, 30(5), 714–731. Schnurr, B. (2017). How my product works and how it looks: Effects of functional and aesthetic co‐creation and the role of product expertise. Creativity and Innovation Management, 26(2), 152–159. Tang, C., & Kaufman, J. C. (2017). Personal characteristics that distinguish creative scientists from less creative scientists. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(3), 204–215. Tang, C., Baer, J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Implicit theories of creativity in computer science in the United States and China. The Journal of 41
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Creative Behavior, 49(2), 137–156. Tortoriello, M., McEvily, B., & Krackhardt, D. (2014). Being a catalyst of innovation: The role of knowledge diversity and network closure. Organization Science, 26(2), 423–438. Vincent, L. C., & Kouchaki, M. (2016). Creative, rare, entitled, and dishonest: How commonality of creativity in one’s group decreases an individual’s entitlement and dishonesty. Academy of Management Journal, 59(4), 1451–1473. Wang, C. C., Ho, H. C., Cheng, C. L., & Cheng, Y. Y. (2014). Application of the Rasch Model to the measurement of creativity: The Creative Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 62–71. Wang, B., Duan, H., Qi, S., Hu, W., & Zhang, H. (2017). When a Dog Has a Pen for a Tail: the Time Course of Creative Object Processing. Creativity Research Journal, 29(1), 37–42. Zhang, D. (2015). Industrial designers: are you ready for foreign markets? Assessing designer confidence and prediction accuracy in a transnational marketing context. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 449–463. Zhou, J., Wang, X. M., Song, L. J., & Wu, J. (2017). Is it new? Personal and contextual influences on perceptions of novelty and creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(2), 180. Excluded Because Effect Sizes Could not be Coded into One of the Three Considered Amabile’s Creativity Components (Fifty-eight) Acar, S., Burnett, C., & Cabra, J. F. (2017). Ingredients of creativity: Originality and more. Creativity Research Journal, 29(2), 133–144. Açıkgöz, A., & Günsel, A. (2016). Individual creativity and team climate in software development projects: The mediating role of team decision processes. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(4), 445–463. Ahmed, F., Shahzad, K., Aslam, H., Bajwa, S. U., & Bahoo, R. (2016). The Role of Collaborative Culture in Knowledge Sharing and Creativity among Employees. Pakistan Journal of Commerce & Social Sciences, 10(2). Ahmetoglu, G., Dobbs, S., Furnham, A., Crump, J., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Bakhshalian, E. (2016). Dark side of personality, intelligence, creativity, and managerial level. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 31(2), 391–404. Ali Taha, V., Sirkova, M., & Ferencova, M. (2016). The impact of organizational culture on creativity and innovation. Polish Journal of Management Studies, 14. Amabile, T. M., Schatzel, E. A., Moneta, G. B., & Kramer, S. J. (2004). Leader behaviors and the work environment for creativity: Perceived leader support. Leadership Quarterly, 15(1), 5–32. Aubke, F. (2014). Creative Hot Spots: A Network Analysis of German Michelin‐Starred Chefs. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(1), 3–14. Audia, P. G., & Goncalo, J. A. (2007). Past success and creativity over time: A study of inventors in the hard disk drive industry. Management Science, 53 (1), 1–15. Báez, J. M., Bergua, J. A., & Pac, D. (2014). The creative class and the creative economy in Spain. Creativity Research Journal, 26(4), 418–426. Bai, Y., Lin, L., & Li, P. P. (2016). How to enable employee creativity in a team context: A cross-level mediating process of transformational leadership. Journal of Business Research, 69(9), 3240–3250. Beaudry, C., & Kananian, R. (2013). Follow the (industry) money–The Impact of science networks and industry-to-university contracts on academic patenting in nanotechnology and biotechnology. Industry and Innovation, 20(3), 241–260. Bénabou, R., Ticchi, D., & Vindigni, A. (2015). Religion and innovation. American Economic Review, 105(5), 346–51. Bennett, R., & Savani, S. (2011). Sources of New Ideas for Charity Fundraising: An Empirical Study. Creativity and Innovation Management, 20(2), 121–138. Björk, J., Di Vincenzo, F., Magnusson, M., & Mascia, D. (2011). The impact of social capital on ideation. Industry and Innovation, 18(6), 631–647. Caniëls, M. C., & Rietzschel, E. F. (2015). Organizing creativity: Creativity and innovation under constraints. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 184–196. Casanueva, C., & Gallego, Á. (2010). Social capital and individual innovativeness in university research networks. Innovation, 12(1), 105–117. Černe, M., Nerstad, C. G., Dysvik, A., & Škerlavaj, M. (2014). What goes around comes around: Knowledge hiding, perceived motivational climate, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 57(1), 172–192. Charyton, C., DeDios, S. L., & Nygren, T. E. (2015). Scientific productivity and idea acceptance in Nobel laureates. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 49(4), 245–262. Chen, M. H., & Chang, Y. C. 2005. The dynamics of conflict and creativity during a project's life cycle: a comparative study between service-driven and technology-driven teams in Taiwan. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 13(2), 127–150. Cheung, M. F., & Wong, C. S. (2011). Transformational leadership, leader support, and employee creativity. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 32(7), 656–672. Cirella, S. (2016). Organizational variables for developing collective creativity in business: A case from an Italian fashion design company. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(3), 331–343. Ekvall, G., & Ryhammar, L. (1998). Leadership style, social climate and organizational outcomes: A study of a Swedish University College. Creativity and Innovation Management, 7(3), 126–130. Ekvall, G., & Ryhammar, L. (1999). The creative climate: Its determinants and effects at a Swedish university. Creativity Research Journal, 12(4), 303–310. Forti, E., Franzoni, C., & Sobrero, M. (2013). Bridges or isolates? Investigating the social networks of academic inventors. Research Policy, 42(8), 1378–1388. Gevers, J. M., &Demerouti, E. (2013). How supervisors' reminders relate to subordinates' absorption and creativity. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 28(6), 677–698. Ghorbani, M., & Ahmadi, S. (2011). Relationship between employee's empowerment dimensions and creativity Improvement in educational organizations. Middle-East Journal of Scientific Research, 10(2), 213–217. Glăveanu, V. P. (2014). Revisiting the “art bias” in lay conceptions of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 11–20. Gong, Y., Kim, T. Y., Zhu, J., & Lee, D. R. (2013). A multilevel model of team goal orientation, information exchange, and creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 56(3), 827–851. Han, G. H., Harms, P. D., & Bai, Y. (2017). Nightmare bosses: The impact of abusive supervision on employees’ sleep, emotions, and creativity. 42
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Journal of Business Ethics, 145(1), 21–31. Hirst, G., Van Knippenberg, D., Zhou, J., Quintane, E., & Zhu, C. (2015). Heard it through the grapevine: Indirect networks and employee creativity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(2), 567. Hon, A. H. (2011). Enhancing employee creativity in the Chinese context: The mediating role of employee self-concordance. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 30(2), 375–384. Im, S., & Workman Jr, J. P. (2004). Market orientation, creativity, and new product performance in high-technology firms. Journal of Marketing, 68(2), 114–132. Kathan, W., Hutter, K., Füller, J., & Hautz, J. (2015). Reciprocity vs. Free‐Riding in Innovation Contest Communities. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(3), 537–549. Khalili, A. (2016). Linking transformational leadership, creativity, innovation, and innovation-supportive climate. Management Decision, 54(9), 2277–2293. Kirkhaug, R. (2009). Loyalty and creativity in a disciplinary organization. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 43(2), 135–147. Lebuda, I., & Karwowski, M. (2016). Written on the Writer’s Face: Facial Width-to-Height Ratio among Nominees and Laureates of the Nobel Prize in Literature. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 207–211. Leon, S. A., Altmann, L. J., Abrams, L., Gonzalez Rothi, L. J., & Heilman, K. M. (2014). Divergent task performance in older adults: declarative memory or creative potential?. Creativity research journal, 26(1), 21–29. Li, W., Yang, W., Li, W., Li, Y., Wei, D., Li, H., … & Zhang, Q. (2015). Brain structure and resting-state functional connectivity in university professors with high academic achievement. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 139–150. Liao, C. H. (2010). How to improve research quality? Examining the impacts of collaboration intensity and member diversity in collaboration networks. Scientometrics, 86(3), 747–761. Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., Liao, C., & Meuser, J. D. (2014). Servant leadership and serving culture: Influence on individual and unit performance. Academy of Management Journal, 57(5), 1434–1452. Malingumu, W., Stouten, J., Euwema, M., & Babyegeya, E. (2016). Servant leadership, organisational citizenship behavior and creativity: The mediating role of team-member exchange. Psychologica Belgica, 56(4), 1–15. McFadyen, M. A., Semadeni, M., & Cannella Jr, A. A. (2009). Value of strong ties to disconnected others: Examining knowledge creation in biomedicine. Organization Science, 20(3), 552–564. Mittal, S., & Dhar, R. L. (2016). Effect of green transformational leadership on green creativity: A study of tourist hotels. Tourism Management, 57, 118–127. Mobarakeh, N. S. (2011). The relation between the organizational culture and creativity of managers and experts of Khuzestan physical education organization. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 15, 3648–3650. Mohamed, A. D. (2016). The effects of modafinil on convergent and divergent thinking of creativity: a randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(4), 252–267. Mousavi, S. H., Heidary, A., & Pour, F. K. (2011). The relationship between leadership styles and physical education teachers' creativity. International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences, 1(3), 82–84. Muñoz-Doyague, M. F., & Nieto, M. (2012). Individual creativity performance and the quality of interpersonal relationships. Industrial Management & Data Systems, 112(1), 125–145. Palmiero, M., Di Giacomo, D., & Passafiume, D. (2016). Can creativity predict cognitive reserve?. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 50(1), 7–23. Rahim, M. A. (2014). A structural equations model of leaders' social intelligence and creative performance. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(1), 44–56. Randel, A. E., Jaussi, K. S., & Wu, A. (2011). When does being creative lead to being rated as creative? The moderating role of perceived probability of successfully bringing ideas to a supervisor's attention. Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 1–8. Rasulzada, F., & Dackert, I. (2009). Organizational creativity and innovation in relation to psychological well-being and organizational factors. Creativity Research Journal, 21(2–3), 191–198. Ren, F., & Zhang, J. (2015). Job stressors, organizational innovation climate, and employees’ innovative behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 16–23. Schulze, A. D., Stade, M. J. C., & Netzel, J. (2014). Conflict and conflict management in innovation processes in the life sciences. Creativity and Innovation Management, 23(1), 57–75. Son, S., & Kim, D. Y. (2016). The Role of Perceived Feedback Sources’ Learning-Goal Orientation on Feedback Acceptance and Employees’ Creativity. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 23(1), 82–95. Tang, C., & Naumann, S. E. (2016). The impact of three kinds of identity on research and development employees’ incremental and radical creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 123–131. Van Dyne, L., Jehn, K. A., & Cummings, A. (2002). Differential effects of strain on two forms of work performance: individual employee sales and creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23(1), 57–74. Yu, F., Peng, T., Peng, K., Zheng, S. X., & Liu, Z. (2016). The Semantic Network Model of creativity: Analysis of online social media data. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 268–274. Zhang, S., Zhang, M., & Zhang, J. (2014). An exploratory study on DRD2 and creative potential. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 115–123. Excluded Because There Were Not Hofstede Dimensions for the Country (One) Nejadirani, F., Abbaszadeh, M. S., & Rasouli, R. (2012). The relationship between knowledge culture and employee creativity in the state-run organizations of West Azerbaijan Province. African Journal of Business Management, 6(38), 10371–10380. Excluded Because of Insufficient Information on Effect Sizes (Eight) Arrighi, P. A., Le Masson, P., & Weil, B. (2015). Addressing constraints creatively: how new design software helps solve the dilemma of originality and feasibility. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 247–260. 43
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
Azadegan, A., Bush, D., & Dooley, K. J. (2008). Design creativity: static or dynamic capability?. International Journal of Operations & Production Management, 28(7), 636–662. Barrett, H., Balloun, J. L., & Weinstein, A. (2005). The impact of creativity on performance in non-profits. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 10(4), 213–223. Baumann, O., & Stieglitz, N. (2014). Rewarding value‐creating ideas in organizations: The power of low‐powered incentives. Strategic Management Journal, 35(3), 358–375. Kwon, J., Lee, M., & Kim, H. R. (2015). Does a creative designer necessarily translate into the creative design of a product? Exploring factors facilitating the creativity of a new product. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(4), 675–692. Lee, I. R., & Kemple, K. (2014). Preservice teachers' personality traits and engagement in creative activities as predictors of their support for children's creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 82–94. Marín‐Idárraga, D. A., Hurtado González, J. M., & Cabello Medina, C. (2016). The Antecedents of Exploitation‐Exploration and Their Relationship with Innovation: A Study of Managers' Cognitive Maps. Creativity and Innovation Management, 25(1), 18–37. Travis, F., & Lagrosen, Y. (2014). Creativity and brain-functioning in product development engineers: a canonical correlation analysis. Creativity Research Journal, 26(2), 239–243. Appendix C. Supplementary material Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2019.12.004.
De Dreu, C. K. W. (2010). Human creativity: Reflections on the role of culture. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 437–446. Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455–463. Earley, P. C. (1994). Self or group? Cultural effects of training on self-efficacy and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39(1), 89–117. Eaton, M. J., & Dembo, M. H. (1997). Differences in the motivational beliefs of Asian American and non-Asian students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 433–440. Elliot, A. J., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y., & Sheldon, K. M. (2001). A cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach) personal goals. Psychological Science, 12(6), 505–510. Erez, M., & Earley, P. C. (1987). Comparative analysis of goal-setting strategies across cultures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72(4), 658–665. Erez, M., & Nouri, R. (2010). Creativity: The influence of cultural, social, and work contexts. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 351–370. Eylon, D., & Au, K. Y. (1999). Exploring empowerment cross-cultural differences among the power distance dimension. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23(3), 373–385. Fischer, R., & Mansell, A. (2009). Commitment across cultures: A meta-analytical approach. Journal of International Business Studies, 40(8), 1339–1358. Fiss, P. C. (2007). A set-theoretic approach to organizational configurations. Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1180–1198. Fiss, P. C. (2011). Building better causal theories: A fuzzyset approach to typologies in organizational research. Academy of Management Journal, 54(2), 393–420. Fiss, P. C., Sharapov, D., & Cronqvist, L. (2013). Opposites attract? Opportunities and challenges for integrating large-N QCA and econometric analysis. Political Research Quarterly, 191–198. Florida, R., & Goodnight, J. (2005). Managing for creativity. Harvard Business Review, 83(7), 124–133. Fleming, L., Mingo, S., & Chen, D. (2007). Collaborative brokerage, generative creativity, and creative success. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52(3), 443–475. Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., & Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and importance of cultural tightness-looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6), 1225–1244. Gelfand, M. J., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nations study. Science, 332(6033), 1100–1104. George, J. M. (2007). Creativity in organizations. Academy of Management Annals, 1(1), 439–477. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interactional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 513–524. Goncalo, J. A., & Staw, B. M. (2006). Individualism-collectivism and group creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(1), 96–109. Greckhamer, T., Furnari, S., Fiss, P. C., & Aguilera, R. V. (2018). Studying configurations with qualitative comparative analysis: Best practices in strategy and organization research. Strategic Organization, 16(4), 482–495. Greckhamer, T., Misangyi, V. F., Elms, H., & Lacey, R. (2008). Using qualitative comparative analysis in management research: An examination of combinations of industry, corporate and business-unit effects. Organizational Research Methods, 11(4), 695–726. Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5(9), 444–454. Harzing, A., & Hofstede, G. (1996). Planned change in organizations. Research in the Sociology of Organizations, 14, 297–340. Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando: Academic Press. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broadbased behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 111–135. Heugens, P. P., & Lander, M. W. (2009). Structure! Agency!(and other quarrels): A metaanalysis of institutional theories of organization. Academy of Management Journal,
References Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 10(1), 123–167 Greenwich, CN: JAI Press. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Amabile, T. (2013). Componential theory of creativity. In E. Kessler (Ed.). Encyclopedia of management theory (pp. 135–140). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50(3), 367–403. Amabile, T. M., & Mueller, J. (2008). Assessing creativity and its antecedents: An exploration of the componential theory of creativity. In J. Zhou, & C. E. Shalley (Eds.). Handbook of organizational creativity (pp. 33–64). Lawrence: Erlbaum Associates. Amabile, T. M., & Pratt, M. G. (2016). The dynamic componential model of creativity and innovation in organizations: Making progress, making meaning. Research in Organizational Behavior, 36, 157–183. Anderson, N., Potočnik, K., & Zhou, J. (2014). Innovation and creativity in organizations: A state-of-the-science review, prospective commentary, and guiding framework. Journal of Management, 40(5), 1297–1333. Aycan, Z., Kanungo, R. N., & Sinha, J. B. (1999). Organizational culture and human resource management practices: The model of culture fit. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30(4), 501–526. Bandura, A. (1986). The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4(3), 359–373. Barron, F., & Harrington, D. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32(1), 439–447. Becker, H. S. (1982). Culture: A sociological view. Yale Review, 71(4), 513–527. Becker, M., et al. (2012). Culture and the distinctiveness motive: Constructing identity in individualistic and collectivistic contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(4), 833–855. Bell, R. G., Filatotchev, I., & Aguilera, R. V. (2014). Corporate governance and investors’ perceptions of foreign IPO value: An institutional perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 301–320. Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y. R. (2007). Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychological Review, 114(1), 133–151. Burt, R. S. (2004). Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology, 110(2), 349–399. Cable, D. M., & Edwards, J. R. (2004). Complementary and supplementary fit: A theoretical and empirical integration. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(5), 822–834. Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380–400. Chua, R. Y., Roth, Y., & Lemoine, J. F. (2015). The impact of culture on creativity: How cultural tightness and cultural distance affect global innovation crowdsourcing work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 60(2), 189–227. Chua, R. Y., Huang, K. G., & Jin, M. (2019). Mapping cultural tightness and its links to innovation, urbanization, and happiness across 31 provinces in China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(14), 6720–6725. Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 37–46. Cousins, S. (1989). Culture and selfhood in Japan and the U.S. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 124–131. Crook, T. R., Ketchen, D. J., Combs, J. G., & Todd, S. Y. (2008). Strategic resources and performance: A meta-analysis. Strategic Management Journal, 29(11), 1141–1154. Dalton, D. R., & Dalton, C. M. (2005). Strategic management as a special case for metaanalysis. In D. J. Ketchen, & D. D. Bergh (Vol. Eds.), Research methodology in strategy and management: Vol. 3, (pp. 31–63). New York, NY: Elsevier. Dalton, D. R., Aguinis, H., Dalton, C. A., Bosco, F. A., & Pierce, C. A. (2012). Revisiting the file drawer problem in meta-analysis: An assessment of published and non-published correlation matrices. Personnel Psychology, 65(2), 221–249.
44
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 157 (2020) 1–45
K. Yong, et al.
27(4), 753–779. Ng, A. K. (2003). A cultural model of creative and conforming behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2–3), 223–233. Nouri, R., Erez, M., Lee, C., Liang, J., Bannister, B. D., & Chiu, W. (2015). Social context: Key to understanding culture's effects on creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36(7), 899–918. Peterson, M. F. (2016). A culture theory commentary on Meyer and Peng’s theoretical probe into Central and Eastern Europe. Journal of International Business Studies, 47(1), 33–43. Perry-Smith, J. E., & Mannucci, P. V. (2017). From creativity to innovation: The social network drivers of the four phases of the idea journey. Academy of Management Review, 42(1), 53–79. Ragin, C. C. (2008). Redesigning social inquiry: Fuzzy sets and beyond. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Ragin, C. C., & Fiss, P. C. (2008). Net effects analysis versus configurational analysis: An empirical demonstration. In C. C. Ragin (Ed.). Redesigning social inquiry: Fuzzy sets and beyond (pp. 190–212). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Ragin, C. C., & Rihoux, B. (2009). Qualitative comparative analysis (QCA) and related systematic comparative methods. International Sociology, 21(5), 679–706. Rank, J., Pace, V. L., & Frese, M. (2004). Three avenues for future research on creativity, innovation, and initiative. Applied Psychology, 4(4), 518–528. Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Oakland, CA: Sage. Raudenbush, S., Bryk, A., Cheong, Y. F., & Congdon, R. (2004). HLM6. Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International Inc. Rietzschel, E. F., De Dreu, C. K., & Nijstad, B. A. (2007). Personal need for structure and creative performance: The moderating influence of fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(6), 855–866. Ronen, S., & Shenkar, O. (2013). Mapping world cultures: Cluster formation, sources and implications. Journal of International Business Studies, 44(9), 867–897. Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Schwarzer, R., Bäßler, J., Kwiatek, P., Schröder, K., & Zhang, J. X. (1997). The assessment of optimistic self-beliefs: Comparison of the German, Spanish, and Chinese versions of the general self-efficacy scale. Applied Psychology, 46(1), 69–88. Shin, J. S., & Zhou, J. (2007). When is educational specialization heterogeneity related to creativity in research and development teams? Transformational leadership as a moderator. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(6), 1709–1721. Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder, & R. A. LeVine (Eds.). Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158–199). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Simonton, D. K., & Ting, S.-S. (2010). Creativity in Eastern and Western civilizations: The lessons of historiometry. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 329–350. Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (1995). The creative cognition approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Sue-Chan, C., & Ong, M. (2002). Goal assignment and performance: Assessing the mediating roles of goal commitment and self-efficacy and the moderating role of power distance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89(2), 1140–1161. Suurmond, R., van Rhee, H., & Hak, T. (2017). Introduction, comparison, and validation of Meta-Essentials: A free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(4), 537–553. Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in action: Symbols and strategies. American Sociological Review, 51, 273–286. Taras, V., Kirkman, B. L., & Steel, P. (2010). Examining the impact of culture's consequences: A three-decade, multilevel, meta-analytic review of Hofstede's cultural value dimensions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(3), 405–439. Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 500–517. Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its potential antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45(6), 1137–1148. Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96(3), 506–520. Westwood, R., & Low, D. R. (2003). The multicultural muse: Culture, creativity and innovation. International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 3(2), 235–259. White, H. D. (1994). Scientific communication and literature retrieval. In H. Cooper, & L. V. Hedges (Eds.). The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 41–55). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. Zhou, J., & Shalley, C. E. (2010). Deepening our understanding of creativity in the workplace: A review of different approaches to creativity research. In S. Zedeck (Vol. Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology: Vol. 1, (pp. 275–302). Washington, DC: APA. Zhou, J., & Su, Y. (2010). A missing piece of the puzzle: The organizational context in cultural patterns of creativity. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 391–413.
52(1), 61–85. Hofstede, G. (1980). Motivation, leadership, and organization: Do American theories apply abroad? Organizational Dynamics, 9(1), 42–63. Hofstede, G. (1994). The business of international business is culture. International Business Review, 3(1), 1–14. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Hofstede, G., & McCrae, R. R. (2004). Personality and culture revisited: Linking traits and dimensions of culture. Cross-Cultural Research, 38(1), 52–88. Hu, J., Erdogan, B., Jiang, K., Bauer, T. N., & Liu, S. (2018). Leader humility and team creativity: The role of team information sharing, psychological safety, and power distance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(3), 313. Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 349–366. Joshi, A., Son, J., & Roh, H. (2015). When can women close the gap? A meta-analytic test of sex differences in performance and rewards. Academy of Management Journal, 58(5), 1516–1545. Kirkman, B. L., Lowe, K. B., & Gibson, C. B. (2006). A quarter century of culture's consequences: A review of empirical research incorporating Hofstede's cultural values framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 37(3), 285–320. Konstantopoulos, S. (2011). Fixed effects and variance components estimation in threelevel meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 2(1), 61–76. Kruglanski, A. W., & Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and unfreezing of impression primacy: Effects of the need for structure and the fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(4), 479–487. Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and “freezing”. Psychological Review, 103(2), 263–283. Lam, S. S., Chen, X. P., & Schaubroeck, J. (2002). Participative decision making and employee performance in different cultures: The moderating effects of allocentrism/ idiocentrism and efficacy. Academy of Management Journal, 45(5), 905–914. Lander, M. W., & Heugens, P. P. (2017). Better together: Using meta-analysis to explore complementarities between ecological and institutional theories of organization. Organization Studies, 38(11), 1573–1601. Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. Leonardi, P. M. (2011). Innovation blindness: Culture, frames, and cross-boundary problem construction in the development of new technology concepts. Organization Science, 22(2), 347–369. Leung, K., Bhagat, R. S., Buchan, N. R., Erez, M., & Gibson, C. B. (2005). Culture and international business: Recent advances and their implications for future research. Journal of International Business Studies, 36(4), 357–378. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Loewenstein, J., & Mueller, J. (2016). Implicit theories of creative ideas: How culture guides creativity assessments. Academy of Management Discoveries, 2(4), 320–348. Liu, D., Jiang, K., Shalley, C. E., Keem, S., & Zhou, J. (2016). Motivational mechanisms of employee creativity: A meta-analytic examination and theoretical extension of the creativity literature. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137, 236–263. Madjar, N., Oldham, G. R., & Pratt, M. G. (2002). There's no place like home? The contributions of work and non-work creativity support to employees' creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45(4), 757–767. Mannucci, P. V., & Yong, K. (2018). The differential impact of knowledge depth and knowledge breadth on creativity over individual careers. Academy of Management Journal, 61(5), 1741–1763. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. Meuer, J., Rupietta, C., & Backes-Gellner, U. (2015). Layers of co-existing innovation systems. Research Policy, 44(4), 888–910. Miron-Spektor, E., Erez, M., & Naveh, E. (2011). The effect of conformist and attentive-todetail members on team innovation: Reconciling the innovation paradox. Academy of Management Journal, 54(4), 740–760. Misangyi, V. F., Greckhamer, T., Furnari, S., Fiss, P. C., Crilly, D., & Aguilera, R. (2017). Embracing causal complexity: The emergence of a neo-configurational perspective. Journal of Management, 43(1), 255–282. Morris, M. W., & Leung, K. (2010). Creativity east and west: Perspectives and parallels. Management and Organization Review, 6(3), 313–327. Newman, K. L., & Nollen, S. D. (1996). Culture and congruence: The fit between management practices and national culture. Journal of International Business Studies,
45