Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 89 (3rd series) Dementias C. Duyckaerts, I. Litvan, Editors # 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved
Frontotemporal dementias Chapter 33
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: clinical and pathologic overview GIL D. RABINOVICI *, KATYA RASCOVSKY AND BRUCE L. MILLER UCSF Memory and Aging Center, San Francisco, CA, USA
33.1. Historical perspective “Circumscribed atrophies” were first described by the Czech-German neurologist and psychiatrist Arnold Pick, who in 1892 reported the case of a 71-year-oldman with behavioral symptoms and a progressive aphasia associated with focal left temporal lobe atrophy (Pick, 1892). Pick reported several other cases of circumscribed atrophies presenting with combined behavioral and language disturbances (Pick, 1904). In one (initially unpublished) case with a frontal syndrome, Alois Alzheimer (Alzheimer, 1911) observed swollen neurons and the argyrophilic “kugel”, later known as Pick’s body. Onari and Spatz performed extensive pathological studies on Pick’s case examined by Alzheimer and on similar cases (Onari and Spatz, 1926). Significantly, Onari and Spatz noted that in many cases characteristic “Pick bodies” (Fig. 33.3C), were entirely absent, without altering the remainder of the clinical or pathologic picture. Another salient observation was that cases could be divided into those with primarily frontal or primarily temporal atrophy. Tissot and Constantinidis confirmed these data and recognized three types of so-called “Pick disease” (clinically defined): (A) with swollen neurons and Pick bodies, (B) with only swollen neurons and (C) without swollen neurons or Pick bodies (Constantinidis et al., 1974). In 1982, Marsel Mesulam coined the term “primary progressive aphasia” to describe patients with focal left hemisphere degeneration who presented with either non-fluent or fluent aphasia, initially without additional cognitive deficits (Mesulam, 1982). In the late 1970s, Arne Brun and Lars Gustafson in Sweden,
and later David Neary and Julie Snowden in the UK, described the clinical, neuroimaging and pathologic features of a dementia syndrome characterized by focal frontal lobe degeneration, coined “frontal lobe dementia of the non-Alzheimer’s type” by the Swedish group (Brun, 1987), and “dementia of the frontal type” by the British group (Neary et al., 1988). Snowden and John Hodges described the language disturbance in patients with focal left anterior temporal atrophy, a syndrome they called “semantic dementia” (Snowden et al., 1989). Pathologically, these syndromes were all characterized by a typical gross and microscopic appearance, the absence of Alzheimer’s pathology, and the variable presence of Pick bodies. In 1994 the Swedish and British groups published the first consensus clinical and neuropathological criteria for “frontotemporal dementia” (Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia; The Lund and Manchester Groups, 1994). The clinical criteria were refined in 1998, using the term “frontotemporal lobar degeneration” (FTLD) to encompass three distinct clinical syndromes: frontotemporal dementia (FTD), a primary disturbance of behavior and comportment; progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA), a disturbance of expressive language; and semantic dementia (semantic dementia), a syndrome characterized by loss of meaning of verbal and non-verbal concepts (Neary et al., 1998). Further insight into the biology of the disease was provided by Kirk Wilhelmsen and colleagues, who discovered the link between familial frontotemporal dementia and a locus on chromosome 17 containing the gene for the microtubule-associated protein tau (Wilhelmsen et al., 1994b; Wilhelmsen, 1997).
*Correspondence to: Gil Rabinovici, MD, UCSF Memory & Aging Center, 350 Parnassus Ave., Suite 706, San Francisco, CA 94143, USA. E-mail:
[email protected], Tel: þ1-(415) 514-9320, Fax: þ1-415-476-4800.
344
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
The subsequent unveiling of multiple tau mutations associated with various FTLD phenotypes (Foster et al., 1997; Clark et al., 1998; Hutton et al., 1998; Poorkaj et al., 1998; Spillantini et al., 1998, 2000a, b) has strengthened the link between FTLD and other “tauopathies”, such as corticobasal degeneration (CBD) and progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) (Kertesz et al., 1999). While a link between motor-neuron disease and FTLD had long been suspected (Hudson et al., 1993), this association was found to be much more common than previously thought (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2002), and to be associated with a distinctive pathologic (Cooper et al., 1995; Jackson et al., 1996) and genetic (Hosler et al., 2000) profile. Most recently, the discovery of causative mutations in the secreted growth factor progranulin and the identification of the DNA-binding protein TDP-43 as a key component in the inclusions found in tau-negative FTLD cases have provided new and important clues towards understanding the pathophysiology of this clinically and pathologically heterogeneous disorder (Baker et al., 2006; Cruts et al., 2006; Gass et al., 2006, Neumann et al., 2006).
33.2. Epidemiology Frontotemporal lobar degeneration comprises 5–6% of all dementias, but 8–17% of early-onset (under 70) dementias in autopsy series (Barnett and Zucker, 1977; Knopman et al., 1990; Barker et al., 2002). The disease most commonly presents in the sixth decade, though presentation may occur as early as the third and as late as the ninth decade of life (Knopman et al., 1990; John and Srivastava, 1999; Barker et al., 2002; Ratnavalli et al., 2002; Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005). FTLD is generally considered an early-onset dementia, but up to one-quarter of cases can present after age 65 (Rosso et al., 2003a; Johnson et al., 2005). Progressive non-fluent aphasia presents later than other clinical syndromes in some series (Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005). FTLD approaches Alzheimer’s disease (AD) as the leading cause of dementia among patients under 65 (Ratnavalli et al., 2002). Population-based studies have estimated prevalence rates of 4.0–15/100,000 person years in the 45–64-year-old population in the Netherlands and Cambridge, UK, respectively. A number of large studies have detected a male preponderance in FTD, while some studies have suggested a male predominance for semantic dementia and a female preponderance in PNFA (Ratnavalli et al., 2002; Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005; Roberson et al., 2005). Median survival from symptom onset and diagnosis are 6 and 3 years respectively, with the clinical syndrome of FTD-MND associated with a more
rapidly progressive course (2 and 1 years respectively) (Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005; Roberson et al., 2005). Significantly, mean time from diagnosis to institutionalization can be as short as 1 year, probably due to the high prevalence of behavioral symptoms. Survival is shorter and cognitive and functional decline are much more rapid than in AD (Rascovsky et al., 2005; Roberson et al., 2005).
33.3. Genetics Up to 40% of FTLD cases are genetic, with most following an autosomal dominant pattern of inheritance (Stevens et al., 1998; Chow et al., 1999). FTD and FTD with motor neuron disease are more strongly familial than the other clinical syndromes (Goldman et al., 2005). The first association between autosomaldominant FTLD and chromosome 17 was described in 1994 (Lynch et al., 1994; Wilhelmsen et al., 1994a), giving rise to the clinical syndrome frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17 (FTDP-17). In 1998 it was found that many cases of FTDP-17 were caused by mutations in the microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT) (Wilhelmsen, 1997; Hutton et al., 1998; Spillantini et al., 1998). The causative gene in FTDP-17 with tau-negative, ubiquitin-positive pathology (see section 33.6: Pathology) was unknown until 2006, when mutations in the progranulin gene (PGRN) were found to account for most cases (Baker et al., 2006; Cruts et al., 2006; Gass et al., 2006). Over 30 mutations in MAPT and over 20 mutations in PGRN have been described to date (Goldman et al., 2004; Gass et al., 2006). Mutations in MAPT or PGRN can lead to a broad range of phenotypes, including all three variants of FTLD, CBD and PSP (the latter has only been described with MAPT mutations thus far). Clinical presentation can vary broadly even within single pedigrees (Benussi et al., 2006). Conversely, other families maintain a similar phenotype across generations (Masellis et al., 2006; Mesulam et al., 2007). Neuropathologic findings may also vary considerably between patients with MAPT mutations, while the pathologic findings associated with PGRN mutations are now being defined (Bird et al., 1999; Nasreddine et al., 1999; Mackenzie et al., 2006b; Spina et al., 2007). MAPT mutations can lead to disease by various mechanisms. Mutant protein may have an increased propensity to self-aggregate into Pick bodies or other neurofibrillary inclusions composed of various species of insoluble tau (Hasegawa et al., 1998; Rizzini et al., 2000; Neumann et al., 2001; Hayashi et al., 2002; Hogg et al., 2003; Pickering-Brown et al., 2004). Alternatively, mutations may lead to impaired microtubule
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 345 binding, assembly or axonal transport (Hasegawa et al., 1998; Hong et al., 1998; Murrell et al., 1999; Rizzu et al., 1999; Rizzini et al., 2000; Hayashi et al., 2002; Hogg et al., 2003). Many mutations have been shown to cause disease via both mechanisms. For example, mutations in the alternatively spliced exon 10 or in the intron that follows it can lead to over-representation of 4R isoforms (see section 33.6: Pathology), leading to poor microtubule binding and function, and aggregation of 4R insoluble tau (Hutton et al., 1998; Spillantini et al., 1998; D’Souza et al., 1999). The extended tau haplotypes H1 and H2 (Baker et al., 1999) may also affect tau processing, as the H1/H1 genotype is over-represented in PSP and CBD (Baker et al., 1999; Houlden et al., 2001), both 4R tauopathies. Finally, pre-symptomatic gene carriers have frontal/executive deficits on neuropsychologic tests decades before the predicted onset of dementia (Geschwind et al., 2001), raising the possibility of a neurodevelopmental component to what had hitherto been considered a purely degenerative disorder. Progranulin is a widely expressed secreted growth factor that plays a role in development, tumorigenesis, wound repair and inflammation (He and Bateman, 2003; He et al., 2003). Interestingly, most pathogenic mutations identified thus far are nonsense, splice-site or frameshift mutations that lead to premature coding termination and null alleles. Since most inheritance patterns are autosomal dominant, the hypothesized pathogenic mechanism involves loss of function, with inadequate growth factor support leading to neurodegeneration (Baker et al., 2006). The mechanisms by which this occurs have not yet been elucidated. Furthermore, though progranulin mutations lead to intra-neuronal and cytoplasmic inclusions composed of the DNA-binding protein TDP-43 (see section 33.6: Pathology), a direct relationship between TDP-43 and progranulin has not yet been established (Baker et al., 2006; Neumann et al., 2006). Familial FTLD has also been associated with other, rarer mutations. In one Dutch family an FTD-like phenotype is associated with a mutation in the endosomal ESCRTIII-complex subunit on chromosome 3 (Gydesen et al., 2002; Skibinski et al., 2005). Mutations in the valosin protein are linked to an autosomal dominant FTD syndrome associated with Paget’s disease and inclusion body myositis (Schroder et al., 2005). Families with FTD-ALS have been linked to chromosome 9 (Hosler et al., 2000). Of note, the apolipoprotein E4 allele, an established risk factor for AD, does not appear to be a risk factor for FTLD (Geschwind et al., 1998; Pickering-Brown et al., 2000; Riemenschneider et al., 2002).
33.4. Clinical syndromes Considerable confusion exists in the literature regarding the nomenclature of frontotemporal dementia, with similar clinical syndromes referred to by a variety of different names (Gustafson, 1987; Neary et al., 1988; Snowden et al., 1989; Mesulam, 2001). For the purposes of this chapter we have adopted the nomenclature used in the Neary diagnostic criteria (Neary et al., 1998). Under this nomenclature, the term frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) encompasses three distinct clinical syndromes: FTD, semantic dementia and PNFA. Each syndrome has a unique anatomy: FTD is characterized by bifrontal atrophy, semantic dementia by anterior temporal atrophy, and PNFA by left peri-sylvian atrophy (Fig. 33.1). While one clinical syndrome tends to predominate early on, with time atrophy tends to spread to previously unaffected brain regions, and the clinical syndromes may overlap (Neary et al., 1998). In addition to common pathologic features and genetics, the three syndromes share several clinical features. Onset of the disorder is insidious and gradually progressive, with first symptoms occurring most commonly (though not exclusively) before age 65. Notably absent from all syndromes are early, severe amnesia, spatial disorientation, and the physical signs of myoclonus, pyramidal weakness, cerebellar ataxia and choreoathetosis. Evidence of motor-neuron disease in a patient with a behavioral or language disorder is highly suggestive of FTLD (Neary et al., 1998). 33.4.1. Frontotemporal dementia (frontal-variant FTLD) Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a behavioral syndrome characterized by changes in personal and interpersonal conduct (Gustafson, 1987, 1993; Neary et al., 1988, 1998; Miller et al., 1991; The Lund and Manchester Groups, 1994). When FTD affects dorsomedial prefrontal structures, patients are apathetic and passive. They become withdrawn, losing interest in personal affairs and neglecting hygiene and personal responsibilities. Affect is blunted, and there is a decrease in spontaneous speech. Conversely, when the disease affects orbitofrontal and ventromedial frontal cortex, patients become disinhibited and childlike, with an inappropriately jovial, euphoric affect (Gustafson, 1987, 1993; Neary et al., 1988, 1998; Miller et al., 1991; The Lund and Manchester Groups, 1994; Levy et al., 1996; Liu et al., 2004). There is a decline in manners and social decorum, with a tendency to make rude or tactless comments to others.
346
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
Fig. 33.1. MRI findings in FTLD syndromes. (A)–(C) are coronal T1-weighted images, (D) is a coronal fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) image. All images are in radiologic orientation. (A) FTD: severe bilateral atrophy of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, insula, anterior cingulate, orbitofrontal cortex and caudate nuclei. (B) PNFA: asymmetric atrophy of left peri-sylvian cortex, including insula and frontal operculum. (C) semantic dementia: left greater than right anterior temporal atrophy involving amygdala, anterior hippocampus, inferior and middle greater than superior temporal gyri. (D) FLAIR image reveals white matter gliosis in left temporal lobe of a patient with semantic dementia.
Patients violate interpersonal space and show a new tendency to approach strangers. Disinhibition can lead to confrontation-seeking, and at times to frankly sociopathic behaviors such as unsolicited sexual approaches, traffic violations, shoplifting and physical assault (Gustafson, 1993; Miller et al., 1997; Mendez et al., 2005a). Patients may be aware that their behavior is wrong, but either lack emotional concern for its consequences, or fail to inhibit it (Mendez et al., 2005a). Patients may exhibit psychomotor hyperactivity, with a tendency to pace and wander. Speech output is increased and pressured, and patients often display “verbal dysdecorum”, speaking in an inappropriately high volume, and tending to interrupt others and manipulate conversation (Neary et al., 1998). Often, features of apathy and disinhibition are inter-mixed (Swartz et al., 1997a). Given the dramatic changes in behavior and personality, patients may first present to a psychiatric clinic (Gregory and Hodges, 1996). Apathetic behavior is confused with depression, while disinhibited behavior is mistaken for mania or hypomania (Gustafson,
1987). The presentation is one of an atypical mood disorder, with irritability, increased appetite and weight gain (as opposed to the anorexia and weight loss typical of AD). Suicidal ideations and guilt, which are common in late-life depression, are absent in FTD (Perry and Miller, 2001). In contrast to AD, where apathy and depression coexist, FTD patients have high apathy scores, often without depression (Levy et al., 1996). Delusions can be a presenting feature (in contrast to AD, where they appear late in disease). When present, delusions are often bizarre, and, in contrast to AD, rarely persecutory (Gustafson et al., 1987; Miller et al., 1991). Hallucinations and delusions of reference are rare (Swartz et al., 1997a). Patients universally show an emotional shallowness, with loss of warmth and empathy towards others (Gregory and Hodges, 1996; Neary et al., 1998; Rankin et al., 2005). They appear self-centered, with an inability to take others’ perspectives and a lack of concern for the implications of their behavior on others. There is a dramatic loss of insight, with either frank denial or a very shallow recognition of the illness
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 347 (often described as mild memory or word-finding difficulties) and no concern for its impact on social, occupational or financial matters. In extreme cases patients change religious beliefs, political conviction, dress and social style. These dramatic personality changes, likened to a “change in self”, are associated with right-predominant lesions (Miller et al., 2001). The combination of loss of empathy, apparent coldheartedness, loss of humor and abnormal non-verbal communication (e.g., loss of facial expression and prosody) leads to an “alien” affect that makes other people uneasy. This is in stark contrast to AD, where social graces are preserved late into the course. Stereotyped behaviors range from aberrant motor behaviors to complex compulsions (Ames et al., 1994; Miller et al., 1995; Mendez et al., 1997). Repetitive motor behaviors have similarities to those encountered in autism and schizophrenia (rubbing, picking, clapping, rocking) (Mendez et al., 2005b), and are more complex and idiosyncratic than the simple, stimulus-bound motor behaviors seen in AD (Nyatsanza et al., 2003). Compulsions are complex, and include repetitive checking of clocks, locks or windows, hygiene rituals, repetitive urination, throatclearing, counting, collecting and hoarding (Perry and Miller, 2001). Changes in eating behavior are highly characteristic, and include overeating (despite endorsing satiety), weight gain, overstuffing the mouth, and idiosyncratic food fads. Craving for sweet, carbohydrate- or lipid-rich foods is common, but less specific for FTD (Miller et al., 1995; Swartz et al., 1997a; Ikeda et al., 2002). Hyperorality may also manifest as excessive drinking, alcohol intake, cigarette smoking and gum- or tobacco-chewing (Gustafson, 1987). Oral exploration of inedible objects, as described in the Kluver-Bucy syndrome, may occur (Kluver and Bucy, 1937). Early in the disease patients may display increased somatic concern or even hypochondriasis (Gustafson, 1987). Later, the valence of disturbing signals from the internal milieu is lost, and patients show loss of disgust or pain (Bathgate et al., 2001; Snowden et al., 2001; Scherder et al., 2003). Patients willingly pick objects off the ground and place them in their mouth. A patient recently walked into our clinic completely unconcerned about the purulent burns on her arm. Indifference to bladder or bowel incontinence is also common. Patients may have an uncontrollable urge to explore the environment (utilization behavior), manifested by grasping at items in view, repeatedly switching lights on and off or opening and closing doors (Gustafson, 1993). Patients are often perseverative and discussions focus on recurrent themes. They develop characteristic “catch phrases”, and, in later
disease stages, manifest echolalia and mutism (Neary et al., 1998). Cognitive complaints are typically less dramatic than behavioral changes, and patients may require custodial care despite normal scores on screening cognitive tests such as the Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al., 1975; Gregory et al., 1999). Families may note that patients are disorganized, have difficulty with planning, multi-tasking and problem-solving, or are inattentive and distractible. Poor judgment is common. On neuropsychologic testing patients have deficits on frontal/executive tasks including tests of working memory, attention, set-shifting, mental flexibility, verbal fluency, response inhibition and abstract reasoning (Neary et al., 1988, 2005; Kramer et al., 2003; Rosen et al., 2004). However, performance on these tasks does not reliably differentiate FTD from AD (Frisoni et al., 1995b; Pachana et al., 1996; Gregory et al., 1997; Thomas-Anterion et al., 2000; Pasquier et al., 2001; Kertesz et al., 2003; Kramer et al., 2003; Rosen et al., 2004). There are at least three possible explanations for this: (1) impairments in executive skills, while present in FTD, are not specific to this condition (Kumar et al., 1990); (2) the complexity of these tasks causes poor performance by both patient groups; or (3) these tests measure impairment in dorsolateral prefrontal functioning, while it is the orbitofrontal and ventromedial frontal injury that occurs early and is more specific to FTD (Sarazin et al., 1998; Rahman et al., 1999a, b) (see Fig. 33.2B). Supporting this third hypothesis, FTD patients are more impaired than AD patients in theory-of-mind tasks (Gregory et al., 2002) that require making inferences about the mental status of others, and are mediated at least in part by ventromedial frontal cortex (Stuss et al., 2001). Memory complaints are variable, and often have a “frontal” flavor (paramnesias mistaking timing and context, confabulation) (Neary et al., 1988, 1998). Several studies have found that declarative verbal (Elfgren et al., 1994; Lindau et al., 1998; ThomasAnterion et al., 2000; Kramer et al., 2003; Rosen et al., 2004) and visual (Frisoni et al., 1995a; Pachana et al., 1996; Kertesz et al., 2003; Kramer et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2003) memory are preserved in FTD compared with AD. Temporal and spatial orientation and praxis are also spared (Elfgren et al., 1994; Mendez et al., 1996; Rascovsky et al., 2002). Driving difficulties are due to impulsivity and non-compliance with traffic regulations rather than topographical disorientation (Gustafson, 1987). Thus, a “cognitive profile” showing impairment on frontal/executive tasks with relative preservation of memory and visuospatial function discriminates FTD from AD in most cases
348
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
Fig. 33.2. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) demonstrates regional atrophy patterns in autopsy-proven cases of FTLD (n ¼ 13) and AD (n ¼ 7) (Rabinovici et al., unpublished results). (A) Compared to controls, both FTLD and AD show dorsolateral prefrontal volume loss. FTLD also demonstrates atrophy of medial frontal structures and the temporal poles, while in AD atrophy extends more posteriorly in the temporal and parietal lobes. (B) Direct comparison of FTLD with AD reveals selective atrophy of bilateral anterior cingulate, insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and striatum in FTLD. There was no significant difference in dorsolateral prefrontal regions. All images are displayed at P < 0.001 uncorrected.
(Elfgren et al., 1994; Pachana et al., 1996; Perry and Hodges, 2000; Rascovsky et al., 2002; Kramer et al., 2003; Diehl et al., 2005). Importantly, quantitative scores on visuospatial and memory tasks may be confounded by poor attention, organization or strategy and should be interpreted with caution (Varma et al., 1999). Qualitative observations of performance on cognitive testing, and especially the presence of rule violations, perseverative errors and confabulations, may be more helpful than quantitative scores in differentiating FTD from AD (Kramer et al., 2003; Nedjam et al., 2004). 33.4.2. Semantic dementia (temporal-variant FTLD) Semantic dementia is a disorder characterized by progressive loss of knowledge about words and concepts (Neary et al., 1998; Hodges, 2001). The syndrome was first characterized by Snowden et al. (1989), though in retrospect Pick’s initial case may have had semantic dementia (Hodges et al., 1992). Anatomically, semantic dementia is defined by progressive anterior temporal atrophy (Fig. 33.1C), with the presenting clinical syndrome strongly influenced by the
side of the brain with the greatest atrophy (Thompson et al., 2003). Left-predominant cases present as a fluent, anomic aphasia. Patients complain of wordfinding difficulties and trouble naming people, places and things (Seeley et al., 2005). Often this is perceived as a memory problem, but when the history is probed, episodic day-to-day memory is preserved. Rather, it is “semantic memory”, the knowledge of words, facts and concepts that is impaired. This form of memory depends less on context, is culturally shared, not temporally specific and acquired to a large extent early in life (Hodges et al., 1992). Speech remains fluent and effortless, with intact syntax and prosody, but impoverished content and semantic paraphasic errors. The meaning of words is lost despite preserved ability to read, write and spell them (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997). Loss of meaning follows a hierarchical pattern: first, patients lose the ability to differentiate between members of a subordinate category (e.g., types of apples), then within a category (e.g., apple versus orange) and finally within a supraordinate category (e.g., fruits versus vegetables). Initially a “poodle” is called a “dog”, then all dogs are called “animals”, and finally all animals are
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 349 referred to as “things” (Hodges et al., 1992). Eventually, broad generic terms entirely replace precise nominal terms. Word usage may be idiosyncratic— for example the word “container” may be used to describe all small objects, regardless of their facility to contain (Neary et al., 1998). Loss of knowledge extends beyond language, and patients lose the ability to put objects in their appropriate context, as demonstrated by the non-verbal Pyramids and Palm Tree task (Hodges et al., 1992; Howard and Patterson, 1992). This differentiates the anomia of semantic dementia from AD. When an AD patient cannot name a pencil, he may respond with a circumlocution (“that’s the thing you write with”). In turn, a semantic dementia patient might respond with “what is a pencil?” Eventually, patients are impaired in recognizing objects and non-verbal sounds and in demonstrating correct object use (Hodges and Graham, 1998; Lambon Ralph et al., 1998; Bozeat et al., 2000; Hodges et al., 2000). Surface dyslexia (reading “PINT” to rhyme with “MINT”) and surface dysgraphia with regularization errors (e.g., “CAUGHT” written as “CORT”) are common and easy to demonstrate at the bedside (Neary et al., 1998). These phenomena probably represent the loss of semantic knowledge of the correct pronunciation and spelling of irregular words (Patterson et al., 1994). On cognitive testing, patients fail at confrontation naming, word-to-picture matching, and category fluency tasks, with preservation of spatial abilities and executive functions (Hodges et al., 1992, 1999; Perry and Hodges, 2000; Thompson et al., 2003). Episodic memory is preserved early in the disease, as evidenced by patients’ ability to remember prospective appointments and keep track of the preceding days and weeks (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997). Interestingly, patients may show a reversal of the typical pattern of retrograde amnesia, with preserved recollection of recent personal events, coupled with significantly impaired recollection of remote autobiographical events from childhood and early adulthood (Graham and Hodges, 1997). Behavioral disturbances are usually not apparent early in the disease, and insight is typically preserved (Snowden et al., 2001). Depression is the most common affective disorder (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997). In contrast, patients who have predominantly right anterior temporal atrophy present with a behavioral syndrome characterized by a flat and sometimes bizarre affect, emotional blunting, and alterations in social conduct with rude, tactless, or awkward behavior in social situations (Mychack et al., 2001; Thompson et al., 2003). Initially, this is difficult to differentiate from a psychiatric illness. The first symptom may be somatic (e.g., weight changes (gain or loss), sleep
disturbance, decreased libido) or affective (e.g., irritability, apathy) (Seeley et al., 2005). Insight is typically absent (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997). Patients may exhibit elements of the “Geschwind syndrome” described in temporal-lobe epilepsy patients, including excessive verbal output, hypergraphia, intensification of philosophical or religious feelings and hyposexuality (Waxman and Geschwind, 1975; Benson, 1991). Early reports of right temporal variants emphasized the importance of prosopagnosia or associative agnosia (visual and tactile) (Evans et al., 1995), and these remain part of the diagnostic criteria (Neary et al., 1998). However, a behavioral syndrome usually precedes the agnosia by years (Seeley et al., 2005). The behavioral changes are caused, at least in part, by a breakdown in the recognition of emotions in others (Rosen et al., 2002b). Loss of fear is also characteristic—two patients followed in our clinic picked up rattlesnakes without any concern for injury to themselves or others. Patients who present with a semantic syndrome typically develop behavioral symptoms after a mean of 3 years, while the patients who begin with behavioral symptoms develop semantic deficits within a similar time frame. The progression of symptoms probably represents spread of atrophy to the contralateral temporal pole (Seeley et al., 2005). After a mean of 5–7 years, patients develop additional behavioral symptoms reminiscent of FTD, perhaps coincident with spread of disease to ventromedial and orbitofrontal cortex (Liu et al., 2004). These include changes in eating, such as restrictive dieting or food fads, that are distinct from the over-eating and hyperorality seen in FTD. Compulsions are also characteristic, with left temporal patients developing visually oriented compulsions (e.g., collecting bright objects or coins), while right temporal patients develop compulsions involving verbal or symbolic stimuli (e.g., playing word jumbles, quoting the bible, or playing solitaire) (Seeley et al., 2005). New artistic interests or talent may emerge in left temporal patients (Miller et al., 1998). The ratio of left to right temporal variants is around 3:1 in most series (Thompson et al., 2003; Seeley et al., 2005). This may represent a referral bias, since many right temporal patients are initially diagnosed with a psychiatric illness (Thompson et al., 2003). 33.4.3. Progressive nonfluent aphasia Progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA) is as a disorder of expressive language and speech production, characterized anatomically by left peri-sylvian atrophy (Fig. 33.1) (Hodges and Patterson, 1996; Turner et al., 1996; Neary et al., 1998; Hodges, 2001). Speech
350
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
is slow, effortful and telegraphic, with a relative increase in content (nouns and verbs) compared to function (articles, verb auxiliaries, prepositions) words. Patients make errors in word order, tense and conjugation. For example, “boy falled chair” may be used to express “the boy fell off the chair”. Collectively, these errors are known as “agrammatism”. Phonemic paraphasic errors are common—these may involve incorrect phoneme use (“kair” for “chair”) or phoneme transposition (“aminal” for “animal”). As in semantic dementia, word-retrieval deficits and anomia are common. Repetition is impaired, but comprehension of single word meaning and all but the most complex syntactic structures is preserved (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004b). Speech may be more fluent when a referrant is used (e.g., when describing a picture) than when attempting spontaneous speech (Mesulam et al., 2005). Reading is non-fluent and effortful, and writing is agrammatic with phonemic errors. Speech apraxia, defined as a failure of correct motor production of speech in the absence of primary motor weakness, is a common feature. This may present as difficulty in initiating speech, a slow rate of speech, or incorrect sequencing or omission of syllables (Duffy, 1995). The absence of motor weakness is confirmed by cranial nerve examination and by intact production of elemental sounds. Subtle speech apraxia can be demonstrated at the bedside by rapid repetition of multi-syllabic, consonant-rich words that require moving quickly from the back to the front of the mouth (e.g., “artillery” or “Methodist Episcopal”). Thus, impaired speech production in PNFA represents the merging of a language disorder with the failure of sensorimotor integration of speech, i.e., speech apraxia. Anatomically, the aphasia relates to atrophy of the left frontal operculum (Broca areas 44, 45 and 47), while the apraxia may relate to atrophy of the left anterior insula (Fig. 33.1B) (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004b). Interestingly, an identical clinical syndrome has been reported in left-handers with focal atrophy or hypometabolism of the corresponding structures in the right hemisphere (Drzezga et al., 2002; Mesulam et al., 2005). PNFA can be readily differentiated from the aphasia of semantic dementia, which is fluent, with intact grammar, semantic rather than phonemic paraphasic errors, and frequent use of nonspecific or supraordinate nouns. PNFA is more difficult to distinguish from “logopenic aphasia”, in which speech is slow, with frequent word-finding pauses. Logopenic aphasia is characterized by more severely impaired comprehension and the absence of agrammatism or speech apraxia. Anatomically, atrophy in logopenic aphasia occurs more posteriorly, in parietotemporal regions.
This anatomy, along with the high prevalence of ApoE4 carriers in one series, raises suspicion that logopenic aphasia may be associated with AD pathology (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004b). In contrast to FTD, personal and interpersonal conduct, behavior and insight are preserved early in PNFA. Spatial skills and short-term memory are also preserved until late in the course (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004b). However, neuropsychologic test performance in these domains may be compromised by the aphasia. Non-verbal testing should be performed to the extent possible. There may be a large discrepancy between apparently poor cognitive test results (including on the verbally oriented Mini-Mental State Exam) and normal function in daily life. To the extent they can compensate for the aphasia, PNFA patients continue to work, drive, and remain independent in many domains (Mesulam, 2003). Typically, the elemental neurologic examination is either normal, or may show subtle signs of hemiparesis or reflex asymmetry. However, the presence of rigidity, supranuclear gaze palsies or gait disturbance, in addition to more obvious signs such as limb apraxia, alien hand or myoclonus, should raise suspicion of underlying CBD or PSP pathology (Kertesz et al., 2000; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004c). As in all FTLD syndromes, a careful neuromuscular examination to exclude motor-neuron disease is paramount (Caselli et al., 1993). 33.4.4. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis The relationship between dementia and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) was first noted soon after the initial description of ALS in the late 1800s, but was underappreciated for many years. The emergence of geographic (Plato et al., 2002) and familial clustering (McGeer et al., 1997; Plato et al., 2002), as well as numerous reports of co-occurrence with the newly described frontal lobe dementias (Caselli et al., 1993; Cavalleri and De Renzi, 1994; Niizato et al., 1997; Portet et al., 2001), refocused attention on this possible link. Finally, the discovery of overlapping genetics (Foster et al., 1997) and neuropathology (Cooper et al., 1995; Jackson et al., 1996) strongly suggested a pathophysiologic connection between the two syndromes. In prospective studies, up to 50% of FTD patients had possible or probable ALS (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2002), while conversely 50% of ALS patients who underwent behavioral and neuropsychologic evaluation had measurable (mainly frontal/executive and behavioral) cognitive deficits, and many met criteria for an FTLD syndrome (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2003).
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 351 Patients who develop frontotemporal lobar degeneration with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (FTLD-ALS) may present with either cognitive or motor symptoms (Lomen-Hoerth, 2004). ALS can co-exist with any of the clinical FTLD syndromes, though FTD is most common (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2003). Patients with bulbar-onset ALS may be at increased risk of developing dementia compared to limb-onset patients (Neary et al., 2000; Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2003). FTLD can occur in association with a variety of upper-motor neuron (UMN) or lower-motor neuron (LMN) syndromes (Strong et al., 2003). While early data suggested that LMN presentations are more common in FTLD (Neary et al., 2000), recent data suggest that UMN signs are at least as frequent (Lomen-Hoerth and Miller, unpublished data). Fasciculations, generally considered benign in isolation, may herald the emergence of ALS in an FTLD patient (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2002). The presence of motor-neuron disease significantly worsens the prognosis of FTLD (Hodges et al., 2003; Roberson et al., 2005) and requires preventive interventions, including early evaluations of swallowing, nutrition and respiratory status. Every FTLD patient should undergo a thorough neuromuscular exam, with electrodiagnostic studies pursued in patients with even subtle physical signs. Similarly, the evaluation of all ALS patients should include a thorough behavioral history and screening cognitive testing. The MMSE alone is insufficient, as scores are often normal in early FTD. Word-generation tasks (which can be completed in the office in 2 minutes) are a more sensitive marker of cognitive dysfunction (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2003).
33.5. Diagnostic tests 33.5.1. Neuroimaging Structural neuroimaging, preferably with MRI, is an essential part of the evaluation of suspected FTLD (Fig. 33.1). Early in the course of disease, MRI may be essentially normal (Miller et al., 1991). As the disease progresses, focal atrophy of frontal and anterior temporal structures is nearly universally present (Miller and Gearhart, 1999). The ventricles enlarge, and the head of the caudate becomes atrophic. With advanced disease, atrophy becomes severe, and may produce a “knife blade” appearance of affected gyri (Fig. 33.1A). These findings are usually also apparent on CT (McGeachie et al., 1979; Kobayashi et al., 1984; Tobo et al., 1984). On MRI, T2 signal hyperintensity may be seen in the subcortical white matter of affected gyri, and may extend into the deep white matter (Fig. 33.1D) (Kitagaki et al., 1997). The corpus
callosum can appear atrophic either anteriorly or diffusely, differentiating FTD from AD, in which the posterior callosum is more prominently affected (Kaufer et al., 1997; Yamauchi et al., 2000). Quantitative volume comparisons between FTLD and controls, utilizing both region of interest analysis (Frisoni et al., 1996; Fukui and Kertesz, 2000; Chan et al., 2001; Galton et al., 2001) and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) (Mummery et al., 2000; Rosen et al., 2002a; Boxer et al., 2003; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004a; Boccardi et al., 2005), have detected distinct focal atrophy patterns for each of the FTLD syndromes. Both FTD and semantic dementia patients have volume loss in ventromedial frontal and insular cortices, anterior insula and anterior cingulate (Rosen et al., 2002a; Boccardi et al., 2005). FTD patients also show widespread atrophy throughout dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and medial premotor regions (Rosen et al., 2002a). Semantic dementia patients demonstrate anterior temporal atrophy involving the amygdala and anterior hippocampus, and anterior portions of the middle and inferior temporal gyri (Mummery et al., 2000; Chan et al., 2001; Galton et al., 2001; Rosen et al., 2002a). Atrophy in semantic dementia is often asymmetric, with left-sided predominance in most cases (Fig. 33.1C). In FTD atrophy is either symmetric, or with a right-hemisphere bias. Patients with PNFA show selective atrophy in the left peri-sylvian region, including the insula, middle and inferior frontal gyri and precentral gyrus (Fig. 33.1B) (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004b). FTD/ALS patients demonstrate atrophy in motor and premotor cortex, anterior temporal lobes, and middle and inferior frontal gyri (Chang et al., 2005). While most of these studies were based on clinical diagnosis, results have been confirmed in autopsy-proven cases (Fig. 33.2A) (Whitwell et al., 2005; Rabinovici et al., in press). Several studies have correlated regional atrophy with specific symptoms. Patients with right hemisphere degeneration tend to have a greater degree of behavioral disturbance (Miller et al., 1993, 2001; Miller and Gearhart, 1999; Liu et al., 2004; Seeley et al., 2005). Atrophy of ventromedial frontal cortex is correlated with disinhibition, ventral anterior cingulate with apathy, dorsal anterior cingulate and premotor cortex with aberrant motor behavior, left anterior temporal lobe with loss of semantic memory, and right amygdala with inability to recognize facial emotions (Mummery et al., 2000; Rosen et al., 2002b, 2005; Liu et al., 2004). Selective atrophy of medial and ventral frontal structures, including ventromedial and orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and anterior insula (but not dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) discriminates between FTLD and AD (Fig. 33.2B) (Rabinovici et al., in press).
352
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
These structures, in association with the amygdala and ventral striatum (which are also affected in FTLD), constitute the rostral limbic system. This network is involved in evaluating internal and external stimuli for motivational and emotional valence, and ultimately in regulating context-dependent, adaptive behavior (Devinsky et al., 1995; Boccardi et al., 2005). Dysfunction of this network may cause the FTLD behavioral syndrome. Functional imaging using xenon 133 (Stigsby et al., 1981; Gustafson, 1987), 99mTC-HMPAO SPECT (Neary et al., 1987; Talbot et al., 1995; Talbot and Morgan, 1998; Talbot et al., 1998) and 18FDG (Kamo et al., 1987) has been instrumental in elucidating the anatomical signatures of FTD (Neary et al., 1987; Miller et al., 1991; Salmon et al., 2003), semantic dementia (Edwards-Lee et al., 1997), PNFA (Chawluk et al., 1986) and FTLD-ALS (Caselli et al., 1993; Talbot et al., 1995; Garraux et al., 1999a). Functional imaging results essentially mirror the structural imaging findings for each clinical subtype. Some studies report extension of functional deficits to sub-cortical structures, including the striatum, globus pallidus and thalamus (Ishii et al., 1998; Jervis and Manson, 2002; Jeong et al., 2005). Functional imaging may be more sensitive than MRI in early disease (Miller et al., 1991; Miller and Gearhart, 1999) and is also helpful in differentiating FTLD from other dementias (Talbot et al., 1995, 1998; Ishii et al., 1998; Garraux et al., 1999a; Silverman et al., 2001). While characteristic of FTD, bifrontal hypoperfusion or hypometabolism has also been reported in AD (though usually accompanied by more posterior deficits) (Silverman et al., 2001), vascular dementia (Kim et al., 1996; Talbot et al., 1998), HIV dementia (Kim et al., 1996), PSP (Garraux et al., 1999b), Jakob-Creutzfeldt disease, psychiatric disorders and toxic-metabolic encephalopathies (Risberg et al., 1993), among others. As in structural imaging, focal functional deficits correlate with behavior and are helpful in elucidating brain– behavior relationships (Elfgren et al., 1996; Pasquier et al., 2003; Franceschi et al., 2005; Mendez et al., 2005a). Novel functional and molecular imaging techniques will improve diagnostic accuracy and provide further insight into FTLD pathophysiology. White matter changes in FTLD are more apparent using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) than on conventional MRI, correlating pathologically with frontal white matter gliosis and demyelination (Larsson et al., 2004). PET imaging of the nigrostiatal pathway using 11CCIT PET correlated extrapyramidal signs with a decrease in pre-synaptic dopaminergic neurons (Rinne et al., 2002). PET imaging with 11CMDL found a decrease in frontal 5HT2A receptors in FTD compared to con-
trols (Franceschi et al., 2005), as has been described in pathologic series. Molecular imaging of amyloid and tau (Shoghi-Jadid et al., 2002; Klunk et al., 2004; Nordberg, 2004), currently under active investigation in AD, may prove helpful in diagnosing taupositive forms of FTLD and differentiating between FTLD and AD (Rabinovici et al., 2007). 33.5.2. EEG The EEG in FTD is often normal (Johannesson et al., 1977, 1979; Neary et al., 1988), though diminution or slowing of normal background rhythm can be seen, especially in temporal variants and advanced disease (Chan et al., 2004). Quantitative spectral comparisons have shown significant differences between EEGs in FTD and AD, but these differences may not be apparent on qualitative assessment (Yener et al., 1996; Neary et al., 1998; Lindau et al., 2003; Chan et al., 2004). 33.5.3. CSF Lumbar puncture should be pursued only to exclude infectious or inflammatory conditions, as the basic cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) profile in FTLD is normal. CSF levels of unphosphorylated and phosphorylated tau have generated interest as potential biomarkers for FTLD. Results have varied, with CSF tau levels found to be lower (Grossman et al., 2005), equal (Sjogren et al., 2000; Clark et al., 2003; Grossman et al., 2005), or higher (Green et al., 1999; Fabre et al., 2001) in FTD than in controls. The majority of studies were based on clinical diagnosis, and thus represent a pathologically heterogeneous group. Of interest, CSF tau levels were found to be normal in patients with tau mutations (Rosso et al., 2003b), and low in a small number of patients with non-tau pathology (Grossman et al., 2005). These equivocal results do not justify the use of CSF tau for diagnosis in routine clinical practice.
33.6. Pathology 33.6.1. Common gross and microscopic changes FTLD is pathologically heterogeneous, but all subtypes share common gross and microscopic features. At autopsy, there is grossly apparent, circumscribed atrophy of the frontal or anterior temporal lobes (Fig. 33.3A) (McKhann et al., 2001). The pattern of atrophy in early disease determines which clinical syndrome predominates, with bifrontal atrophy leading to
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 353
Fig. 33.3. Pathology in FTLD. (A) Gross specimen shows selective frontal lobe atrophy. (B) Low-power H & E stain revealing neuronal loss, spongiosis and gliosis in frontal cortex. (C) Tau immunohistochemistry stains classic Pick bodies. (D) Ballooned tau-positive neuron (Pick cell), seen in Pick’s disease and other tauopathies. (E)–(F) Low- (E) and high-powered (F) views of tau-negative, ubiquitin-positive inclusions in dentate fascia in a patient with FTLD-U. Slides provided courtesy of Drs Stephen J. DeArmond, Mark S. Forman and John Q. Trojanowski.
FTD, anterior temporal presenting as semantic dementia, and dominant hemisphere peri-sylvian atrophy presenting as PNFA. Posterior cortical structures are spared until the advanced stages of the disease. Atrophy can be severe, with brain weights reported as low as 750 g (Dickson, 2001). Other grossly apparent changes include thinning of the cortical ribbon, blurring of the gray–white junction, discoloration and softening of white matter, and variable atrophy of the basal ganglia and thalamus (Dickson, 2001; Munoz et al., 2003).
A gross pathologic staging system has been proposed based primarily on findings in patients diagnosed with frontal-variant FTLD (FTD) during life (Broe et al., 2003). The earliest atrophy was found in orbital and superior medial frontal cortex and hippocampus (Stage 1). Atrophy then spread to adjacent anterior frontal structures, the temporal poles, inferior temporal cortex and the basal ganglia (caudate > putamen > globus pallidus; Stage 2). Further progression led to grossly apparent white matter atrophy, ventricular dilatation and thinning of the corpus callosum
354
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
(Stage 3). In advanced disease there was global cortical atrophy (though more severe anteriorly), further atrophy of the basal ganglia, involvement of the thalamus, and near dissolution of medial inferior temporal cortex (Stage 4). Hemispheric asymmetry was not apparent at the end-stage of disease. Progression of atrophy was independent of specific histopathologic subtype, and correlated with clinical severity (Broe et al., 2003; Kril and Halliday, 2004). Microscopically, routine stains reveal microvacuolar degeneration and pyramidal cell loss in superficial frontal and temporal cortex, creating a spongiform appearance (Fig. 33.3B). There is a variable degree of gliosis within cortex. Subcortical white matter, especially subjacent to the areas of greatest cortical atrophy, shows loss of myelinated fibers, gliosis, and axonal loss (Constantinidis et al., 1974; Brun, 1987, 1993; Brun and Passant, 1996; Dickson, 2001; McKhann et al., 2001; Munoz et al., 2003; Kertesz et al., 2005).
33.7. FTLD histologic subtypes After excluding Alzheimer’s and a-synuclein pathology, cases can be further classified pathologically into three subtypes based on the patterns of intracellular inclusions and immunohistochemical staining: (1) tau-positive pathology with or without inclusions (Pick’s disease and related disorders); (2) tau-negative, ubiquitinpositive inclusions (FTLD with motor-neuron disease inclusions); and (3) tau-negative, ubiquitin-negative pathology (dementia lacking distinctive histology) (McKhann et al., 2001). 33.7.1. Pick’s disease and other tauopathies (FTLD-T) Pick’s disease is characterized by the presence of classic Pick bodies—round or oval, intensely argyrophilic, cytoplasmic neuronal inclusions (Fig. 33.3C). Pick bodies are stained by Bielschowsky but not Gallyas stains, and are most readily detected by tau immunohistochemical stains. They are usually found in hippocampus (including dentate fascia), amygdala, and in superficial frontal and temporal neocortex (Dickson, 2001; McKhann et al., 2001; Munoz et al., 2003; Kertesz et al., 2005). They are uncommon in parietal and occipital cortex, and are variably found in the basal ganglia, monoaminergic nuclei, hypothalamus and brainstem. They stain variably for ubiquitin (Dickson, 2001). Pick bodies are only one of many tau-immunoreactive pathologic features. Others include ballooned achromatic neurons (Pick cells, Fig. 33.3D), and
intracytoplasmic neuronal and glial inclusions of various morphologies, including gray and white matter coils, threads and neurofibrillary tangles (Dickson, 2001). These inclusions are variably present in Pick’s disease, but are also found in the absence of Pick bodies in the family of diseases known as “tauopathies”. Given considerable overlap in morphology, further discrimination between syndromes can be achieved by tau biochemical analysis (Munoz et al., 2003). In humans, tau exists as six isoforms, each expressing either three or four repeated amino-acid sequences that serve as microtubulebinding sites. Exon 10 encodes the fourth repeated sequence, and alternative splicing includes or excludes exon 10, leading to 4-repeat (4R) or 3-repeat (3R) isoforms respectively. Pathologic tau may be predominantly composed of 3R, 4R or a mix of the two (Munoz et al., 2003). Pick’s disease is usually (but not always) a 3R tauopathy, argyrophilic grain disease is a 4R tauopathy (Braak and Braak, 1998; Jellinger and Bancher, 1998), and tangle-predominant dementia has a mix of 3R and 4R isoforms (Jellinger and Bancher, 1998). Frontotemporal dementia with Parkinsonism associated with chromosome 17 (FTDP-17) may include 3R, 4R, or mixed isoforms (McKhann et al., 2001). Corticobasal degeneration (CBD) and progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) are 4R tauopathies that have considerable clinical and pathologic overlap with FTLD (Kertesz, 1997). These diseases are covered elsewhere in this volume. 33.7.2. FTLD with ubiquitin- and TDP-43-positive inclusions (FTLD-U) Okamoto and colleagues first described ubiquitinpositive inclusions outside motor cortex in patients with ALS (Okamoto et al., 1991). Jackson et al. found similar inclusions associated with dementia, with or without clinically apparent motor-neuron disease (Cooper et al., 1995; Jackson et al., 1996). Inclusions are small, rounded or crescent-shaped, tau- and synuclein-negative, and ubiquitin-positive. They are most abundant in dentate gyrus granule cells in hippocampus (Fig. 33.3E,F), and in layer II neurons in frontotemporal cortex (McKhann et al., 2001). Inclusions may be cytoplasmic or nuclear, and may be accompanied by coiled or curvilinear ubiquitin-staining bodies within dystrophic neurites. Skein-like ubiquitin inclusions are variably present in anterior horn cells and cranial nerve nuclei (Shi et al., 2005). There is a wide range in the morphology and distribution of ubiquitin inclusions, with significant overlap between the clinical syndromes of ALS, FTD and FTD-ALS, suggesting that these syndromes are strongly related
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 355 (Mackenzie and Feldman, 2005). Three distinct patterns of ubiquitin inclusion pathology have recently been demonstrated: (i) long neuritic inclusions in superficial cortex; (ii) cytoplasmic inclusions in both superficial and deep cortex; and (iii) predominantly ring-shaped cytoplasmic inclusions in superficial cortex (Mackenzie et al., 2006a; Sampathu et al., 2006). The relationship between these heterogeneous pathologic patterns, clinical presentation and pathogenesis is not yet understood. For years the ubiquitin-positive inclusions found in FTLD were hypothesized to represent a proteolytic tag for another, unknown pathogenic protein. Recent work by Neumann and colleagues suggests this mysterious protein may be the TAR DNA-binding protein TDP43 (Neumann et al., 2006). TDP-43 is a ubiquitously expressed nuclear protein that plays a role in DNA transcription and splicing. TDP-43 is found in all forms of ubiquitin-inclusions in sporadic and familial FTLD-U, FTLD-U with motor-neuron disease, and in sporadic ALS, whereas it is not commonly found in inclusions in FTLD-tau or other neurodegenerative disorders (Arai et al., 2006; Neumann et al., 2006; Davidson et al., 2007). Interestingly, under pathologic conditions TDP-43 is displaced from the cell nucleus to the cytoplasm (Neumann et al., 2006). The sensitive and specific association between TDP-43 and FTLD-U supports the hypothesis that TDP-43 is the pathogenic protein in FTLD-U, but it is important to emphasize that a direct causal relationship between TDP-43 and FTLD has not yet been established. Nevertheless, the discovery of this protein represents a potential major breakthrough towards understanding FTLD pathophysiology. 33.7.3. Dementia lacking distinctive histology and other histopathologic forms The term dementia lacking distinctive histology (DLDH) describes cases in which gross and microscopic atrophic changes are clearly evident, yet tau and ubiquitin immunohistochemistry is negative (McKhann et al., 2001). The term was originally coined by Knopman to describe the bland pathology found in non-Pick’s cases, which initially appeared to represent the majority of FTLD cases (Knopman et al., 1990; Knopman, 1993). A significant number of cases initially diagnosed as DLDH have been restained for ubiquitin and found to be positive (Josephs et al., 2004; Lipton et al., 2004), and DLDH is now found only in a minority of cases (see below). While FTLD-T, FLTD-U and DLDH encompass the vast majority of pathology in FTLD, rare cases of other pathologic subtypes have been reported. An early-onset,
rapidly progressive behavioral syndrome characterized by various degrees of extrapyramidal and motor-neuron signs is associated with inclusions containing neurofilament and alpha-internexin (Josephs et al., 2003; Cairns et al., 2004). A familial syndrome of inclusion body myositis, Paget’s disease and FTD is associated with a mutation in the valosin-containing protein, with valosin and TDP-43 inclusions found in muscle and neuronal nuclei (Schroder et al., 2005; Neumann et al., 2007). Finally, some patients who present with the FTLD clinical syndrome will have non-FTLD neuropathology. AD, vascular dementia, Lewy body dementia, multiple systems atrophy, prion disease and normal brains have all been found on autopsy in patients clinically diagnosed with an FTLD syndrome (Galton et al., 2000; Davies et al., 2005; Kertesz et al., 2005; Forman et al., 2006). 33.7.4. Clinicopathologic correlations A number of large series of autopsy-proven FTLD have recently been published (Hodges et al., 2003, 2004; Lipton et al., 2004; Davies et al., 2005; Kertesz et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2005; Forman et al., 2006). Direct comparison of data from these series is limited by their different methodologies: some studies prospectively studied the pathological diagnosis in all patients clinically diagnosed as an FTLD syndrome, while others retrospectively examined clinical data in autopsy-proven FTLD. Some studies included CBD and PSP while others did not. Nevertheless, a number of common themes have emerged. First, all forms of FTLD pathology can present as any of the FTLD clinical syndromes. Gender distributions are equal amongst all pathologies. A number of studies have found an older age of onset and longer survival in FTLD-T (Hodges et al., 2003, 2004; Roberson et al., 2005), though this has been an inconsistent finding (Kertesz et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2005). Survival is shortened in FTLD-U (Hodges et al., 2003, 2004), an effect that is most prominent in cases of clinically apparent ALS, and does not hold true for semantic dementia (Davies et al., 2005). FTLD-U is the most prevalent pathology, ranging from 26% to 62% in the various series, and accounting for 140/305 (46%) of autopsy-proven FTLD cases reported in the literature (Shi et al., 2005). Prevalence of classic Pick’s disease (not including other tauopathies) is 11–33%, while DLDH is present 13–23% of the time. FTLD-U accounts for a large proportion of semantic dementia presentations (13/18 cases in one series) (Davies et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2005; Forman et al., 2006). Of the FTLD clinical syndromes, PNFA is more likely to be associated with non-FTLD
356
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
pathology, such as CBD or AD (Hodges et al., 2004; Kertesz et al., 2005; Knibb et al., 2006). When FTLD pathology is implicated in PNFA, one study found a high (6/8) proportion of Pick’s disease (Hodges et al., 2004). Early onset of a movement disorder makes tau pathology more likely (Kertesz et al., 2005; Forman et al., 2006), and the clinical syndromes of CBD and PSP often correlate with either CBD or PSP pathology (Hodges et al., 2004; Kertesz et al., 2005). Clinically apparent motor-neuron disease virtually ensures tau-negative pathology, and very strongly predicts FTLD-U (Hodges et al., 2004; Kertesz et al., 2005). The behavioral FTD syndrome does not appear to have a predilection for a specific pathology.
33.8. Treatment 33.8.1. Pharmacotherapy The absence of animal models and the heterogeneous pathology of FTLD have hampered efforts to identify effective therapies. Symptomatic treatment has focused on treating behavioral symptoms, including disinhibition, impulsiveness, altered appetite, and compulsive and stereotypical behaviors. Drugs that modify serotonin have the strongest theoretical rationale, given the serotonergic deficits identified by neurochemical studies (Sparks and Markesbery, 1991; Procter et al., 1999) and functional imaging (Franceschi et al., 2005). Moreover, these drugs are effective in treating similar behaviors in non-demented individuals. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) were effective at controlling behavior in small studies without placebo controls (Swartz et al., 1997b; Moretti et al., 2003), though a recent placebo-controlled study has challenged these findings (Deakin et al., 2004). Trazodone, another serotonergic drug, was effective in a placebo-controlled crossover study (Lebert et al., 2004). Rivastigmine, an inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase and butyrylcholinesterase used in Alzheimer’s disease (Farlow et al., 2000), was found to be effective in one open-label study (Moretti et al., 2004). However, as opposed to AD, there is no cortical cholinergic deficit in FTLD (Yates et al., 1980; Meier-Ruge et al., 1984; Hansen et al., 1988), and in our experience cholinesterase inhibitors may exacerbate agitation in FTLD patients (Perry and Miller, 2001). In our clinical practice, SSRIs are the first-line treatment for behavioral symptoms. SSRIs can also be helpful in curbing stereotypical movements (Mendez et al., 2005b). Low-dose atypical neuroleptics (e.g., quetiapine) are sometimes necessary to control aggressive behaviors, but these should be used sparingly as they may exacerbate parkinsonism, and slightly increase
mortality in elderly dementia patients (http://www.fda. gov/cder/drug/infopage/antipsychotics/default.htm). Anticonvulsants such as carbamazepine and valproic acid can also be helpful in modulating agitation and aggression, while benzodiazepines should be avoided (Perry and Miller, 2001). Memantine, an NMDA antagonist used in AD (Reisberg et al., 2003) has sparked recent interest, especially given the effectiveness of anti-glutamatergic therapy in ALS (Bensimon et al., 1994), and is under active investigation. 33.8.2. Non-pharmacologic therapy As always, the treating physician can alleviate stress by educating families and caregivers about the disease. Understanding the biological basis of difficult behaviors helps put these behaviors in a different perspective. Anticipating the progression of illness can help families reach difficult decisions (e.g., transition to an assisted-living or long-term care facility) in a more gradual, controlled manner. Participation in support groups, ideally with family members of other FTD patients, can be extremely helpful. Semantic dementia and PNFA patients may benefit from speech therapy or the use of augmentative or alternative communication devices. Caregiver and family education about aphasia helps facilitate effective communication. Early in FTD, the physician should stress the importance of avoiding situations in which a patient’s poor judgment or impulsivity can have dire consequences (e.g., driving, financial decision-making) (Perry and Miller, 2001). Later in the disease, environmental modification can be helpful by removing cues that trigger undesired behaviors, and utilizing stimuli that trigger adaptive behaviors (e.g., setting out clothes in the order they are to be worn) (Talerico and Evans, 2001). Caregivers of dementia patients are at risk for depression and medical illness (Schulz et al., 1995), and the physician must ensure that caregivers are obtaining sufficient help and respite and addressing their own health needs. Physical activity in the form of a structured exercise program is a universal recommendation for both patients and caregivers, and it has been our anecdotal experience that it helps delay or slow motor symptoms. In the end-stages of disease, the physician should guide families towards setting goals that maximize function and quality of life, rather than “safety at all costs” (Talerico and Evans, 2001).
Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Dr. William W. Seeley for helpful contributions to this manuscript.
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 357 GDR is a fellow of the John Douglas French Alzheimer’s Foundation. Supported by NIA grant P01AG1972403, ADRC grant P50-AG023501 and the Hillblom Foundation.
References Alzheimer A (1911). Uber eigenartige Krankheitsfalle des sparteren Alters. Zeitscrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 4: 356–385. Ames D, Cummings JL, Wirshing WC, et al. (1994). Repetitive and compulsive behavior in frontal lobe degenerations. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6: 100–113. Arai T, Hasegawa M, Akiyama H, et al. (2006). TDP-43 is a component of ubiquitin-positive tau-negative inclusions in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 351: 602–611. Baker M, Litvan I, Houlden H, et al. (1999). Association of an extended haplotype in the tau gene with progressive supranuclear palsy. Hum Mol Genet 8: 711–715. Baker M, Mackenzie IR, Pickering-Brown SM, et al. (2006). Mutations in progranulin cause tau-negative frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17. Nature 442: 916–919. Barker WW, Luis CA, Kashuba A, et al. (2002). Relative frequencies of Alzheimer disease, Lewy body, vascular and frontotemporal dementia, and hippocampal sclerosis in the State of Florida Brain Bank. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 16: 203–212. Barnett DW, Zucker KB (1977). Validating a measure of children’s others-concept through population and behavior variables. J Pers Assess 41: 131–143. Bathgate D, Snowden JS, Varma A, et al. (2001). Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Acta Neurol Scand 103: 367–378. Bensimon G, Lacomblez L, Meininger V (1994). A controlled trial of riluzole in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. ALS/Riluzole Study Group. N Engl J Med 330: 585–591. Benson DF (1991). The Geschwind syndrome. Adv Neurol 55: 411–421. Benussi L, Binetti G, Sina E, et al. (2006). A novel deletion in progranulin gene is associated with FTDP-17 and CBS. Neurobiol Aging [Epub ahead of print]. Bird T, Nochlin D, Poorkaj P, et al. (1999 April). A clinical pathological comparison of three families with frontotemporal dementia and identical mutations in the tau gene (P301L). Brain 74: 1–756. Boccardi M, Sabattoli F, Laakso MP, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal dementia as a neural system disease. Neurobiol Aging 26: 37–44. Boxer AL, Rankin KP, Miller BL, et al. (2003). Cinguloparietal atrophy distinguishes Alzheimer disease from semantic dementia. Arch Neurol 60: 949–956. Bozeat S, Lambon Ralph MA, Patterson K, et al. (2000). Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 38: 1207–1215.
Braak H, Braak E (1998). Argyrophilic grain disease: frequency of occurrence in different age categories and neuropathological diagnostic criteria. J Neural Transm 105: 801–819. Broe M, Hodges JR, Schofield E, et al. (2003). Staging disease severity in pathologically confirmed cases of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 60: 1005–1011. Brun A (1987). Frontal Lobe Degeneration of the NonAlzheimer Type. I. Neuropathology. Arch Gerontol Geriatrics 6: 193–208. Brun A (1993). Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type revisited. Dementia 4: 126–131. Brun A, Passant U (1996). Frontal lobe degeneration of nonAlzheimer’s type: Structural characteristics, diagnostic criteria, and relation to other frontotemporal dementias. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 168: 28–30. Cairns NJ, Grossman M, Arnold SE, et al. (2004). Clinical and neuropathologic variation in neuronal intermediate filament inclusion disease. Neurology 63: 1376–1384. Caselli RJ, Windebank AJ, Petersen RC, et al. (1993). Rapidly progressive aphasic dementia and motor neuron disease [see comments]. Ann Neurol 33: 200–207. Cavalleri F, De Renzi E (1994). Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with dementia. Acta Neurol Scand 89: 391–394. Chan D, Fox NC, Scahill RI, et al. (2001). Patterns of temporal lobe atrophy in semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 49: 433–442. Chan D, Walters RJ, Sampson EL, et al. (2004). EEG abnormalities in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 62: 1628–1630. Chang JL, Lomen-Hoerth C, Murphy J, et al. (2005). A voxel-based morphometry study of patterns of brain atrophy in ALS and ALS/FTLD. Neurology 65: 75–80. Chawluk JB, Mesulam M-M, Hurtig H, et al. (1986). Slowly progressive aphasia without generalised dementia: studies with positron emission tomography. Ann Neurol 19: 68–74. Chow TW, Miller BL, Hayashi VN, et al. (1999). Inheritance of frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 56: 817–822. Clark CM, Xie S, Chittams J, et al. (2003). Cerebrospinal fluid tau and beta-amyloid: how well do these biomarkers reflect autopsy-confirmed dementia diagnoses? Arch Neurol 60: 1696–1702. Clark LN, Poorkaj P, Wszolek Z, et al. (1998). Pathogenic implications of mutations in the tau gene in pallido-pontonigral degeneration and related neurodegenerative disorders linked to chromosome 17. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 13103–13107. The Lund and Manchester Groups (1994). Clinical and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57: 416–418. Constantinidis J, Richard J, Tissot R (1974). Pick’s disease: histological and clinical correlations. Eur Neurol 11: 208–217. Cooper PN, Jackson M, Lennox G, et al. (1995). Tau, ubiquitin, and alpha B-crystallin immunohistochemistry define the principal causes of degenerative frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 52: 1011–1015.
358
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
Cruts M, Kumar-Singh S, Van Broeckhoven C (2006). Progranulin mutations in ubiquitin-positive frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 17q21. Curr Alzheimer Res 3: 485–491. Davidson Y, Kelley T, Mackenzie IR, et al. (2007). Ubiquitinated pathological lesions in frontotemporal lobar degeneration contain the TAR DNA-binding protein, TDP-43. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 113: 521–533. Davies RR, Hodges JR, Kril JJ, et al. (2005). The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain 128: 1984–1995. Deakin JB, Rahman S, Nestor PJ, et al. (2004). Paroxetine does not improve symptoms and impairs cognition in frontotemporal dementia: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 172: 400–408. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA (1995). Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 118: 279–306. Dickson DW (2001). Neuropathology of Pick’s disease. Neurology 56: S16–20. Diehl J, Monsch AU, Aebi C, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease: the contribution of standard neuropsychological tests to differential diagnosis. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 18: 39–44. Drzezga A, Grimmer T, Siebner H, et al. (2002). Prominent hypometabolism of the right temporoparietal and frontal cortex in two left-handed patients with primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol 249: 1263–1267. D’Souza I, Poorkaj P, Hong M, et al. (1999). Missense and silent tau gene mutations cause frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism-chromosome 17 type, by affecting multiple alternative RNA splicing regulatory elements. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 5598–5603. Duffy JR (1995). Motor speech disorders: substrates, differential diagnosis, and management. Mosby, St. Louis. Edwards-Lee T, Miller BL, Benson DF, et al. (1997). The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 120: 1027–1040. Elfgren C, Brun A, Gustafson L, et al. (1994). Neuropsychological tests as discriminators between dementia of Alzheimer’s type and Frontotemporal dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 9: 635–642. Elfgren CI, Ryding E, Passant U (1996). Performance on neuropsychological tests related to single photon emission computerised tomography findings in frontotemporal dementia. Br J Psychiatry 169: 416–422. Evans JJ, Heggs AJ, Antoun N, et al. (1995). Progressive prosopagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy: a new syndrome? Brain 118: 1–13. Fabre SF, Forsell C, Viitanen M, et al. (2001). Clinic-based cases with frontotemporal dementia show increased cerebrospinal fluid tau and high apolipoprotein E epsilon4 frequency, but no tau gene mutations. Exp Neurol 168: 413–418. Farlow M, Anand R, Messina J Jr, et al. (2000). A 52-week study of the efficacy of rivastigmine in patients with mild to moderately severe Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Neurol 44: 236–241.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the mental state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res 12: 189–198. Forman MS, Farmer J, Johnson JK, et al. (2006). Frontotemporal dementia: clinicopathological correlations. Ann Neurol 59: 952–962. Foster NL, Wilhelmsen K, Sima AA, et al. (1997). Frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17: a consensus conference. Conference Participants. Ann Neurol 41: 706–715. Franceschi M, Anchisi D, Pelati O, et al. (2005). Glucose metabolism and serotonin receptors in the frontotemporal lobe degeneration. Ann Neurol 57: 216–225. Frisoni GB, Pizzolato G, Geroldi C, et al. (1995a). Dementia of the frontal type: neuropsychological and 99 TC HMPAO SPECT features. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 8: 42–48. Frisoni GB, Pizzolato G, Geroldi C, et al. (1995b). Dementia of the Frontal Type: Neuropsychological and [99Tc]-HMPAO SPET features. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 8: 42–48. Frisoni GB, Beltramello A, Geroldi C, et al. (1996). Brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 61: 157–165. Fukui T, Kertesz A (2000). Volumetric study of lobar atrophy in Pick complex and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci 174: 111–121. Galton CJ, Patterson K, Xuereb JH, et al. (2000). Atypical and typical presentations of Alzheimer’s disease: a clinical, neuropsychological, neuroimaging and pathological study of 13 cases. Brain 3: 484–498. Galton CJ, Patterson K, Graham K, et al. (2001). Differing patterns of temporal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology 56: 216–225. Garraux G, Salmon E, Degueldre C, et al. (1999a). Medial temporal lobe metabolic impairment in dementia associated with motor neuron disease. J Neurol Sci 168: 145–150. Garraux G, Salmon E, Degueldre C, et al. (1999b). Comparison of impaired subcortico-frontal metabolic networks in normal aging, subcortico-frontal dementia, and cortical frontal dementia. Neuroimage 10: 149–162. Gass J, Cannon A, Mackenzie IR, et al. (2006). Mutations in progranulin are a major cause of ubiquitin-positive frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Hum Mol Genet 15: 2988–3001. Geschwind D, Karrim J, Nelson SF, et al. (1998). The apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele is not a significant risk factor for frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 44: 134–138. Geschwind DH, Robidoux J, Alarcon M, et al. (2001). Dementia and neurodevelopmental predisposition: cognitive dysfunction in presymptomatic subjects precedes dementia by decades in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 50: 741–746. Goldman JS, Farmer JM, Van Deerlin VM, et al. (2004). Frontotemporal dementia: genetics and genetic counseling dilemmas. Neurologist 10: 227–234. Goldman JS, Farmer JM, Wood EM, et al. (2005). Comparison of family histories in FTLD subtypes and related tauopathies. Neurology 65: 1817–1819.
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 359 Gorno-Tempini ML, Chang J, Miller B, et al. (2004a). A voxel-based morphometry study of patterns of brain atrophy in patients with ALS and ALS/FTD. 56th Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Neurology. Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, et al. (2004b). Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 55: 335–346. Gorno-Tempini ML, Murray RC, Rankin KP, et al. (2004c). Clinical, cognitive and anatomical evolution from nonfluent progressive aphasia to corticobasal syndrome: a case report. Neurocase 10: 426–436. Graham KS, Hodges JR (1997). Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex in long-term memory storage: evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 11: 77–89. Green AJ, Harvey RJ, Thompson EJ, et al. (1999). Increased tau in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Lett 259: 133–135. Gregory CA, Hodges JR (1996). Clinical features of frontal lobe dementia in comparision to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neural Transm 47: 103–123. Gregory CA, Orrell M, Sahakian B, et al. (1997). Can frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease be differentiated using a brief battery of tests? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12: 375–383. Gregory CA, Serra-Mestres J, Hodges JR (1999). Early diagnosis of the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia: how sensitive are standard neuroimaging and neuropsychologic tests? Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 12: 128–135. Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, et al. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain 125: 752–764. Grossman M, Farmer J, Leight S, et al. (2005). Cerebrospinal fluid profile in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 57: 721–729. Gustafson L (1987). Frontal lobe degeneration of nonAlheimer type. II. Clinical picture and differential diagnosis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 6: 209–223. Gustafson L (1993). Clinical picture of frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. Dementia 4: 143–148. Gustafson L, Brun A, Risberg J (1987). Frontal lobe degeneration of non-Alzheimer type. II. Clinical picture and differential diagnosis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 6: 209–223. Gydesen S, Brown JM, Brun A, et al. (2002). Chromosome 3 linked frontotemporal dementia (FTD-3). Neurology 59: 1585–1594. Hansen LA, Deteresa R, Tobias H, et al. (1988). Neocortical morphometry and cholinergic neurochemistry in Pick’s disease. Am J Pathol 131: 507–518. Hasegawa M, Smith MJ, Goedert M (1998). Tau proteins with FTDP-17 mutations have a reduced ability to promote microtubule assembly. FEBS Lett 437: 207–210. Hayashi S, Toyoshima Y, Hasegawa M, et al. (2002). Lateonset frontotemporal dementia with a novel exon 1 (Arg5His) tau gene mutation. Ann Neurol 51: 525–530.
He Z, Bateman A (2003). Progranulin (granulin-epithelin precursor, PC-cell-derived growth factor, acrogranin) mediates tissue repair and tumorigenesis. J Mol Med 81: 600–612. He Z, Ong CH, Halper J, et al. (2003). Progranulin is a mediator of the wound response. Nat Med 9: 225–229. Hodges JR (2001). Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease) Clinical features and assessment. Neurology 56: 6S–10. Hodges JR, Graham KS (1998). A reversal of the temporal gradient for famous person knowledge in semantic dementia: implications for the neural organisation of long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 36: 803–825. Hodges JR, Patterson K (1996). Nonfluent progressive aphasia and semantic dementia: a comparative neuropsychological study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2: 511–524. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Oxbury S, et al. (1992). Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 115: 1783–1806. Hodges JR, Patterson K, Ward R, et al. (1999). The differentiation of semantic dementia and frontal lobe dementia (temporal and frontal variants of frontotemporal dementia) from early Alzheimer’s disease: A comparative neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology 13: 31–40. Hodges JR, Bozeat S, Lambon Ralph MA, et al. (2000). The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 123: 1913–1925. Hodges JR, Davies R, Xuereb J, et al. (2003). Survival in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 61: 349–354. Hodges JR, Davies RR, Xuereb JH, et al. (2004). Clinicopathological correlates in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 56: 399–406. Hogg M, Grujic ZM, Baker M, et al. (2003). The L266V tau mutation is associated with frontotemporal dementia and Pick-like 3R and 4R tauopathy. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 106: 323–336. Hong M, Zhukareva V, Vogelsberg-Ragaglia V, et al. (1998). Mutation-specific functional impairments in distinct tau isoforms of hereditary FTDP-17. Science 282: 1914–1917. Hosler BA, Siddique T, Sapp PC, et al. (2000). Linkage of familial amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with frontotemporal dementia to chromosome 9q21-q22. JAMA 284: 1664–1669. Houlden H, Baker M, Morris HR, et al. (2001). Corticobasal degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy share a common tau haplotype. Neurology 56: 1702–1706. Howard D, Patterson K (1992). Pyramids and Palm trees: a test of semantic access from pictures and words Thames Valley Publishing Company, Bury St Edmunds, Suffolk. Hudson LP, Munoz DG, Miller L, et al. (1993). Amygdaloid sclerosis in temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 33: 622–631. Hutton M, Lendon CL, Rizzu P, et al. (1998). Association of missense and 50 -splice-site mutations in tau with the inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 393: 702–705. Ikeda M, Brown J, Holland AJ, et al. (2002). Changes in appetite, food preference, and eating habits in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73: 371–376.
360
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
Ishii K, Sakamoto S, Sasaki M, et al. (1998). Cerebral glucose metabolism in patients with frontotemporal dementia. J Nucl Med 39: 1875–1878. Jackson M, Lennox G, Lowe J (1996). Motor neurone diseaseinclusion dementia. Neurodegeneration 5: 339–350. Jellinger KA, Bancher C (1998). Senile dementia with tangles (tangle predominant form of senile dementia). Brain Pathol 8: 367–376. Jeong Y, Cho SS, Park JM, et al. (2005). 18F-FDG PET findings in frontotemporal dementia: an SPM analysis of 29 patients. J Nucl Med 46: 233–239. Jervis LL, Manson SM (2002). American Indians/Alaska Natives and dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 16: S89–S95. Johannesson G, Brun A, Gustafson I, et al. (1977). EEG in presenile dementia related to cerebral blood flow and autopsy findings. Acta Neurol Scand 56: 89–103. Johannesson G, Hagberg B, Gustafson L, et al. (1979). EEG and cognitive impairment in presenile dementia. Acta Neurol Scand 59: 225–240. John OP, Srivastava S (1999). The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In: LA Pervin, OP John (Eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 102–138. Johnson JK, Diehl J, Mendez MF, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: demographic characteristics of 353 patients. Arch Neurol 62: 925–930. Josephs KA, Holton JL, Rossor MN, et al. (2003). Neurofilament inclusion body disease: a new proteinopathy? Brain 126: 2291–2303. Josephs KA, Holton JL, Rossor MN, et al. (2004). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration and ubiquitin immunohistochemistry. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 30: 369–373. Kamo H, McGeer PL, Harrop R, et al. (1987). Positron emission tomography and histopathology in Pick’s disease. Neurology 37: 439–445. Kaufer DI, Miller BL, Itti L, et al. (1997). Midline cerebral morphometry distinguishes frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 48: 978–985. Kertesz A (1997). Frontotemporal dementia, Pick disease, and corticobasal degeneration. Arch Neurol 54: 1427–1429. Kertesz A, Davidson W, Munoz D (1999). Clinical and pathological overlap between frontotemporal dementia, primary progressive aphasia and corticobasal degeneration: the Pick complex. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10: 46–49. Kertesz A, Martinez-Lage P, Davidson W, et al. (2000). The corticobasal degeneration syndrome overlaps progressive aphasia and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55: 1368–1375. Kertesz A, Davidson W, McCabe P, et al. (2003). Behavioral quantitation is more sensitive than cognitive testing in frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 17: 223–229. Kertesz A, McMonagle P, Blair M, et al. (2005). The evolution and pathology of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 128: 1996–2005.
Kim DM, Tien R, Byrum C, et al. (1996). Imaging in acquired immune deficiency syndrome dementia complex (AIDS dementia complex): a review. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 20: 349–370. Kitagaki H, Mori E, Hirono N, et al. (1997). Alteration of white matter MR signal intensity in frontotemporal dementia. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 18: 367–378. Klunk WE, Engler H, Nordberg A, et al. (2004). Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann Neurol 55: 306–319. Kluver H, Bucy PC (1937). “Psychic Blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. Am J Physiol 1937: 352–353. Knibb JA, Xuereb JH, Patterson K, et al. (2006). Clinical and pathological characterization of progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 59: 156–165. Knopman DS (1993). Overview of dementia lacking distinctive histology: pathological designation of a progressive dementia. Dementia 4: 132–136. Knopman DS, Mastri AR, Frey WHd, et al. (1990). Dementia lacking distinctive histologic features: a common nonAlzheimer degenerative dementia. Neurology 40: 251–256. Kobayashi K, Arai H, Ikeda K, et al. (1984). [A computed tomography study of Pick’s disease]. Seishin Shinkeigaku Zasshi 86: 401–416. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, et al. (2003). Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 16: 211–218. Kril JJ, Halliday GM (2004). Clinicopathological staging of frontotemporal dementia severity: correlation with regional atrophy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 311–315. Kumar A, Schapiro MB, Haxby JV, et al. (1990). Cerebral metabolic and cognitive studies in dementia with frontal lobe behavioral features. J Psychiatr Res 24: 97–109. Lambon Ralph MA, Graham KS, Ellis AW, et al. (1998). Naming in semantic dementia–what matters? Neuropsychologia 36: 775–784. Larsson EM, Englund E, Sjobeck M, et al. (2004). MRI with diffusion tensor imaging post-mortem at 3.0 T in a patient with frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 316–319. Lebert F, Stekke W, Hasenbroekx C, et al. (2004). Frontotemporal dementia: a randomised, controlled trial with trazodone. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 355–359. Lee AC, Rahman S, Hodges JR, et al. (2003). Associative and recognition memory for novel objects in dementia: implications for diagnosis. Eur J Neurosci 18: 1660–1670. Levy ML, Miller BL, Cummings JL, et al. (1996). Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal dementias. Behavioral distinctions. Arch Neurol 53: 687–690. Lindau M, Almkvist O, Johansson SE, et al. (1998). Cognitive and behavioral differentiation of frontal lobe degeneration of the non-Alzheimer’s type and Alzheimer’s Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 9: 205–213. Lindau M, Jelic V, Johansson SE, et al. (2003). Quantitative EEG abnormalities and cognitive dysfunctions in fronto-
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 361 temporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 15: 106–114. Lipton AM, White CL, 3rd, Bigio EH (2004). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration with motor neuron disease-type inclusions predominates in 76 cases of frontotemporal degeneration. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 108: 379–385. Liu W, Miller BL, Kramer JH, et al. (2004). Behavioral disorders in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 62: 742–748. Lomen-Hoerth C (2004). Characterization of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 17: 337–341. Lomen-Hoerth C, Anderson T, Miller B (2002). The overlap of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 59: 1077–1079. Lomen-Hoerth C, Murphy J, Langmore S, et al. (2003). Are amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients cognitively normal? Neurology 60: 1094–1097. Lynch T, Sano M, Marder KS, et al. (1994). Clinical characteristics of a family with chromosome 17-linked disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy complex [see comments]. Neurology 44: 1878–1884. Mackenzie IR, Feldman HH (2005). Ubiquitin immunohistochemistry suggests classic motor neuron disease, motor neuron disease with dementia, and frontotemporal dementia of the motor neuron disease type represent a clinicopathologic spectrum. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 64: 730–739. Mackenzie IR, Baborie A, Pickering-Brown S, et al. (2006a). Heterogeneity of ubiquitin pathology in frontotemporal lobar degeneration: classification and relation to clinical phenotype. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 112: 539–549. Mackenzie IR, Baker M, Pickering-Brown S, et al. (2006b). The neuropathology of frontotemporal lobar degeneration caused by mutations in the progranulin gene. Brain 129: 3081–3090. Masellis M, Momeni P, Meschino W, et al. (2006). Novel splicing mutation in the progranulin gene causing familial corticobasal syndrome. Brain 129: 3115–3123. McGeachie RE, Fleming JO, Sharer LR, et al. (1979). Case report. Diagnosis of Pick’s disease by computed tomography. J Comput Assist Tomogr 3: 113–115. McGeer PL, Schwab C, McGeer EG, et al. (1997). Familial nature and continuing morbidity of the amyotrophic lateral sclerosis-parkinsonism dementia complex of Guam. Neurology 49: 400–409. McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, et al. (2001). Clinical and pathological diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia: report of the Work Group on Frontotemporal Dementia and Pick’s Disease. Arch Neurol 58: 1803–1809. Meier-Ruge W, Iwangoff P, Reichlmeier K (1984). Neurochemical enzyme changes in Alzheimer’s and Pick’s disease. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 3: 161–165. Mendez MF, Cherrier M, Perryman KM, et al. (1996). Frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease: differential cognitive features. Neurology 47: 1189–1194. Mendez MF, Perryman KM, Miller BL, et al. (1997). Compulsive behaviors as presenting symptoms of frontotemporal dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 10: 154–157.
Mendez MF, Chen AK, Shapira JS, et al. (2005a). Acquired sociopathy and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 20: 99–104. Mendez MF, Shapira JS, Miller BL (2005b). Stereotypical movements and frontotemporal dementia. Mov Disord 20: 742–745. Mesulam MM (1982). Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol 11: 592–598. Mesulam MM (2001). Primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol 49: 425–432. Mesulam MM (2003). Primary progressive aphasia–a language-based dementia. N Engl J Med 349: 1535–1542. Mesulam M, Weintraub S, Parrish T, et al. (2005). Primary progressive aphasia: reversed asymmetry of atrophy and right hemisphere language dominance. Neurology 64: 556–557. Mesulam M, Johnson N, Krefft TA, et al. (2007). Progranulin mutations in primary progressive aphasia: the PPA1 and PPA3 families. Arch Neurol 64: 43–47. Miller BL, Gearhart R (1999). Neuroimaging in the diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 10: 71–74. Miller BL, Cummings JL, Villanueva-Meyer J, et al. (1991). Frontal lobe degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological, and SPECT characteristics. Neurology 41: 1374–1382. Miller BL, Chang L, Mena I, et al. (1993). Progressive right frontotemporal degeneration: clinical, neuropsychological and SPECT characteristics. Dementia 4: 204–213. Miller BL, Darby AL, Swartz JR, et al. (1995). Dietary changes, compulsions and sexual behavior in frontotemporal degeneration. Dementia 6: 195–199. Miller BL, Darby A, Benson DF, et al. (1997). Aggressive, socially disruptive and antisocial behaviour associated with fronto-temporal dementia. Br J Psychiatry 170: 150–154. Miller BL, Cummings J, Mishkin F, et al. (1998). Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 51: 978–982. Miller BL, Seeley WW, Mychack P, et al. (2001). Neuroanatomy of the self: Evidence from patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 57: 817–821. Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, et al. (2003). Frontotemporal dementia: paroxetine as a possible treatment of behavior symptoms. A randomized, controlled, open 14-month study. Eur Neurol 49: 13–19. Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, et al. (2004). Rivastigmine in frontotemporal dementia: an open-label study. Drugs Aging 21: 931–937. Mummery C, Patterson K, Price C, et al. (2000). A voxelbased morphometry study of semantic demntia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory. Ann Neurol 47: 36–45. Munoz DG, Dickson DW, Bergeron C, et al. (2003). The neuropathology and biochemistry of frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 54: S24–28. Murrell JR, Spillantini MG, Zolo P, et al. (1999). Tau gene mutation G389R causes a tauopathy with abundant pick body-like inclusions and axonal deposits. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 58: 1207–1226.
362
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
Mychack P, Kramer JH, Boone KB, et al. (2001). The influence of right frontotemporal dysfunction on social behavior in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 56: S11–15. Nasreddine ZS, Loginov M, Clark LN, et al. (1999). From genotype to phenotype: a clinical pathological, and biochemical investigation of frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism (FTDP-17) caused by the P301L tau mutation. Ann Neurol 45: 704–715. Neary D, Snowden J, Shields R, et al. (1987). Single photon emission tomography using 99mtc-HM-PAO in the investigation of dementia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 50: 1101–1109. Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al. (1988). Dementia of frontal lobe type. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51: 353–361. Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51: 1546–1554. Neary D, Snowden JS, Mann DM (2000). Cognitive change in motor neurone disease/amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (MND/ALS). J Neurol Sci 180: 15–20. Neary D, Snowden J, Mann D (2005). Frontotemporal dementia. Lancet Neurol 4: 771–780. Nedjam Z, Devouche E, Dalla Barba G (2004). Confabulation, but not executive dysfunction discriminate AD from frontotemporal dementia. Eur J Neurol 11: 728–733. Neumann M, Schulz-Schaeffer W, Crowther RA, et al. (2001). Pick’s disease associated with the novel Tau gene mutation K369I. Ann Neurol 50: 503–513. Neumann M, Sampathu DM, Kwong LK, et al. (2006). Ubiquitinated TDP-43 in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Science 314: 130–133. Neumann M, Mackenzie IR, Cairns NJ, et al. (2007). TDP43 in the ubiquitin pathology of frontotemporal dementia with VCP gene mutations. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 66: 152–157. Niizato K, Tsuchiya K, Tominaga I, et al. (1997). Pick’s disease with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS): report of two autopsy cases and literature review. J Neurol Sci 148: 107–112. Nordberg A (2004). PET imaging of amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 3: 519–527. Nyatsanza S, Shetty T, Gregory C, et al. (2003). A study of stereotypic behaviours in Alzheimer’s disease and frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 74: 1398–1402. Okamoto K, Hirai S, Yamazaki T, et al. (1991). New ubiquitin-positive intraneuronal inclusions in the extramotor cortices in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurosci Lett 129: 233–236. Onari K, Spatz H (1926). Anatomische Beitragezur Lehre von der Pickschen umschriebene-Grosshirnriden-Atrophie (‘Picksche Krankheit’). Zeitscrift fur die Gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 101: 470–511. Pachana NA, Boone KB, Miller BL, et al. (1996). Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s dis-
ease and frontotemporal dementia. J Int NeuropsycholSoc 2: 505–510. Pasquier F, Grymonprez L, Lebert F, et al. (2001). Memory impairment differs in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurocase 7: 161–171. Pasquier F, Fukui T, Sarazin M, et al. (2003). Laboratory investigations and treatment in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 54: S32–35. Patterson K, Graham N, Hodges JR (1994). The impact of semantic memory loss on phonological representations. J Cogn Neurosci 6: 57–69. Perry RJ, Hodges JR (2000). Differentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 54: 2277–2284. Perry RJ, Miller BL (2001). Behavior and treatment in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 56: S46–51. Pick A (1892). Uber die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prager Medizinische Wochenschrift 17: 165–167. Pick A (1904). Zur symptomatologie der linksseitigen schlafenappenatrophie. Monatsschrift fur Psychiatrie und Neurologie 16: 378–388. Pickering-Brown SM, Owen F, Isaacs A, et al. (2000). Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele has no effect on age at onset or duration of disease in cases of frontotemporal dementia with pick- or microvacuolar-type histology. Exp Neurol 163: 452–456. Pickering-Brown SM, Baker M, Nonaka T, et al. (2004). Frontotemporal dementia with Pick-type histology associated with Q336R mutation in the tau gene. Brain 127: 1415–1426. Plato CC, Galasko D, Garruto RM, et al. (2002). ALS and PDC of Guam: forty-year follow-up. Neurology 58: 765–773. Poorkaj P, Bird TD, Wijsman E, et al. (1998). Tau is a candidate gene for chromosome 17 frontotemporal dementia [published erratum appears in Ann Neurol 1998 Sep;44:428]. Ann Neurol 43: 815–825. Portet F, Cadilhac C, Touchon J, et al. (2001). Cognitive impairment in motor neuron disease with bulbar onset. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Other Motor Neuron Disord 2: 23–29. Procter AW, Qurne M, Francis PT (1999). Neurochemical features of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1: 80–84. Rabinovici GD, Furst AJ, O’Neil JP, et al. (2007). 11C-PIB PET imaging in Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 68: 1205–1212. Rabinovici GD, Seeley WW, Kim EJ, et al (in press). Distinct MRI atrophy patterns in autopsy-proven Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Am J Alzheimer’s Dis Other Dement 22: 474–488. Rahman S, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (1999a). Comparative cognitive neuropsychological studies of frontal lobe function: implications for therapeutic strategies in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn. Disord 1: 15–28. Rahman S, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, et al. (1999b). Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain 122: 1469–1493.
FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION: CLINICAL AND PATHOLOGIC OVERVIEW 363 Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Miller BL (2005). Patterns of cognitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cogn Behav Neurol 18: 28–36. Rascovsky K, Salmon DP, Ho GJ, et al. (2002). Cognitive profiles differ in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and AD. Neurology 58: 1801–1808. Rascovsky K, Salmon DP, Lipton AM, et al. (2005). Rate of progression differs in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Neurology 65: 397–403. Ratnavalli E, Brayne C, Dawson K, et al. (2002). The prevalence of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 58: 1615–1621. Reisberg B, Doody R, Stoffler A, et al. (2003). Memantine in moderate-to-severe Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 348: 1333–1341. Riemenschneider M, Diehl J, Muller U, et al. (2002). Apolipoprotein E polymorphism in German patients with frontotemporal degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72: 639–641. Rinne JO, Laine M, Kaasinen V, et al. (2002). Striatal dopamine transporter and extrapyramidal symptoms in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 58: 1489–1493. Risberg J, Passant U, Warkentin S, et al. (1993). Regional cerebral blood flow in frontal dementia of Non-Alzheimer type. Dementia 4: 186–187. Rizzini C, Goedert M, Hodges JR, et al. (2000). Tau gene mutation K257T causes a tauopathy similar to Pick’s disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 59: 990–1001. Rizzu P, Van Swieten JC, Joosse M, et al. (1999). High prevalence of mutations in the microtubule-associated protein tau in a population study of frontotemporal dementia in the Netherlands. Am J Hum Genet 64: 414–421. Roberson ED, Hesse JH, Rose KD, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal dementia progresses to death faster than Alzheimer disease. Neurology 65: 719–725. Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Goldman WP, et al. (2002a). Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Neurology 58: 198–208. Rosen HJ, Perry RJ, Murphy J, et al. (2002b). Emotion comprehension in the temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 125: 2286–2295. Rosen HJ, Narvaez JM, Hallam B, et al. (2004). Neuropsychological and functional measures of severity in Alzheimer disease, frontotemporal dementia, and semantic dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 18: 202–207. Rosen HJ, Allison SC, Schauer GF, et al. (2005). Neuroanatomical correlates of behavioural disorders in dementia. Brain 128: 2612–2625. Rosso SM, Donker Kaat L, Baks T, et al. (2003a). Frontotemporal dementia in The Netherlands: patient characteristics and prevalence estimates from a population-based study. Brain 126: 2016–2022. Rosso SM, van Herpen E, Pijnenburg YA, et al. (2003b). Total tau and phosphorylated tau 181 levels in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with frontotemporal dementia due to P301L and G272V tau mutations. Arch Neurol 60: 1209–1213.
Salmon E, Garraux G, Delbeuck X, et al. (2003). Predominant ventromedial frontopolar metabolic impairment in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 20: 435–440. Sampathu DM, Neumann M, Kwong LK, et al. (2006). Pathological heterogeneity of frontotemporal lobar degeneration with ubiquitin-positive inclusions delineated by ubiquitin immunohistochemistry and novel monoclonal antibodies. Am J Pathol 169: 1343–1352. Sarazin M, Pillon B, Giannakopoulos MD, et al. (1998). Clinicometabolic dissociation of cognitive functions and social behavior in frontal lobe lesions. Neurology 51: 142–148. Scherder EJ, Sergeant JA, Swaab DF (2003). Pain processing in dementia and its relation to neuropathology. Lancet Neurol 2: 677–686. Schroder R, Watts GD, Mehta SG, et al. (2005). Mutant valosin-containing protein causes a novel type of frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 57: 457–461. Schulz R, O’Brien AT, Bookwala J, et al. (1995). Psychiatric and physical morbidity effects of dementia caregiving: prevalence, correlates, and causes. Gerontologist 35: 771–791. Seeley WW, Bauer AM, Miller BL, et al. (2005). The natural history of temporal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 64: 1384–1390. Shi J, Shaw CL, Du Plessis D, et al. (2005). Histopathological changes underlying frontotemporal lobar degeneration with clinicopathological correlation. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 110: 501–512. Shoghi-Jadid K, Small GW, Agdeppa ED, et al. (2002). Localization of neurofibrillary tangles and betaamyloid plaques in the brains of living patients with Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 10: 24–35. Silverman DH, Small GW, Chang CY, et al. (2001). Positron emission tomography in evaluation of dementia: Regional brain metabolism and long-term outcome. JAMA 286: 2120–2127. Sjogren M, Minthon L, Davidsson P, et al. (2000). CSF levels of tau, beta-amyloid(1–42) and GAP-43 in frontotemporal dementia, other types of dementia and normal aging. J Neural Transm 107: 563–579. Skibinski G, Parkinson NJ, Brown JM, et al. (2005). Mutations in the endosomal ESCRTIII-complex subunit CHMP2B in frontotemporal dementia. Nat Genet 37: 806–808. Snowden J, Goulding P, Neary D (1989). Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behav Neurol 2: 167–182. Snowden JS, Bathgate D, Varma A, et al. (2001). Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 70: 323–332. Sparks D, Markesbery WR (1991). Altered serotonergic and cholinergic synaptic markers in Pick’s disease. Arch Neurol 48: 796–799. Spillantini MG, Murrell JR, Goedert M, et al. (1998). Mutation in the tau gene in familial multiple system tauopathy
364
G.D. RABINOVICI ET AL.
with presenile dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 7737–7741. Spillantini MG, Van Swieten JC, Goedert M (2000a). Tau gene mutations in frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17 (FTDP-17). Neurogenetics 2: 193–205. Spillantini MG, Yoshida H, Rizzini C, et al. (2000b). A novel tau mutation (N296N) in familial dementia with swollen achromatic neurons and corticobasal inclusion bodies. Ann Neurol 48: 939–943. Spina S, Murrell JR, Huey ED, et al. (2007). Clinicopathologic features of frontotemporal dementia with Progranulin sequence variation. Neurology; Epub ahead of print. PMID: 17202431 Stevens M, van Duijn CM, Kamphorst W, et al. (1998). Familial aggregation in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 50: 1541–1545. Stigsby B, Johannesson G, Ingvar DH (1981). Regional EEG analysis and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s and Pick’s diseases. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 51: 537–547. Strong MJ, Lomen-Hoerth C, Caselli RJ, et al. (2003). Cognitive impairment, frontotemporal dementia, and the motor neuron diseases. Ann Neurol 54: S20–23. Stuss DT, Gallup GG Jr, Alexander MP (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain 124: 279–286. Swartz JR, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al. (1997a). Behavioural phenomenology in Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal dementia, and late-life depression: A retrospective analysis. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 10: 67–74. Swartz JR, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al. (1997b). Frontotemporal dementia: treatment response to serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors [published erratum appears in J Clin Psychiatry 1997 Jun;58:275]. J Clin Psychiatry 58: 212–216. Talbot JA, Morgan DL (1998). The effects of stretch parameters on eccentric exercise-induced damage to toad skeletal muscle. J Muscle Res Cell Motil 19: 237–245. Talbot PR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, et al. (1995). The contribution of single photon emission tomography to the clinical differentiation of degenerative cortical brain disorders. J Neurol 242: 579–586.
Talbot PR, Lloyd JJ, Snowden JS, et al. (1998). A clinical role for 99mtc-HMPAO SPECT in the investigation of dementia? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 64: 306–313. Talerico KA, Evans LK (2001). Responding to safety issues in frontotemporal dementias. Neurology 56: S52–55. Thomas-Anterion C, Jacquin K, Laurent B (2000). Differential mechanisms of impairment of remote memory in Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 11: 100–106. Thompson SA, Patterson K, Hodges JR (2003). Left/right asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia: behavioralcognitive implications. Neurology 61: 1196–1203. Tobo M, Fujii I, Hoaki T (1984). Computed tomography in Pick’s disease. Folia Psychiatr Neurol Jpn 38: 137–141. Turner RS, Kenyon LC, Trojanowski JQ, et al. (1996). Clinical, neuroimaging, and pathologic features of progressive nonfluent aphasia. Ann Neurol 39: 166–173. Varma AR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, et al. (1999). Evaluation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria in the differentiation of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 66: 184–188. Waxman SG, Geschwind N (1975). The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 32: 1580–1586. Whitwell JL, Josephs KA, Rossor MN, et al. (2005). Magnetic resonance imaging signatures of tissue pathology in frontotemporal dementia. Arch Neurol 62: 1402–1408. Wilhelmsen KC (1997). Frontotemporal dementia is on the maptau. Ann Neurol 41: 139–140. Wilhelmsen K, Lynch T, Pavlou E, et al. (1994). Localization of disinhibition-dementia-parkinsonism-amyotrophy complex to 17q21–22. Am J Hum Genet 6: 1159–1165. Yamauchi H, Fukuyama H, Nagahama Y, et al. (2000). Comparison of the pattern of atrophy of the corpus callosum in frontotemporal dementia, progressive supranuclear palsy, and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 69: 623–629. Yates CM, Simpson J, Maloney AFJ, et al. (1980). Neurochemical observations in a case of Pick’s disease. Neurol Sci 48: 257–263. Yener GG, Leuchter AF, Jenden D, et al. (1996). Quantitative EEG in frontotemporal dementia. Clinical Electroencephalogr 27: 61–68.