Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear responses in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii)

Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear responses in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii)

Accepted Manuscript Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear responses in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii) Ibrahim M. Hegab, ...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 51 Views

Accepted Manuscript Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear responses in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii) Ibrahim M. Hegab, Zhang Qian, Qiangsheng Pu, Zhicheng Wang, Kang yukun, Cai zhiyuan, Guo huailiang, Haifang Wang, Weihong Ji, A.M. Hanafy, Junhu Su PII:

S2351-9894(18)30359-7

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gecco.2018.e00503

Article Number: e00503 Reference:

GECCO 503

To appear in:

Global Ecology and Conservation

Received Date: 3 October 2018 Revised Date:

7 December 2018

Accepted Date: 7 December 2018

Please cite this article as: Hegab, I.M., Qian, Z., Pu, Q., Wang, Z., yukun, K., zhiyuan, C., huailiang, G., Wang, H., Ji, W., Hanafy, A.M., Su, J., Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear responses in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii), Global Ecology and Conservation (2019), doi: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.gecco.2018.e00503. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Gender difference in unconditioned and conditioned predator fear

2

responses in Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii)

3

Ibrahim M. Hegab1,2,3, Zhang Qian1, Qiangsheng Pu1, Zhicheng Wang1, Kang yukun1, Cai

4

zhiyuan1, Guo huailiang1, Haifang Wang1, Weihong Ji2,4, Hanafy A.M1,2,5, Junhu Su1,2*

5

1

6

Gansu Agricultural University, Lanzhou 730070, China

7

2

8

Gansu Agricultural University, Lanzhou 730070, China

9

3

RI PT

1

SC

College of Grassland Science, Key Laboratory of Grassland Ecosystem (Ministry of Education),

M AN U

Gansu Agricultural University-Massey University Research Centre for Grassland Biodiversity,

Department of Hygiene, Zoonoses and Animal Behaviour and Management, Faculty of

10

Veterinary Medicine, Suez Canal University, Ismailia 41522, Egypt

11

4

12

Shore Mail Centre 0632, Auckland, New Zealand

13

5

14

Egypt

TE D

Institute of Natural and Mathematical Sciences, Massey University, Private Bag 102904 North

EP

Animal Production Department, Faculty of Agriculture, Suez Canal University, 41522 Ismailia,

15 *

17

College of Grassland Science, Key Laboratory of Grassland Ecosystem (Ministry of Education),

18

Gansu, Agricultural University, Lanzhou 730070, China

19

e-mail: [email protected]

20

Tel. +86-931-7631213

AC C

16

Corresponding author: Dr. Junhu Su

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Abstract

22

Risk taking is imperative for the survival and fitness of animals since they constantly face

23

innumerable threats from various sources. Indeed, the ability of the individual to balance

24

between the costs and benefits of various options and adopt a wise decision is critical for the

25

well-being of the animal. In the current study, we investigated gender differences in risk taking

26

and decision making in male and female Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii), a subterranean

27

rodent species, in the defensive withdrawal apparatus (DWA) under novel, predator and

28

conditioned contexts. Under novel context, males showed more exploratory and less hiding

29

behavior than females especially during the first half of the testing session. However, under

30

predator and conditioned contexts, non-significant sexually dimorphic behavioral differences

31

were observed. Nevertheless, analysis of the performance of each gender separately across the

32

three contexts revealed that males showed strong aversion to the predator and conditioned

33

contexts. By contrast, females did not significantly vary their behavioral responses when exposed

34

to the predator and conditioned cues. Our results suggest that males are more inclined to indulge

35

in riskier exploratory activities while females may be regarded as risk-aversive. The current

36

findings highlighted the functional significance of the trade-off between risk and exploration in

37

the natural environment such that male and female zokors differed in how they gather and

38

process information during risky confrontations that ultimately elicit significant variations in

39

their decision-making and coping strategies to dangerous events.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

40

RI PT

21

41

42

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

43

Keyword: conditioning, behavior, sex differences, risk-taking, subterranean rodents

44

1. Introduction Rodents are daily jeopardized with a myriad of threats, and as dangerous confrontations

46

escalate, they have to constantly trade-off risk-prone behaviors such as feeding with risk-averse

47

ones such as vigilance to build up a series of assessments and decisions to adopt a sound strategy

48

for surviving with unanticipated events (Blanchard et al. 2005) and can thus have greater fitness

49

outcomes. This fact may be true for aboveground dwelling rodents that usually encounter ever-

50

changing events by showing adaptive alterations in their foraging, feeding, general activity and

51

shelter-seeking behaviors, which ultimately may affect their survival and fitness (Drakeley et al.

52

2015). Nevertheless, the underground ecotope represents a unique habitat for nearly 250

53

subterranean rodent species in which the underground dwellers inhabit, for most of their lives, a

54

complex sealed burrow system separated from the aboveground environment by mounds of soil

55

(Shams et al. 2005). This relatively secured habitat structure with a monotonous and stable

56

environment offers shelter to the subterranean dwellers against predators and adverse

57

environmental conditions (Šumbera et al. 2006). Moreover, this environment is relatively

58

constant and predictable, in comparison with the aboveground niche (Burda et al. 2007). Indeed,

59

this environmental predictability may exert ecological and evolutionary pressures on the risk

60

assessment and state anxiety in subterranean species in such a way that the more predictable

61

environment would require more preparations be launched for a possible dangerous encounter

62

(Parihar et al. 2009). Likewise, empirical studies demonstrate the effects of different

63

environmental sensory stimuli on risk assessment and threat perception (Munoz and Blumstein

64

2012; Fischer et al. 2017). For aboveground species, various sensory cues may facilitate the

65

acquisition of valid information from the environment in which the visual capacity of the animal

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

45

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

is sufficient to permit robust scanning of the surroundings concurrently with other sensory

67

systems, including hearing and olfaction. By contrast, the unchanging, extraordinary

68

underground niche exerts restriction on the sensory perception of the subterranean dwellers,

69

compared with that of aboveground taxa, because the underground burrows lack sufficient light

70

and ventilation, which eliminate vision and lessen the magnitude of olfactory stimuli by the soil

71

(Kimchi and Terkel 2004). Thus, a reasonable assumption is that these constraints on sensory

72

perception may hamper the cognitive ability of underground inhabitants to extract rational

73

meanings from common sensory inputs, which in turn might affect their risk assessment,

74

rationalizing and behavior guidance (Hegab et al. 2018). Bearing all this in mind, we assume

75

that the subterranean rodents may show atypical risk-assessment and anxiety-related behaviors

76

when challenged with risky and threatening circumstances, inconsistent with those previously

77

described in aboveground species (Jin et al. 2018; Sievert and Laska 2016).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

66

Predator odors are non-intrusive naturalistic stressors of high ethological relevance that

79

provoke various defensive responses in prey species (Hegab et al. 2015). The anti-predator

80

behavioral responses to a predation threat include reduced locomotor activities and retreating to a

81

safer shelter which, on one hand, minimize the probability of a possible fatal prey-predator

82

conflicts and, on the other hand, monitor of the predator threat from a strategic location (Stryjek

83

et al. 2018). Unequivocally, prior to the initiation and expression of the adaptive defensive

84

behaviors, first and foremost to avoid predation risk is to detect and identify the predator and the

85

source of threat perception as well. Equally important, remembering the place where a predator

86

or its odor had been previously encountered facilitates the avoidance of potential predators thus

87

protecting individuals from danger (Ferrari et al. 2010). This association between an

88

environmental context and aversive stimuli is known as contextual fear conditioning (Endres

AC C

EP

TE D

78

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

and Fendt 2007). Contextual fear conditioning paradigm is widely used to study predator odor

90

emotional associative learning and memory. The basic protocol of this model is very

91

straightforward (Takahashi et al. 2008); first it involves adapting the individual to the test

92

apparatus before exposure to the predator stimulus. Then, animals are exposed to the predator

93

stimulus within the same context and the unconditioned fear-related behavioral responses are

94

measured. Following exposure to the predator odor, the animal is returned to its home-cage and

95

tested the next day(s) for retention of contextual fear behavior in the same conditioning apparatus

96

with predator odor had been removed. A generous proportion of the literature described the

97

behavioral defensive responses in many aboveground rodent species following exposure to

98

predator odors, for example, Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus, Yin et al. 2013), Brandt’s voles

99

(Lasiopodomys brandtii, Hegab et al. 2014a), Pateau pika (Ochotona curzoniae, Yin et al.

100

2017), Wood mice (Apodemus sylvaticus, Navarro-Castilla et al. 2018), Bank voles

101

(Clethrionomys Glareolus, Borowski and Owadowska 2001) and Ship Rats (Rattus rattus,

102

Bramley and Waas 2001) and to a predator odor fear conditioning paradigm (Takahashi et al.

103

2005, Takahashi 2014, Halonen et al. 2016). However, very few studies have investigated the

104

effect of predator odors on the underground rodents such as in Talas Tuco-tuco (Ctenomys

105

talarum, Brachetta et al. 2016) and to the best of our knowledge, the predator odor contextual

106

conditioning paradigm had not been performed previously on a subterranean species.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

107

RI PT

89

Emerging evidences have demonstrated that males and females differ in some forms of

108

risky decision-making (Ward et al. 2004; Cavigelli et al. 2011; Orsini et al. 2016). Generally,

109

individuals could be classified based on their risk preference as “risk prone”, “risk neutral” or

110

“risk aversive” (Blumstein and Bouskila 1996; Platt and Huettel 2008). These sex variations

111

in the willingness to take risk are not only explained by the disparities in the morphological

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

(Samia et al. 2015), physiological or neural (van den Bos et al. 2013) architectures between

113

genders, but extended beyond to the differences in life-history trade-offs that may also divulge

114

how males and females process and utilize information, and thus affect their risk taking and

115

learning responses. The speed of decision making during exposure to a predation threat may

116

seems to be advantageous, for the first instance, for the individuals’ fitness as being faster at

117

information collection may help earning more rewards but, by contrast, may hinder an interactive

118

and thorough exploration of the risky context and increase the probability of predation encounter

119

as well which ultimately putting their life at stake (Jolles et al. 2015). On the other hand,

120

although meticulous information gathering and data acquisition may incur in a slower decision

121

making which might minimize the likelihood to grasp valuable rewards from the surrounding

122

environment, it may be a safer strategy that preserve the animals’ survival and provides more

123

detailed information about the nature of risk (Mathot et al. 2012). Therefore, it is reasonable to

124

assume that during many real-world decision, the dynamics of decision making are not only

125

curtailed on the potential rewards the animals can benefit, but also on the potential negative

126

reinforcements and outcomes. While males tend to be risk-inclined by being proactive and

127

rapidly performing more risky choices than females (Harris et al. 2010), females may be more

128

motivated to collect comprehensive information about their environment and making them more

129

flexible learners (Jolles et al. 2015). However, several decision-making models that have been

130

commonly used to explore gender differences do not involve the potential for some life-

131

threatening consequences that is sometimes encountered during exposure to dangerous events

132

such as a predatory encounter. Besides, the vast variations in the species (Bettis and Jacobs

133

2009) or even the strains (Rex et al. 1996) used in different risky-decision tests make a general

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

112

6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

134

speculation on the magnitude and direction of dimorphic sexual behavioral responses to risky

135

situations invalid. Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii) are a typical subterranean rodent species that dwell in

137

areas above 2000 m on the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau with other zokor species such as plateau

138

zokors (Eospalax baileyi) and (Eospalax rufescents), which show closely related morphological

139

and phylogenetic relationships (Su et al. 2014). Zokors live in very intricate burrow systems

140

with closed endings separated from the underground ecotope with mounds of soil (Su et al.,

141

2018). Zokors show frequent aboveground trips, but they remain in tunnels most of their lives

142

(Zhou and Dou 1990). Their skulls are also found in the pellets of many aerial predators

143

revealing that they most likely disperse aboveground (Cui et al. 2003). During the aboveground

144

dispersal, zokors would have increased vulnerability to multiple risks including both terrestrial

145

and flying predators (Schaller 1998). In the current study, we repeatedly exposed male and

146

female Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii) to a defensive withdrawal apparatus (Hegab et al.

147

2014b) which composed of two parts; a testing arena and a hide box, and observed their

148

behavioral responses under three conditions based on the classical fear conditioning paradigm

149

(Takahashi et al. 2008); (1) when the context is novel at the first exposure to measure the

150

responses of both sexes to novelty, (2) when the context comprised a predator odor (Cat odor) to

151

gauge the anti-predator behavioral responses in both genders, and finally (3) when the odor of

152

the predator was removed, to quantify responses to associatively learned risk. Within the

153

breeding season, males zokors are more inclined to indulge in territorial behaviors and

154

competing endeavors over access to burrows and mates (Zhang 2007), whereas females may

155

invest more time and activities in parental care. Additionally, males and females show a

156

significant difference in their home range size in the breeding season, with males exhibiting

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

136

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

wider home ranges than those of females (Hegab et al. 2018). Initially, we assume that these

158

sex-specific disparities in life history strategies and priorities may reflect differences in risk-

159

assessment/taking behavioral responses in zokors, and therefore, we hypothesized that male

160

zokors, when exposed to critical situations, will perform more risky behaviors whereas females

161

will display more risk-aversive actions to ensure their safety. Nevertheless, we may also present

162

an alternative hypothesis, contrary to the previous one, as zokors remained most or, even nearly,

163

all their entire life locked to a well-defined and a well-structured burrow system like a “train to

164

its rails” or a like a “spider to its web” except for few aboveground dispersal events (Zhang

165

2007) and , therefore, the limited space in which the zokors can utilize to launch various anti-

166

predator behavioral responses and maneuvering the predation threat would be limited and

167

ultimately this will abolish any apparent gender dimorphic variations in their threat perception,

168

decision making and the displayed behavioral responses to a predation risk.

169

2. Materials and Methods

170

2.1. Subjects and housing

SC

M AN U

TE D

Forty-three adult Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii) [Twenty-two males and twenty-one

EP

171

RI PT

157

females] were captured alive in specialized tube traps (Baoji Ludixincheng Co., Ltd, Xian, China)

173

from the grassland fields of Minxian County in the northwest part of the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau

174

(34.43807° N, 104.03688° S), Gansu Province. Neither trap deaths nor injuries were recorded.

175

Experts trapped the zokors during the breeding season (March-April 2018). Animals were then

176

transported to a laboratory animal room in which their health status was checked, and they were

177

sexed and individually housed in plastic cages (75 × 50 × 40 cm) with soil (20 cm depth) as

178

bedding material to mimic their natural habitat. Animals were fed ad libitum with carrots,

AC C

172

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

potatoes and sweet potatoes. Soil change and cage cleaning was performed as required. Animals

180

were housed in controlled conditions at 23 ± 1 °C and humidity of 55 ± 5% in a remote animal

181

room to avoid noises at Gansu Agricultural University.

182

2.2. Experimental set-up

RI PT

179

All animals were experimentally naïve and had not undergone any previous behavioral

184

experiments. Testing was conducted in a separate room connected to the animal room with a

185

swinging door. Animals were always handled and carried from the animal room to the testing

186

room by the same experimenter, thereby reducing stress and increasing the validity of the results.

187

To investigate the zokors’ behavioral responses to risk, we subjected them repeatedly to one of

188

three identical defensive withdrawal apparatus (DWA). The apparatus consisted of an opaque

189

glass wall divided the box into two compartments; the ‘testing arena’ consisted of a rectangular

190

area (60 × 30 × 30 cm), and the second compartment, termed the ‘hide box,’ was constructed

191

from black glass (15 x 30 x 30 cm). A small, square hole (8 × 8 cm) in the front wall of the hide

192

box allowed the tested individuals to enter the arena (Hegab et al. 2014b). The front wall of the

193

arena contained a double-face adhesive tape above the floor that held a piece of clothes (2×4 cm

194

strip), termed the ‘stimulus’. We used either a ‘cat odour stimulus’, created by placing the

195

clothes on laboratory cat for 10 consecutive days, or a ‘control stimulus’ that had not been in

196

contact with a cat. Both control and cat odor stimuli were kept separately in airtight plastic bags

197

at −20°C and were always manipulated with latex gloves. The zokors’ movements were recorded

198

using a camera in the center of the top-unit of the apparatus.

199

2.3. Experimental procedure

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

183

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Zokors were subjected to the DWA set-up for three 20 min sessions on three subsequent

201

days. On day 1, the testing arena contained the control stimulus and was new to the zokors

202

‘novel context’. On day 2, the testing arena contained the cat odour stimulus ‘predator odour

203

context’. On day 3, the testing arena again contained the control stimulus, thus considered as a

204

‘conditioned context’. Zokors were placed in the hiding compartment of the apparatus at the

205

beginning of each trial. Both control and cat odour cues were replaced using disposable gloves

206

before testing a new individual. To avoid the transfer of zokor odours between trials, the DWA

207

(both the testing arena and hide box) was thoroughly cleaned with 50% ethanol solution and

208

paper towels after each trial. Between test days the arenas were thoroughly cleaned an extra time.

209

2.4. Behavioral measures

M AN U

SC

RI PT

200

All testing sessions were recorded with an infrared video network camera (Model.

211

ZKXC.TD136U2RZT, Zhongke Electric Co., Ltd, Shenzhen, China) mounted above the

212

apparatus and connected to a monitor and a computer in an adjacent room for recording and

213

monitoring animal behavior. Videos were analyzed using Behavioral Observation Research

214

Interactive Software (BORIS, v. 2.95, Friard and Gamba 2016). Data were collectively

215

categorized as follows: (1) the proportion of times zokors were completely hidden inside the

216

hide-box (‘hidden’) including the tail, (2) the individual’s body is in the hide-box with its head

217

outside the opening, a typical risk-assessment position (Head-out; Dielenberg McGregor 2001)

218

and (3) the individual’s body completely outside the hide-box (‘out’). These measures are

219

mutually exclusive and reflect different levels of risk-taking and/or engagement with the

220

environment: low (hidden), intermediate (head-out) and high (out). Additionally, the latency

221

from placing the individual inside the hide box at the start of the experiment till the whole zokor

AC C

EP

TE D

210

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

or part of it appear outside the box was also recorded alongside the duration of locomotor

223

activities and contact to the neutral or predator stimulus at the relevant contexts (Hegab et al.

224

2014c).

225

2.5. Data analysis

RI PT

222

The SPSS 22.00 statistical software package (Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.) was used for all

227

analyses. In each context, Student’s test was used to examine the differences between males and

228

females for different behavioral variables. Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs)

229

were used within each context to analyze the difference between the first and second halves of

230

the testing sessions (10 minutes each) with sex (between-subjects factor) and time (first and

231

second halves as within-subjects factor) effects on the different behavioral responses. Also, One-

232

Way repeated-measures ANOVAs were run separately for each gender across the different

233

contexts. Duncan’s post hoc test was used to compare different behavioral responses if

234

necessarily. Effect size [partial-eta squared (η2) values for repeated measures ANOVA were

235

calculated. The level of significance at which the null hypothesis was rejected was α= 0.05.

236

3. Results

237

3.1. Gender differences inside the novel context

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

226

238

When male and female zokors firstly placed in the novel context with the control stimulus,

239

males spent more time outside the hide box (t41= 2.19, p= 0.03; Fig. 1-C) and were less time

240

hidden (t41=−2.43, p= 0.02; Fig. 1-A) than females. The mean duration of locomotor activities

241

was higher in males (t41=−2.96, p= 0.01; Fig. 1-D), but females showed longer latencies till leave

242

the hide box for the first time (t41=−3.84, p< 0.01; Fig. 1-F). Both genders did not display

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

243

significant differences in the time spent neither for head-out (t41= 1.66, p= 0.10; Fig. 1-B) nor

244

sniffing the control stimulus (t41= 0.76, p= 0.45; Fig. 1-E). Means of different behavioral activities of male and female Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii)

RI PT

245

during the first and second 10 minutes of the testing session under the novel context were

247

presented in Supplementary table (1). Analysis of the behavioral variables in both genders

248

between the first and second 10 minutes of the observation session using repeated measures

249

ANOVA also yielded clear sex differences. Comparison of the hiding levels showed that there

250

was no significant time (F1,41= 0.52, p= 0.48, η2= 0.012) effect on the mean duration of hiding

251

but there were significant effects of sex (F1,41= 5.50, p= 0.024, η2= 0.12) and Time x Sex

252

interaction (F1,41= 5.06, p= 0.03, η2= 0.11) that males hid more than females and Duncan’s post

253

hoc test showed that the highest proportion of time spent in the hide box was in the first half of

254

the testing trial for females (0.70 ± 0.07) while males spent the lowest proportion of time hiding

255

during the first half of the trial (0.41 ± 0.05). A significant effect of time (F1,41= 8.68, p= 0.005,

256

η2= 0.18) on head-out behavior was observed that zokors tend to significantly decrease (t84= 2.92,

257

p= 0.02) head out in the second half of the testing session (First half; 0.03 ± 0.007, Second half:

258

0.02 ± 0.004), but there were neither significant sex (F1,41= 2.71, p= 0.11, η2= 0.06) nor Sex ×

259

Time interaction (F1,41= 0.21, p= 0.65, η2= 0.005) effects. Our results also showed significant sex

260

(F1,41= 4.77, p= 0.04, η2= 0.104) and Sex × Time interaction (F1,41= 4.98, p= 0.03, η2= 0.108)

261

effects that males spent more time out of cover during the testing session than females (Male;

262

0.49 ± 0.05, Females; 0.31 ± 0.07) and Duncan’s post hoc test showed that the highest proportion

263

of time spent out of cover was recorded during the first half of the testing session for males (0.54

264

± 0.05) while females displayed the lowest time (0.27 ± 0.06) out of cover during the first half

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

246

12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

265

of the testing session, whereas there was no significant effect of the time of the trial (F1,41= 0.01,

266

p= 0.76, η2= 0.002). Zokors tended to significantly (Time: F1,41= 8.24, p= 0.006, η2= 0.167) display more

268

locomotor activities (sec) during the first half of the trial (83.70 ± 9.34) compared to the second

269

one (55.98 ± 8.19) and males significantly (Sex: F1,41= 8.52, p= 0.006, η2= 0.172) showed more

270

locomotion than females (178.40 ± 17.63 and 98.18 ± 20.66, respectively), while there was no

271

significant Time × Sex interaction (F1,41= 0.81, p= 0.37, η2= 0.019) was observed. Finally,

272

neither Time (F1,41= 0.45, p= 0.51, η2= 0.011), Sex (F1,41= 0.58, p= 0.45, η2= 0.014) nor Time ×

273

Sex interaction (F1,41= 0.05, p= 0.80, η2= 0.001) showed any effects on the mean duration of

274

contact time (sec) to the control stimulus in males (First half; 7.88 ± 2.14, Second half; 9.21 ±

275

4.95) and females (First half; 4.71 ± 1.50, Second half; 7.29 ± 2.67).

276

3.2. Gender differences inside the predator odor context

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

267

Upon exposure to the field test arena containing the cat odour stimulus, male and female

278

zokors showed non-significant differences in their responses to the predator threat. Both genders

279

spent nearly the same proportion of hiding time (t41=−1.42, p= 0.16; Fig. 1-A), head out (t41=

280

1.19, p= 0.24; Fig. 1-B) and out of cover (t41= 1.36, p= 0.18; Fig. 1-C). Similarly, the mean

281

duration of both locomotor activities (t41=0.88, p=0.38; Fig. 1-D) and contact to the odor source

282

(t41= 1.47, p= 0.15; Fig. 1-E) did not significantly change between genders. However, females

283

tended to spend longer latencies till first emerge from the hide box than males (t41=−2.76, p=

284

0.01; Fig. 1-F).

AC C

EP

277

285

Similarly, repeated measures analysis of within testing session (10 minutes interval) of the

286

behavioral responses of both sexes revealed absence of significance sex differences in the zokors

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

responses toward predator odors over time. Neither Sex nor the Time × Sex interaction showed

288

any significant effects on the proportion of hiding time (Sex: F1,41= 2.21, p= 0.15, η2= 0.05/

289

Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.12, p= 0.73, η2= 0.003), head out (Sex: F1,41= 1.08, p= 0.31, η2= 0.03/

290

Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.36, p= 0.55, η2= 0.009), out of cover (Sex: F1,41= 2.05, p= 0.16, η2= 0.05/

291

Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.17, p= 0.68, η2= 0.004), the mean duration of locomotion (Sex: F1,41= 0.92,

292

p= 0.34, η2= 0.02/ Time × Sex: F1,41= 1.42, p= 0.24, η2= 0.03) and contact to the predator

293

stimulus (Sex: F1,41= 2.13, p= 0.15, η2= 0.05/ Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.96, p= 0.33, η2= 0.02).

294

Means of different behavioral activities of male and female Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii)

295

during the first and second 10 minutes of the testing session under the predator odor context were

296

presented in Supplementary table (1)

M AN U

SC

RI PT

287

However, zokors significantly decrease their head out (F1,41= 9.65, p= 0.003, η2= 0.190)

298

during the second half of the testing session (First half: 0.03 ± 0.004, Second half: 0.02 ± 0.004),

299

and similarly showed reduced locomotion (F1,41= 11.29, p= 0.002, η2= 0.22) activities over time

300

during the second half of the testing session (First half: 72.39 ± 8.87, Second half: 47.35 ± 7.05).

301

zokors neither significantly vary their proportion of hiding time (F1,41= 0.33, p= 0.57, η2= 0.008)

302

and time spent out of cover (F1,41= 0.07, p= 0.80, η2= 0.002) nor the mean contact time to the

303

odor source (F1,41= 3.23, p= 0.08, η2= 0.07) between the first and second halves of the testing

304

session.

305

3.3. Gender differences to the conditioned context

AC C

EP

TE D

297

306

Similar to the results of the predator context, testing zokor in the conditioned context

307

disclosed absence of gender differences to the conditioned context regarding the proportion of

308

hiding time (t41=−0.07, p= 0.94; Fig. 1-A), head out (t41= 1.07, p= 0.29; Fig. 1-B), out of cover

309

(t41=−0.06, p= 0.95; Fig. 1-C) and the mean duration of both locomotor activities (t41= 0.28, p= 14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

0.78; Fig. 1-D) and contact to the neutral stimulus as well (t41= 0.21, p= 0.83; Fig. 1-E) did not

311

significantly change between genders. However, females tended to spend longer latencies till

312

first emerge from the hide box than males (t41=−2.62, p= 0.01; Fig. 1-F).

RI PT

310

Repeated measures analysis of within testing session of the behavioral responses of both

314

sexes revealed absence of significance sex differences in the zokors responses toward the

315

conditioned context over time. Neither Sex nor the Time × Sex interaction showed any

316

significant effects on the proportion of hiding time (Sex: F1,41= 0.005, p= 0.94, η2= 0.0001/ Time

317

× Sex: F1,41= 3.50, p= 0.07, η2= 0.08), head out (Sex: F1,41= 1.15, p= 0.29, η2= 0.03/ Time × Sex:

318

F1,41= 1.33, p= 0.26, η2= 0.03), out of cover (Sex: F1,41= 0.003, p= 0.96, η2= 0.00008/ Time ×

319

Sex: F1,41= 2.49, p= 0.12, η2= 0.06), the mean duration of locomotion (Sex: F1,41= 0.08, p= 0.78,

320

η2= 0.002/ Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.10, p= 0.75, η2= 0.002) and contact to the predator stimulus

321

(Sex: F1,41= 0.05, p= 0.83, η2= 0.001/ Time × Sex: F1,41= 0.09, p= 0.76, η2= 0.002). Zokors

322

significantly hid more (F1,41= 5.20, p= 0.03, η2= 0.11) during the second half of the testing

323

session (First half: 0.67 ± 0.04, Second half: 0.76 ± 0.05). Nevertheless, both gender

324

significantly showed less head out (F1,41= 8.03, p= 0.007, η2= 0.16) behavior during the second

325

half of the testing trial (First half: 0.04 ± 0.01, Second half: 0.01 ± 0.002) and significantly

326

lowered (F1,41= 11.63, p= 0.001, η2= 0.22) their locomotion as well over time (First half: 61.40 ±

327

8.63, Second half: 35.49 ± 7.05). Finally, zokors neither alter their time out of cover (F1,41= 2.34,

328

p= 0.13, η2= 0.05) nor contact time to the odor source (F1,41= 0.19, p= 0.66, η2= 0.005) between

329

the first and second parts of the testing trial. Means of different behavioral activities of male and

330

female Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii) during the first and second 10 minutes of the testing

331

session under the conditioned context were presented in Supplementary table (1)

332

3.4. Comparison of gender behavioral responses between the different contexts

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

313

15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

We also conducted separate repeated measures analysis (ANOVA) on the different

334

behavioral responses to gauge the different reactions of each gender separately between the

335

novel, predator and conditioned contexts. Male zokors significantly (F2,42= 8.62, p= 0.001, η2=

336

0.29) hid more in the conditioned context (Fig. 1- A) than they did in the predator and novel ones,

337

while females did not show significant changes in the proportion of hiding time between the

338

different contexts (F2,42= 0.22, p= 0.80, η2= 0.01). Conversely, males significantly (F2,41= 8.22,

339

p= 0.001, η2= 0.28) spent less time out of cover in the conditioned context than they did in the

340

predator and novel ones (Fig. 1-C) while females did not show significant changes in the

341

proportion of time outside the hide box between different contexts. Neither males (F2,42= 0.12,

342

p= 0.79, η2= 0.006) nor females (F2,42= 0.14, p= 0.86, η2= 0.007) showed any significant changes

343

in the proportion time for head-out between the different contexts (Fig. 1-B). Males tended to

344

significantly modify their locomotion activities (F2,42= 12.14, p< 0.0001, η2= 0.37) between

345

different contexts that they showed the lowest locomotor activities during exposure to the

346

conditioned context while females did not significantly alter their locomotor activities (F2,42=

347

0.23, p= 0.80, η2= 0.01) between different contexts (Fig. 1-D). Both males (F2,42= 4.41, p= 0.02,

348

η2= 0.17) and females (F2,42= 3.70, p= 0.03, η2= 0.16) displayed the lowest durations of time to

349

contact the source of the stimulus when exposed to the conditioned context than they did in the

350

predator or novel ones (Fig. 1-E). Finally, the latency to leave the hide box for the first time

351

significantly vary between different contexts in males (F2,42= 2.29, p= 0.14, η2= 0.01) that they

352

showed the higher latency when exposed to the conditioned context (Fig. 1-F) while females did

353

not significantly (F2,42= 1.08, p= 0.34, η2= 0.05) change the latency when exposed to different

354

contexts.

355

4. Discussion

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

333

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

To investigate sex differences in Smith's zokors (Eospalax smithii), a subterranean rodent

357

species, under risky environment, we used the predator-odor fear conditioning paradigm to

358

measure the behavioral responses when the context is novel (Low risk), when a predator odors

359

was presented (High risk) and finally the association between the predator odor context and the

360

conditioned context (Fear-Conditioning) when the predator odor was removed in the next day.

361

Our findings that males displayed more exploration, spend more time in locomotion and hid less

362

than females during exposure to the novel context support the previous claims that males showed

363

more “boldness” and were more risk-prone than females. Our results are consistent with those

364

reported by (King et al. 2013; Anchan et al. 2014) using a range of species; however, other

365

studies show that males when tested in a novel context are more-risk aversive and less

366

exploratory than females (Johnston and File 1991; Kokras and Dalla 2014). Several aspects

367

might be regarded to explain these contrasting findings and interpretation of such studies. Firstly,

368

the nature of the testing apparatus might reflect different states of emotionality and risk

369

perception between the different testing conditions. Rodents have a natural response to risk by

370

hiding or escaping (Kavaliers and Choleris 2001), however, previous studies using the novel

371

contexts without offering a hiding place are more likely to measure the “Compulsory

372

exploration”, while in paradigms (Similar to our study) the motivational state of exploration

373

would be termed “Voluntary exploration” where the animal would have the opportunity to either

374

hide or explore the testing arena. Furthermore, gender differences are not always detected in a

375

consistent direction when testing subjects even within the same behavioral paradigm of anxiety

376

(Meng and Drugan 1993). For example, female Lister hooded rats showed less social

377

interaction than males, which denotes higher levels of anxiety, and in the same test, displayed a

378

higher level of exploration and ambulation that reflects a low anxiety level (Johnston and File

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

356

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1991). Finally, these sexually dimorphic behavioral patterns in during exposure to the novel

380

context might suggest variability in life-history strategies between male and female zokors.

381

Zokors are solitary subterranean rodents, and males during the breeding season are more active

382

and maintain larger home ranges than those of females to seek a mate. In nature, zokors are free-

383

roaming individuals who occupy a large home range of 1790 ± 720 m2 for males and 260 ± 112

384

m2 for females (Zhang, 2007). Therefore, taken into considerations that animals had been

385

captured during the breeding season, these huge differences in home-range sizes may elicit more

386

exploration and less hiding in males that emulate gender difference in the motivational states

387

between males and females when they explored novel areas. Also, close inspection and analysis

388

of the zokors’ activity patterns during the novel context exposure session showed clear gender

389

differences in the behavioral responses between the first and second parts of the trial. Males

390

tended to display more exploratory behaviors and hid less than females during the first half of the

391

trial which support our previous assumption that during exposure to risk, males tended to be

392

more risk-prone while females show more risk-aversive reactions (Anchan et al. 2014).

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

379

Based on the finding that males took more risk in the predator odor and conditioned

394

contexts, we can clearly imply that female zokors did not follow the classical anti-predator

395

defensive responses (Decreasing time out of cover and shelter-seeking behaviors) as commonly

396

observed not only in the above-ground rodents (Bramley and Waas 2001; Staples 2010;

397

Ferrero et al. 2011; Pérez-Gómez et al. 2015), but also compared to their male conspecifics

398

which had been tested under the same testing conditions. This finding suggests that males zokors

399

have clearer anti-predatory and stronger fear conditioning avoidance behavioral responses than

400

females. Several aspects of the life-history strategies would be discussed to explain our results.

AC C

EP

393

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

The nature of zokors as solitary subterranean rodent that live in an intricate underground

402

burrow system mandates dispersal of individuals to new locations rather than staying in their

403

natal home range. Male-biased dispersal had been documented in zokors that males maximize

404

reproductive success by mating with multiple females and should disperse for both social

405

(competition for mates from dominant older males) and genetic (inbreeding avoidance) reasons.

406

However, the pattern of dispersal, if occur underground, would result in more energetically

407

highly demanding ambulatory and digging activities that would eventually incur a heavy energy

408

expenditure (Vleck 1979); thus, males choose the aboveground route for dispersal, because as

409

previously reported in (Zhang 2007), “The energetic costs of a potential dispersal through the

410

soil appear to be impossibly high”. Animals can learn about dangerous stimuli when they

411

directly interact with them. In the wild, young zokors spend the first weeks of life (50 days,

412

Zhang et al. 1995) in an enclosed nesting chamber. When they emerge from their natal burrows

413

for dispersal, they are exposed to predators that pose significant threats or at least exposed to

414

various predatory cues. As mentioned earlier, males might be the more vulnerable sex to the

415

predatory risk due to their predominant above-ground dispersion pattern. Therefore, this former

416

experience, particularly in young or immature individuals, due to the above-ground trips and the

417

earlier acquisition of information regarding the predators’ cues may systematically influence

418

male zokors through consolidating of a strong recognition memory especially during the young

419

ages which help them recalling their previous dangerous confrontations when they exposed to

420

the same threatening stimulus repeatedly during their lifetime even during their adulthood

421

(Wiedenmayer 2009), learning about properties of the situation and adjusting its behavior in

422

successive encounters. This experience in early ontogeny might shape the expression of anti-

423

predator defensive responses in male zokors to mimic those previously reported in the above-

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

401

19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ground taxa, while females did not encounter the same experience due to the fewer or non-

425

aboveground trips compared to males. We hereby cannot assume that female zokors did not

426

recognize and identify the predator odor as the anti-predator responses are innate even the prey

427

had no prior experience to predation cues (Apfelbach et al. 2005), but the risk of exploring a

428

novel area, due to lack of past experience, might provoke an equal perception of threat for

429

females as a predator encounter might do. Therefore, females may prefer to launch another tactic

430

across the different contexts that rely on being more stationary and less reactive to the changing

431

contexts. This is clear in the behavioral response of females in the conditioned context to the

432

odor source where they showed stronger aversion and less contact time to the clean towel than

433

they did in the novel and predator contexts before.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

424

Several Studies have demonstrated sex differences in responses to predation risk

435

(Blanchard et al. 1990; Harris et al. 2010) and learning performance (Jonasson 2005; van den

436

Bos et al. 2013) separately, nevertheless the potential of the predator odor fear-conditioning

437

paradigm involves the use of a natural unconditioned stimulus. Our results clearly revealed that

438

male zokors are more risk-prone and depending more on their former experiences, while females

439

being more sensitive to any environmental changes. The evolution of gender differences in both

440

parental and reproductive investments in zokors may explain the differences in the anti-predator

441

and learning responses. Within the breeding season, males are more inclined to indulge in

442

territorial activities and competing endeavors over access to burrows and females (Zhang 2007),

443

whereas females may invest more time and activities in reproductive and parental care make

444

them valuable resource which ensures the survival of their offspring. Our results are parallel with

445

the asset protection model that predicts that an individual’s past successes at accruing fitness will

446

shape its response to current predation threats (Katwaroo-Andersen et al. 2016). Consequently,

AC C

EP

TE D

434

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

the higher costs and possibly increased predation risk that comes with the higher reproductive

448

and maternal investments of females, especially during the breeding period, may make hiding

449

and decreased exploratory responses more appropriate to female zokors ensure their safety. Thus,

450

prey may modify their behavioral decisions based on both the perceived intensity of acute risk

451

and/or the resulting fitness expectations. Similar results were also found in other species in

452

which females have a higher reproductive and maternal investment. In Spanish terrapins

453

(Mauremys leprosa), gravid females took longer to emerge from their shells following mimicked

454

predator risk, and females with larger clutches took longer to emerge than those with smaller

455

clutches (Ibáñez et al. 2015). Conversely, in Pipefish (Roelke and Sogard 1993), when females

456

invest less in parental care and reproductive fitness, they showed strong risk-taking behavioral

457

responses than males which invest more time in broodiness than females.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

447

Typical odor sources are skin, fur, urine, droppings and secretions of the anal glands are

459

commonly used in different studies (Tidhar et al. 2007; Hacquemand et al. 2010; Sievert and

460

Laska 2016; Yin et al. 2017), and fur/skin source is considered the most potent stimulus

461

providing a strong indication that a predator is nearby now and able to perform a successful

462

strike (Hegab et al. 2015). Therefore, we cannot assume that the odor source was not effective to

463

induce fear-related behavioral responses in zokors since males showed strong anti-predator

464

behavioral changes when tested in the predator context and successfully formed an effective risk-

465

conditioned association when the predator odor has been removed. However, our experimental

466

set-up may have contributed to the absence of behavioral response in female zokors. females live

467

in deeper burrows (2–2.5 m depth) than males (0.8–1.5 m) (Shao et al., 2015), and never or

468

rarely leave their nests neither for dispersal nor for foraging as they forge completely

469

underground (Zhang 2007). Accordingly, it can be argued that the hide box in the defensive

AC C

EP

TE D

458

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

withdrawal apparatus may well have presented the same perception of shelter as do the tunnel

471

system for females in the wild. Therefore, the hide box, which was found fit testing multiple

472

species (Dielenberg and McGregor 1999; Eilam et al. 1999, Hegab et al. 2014 a,b,c), may be

473

inappropriate for when investigating the risk-perception and decision making under predation

474

threat in female zokors, although it was appropriate when testing males. Furthermore, in an

475

elegant study conducted by Eilam et al. (1999), common spiny mouse (Acomys cahirinus) did

476

not display show any behavioral changes after exposure to owl calls despite increased cortisol

477

levels which are indicative of stress. Indeed, predator odors may elicit various anti-predator

478

responses including behavioral, hormonal and molecular changes in defensive-related brain

479

regions (Apfelbach et al. 2005; Kondoh et al. 2016). The lack of the behavioral changes in

480

female zokors between the different context may be systematically accompanied by hormonal

481

and/or gene expression as previously reported in Eilam et al. (1999). Further studies with a

482

different apparatus and experimental procedure concurrently with analysis of the hormonal

483

profiles might be required to unravel the lack of behavioral response in female zokors.

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

470

Although we used a behavioral testing paradigm that has been extensively used in studies

485

on rodents’ defensive behavior, the results obtained in the current study were somewhat different

486

from those obtained either from common laboratory (Ferrero et al. 2011; Sievert and Laska

487

2016)] or laboratory-bred wild species (Yin et al. 2011; Hegab et al. 2014 a,b,c; Yin et al.

488

2017). First, the levels of head-out behavior did not significantly differ between the different

489

contexts. Head-out is behavioral response aims to collect information regarding the surrounding

490

environment before departing the safe shelter (Dielenberg and McGregor 2001). However, it

491

may be considered more risky strategy for zokors than staying concealed completely, especially

492

in the high-risk context of predator odor, which suggests that a predator may be nearby (Jolles et

AC C

EP

484

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

al. 2015). Second, zokors did not show aversion to the predator odor source, although both

494

genders showed strong aversion to the neutral odor source in the conditioned context. Similar

495

results were also found in a recent study conducted on wild Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus),

496

where wild rats did not avoid boxes scented with predator odor (Stryjek et al. 2018). This might

497

denote that laboratory-bred individuals in comparable studies may not display similar defensive

498

behavioral responses under risky situations as their wild counterparts will do.

RI PT

493

In conclusion, male and female zokors adopt distinct behavioral strategies when

500

confronted with novel, predator and conditioned contexts where males showed different

501

behavioral responses across contexts, but females did not vary their responses between different

502

degrees of risk. These variations in risk-taking behaviors may arise from gender differences in

503

information gathering and decision-making processes due to the selective pressure of the

504

underground ecotope and life-history traits. A further investigation of the link between

505

behavioral and neuro-endocrinological responses toward predation risk is needed for better

506

illustration of the evolution of defensive anti-predatory tactics in male and female Smith’s zokors.

507

Acknowledgements

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

499

Funding for this work was supported by the Special funds for discipline construction

509

of Gansu Agricultural University (GAU-XKJS-2018-003); National Natural Science Foundation

510

of China (Nos. 31460566 and 31760706); Gansu Provincial Natural Science Foundation for

511

Distinguished Young Scholars of China (No. 1606RJDA314); “Fuxi Talent” Plan of Gansu

512

Agricultural University (Gaufx-02J03), Longyuan Youth Innovation and Entrepreneurship

513

Talent Project, the Project of the Innovation Team on Gansu Grassland and Animal Husbandry

514

Sustainable Development (2017C-11) and Talented Young Scientists fellowship (TYSP) of the

515

Ministry of Science. The authors would like to thank Professor Shangli Shi (Dean of School of

AC C

508

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Grassland Sciences, Gansu Agricultural University) and the Department of Hygiene, Zoonoses

517

and Animal Behavior and Management, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Suez Canal University

518

for their moral and career support. The authors declare they have no competing interests.

RI PT

516

519 520 References

Anchan, Divya, Sara Clark, Kevin Pollard, and Nandini Vasudevan. 2014. "GPR30 activation

522

decreases anxiety in the open field test but not in the elevated plus maze test in female

523

mice." Brain and Behavior 4(1):51-59.

M AN U

SC

521

524

Apfelbach, Raimund, Caroline D. Blanchard, Robert J. Blanchard, R. Andrew Hayes, and Iain S.

525

McGregor. 2005. "The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: A review of

526

field and laboratory studies." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29(8):1123-44.

528

Bettis, Tania J., and Lucia F. Jacobs. 2009. "Sex-specific strategies in spatial orientation in

TE D

527

C57BL/6J mice." Behavioural Processes 82(3):249-55. Blanchard, D. Caroline, J. Blanchard Robert, and Guy Griebel. 2005. "Defensive Responses to

530

Predator Threat in the Rat and Mouse." Current Protocols in Neuroscience 30(1):8.19.1-

531

8.19.20.

AC C

EP

529

532

Blanchard, D. Caroline, Robert J. Blanchard, Paul Tom, and R. J. Rodgers. 1990. "Diazepam

533

changes risk assessment in an anxiety/defense test battery." Psychopharmacology (Berl)

534

101(4):511-18.

24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Blumstein, Daniel T., and Amos Bouskila. 1996. "Assessment and Decision Making in Animals:

536

A Mechanistic Model underlying Behavioral Flexibility Can Prevent Ambiguity." Oikos

537

77(3):569-76.

RI PT

535

Borowski, Zbigniew, and Edyta Owadowska. 2001. "Spatial Responses of Field (Microtus

539

Agrestis) and Bank (Clethrionomys Glareolus) Voles to Weasel (Mustela Nivalis) Odour

540

in Natural Habitat." Pp. 289-93 in Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 9, edited by Anna

541

Marchlewska-Koj, John J. Lepri, and Dietland Müller-Schwarze. Boston, MA: Springer

542

US.

M AN U

SC

538

543

Brachetta, Valentina, Cristian E. Schleich, and Roxana R. Zenuto. 2016. "Source Odor, Intensity,

544

and Exposure Pattern Affect Antipredatory Responses in the Subterranean Rodent

545

Ctenomys talarum." Ethology 122(12):923-36.

Bramley, Gary N., and Joseph R. Waas. 2001. "Laboratory and Field Evaluation of Predator

547

Odors as Repellents for Kiore (Rattus exulans) and Ship Rats (R. rattus)." Journal of

548

Chemical Ecology 27(5):1029-47.

TE D

546

Burda, Hynek, Radim Šumbera, and Sabine Begall. 2007. "Microclimate in Burrows of

550

Subterranean Rodents — Revisited." Pp. 21-33 in Subterranean Rodents: News from

551

Underground, edited by Sabine Begall, Hynek Burda, and Cristian E. Schleich. Berlin,

AC C

552

EP

549

Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

553

Cavigelli, Sonia A., Kerry C. Michael, Sheila G. West, and Laura Cousino Klein. 2011.

554

"Behavioral responses to physical vs. social novelty in male and female laboratory rats."

555

Behavioural Processes 88(1):56-59.

25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

558 559

560 561

Bubo bubo. Chin J Zool 38:57–63 [In Chinese with English summary] Dielenberg, Robert A., and Iain S. McGregor. 1999. "Habituation of the hiding response to cat

RI PT

557

Cui Q, Lian X, Zhang T, Su J (2003) Food habits comparison between Buteo hemilasius and

odor in rats (Rattus norvegicus)." J Comp Psychol 113(4):376-87.

Dielenberg, R.A., McGregor, I.S. 2001. "Defensive behavior in rats towards predatory odors: a review." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 25(7):597-609.

SC

556

Drakeley, Maximilian, Oriol Lapiedra, and Jason J. Kolbe. 2015. "Predation Risk Perception,

563

Food Density and Conspecific Cues Shape Foraging Decisions in a Tropical Lizard."

564

PLoS One 10(9):e0138016.

M AN U

562

Eilam, D., T. Dayan, S. Ben-Eliyahu, I. I. Schulman, G. Shefer, and C. A. Hendrie. 1999.

566

"Differential behavioural and hormonal responses of voles and spiny mice to owl calls."

567

Animal Behaviour 58(5):1085-93.

569

Endres, Thomas, and Markus Fendt. 2007. "Conditioned behavioral responses to a context paired with the predator odor trimethylthiazoline." Behav Neurosci 121(3):594-601.

EP

568

TE D

565

Ferrari, Maud C. O., Grant E. Brown, Gary R. Bortolotti, and Douglas P. Chivers. 2010.

571

"Linking predator risk and uncertainty to adaptive forgetting: a theoretical framework and

572 573

AC C

570

empirical test using tadpoles." Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277(1691):2205-10.

574

Ferrero, David M., Jamie K. Lemon, Daniela Fluegge, Stan L. Pashkovski, Wayne J. Korzan,

575

Sandeep Robert Datta, Marc Spehr, Markus Fendt, and Stephen D. Liberles. 2011.

26

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

576

"Detection and avoidance of a carnivore odor by prey." Proceedings of the National

577

Academy of Sciences 108(27):11235. Fischer, Stefan, Evelyne Oberhummer, Filipa Cunha-Saraiva, Nina Gerber, and Barbara

579

Taborsky. 2017. "Smell or vision? The use of different sensory modalities in predator

580

discrimination." Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 71(10):143.

RI PT

578

Friard, Olivier, and Marco Gamba. 2016. "BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging

582

software for video/audio coding and live observations." Methods in Ecology and

583

Evolution 7(11):1325-30.

M AN U

SC

581

584

Hacquemand, Romain, Laurence Jacquot, and Gérard Brand. 2010. "Comparative Fear-Related

585

Behaviors to Predator Odors (TMT and Natural Fox Feces) before and after Intranasal

586

ZnSO(4) Treatment in Mice." Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 4:188. Halonen, Joshua D., Phillip R. Zoladz, Collin R. Park, and David M. Diamond. 2016.

588

"Behavioral and Neurobiological Assessments of Predator-Based Fear Conditioning and

589

Extinction." Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science Vol.06No.08:20.

TE D

587

Harris, Sanna, Indar W. Ramnarine, Henrik G. Smith, and Lars B. Pettersson. 2010. "Picking

591

personalities apart: estimating the influence of predation, sex and body size on boldness

AC C

592

EP

590

in the guppy Poecilia reticulata." Oikos 119(11):1711-18.

593

Hegab, I. M., G. Shang, M. Ye, Y. Jin, A. Wang, B. Yin, S. Yang, and W. Wei. 2014a.

594

"Defensive responses of Brandt's voles (Lasiopodomys brandtii) to chronic predatory

595

stress." Physiology & Behavior 126:1-7.

27

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Hegab, I. M., Y. Jin, M. Ye, A. Wang, B. Yin, S. Yang, and W. Wei. 2014b. "Defensive

597

responses of Brandt's voles (Lasiopodomys brandtii) to stored cat feces." Physiology &

598

Behavior 123:193-9.

RI PT

596

Hegab, Ibrahim M., Aiqin Wang, Baofa Yin, Shengmei Yang, and Wei Wanhong. 2014c.

600

"Behavioral and neuroendocrine response of Brandt's voles, Lasiopodomys brandtii, to

601

odors of different species." European Journal of Wildlife Research 60(2):331-40.

SC

599

Hegab, Ibrahim M., Shushu Kong, Shengmei Yang, Walaa I. Mohamaden, and Wanhong Wei.

603

2015. "The ethological relevance of predator odors to induce changes in prey species."

604

acta ethologica 18(1):1-9.

M AN U

602

Hegab, I. M., Y. Tan, C. Wang, B. Yao, H. Wang, W. Ji, and J. Su. 2018. "Examining object

606

recognition and object-in-Place memory in plateau zokors, Eospalax baileyi." Behav

607

Processes 146:34-41.

TE D

605

Ibáñez, Alejandro, Alfonso Marzal, Pilar López, and José Martín. 2015. "Reproductive state

609

affects hiding behaviour under risk of predation but not exploratory activity of female

610

Spanish terrapins." Behavioural Processes 111:90-96.

612 613

614 615

Jin, Shengtao, Yanan Zhao, Yinghong Jiang, Yanyu Wang, Changjiang Li, Deli Zhang, Bo Lian,

AC C

611

EP

608

Zhongde Du, Hongwei Sun, and Lin Sun. 2018. "Anxiety-like behaviour assessments of adolescent rats after repeated maternal separation during early life." Neuroreport 29(8).

Johnston, Amanda L., and Sandra E. File. 1991. "Sex differences in animal tests of anxiety." Physiology & Behavior 49(2):245-50.

28

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Jolles, Jolle Wolter, Neeltje J. Boogert, and Ruud van den Bos. 2015. "Sex differences in risk-

617

taking and associative learning in rats." Royal Society Open Science 2(11):150485.

618

Jonasson, Zachariah. 2005. "Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and

619

memory: a review of behavioral and biological data." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral

620

Reviews 28(8):811-25.

RI PT

616

Katwaroo-Andersen, Jemma, Chris K. Elvidge, Indar Ramnarine, and Grant E. Brown. 2016.

622

"Interactive effects of reproductive assets and ambient predation risk on the threat-

623

sensitive decisions of Trinidadian guppies." Current Zoology 62(3):221-26.

M AN U

SC

621

624

Kavaliers, Martin, and Elena Choleris. 2001. "Antipredator responses and defensive behavior:

625

ecological and ethological approaches for the neurosciences." Neuroscience &

626

Biobehavioral Reviews 25(7):577-86.

Kimchi, Tali, and Joseph Terkel. 2004. "Comparison of the role of somatosensory stimuli in

628

maze learning in a blind subterranean rodent and a sighted surface-dwelling rodent."

629

Behav Brain Res 153(2):389-95.

TE D

627

King, Andrew J., Ines Fürtbauer, Diamanto Mamuneas, Charlotte James, and Andrea Manica.

631

2013. "Sex-Differences and Temporal Consistency in Stickleback Fish Boldness." PLoS

633 634

AC C

632

EP

630

One 8(12):e81116.

Kokras, N., and C. Dalla. 2014. "Sex differences in animal models of psychiatric disorders." British Journal of Pharmacology 171(20):4595-619.

29

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Kondoh, Kunio, Zhonghua Lu, Xiaolan Ye, David P. Olson, Bradford B. Lowell, and Linda B.

636

Buck. 2016. "A specific area of olfactory cortex involved in stress hormone responses to

637

predator odors." Nature 532(7597):103-06.

RI PT

635

Mathot, Kimberley J., J. Wright, B. Kempenaers, and N. J. Dingemanse. 2012. "Adaptive

639

strategies for managing uncertainty may explain personality-related differences in

640

behavioural plasticity." Oikos 121(7):1009-20.

642

643 644

Meng, Ian D., and Robert C. Drugan. 1993. "Sex differences in open-field behavior in response to the β-carboline FG 7142 in rats." Physiology & Behavior 54(4):701-05.

M AN U

641

SC

638

Munoz, Nicole E., and Daniel T. Blumstein. 2012. "Multisensory perception in uncertain environments." Behavioral Ecology 23(3):457-62.

Navarro-Castilla, Álvaro, Isabel Barja, and Mario Díaz. 2018. "Foraging, feeding, and

646

physiological stress responses of wild wood mice to increased illumination and common

647

genet cues." Current Zoology 64(4):409-17.

TE D

645

Orsini, Caitlin A., Markie L. Willis, Ryan J. Gilbert, Jennifer L. Bizon, and Barry Setlow. 2016.

649

"Sex Differences in a Rat Model of Risky Decision Making." Behav Neurosci 130(1):50-

650

61.

AC C

EP

648

651

Parihar, V. K., B. Hattiangady, R. Kuruba, B. Shuai, and A. K. Shetty. 2009. "Predictable

652

chronic mild stress improves mood, hippocampal neurogenesis and memory." Molecular

653

Psychiatry 16:171.

654

Pérez-Gómez, Anabel, Katherin Bleymehl, Benjamin Stein, Martina Pyrski, Lutz Birnbaumer,

655

Steven D Munger, Trese Leinders-Zufall, Frank Zufall, and Pablo Chamero. 2015.

30

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

656

"Innate Predator Odor Aversion Driven by Parallel Olfactory Subsystems that Converge

657

in the Ventromedial Hypothalamus." Current Biology 25(10):1340-46.

660 661

making under uncertainty." Nat Neurosci 11(4):398-403.

RI PT

659

Platt, Michael L., and Scott A. Huettel. 2008. "Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision

Rex, A., U. Sondern, J. P. Voigt, S. Franck, and H. Fink. 1996. "Strain differences in fearmotivated behavior of rats." Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 54(1):107-11.

SC

658

Roelke, Daniel L., and Susan M. Sogard. 1993. "Gender-Based Differences in Habitat Selection

663

and Activity Level in the Northern Pipefish (Syngnathus fuscus)." Copeia 1993(2):528-

664

32.

M AN U

662

Samia, Diogo S. M., Anders Pape Møller, Daniel T. Blumstein, Theodore Stankowich, and

666

William E. Cooper. 2015. "Sex differences in lizard escape decisions vary with latitude,

667

but not sexual dimorphism." Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences

668

282(1805).

TE D

665

Schaller GB (1998) Wildlife of the Tibetan Steppe. Univ Chicago Press, Chicago

670

Shams, Imad, Aaron Avivi, and Eviatar Nevo. 2005. "Oxygen and carbon dioxide fluctuations in

671

burrows of subterranean blind mole rats indicate tolerance to hypoxic–hypercapnic

673

674 675

AC C

672

EP

669

stresses." Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 142(3):376-82.

Shao, Y., J. X. Li, R. L. Ge, L. Zhong, D. M. Irwin, R. W. Murphy, and Y. P. Zhang. 2015. "Genetic adaptations of the plateau zokor in high-elevation burrows." Sci Rep 5:17262.

31

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

676 677

Sievert, Thorbjörn, and Matthias Laska. 2016. "Behavioral Responses of CD-1 Mice to Six Predator Odor Components." Chemical Senses 41(5):399-406. Staples, Lauren G. 2010. "Predator odor avoidance as a rodent model of anxiety: Learning-

679

mediated consequences beyond the initial exposure." Neurobiol Learn Mem 94(4):435-45.

680

Stryjek, Rafał, Berenika Mioduszewska, Ewelina Spaltabaka-Gędek, and Grzegorz R. Juszczak.

681

2018. "Wild Norway Rats Do Not Avoid Predator Scents When Collecting Food in a

682

Familiar Habitat: A Field Study." Sci Rep 8(1):9475.

SC

RI PT

678

Su, Junhu, M. Hegab Ibrahim, Weihong Ji, and Zhibiao Nan. 2018. "Function-related Drivers of

684

Skull Morphometric Variation and Sexual Size Dimorphism in a Subterranean Rodent,

685

Plateau Zokor (Eospalax baileyi)." Ecol Evol 8(9):4631-43.

M AN U

683

Su, Junhu, Weihong Ji, Jing Wang, Dianne M. Gleeson, Janwei Zhou, Limin Hua, and Yanming

687

Wei. 2014. "Phylogenetic relationships of extant zokors (Myospalacinae) (Rodentia,

688

Spalacidae) inferred from mitochondrial DNA sequences." Mitochondrial DNA

689

25(2):135-41.

EP

691

Takahashi, Lorey K. 2014. "Olfactory systems and neural circuits that modulate predator odor fear." Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8:72.

AC C

690

TE D

686

692

Takahashi, Lorey K., Brandy R. Nakashima, Hyechong Hong, and Kendra Watanabe. 2005.

693

"The smell of danger: A behavioral and neural analysis of predator odor-induced fear."

694

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29(8):1157-67.

32

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Takahashi, Lorey K., Megan M. Chan, and Mark L. Pilar. 2008. "Predator odor fear conditioning:

696

Current perspectives and new directions." Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

697

32(7):1218-27.

RI PT

695

Tidhar, Wendy L., Frances Bonier, and John R. Speakman. 2007. "Sex- and concentration-

699

dependent effects of predator feces on seasonal regulation of body mass in the bank vole

700

Clethrionomys glareolus." Hormones and Behavior 52(4):436-44.

SC

698

van den Bos, Ruud, Judith Homberg, and Leonie de Visser. 2013. "A critical review of sex

702

differences in decision-making tasks: Focus on the Iowa Gambling Task." Behav Brain

703

Res 238:95-108.

704 705

M AN U

701

Vleck, David. 1979. "The Energy Cost of Burrowing by the Pocket Gopher Thomomys bottae." Physiological Zoology 52(2):122-36.

Ward, Ashley J. W., Philip Thomas, Paul J. B. Hart, and Jens Krause. 2004. "Correlates of

707

boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus)." Behavioral Ecology and

708

Sociobiology 55(6):561-68.

EP

710

Wiedenmayer, C.P. 2009. Plasticity of defensive behavior and fear in early development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 33: 432-441.

AC C

709

TE D

706

711

Yin, B., C. Gu, Y. Lu, I. M. Hegab, S. Yang, A. Wang, and W. Wei. 2017. "Repeated exposure

712

to cat urine induces complex behavioral, hormonal, and c-fos mRNA responses in

713

Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus)." Naturwissenschaften 104(7-8):64.

33

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Yin, Baofa, Hongmei Fan, Shiping Li, Ibrahim Hegab, Guangyu Lu, and Wanhong Wei. 2011.

715

"Behavioral response of Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) to odors of different

716

mammalian species." Journal of pest science 84(3):265.

RI PT

714

Zhang D., Zhou W., Wei W., Wang Q. 1995 Study on the reproduction and reproductive

718

behaviour of the plateau zokor. In: Cheng Y (ed) Studies on mammal biology in China.

719

Forestry Publishing House, Beijing, China, pp 174–179 [In Chinese with English

720

summary]

SC

717

Zhang, Yanming. 2007. "The Biology and Ecology of Plateau Zokors (Eospalax fontanierii)." Pp.

722

237-49 in Subterranean Rodents: News from Underground, edited by Sabine Begall,

723

Hynek Burda, and Cristian E. Schleich. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

M AN U

721

Zhou WY, Dou FM (1990) Studies on activity and home range of plateau zokor. Acta Theriol

725

Sin 10: 31–39.

728

729

730

731

EP

727

AC C

726

TE D

724

Table captions

732

Supplementary table (1) Means ± SE of different behavioral activities of male and female

733

Smith’s zokors (Eospalax smithii) during the first and second 10 minutes of the testing session

734

under novel, predator and conditioned contexts. 34

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

735

736

RI PT

737

738

SC

739

740

M AN U

741

742

743

747

748

749

EP

746

AC C

745

TE D

744

Figure captions

750

Figure (1) The proportion of time spent in the hide box (A), proportion of time spent in head out

751

(B), proportion of time spent out of cover (C), the mean duration of locomotion (D), mean

752

duration of contact to the stimulus (E) and the latency till first leave the hide box (F) during

35

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

exposure of male (n=22; triangles, dashed line) and female (n=21; circle, solid line) Smith’s

754

zokors to the novel, predator and conditioned contexts. * Represents gender differences within

755

context at p ≤ 0.05. Means with different superscripts for each gender are statistically significant

756

between contexts at p ≤ 0.05.

RI PT

753

757

SC

758

M AN U

759

760

761

765

766

767

768

EP

764

AC C

763

TE D

762

769

36

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

EP

772

Figure (1)

AC C

771

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

770

37

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Highlights • Male and female Smith’s zokors showed different risk-taking responses under different contexts.

RI PT

• Males trended to explore the contexts and collect information more than females.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

• Males are more risk-prone while females are risk-aversive.