Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study

Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study

Accepted Manuscript Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study Y.K. Eugenia Kwok, Marina A.G. von Keyserl...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 44 Views

Accepted Manuscript Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study Y.K. Eugenia Kwok, Marina A.G. von Keyserlingk, Gisele Sprea, Carla Forte Maiolino Molento PII:

S1558-7878(16)30024-7

DOI:

10.1016/j.jveb.2016.03.006

Reference:

JVEB 958

To appear in:

Journal of Veterinary Behavior

Received Date: 8 October 2015 Revised Date:

19 March 2016

Accepted Date: 25 March 2016

Please cite this article as: Kwok, Y.K.E., von Keyserlingk, M.A.G., Sprea, G., Molento, C.F.M., Humananimal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study, Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.03.006. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1 2

RI PT

3 4 5

SC

6

Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil:

8

A descriptive study

M AN U

7

9

Y.K. Eugenia Kwok1, Marina A.G. von Keyserlingk*1, Gisele Sprea2 and Carla Forte Maiolino

11

Molento3

14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26

EP

13

1Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columba, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver BC V6T 1Z6

AC C

12

TE D

10

2 Vigilância em Saúde Ambiental, Prefeitura Municipal de Campo Largo Avenida Padre Natal Pigatto, 925 - Campo Largo - Paraná CEP: 83601-630

3 Laboratório de Bem-estar Animal, Federal University of Paraná, R. dos Funcionários, 1540 CEP 80035-050, Curitiba, Brazil *Corresponding author e-mail:

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2 27 28 29 30

Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: A descriptive study

31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38

2 Vigilância em Saúde Ambiental, Prefeitura Municipal de Campo Largo Avenida Padre Natal Pigatto, 925 Campo Largo - Paraná | CEP: 83601-630 CNPJ: 76.105.618/0001-88 Tel: (41) 3291-5000

39 40

Phone: 1 604 822 4898

Y.K. Eugenia Kwok1, Marina A.G. von Keyserlingk*1, Gisele Sprea2 and Carla Forte Maiolino Molento2 1Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columba, 2357 Main Mall,

RI PT

Vancouver BC V6T 1Z6

CEP 80035-050, Curitiba, Brazil

M AN U

*Corresponding author: [email protected]

SC

3 Laboratório de Bem-estar Animal, Federal University of Paraná, R. dos Funcionários, 1540

41

Free-roaming, ownerless dogs comprise a considerable portion of Brazil’s dog

43

population. To address societal concerns for animal welfare, the Brazilian town of Campo Largo

44

established the ‘community dog program’, where free-roaming dogs are cared for by self-

45

appointed community members, known as maintainers. As this program was established only

46

two years ago, little is known about the interactions that take place between these dogs and

47

people residing in these communities. Thus, the objective of this study was to describe the types

48

of human-animal interactions observed between community dogs and humans in Campo Largo.

49

Dog subjects (n=7), selected by the municipality based on accessibility and community approval,

50

were of mixed breeds, and averaged 4.0±4.16 (mean±SD) years old, ranging from 1 to 10 years

51

old. Over an 18-day period, each dog was observed through continuous focal sampling for six

52

consecutive hours on three separate days, with the exception of two dogs, Pitoco and

53

Moranguinha, who were observed for one and two days, respectively. Interactions were

54

presented as medians and total counts, and grouped as dog-initiated or human-initiated. Human-

55

initiated interactions were further distinguished as either stranger-initiated and community

AC C

EP

TE D

42

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3 member-initiated. Of the 465 total dog-human interactions, 298 were initiated by dogs and 167

57

by humans. Dogs interacted with vehicles a total of 157 times. Relative frequency of dog-

58

initiated interactions towards vehicles was much lower than those directed at humans. While

59

dogs approached humans a median of 9 times per 6 h observation period they approached

60

vehicles 0 times per observation day. Vehicle-chasing was observed a median of 2 times per 6 h

61

period. Avoiding and barking at humans was observed, directed most often towards strangers

62

who had no known previous contact with the dogs. While humans petted, hugged and kissed

63

dogs, they were also seen to kick, scold and attempt to scare them. Both community members

64

and strangers showed affection towards dogs. Kicking was observed a total of 4 times, only

65

performed by strangers. However, strangers were also observed to feed dogs a median of once

66

per observation period. This descriptive study is the first documentation on the types of

67

interactions between community dogs and humans in Campo Largo.

68 69 70 71

Keywords: Community dog, free-roaming, human-animal interactions, animal ethology, animal welfare, Brazil.

74 75 76 77 78 79 80

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

73

AC C

72

RI PT

56

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4 81 82 83

Introduction Free-roaming dogs in Brazil Brazil is home to approximately 37 million of the estimated 500 million domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in the world – a considerable portion of which are free-roaming (Dantas-

85

Torres and Otranto, 2014; Hsu et al., 2003). The term ‘free-roaming’ is used to define domestic

86

dogs that are generally unattended or are not under direct human control (Høgasen et al., 2013;

87

Majumder et al., 2014). These animals can thrive as either solitary individuals or members of

88

large social groups (Sparkes et al., 2014). However, the lives of these dogs are typically believed

89

to be brief and harsh due to their limited access to food, water and shelter on the streets (Amaku

90

et al., 2010). Lack of human ownership also suggests that veterinary care is either insufficient or

91

absent, thereby increasing their susceptibility to disease and malnutrition, compromising welfare

92

and reducing chances of survival (Hsu et al., 2003; Morters et al., 2014).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

84

94 95

TE D

93

The community dog program in Southern Brazil

Although region-specific, Brazilian free-roaming dogs are typically found near impoverished urban areas, rural parks and forested neighborhoods (Dias et al., 2013). One way

97

to address the direct welfare needs of free-roaming dogs is to allow these animals to remain on

98

the streets while tending to their basic needs through establishing them as community residents.

AC C

99

EP

96

In the Southern Brazilian town of Campo Largo, the community dog program has been in

100

effect since 2012. Community dogs differ from free-roaming dogs in that they are collectively

101

cared for by community members and have been sterilized, vaccinated and identified by the local

102

municipal veterinarian (Høgasen et al., 2013). ‘Maintainers’, or self-appointed members of the

103

community, are registered with the municipal government to be legally responsible for

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 5 providing basic necessities such as clean water, food, and shelter to their respective community

105

dogs. Maintainers are also responsible for monitoring the health status of the community dog and

106

contacting the municipal veterinarian if the animal is injured or sick. Additionally, community

107

dogs must not pose significant risk to humans and animals in their neighborhood. This

108

alternative may have a positive impact on public health through addressing animal health issues

109

in the region (Molento, 2014). Since its inception, the Campo Largo community program has

110

registered more than 80 dogs in the community and is expected to increase its capacity in the

111

coming years.

SC

RI PT

104

113 114

M AN U

112 Study objectives

While studies have been conducted on the spatial distribution of free-roaming dogs, research on their behaviors and relationships with their communities has been scant. Likewise,

116

the concept of community dogs is relatively new to the animal population management field and

117

has received little attention. Hence, the aim of this study was to describe the types of human-

118

animal interactions observed between community dogs and humans in Campo Largo, Brazil.

TE D

115

121

Materials and methods

This study was approved by the UBC Animal Care Committee (Protocol # A14-0207),

AC C

120

EP

119

122

and by a joint cooperation term between the Federal University of Paraná and the Campo Largo

123

municipal government.

124 125 126

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 6 127

Study Site This project took place between June and August 2014 in the town of Campo Largo in

129

the state of Paraná, Southern Brazil (latitude: 25°27'33.1"S longitude: 49°31'21.3"W). Campo

130

Largo’s human population of approximately 100,000 residents is distributed across a vast

131

geographic area of over 1 million square kilometers (IBGE, 2008). The exact size of Campo

132

Largo’s dog population remains unknown, although previous studies have estimated that

133

approximately 25,000 dogs reside in the area (Molento, 2014).

SC

134

136

Subjects

M AN U

135

RI PT

128

Data were collected from seven community dogs (Figure 1) that were scattered across the town’s district and lived in various locations within the town’s residential, farm and commercial

138

areas. This was a convenience sample as the dogs were selected by the Campo Largo municipal

139

government officials based on the accessibility to community study sites, and permission given

140

by the local community members to observe the animals at the individual study sites. Details on

141

the age, size and sex of each subject were made available through dog profiles provided by the

142

municipal government (Table 1). All subjects were medium to large in body size and averaged

143

4.0±4.16 years old, ranging from 1 to 10 years old. Information regarding living conditions,

144

access to food, water, and shelter was also provided by the municipal government. Of the seven

145

subjects, three were males and four were females; all were recorded as mixed breed. Subjects

146

were spayed or neutered by the local municipal veterinarian prior to the study, as part of the

147

standard registration procedure of community dogs. The maintainer of each dog was also

148

identified through records provided by the government. Four of the dogs had a single maintainer

149

while three (Tigrão, Negão and Juli), had two maintainers.

AC C

EP

TE D

137

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 7 150 151

Housing conditions All subjects remained on the street prior to and during the observation period with the exception of two dogs: Pitoco, who disappeared after only one observation day, and

153

Moranguinha, who was kept inside her maintainer’s house on the final day of observation. Four

154

subjects lived in residential neighborhoods while three lived in areas that were a mix of

155

residential, farm and commercial settings (Table 1). Dogs were provided water ad libitum in

156

bowls, and provided food periodically throughout each observation day by their maintainers with

157

no intervention by the study investigators.

159

M AN U

158

SC

RI PT

152

Behavioral observations

We undertook an initial pilot study between June 9th and June 13th 2014. We watched

161

three subjects (Negão, Pitoco and Pretinha) to create a general behavioral ethogram. Behaviors

162

were categorized according to dog-initiated and human-initiated interactions (Table 2).

TE D

160

Observations took place over 18 days between June 16th and August 12th, 2014, where

164

each dog was observed on three separate days with the exceptions of Pitoco and Moranguinha.

165

Each dog was observed for 6 hours per day, which were divided into two 3-hour bouts (from

166

10:00-13:00 and 13:30-16:30). We arrived at least 10 minutes prior to the start of every

167

observation day to allow people and community dogs to become acclimatized to our presence.

168

Each subject was randomly assigned a Monday, a Tuesday, and a Friday due to limited access to

169

study sites, except for Negão, who was observed on two Tuesdays and a Monday due to a bus

170

strike which prevented our access to the study site. Contact with the subjects was minimized by

171

remaining at least 7 meters from animals (the approximate length of a street intersection),

172

avoiding eye contact with dogs and refraining from any form of interaction. During observations,

AC C

EP

163

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 8 173

conversation and interaction with local people were kept to a minimum. Live, continuous focal

174

sampling was used to record all interactions during each observation period.

175

177

Behavioral grouping

RI PT

176

Interactions were grouped into two major categories: dog-human interactions and dogvehicle interactions (Table 2). Dog-human interactions were further differentiated into

179

interactions with strangers (humans who did not live in the community) and community

180

members. We were introduced to regular community members during our initial visits to study

181

sites before the start of the study. Strangers were considered people that were not known to

182

researchers or community members (as previously identified through personal communication

183

with residents of the community). As all observed interactions were considered behavioral

184

events, a separate interaction was recorded every time the animal performed the behavior

185

regardless of the duration of time it took to perform the behavior.

186

188

Data analysis

Given that this study was descriptive in nature, all results were presented as medians per

EP

187

TE D

M AN U

SC

178

6 hour observation day and total counts across all observation days. All medians, with the

190

exceptions of the data collected from Pitoco and Moranguinha, represented 3 days with 6 hours

191

of observations taken each day.

192 193 194

AC C

189

Results

Over the 18 days of observation across all seven dogs, a total of 465 dog-human

195

interactions and 157 dog-vehicle interactions were observed. Of the dog-human interactions, 298

196

were initiated by dogs and 167 by humans. Of the dog-vehicle interactions recorded, 104

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 9 197

interactions involved cars and the rest involved bicycles, bulldozers, busses, motorcycles and

198

trucks.

200

Dog-initiated interactions: humans versus vehicles

RI PT

199

The relative frequency of dog-initiated interactions were different (X2 = 216.1, p = 2.2 x

202

10-16) between humans and vehicles. While dogs approached humans a median of 9 times per 6

203

hour observation day with an interquartile range (IQR) between 4.5 and 14.5, they approached

204

vehicles 0 times per observation period (IQR = 0-1.5) (Figure 2). Dogs followed humans

205

frequently during the observation days (Figure 2a); however, this varied considerably across

206

individuals. Pretinha followed humans the most compared to all other dogs – a total of 42 times

207

across all observation days – while Pitoco did not perform this behavior at all (Figure 3a). In

208

contrast, dogs almost never followed vehicles; this was only observed once when Mindinga

209

followed her maintainer’s truck (Figure 3b). Dogs avoided humans a median of 2 times per

210

observation day (IQR = 1.5-3.5) and vehicles a median of 2 times per observation day (IQR =

211

2.0-4.0) (Figure 2). However, Pretinha avoided vehicles the most compared to all other subjects;

212

avoiding them a total of 73 times across all observation days (Figure 3b). This particular dog also

213

avoided humans the most compared to all other subjects (Figure 3a). Approaching, following,

214

and barking at humans were variable across dogs, while other behaviors such as playing, chasing

215

and olfaction were less variable.

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

216

SC

201

Dogs were observed to chase vehicles more than they chased humans. Human-chasing

217

was only performed by Juli, Pretinha and Tigrão on occasions where strangers walked through

218

the street (Figure 3a). Vehicle-chasing was observed a median of 2 times (IQR = 1.5-3.0) per

219

observation day (Figure 2b). However, this was driven almost exclusively by one dog, Juli, who

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 10 engaged in car chasing a total of 28 times over the course of the three observation days (Figure

221

3b). Dogs barked at humans a median of 3 times per observation day (IQR = 2.0-8.5) (Figure

222

2a), although the majority of barking occurrences was performed by Tigrão, who barked 17

223

times across all observation days (Figure 3a). Play with humans by dogs was initiated a median

224

of 4 times per day (IQR = 2.5-5.5) (Figure 2a). However, this behavior varied between dogs, as

225

Mindinga performed this 10 times across the observation days, while Pitoco was never observed

226

to play with humans (Figure 3a). Dogs were not observed to play with vehicles during the entire

227

observation study.

228 229 230

Dogs with fewer observation days

M AN U

SC

RI PT

220

Pitoco interacted with humans and vehicles the least compared to all other dogs. During our observations, Pitoco only approached a human once. The only other interactions he

232

performed were avoiding and barking at strangers and neighbors, and approaching his maintainer

233

once. However, it must be noted that one day of observation may not have reflected the true

234

daily interactions between Pitoco and people in his community. Likewise, as Moranguinha was

235

kept inside her maintainer’s house for the third day of observation, it was not possible to observe

236

the interactions that occurred. It was likely that the maintainer interacted with Moranguinha

237

during this period.

239 240

EP

AC C

238

TE D

231

Human-initiated interactions: strangers versus community members The number of occurrences of human-initiated interactions with dogs were similar (X2 =

241

10.4, p = 0.2) between strangers and community members. Community members and strangers

242

both showed affection towards dogs (Figure 4). However, community members were observed to

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11 call dogs more often than strangers, with a median of 3 times (IQR = 1.0-4.0) per observation

244

day, usually during daily feeds and when dogs barked or chased other humans and vehicles

245

(Figure 4b). While community members were observed to feed portions of beans, rice and meat

246

to dogs, strangers were seen to offer a variety of candies and dog treats to dogs, and fed them a

247

median of once per day (IQR = 0-5.0) (Figure 4a). In contrast, kicking was observed four times

248

throughout the entire study, and was only performed by strangers (Figure 4a). In general, both

249

groups rarely attempted to scare dogs; however, community members attempted to scare

250

Pretinha twice, while strangers were observed to attempt to scare Juli, Pretinha and Tigrão

251

(Figure 5). Both community members and strangers scolded dogs (Figure 4), although this was

252

typically observed when dogs barked or chased humans and vehicles.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

243

253 254

256

Subject variability

TE D

255

Discussion

In this study, interactions with both dogs and humans varied considerably across dogs. While some dogs frequently approached, played with and followed humans, others seldom did.

258

This may in part be due to the different individual experience with humans and diversity in

259

human exposure they received throughout their lives. In many species, experiences during the

260

infant and adolescent period are critical in shaping behavioral development and temperament

261

(Appleby et al., 2002; Foyer et al., 2014). Restricted or lack of socialization during this sensitive

262

growth period may result in abnormal behavioral development and reduced social behaviors

263

towards humans as the animals age (Appleby et al., 2002). Subsequently, the type of previous

264

human experience that each subject may have been exposed to may have influenced their level of

265

association with humans. Another possible factor that influenced the interactions observed in this

AC C

EP

257

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 12 266

study could be differences in personalities among dogs, which Ley et al. (2008) define as “the

267

pattern of characteristic thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that distinguish one individual from

268

another and that persist over time and situations”. Pitoco was rarely observed to associate with humans. He did not follow or play with

RI PT

269

them, and appeared to make every effort to avoid humans throughout the observation day.

271

Information provided by the community revealed that this dog had been previously stabbed and

272

shot at by humans. These negative experiences may have made Pitoco less trusting of humans,

273

and affected his willingness to interact with them. In contrast, our subject Mindinga approached,

274

followed, and played with humans more frequently during observation periods than most of the

275

other dogs, and seemed to reflect a positive relationship with humans. As Mindinga lived near a

276

snack bar with a constant influx of people, she was frequently exposed to humans and may have

277

become habituated to the presence of strangers in her community. Community members also

278

seemed to have more knowledge about Mindinga, and described to us the types of foods, places

279

and people that she preferred. This positive attitude towards Mindinga may explain the high level

280

of affection she received, as she was petted, hugged and kissed far more than any of the other

281

subjects. Moreover, Cooper et al. (2003) explain that strong social bonds between dogs and

282

humans can enhance behavioral development in canines and increase their motivation to interact

283

with people. Arguably, Mindinga’s positive relationship with humans in her community may

284

have contributed to her willingness to interact with humans in general. Based on these two

285

examples, past experience with humans seems to play a large factor in affecting the behavior of

286

these dogs and their association with their communities.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

270

287 288

Differences in human association

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 13 289

The types and degree of human association with dog subjects varied considerably between both dogs and human groups. Interactions between community members and

291

community dogs likely reflected the responsibilities of maintainers as the primary caretakers of

292

these animals. However, community members also showed a certain level of affection towards

293

dogs, suggesting that they tolerated and perhaps enjoyed the presence of these community dogs.

294

Nonetheless, interactions between humans and dogs were typically brief, and neither party

295

seemed to be highly attached to the other. This lack of what appears to be a solid attachment

296

seemed to distinguish the maintainer-community dog relationship from typical owner-dog

297

relationships, as owned dogs tend to display a higher level of bondedness to their owner. This

298

bondedness is characterized by maintaining proximity with certain individuals combined with

299

higher levels of distress when this bond is severed – neither of which were observed in

300

community dogs (Prato-Previde et al., 2003).

SC

M AN U

Only strangers were observed to physically kick the dogs. This may be partly due to

TE D

301

RI PT

290

differences in the dogs’ behavior towards strangers compared to humans they were familiar with

303

(Kerepesi et al., 2015). In a study by Győri et al. (2010) on the behavior of dogs in playful and

304

agonistic situations with owners and strangers, dogs displayed significantly more avoidance and

305

aggression towards strangers when placed in a threatening situation. However, dogs that were

306

placed in the same situation with their owners showed tolerance towards their owners (Győri et

307

al., 2010). This suggested that human-dog interactions depend considerably on the dog’s

308

familiarity with the human. It followed that community dogs in our study were likely more

309

familiar with and trusting of community members that lived in the area compared to strangers

310

passing through the community. Nonetheless, strangers were found to feed dogs more often than

AC C

EP

302

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 14 311

community members, which suggested that some people were more accepting of community

312

dogs than others.

313

315

Dogs and vehicles

RI PT

314

Dog-vehicle interactions in the communities were prevalent. As the study sites were composed of residential, farm and business areas, the presence of cars, busses and trucks were

317

common. Cars were most prevalent in all three areas, and thus it was not surprising that dogs

318

interacted with these vehicles the most. Pretinha avoided vehicles the most, which may have

319

been a consequence of her having been hit by a car just prior to our first day of observation.

320

In contrast to Pretinha, Juli chased vehicles excessively. In fact, the total number of

321

vehicle-chasing events by this dog was greater than the sum of all chasing events observed

322

across all other subjects. Unlike Pretinha’s avoidance of vehicles, Juli actively pursued cars and

323

motorcycles as they passed through the neighborhood. While this behavior was frowned upon

324

by community members and drivers, who scolded Juli for doing so, it may have been associated

325

with her natural prey drive and motivation to chase fast-moving objects (ASPCA, 2015). We

326

noticed that Juli pursued vehicles to the end of her street and would quickly retreat to her

327

sleeping area after the chase. This suggested that she was likely not curious or aggressive toward

328

the vehicles, but simply expressing her motivation to chase or protect her territory. As chasing

329

behavior varied considerably across dogs, the community dog program should consider

330

screening community dog candidates for their motivation to chase, to ensure the safety of both

331

dogs and vehicles when free-roaming dogs are registered to the program.

332 333

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

316

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 15 334

Study limitations Results obtained from the study were limited to a convenience sample of 7 dogs

336

repeatedly observed over several days and thus cannot be considered a representative sample of

337

all community dog interactions in Campo Largo. Thus, we would recommend further studies be

338

done with larger sample sizes in order to validate the human-animal interactions with community

339

dogs in these communities. Another limitation is the exclusion of dog-dog interactions from this

340

study. Although we anecdotally noted numerous dog-dog interactions throughout our study we

341

encourage others to investigate this important aspect of understanding how community dogs

342

interact with their community, including humans and other dogs. Additionally, the observation

343

that only strangers kicked dogs could be due to community members being aware of the presence

344

of researchers in the study area and refraining from kicking dogs, while strangers were unaware

345

of this study. Attempting to understand dog-dog interactions will be challenging given that we

346

predict it will be difficult to differentiate between free-roaming owned dogs (dogs owned by

347

community members that are allowed to roam free) and true stray, ownerless dogs that are not

348

part of the community dog program.

351

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

350

Conclusion

Through this study, it was possible to briefly describe and compare the interactions

AC C

349

RI PT

335

352

between community dogs, humans and vehicles in Campo Largo, Brazil. While some

353

interactions seemed to suggest a positive relationship between certain individuals in the

354

community, others brought about concerns for animal welfare. Nonetheless, preliminary

355

investigation of the community dog program in Campo Largo suggested that this program is a

356

promising step towards improving the welfare of street dogs, and seemed to encourage a form of

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 16 task delegation between the Campo Largo community and the local municipal government. In

358

the future, we would recommend further studies to further explore the impacts of the program on

359

the welfare of community dogs, and the possibility of implementing this program in other

360

regions facing issues with free-roaming dogs. While dog overpopulation and abandonment will

361

likely continue, attention must be brought to the animals that are currently living on the streets.

362

In time, better systems for managing dog overpopulation will hopefully be established to more

363

adequately address the issue of free-roaming dogs in this region of Brazil, as well as other areas

364

around the world, that also encompass animal welfare. Appropriately, the community dog

365

program of Campo Largo may offer one possible method in addressing the needs of these

366

animals in the meantime.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

357

367 368

Acknowledgments

Funding for this project was provided by the Mitacs Globalink Research Award

370

(Vancouver, BC, Canada). We are extremely grateful to all the staff and students at the Animal

371

Welfare Laboratory of Universidade Federal do Paraná for their help in planning this project,

372

collecting field data, and volunteering translators. Many thanks also to the members of the UBC

373

Animal Welfare Program, particularly Dr. David Fraser, Dr. Becca Franks and Nancy Clarke,

374

and the staff at Prefeitura Municipal de Campo Largo. Finally, a sincere thank you to the Campo

375

Largo community members for allowing this work to be done. The original idea for this study

376

was conceived by Dr. Gisele Sprea and then refined by Y.K Eugenia Kwok (YKEK), Drs Marina

377

von Keyserlingk (MvK) and Carla Molento (CM). Data collection was done by YKEK. This

378

paper was completed initially as an undergraduate thesis written by YKEK under the supervision

AC C

EP

TE D

369

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 17 379

of MvK and then adapted for publication. YKEK, MvK and CM contributed to the data analyses,

380

interpretation and manuscript writing.

RI PT

381 382 383

SC

384 385

M AN U

386 387 388 389

393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400

EP

392

AC C

391

TE D

390

References

Amaku, M. Dias, R.A. and Ferreira, F. 2010. Dynamics and control of stray dog populations. Math. Popul. Stud. 17, 69-78.

Appleby, D.L., Bradshaw, J.W.s. and Casey, R.A. 2002. Relationship between aggressive and

401

avoidance behavior by dogs and their experience in the first six months of life. Vet.

402

Record. 150, 434-438.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 18 403 404

ASPCA. 2015. Dogs chasing cars. American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals.

405

Available at: http://www.aspca.org/pet-care/virtual-pet-behaviorist/dog-behavior/dogs-

406

chasing-cars. Accessed March 2015.

RI PT

407 408

Cooper, J.J., Ashton, C., Bishop, S., West, R., Mills, D.S. and Young, R.J. 2003. Clever hounds:

409

social cognition in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 81, 229-

410

244.

412

Dantas-Torres, F. and Otranto, D. 2014. Dogs, cats, parasites and humans in Brazil: opening the

SC

411

black box. Parasit. Vectors. 7, 22.

414

M AN U

413

Dias, R.A., Guilloux, A.G.A., Borba, M.R., Guarnieri, M.C.L., Ferreira, F., Amaku, M., Neto,

415

J.S.F. and Stevenson, M. 2013. Size and spatial distribution of stray dog population in the

416

University of Sao Paulo campus, Brazil. Prev. Vet. Med. 110, 263-273.

417 418

Foyer, P., Bjällerhag, N., Wilsson, E., Jensen, P., Militärtekniska avdelningen (MTA), Militärvetenskapliga, institutionen (MVI) and Försvarshögskolan. 2014. Behavior and

420

experiences of dogs during the first year of life predict the outcome in a later

421

temperament test. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 155, 93-100.

424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432

Győri, B., Gácsi, M. and Miklóski, Á. 2010. Friend or foe: Context dependent sensitivity to

EP

423

human behavior. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 128, 69-77.

AC C

422

TE D

419

Høgasen, H.R., Di Nardo, C.E.A. and Dalla Villa, P. 2013. Free-roaming dog populations: a cost-benefit model for different management options, applied to Abruzzo, Italy. Prev. Vet. Med. 112, 401-413.

Hsu, Y., Severinghaus, L.L. and Serpell, J.A. 2003. Dog keeping in Taiwan: its contribution to the problem of free-roaming dogs. J. Appl. Anim. Welfare. Sci. 6, 1-23.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 19 433

IBGE. 2008. Estimativas da população para 1º de julho de 2008 (Population estimates from 1

434

July 2008). Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Available at:

435

http://www.ibge.gov.br/home/estatistica/populacao/estimativa2008/POP2008_DOU.pdf.

436

Accessed March 2015.

439

Kerepesi, A., Dóka, A. and Milóski, Á. 2015. Dogs and their human companions: The effect of familiarity on dog-human interactions. Behav. Process. 110, 27-36.

440 441 442

Ley, J., Bennett, P. and Coleman, G. 2008. Personality dimensions that emerge in companion canines. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 110, 305-317.

444

M AN U

443

SC

438

RI PT

437

Majumder, S.S., Bhadra, A., Ghosh, A., Mitra, S., Bhattacharjee, D., Chatterjee, J., Nandi, A.K.

445

and Bhadra, A. 2014. To be or not to be social: foraging associations of free-ranging dogs

446

in an urban ecosystem. Acta. Ethol. 17, 1-8.

447 448

Molento, C. F. M. 2014. Public Health and Animal Welfare. In: Appleby, M.C., Weary D.M., and Sandøe, P. (Eds), Dilemmas in Animal Welfare. CABI International, Boston, pp.

450

102-123.

451 452

TE D

449

Morters, M.K., Bharadwaj, S., Whay, H.R., Cleaveland, S., Damriyasa, I.M.D., and Wood, J.L.N. 2014. Participatory methods for the assessment of the ownership status of free-

454

roaming dogs in Bali, Indonesia, for disease control and animal welfare. Prev. Vet. Med.

455

116, 1-2.

457 458 459

AC C

456

EP

453

Prato-Previde, E., Custance, D.M., Spiezio, C. and Sabatini, F. 2003. Is the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth’s strange situation. Behav. 140, 225-254.

460 461

Sparkes, J., Körtner, G., Ballard, G., Fleming, P.J.S. and Brown, W.Y. 2014. Effects of sex and

462

reproductive state on interactions between free-roaming domestic dogs. Plos. One. 9,

463

e116053.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 1 Sex, age, body size and location type of seven community dog subjects in Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil, June to August, 2014. Body Size1 Large Medium Medium Medium Medium Large Medium

RI PT

Age 10 2 3 1 1 10 1

SC

Sex Male Male Female Female Female Male Female

M AN U

Name Tigrão Negão Juli Pretinha Mindinga Pitoco Moranguinha

Location type2 Residence Store Residence Store Store Residence Residence

1 Estimated body sizes categorized as small, medium or large as listed by municipal government records.

AC C

EP

TE D

2 Residence is defined as spaces that serve as living areas for community members. Stores are considered infrastructures where people run their business establishments.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1 Table 2 Behavioral ethogram of a) dog-initiated interactions to humans and vehicles and b) human-initiated interactions to dogs in Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil.

RI PT

a) Dog-initiated interactions Description

Approach

With the head point towards the human/vehicle, using all four limbs to move towards the human in an uninterrupted manner by running or walking. The head may be slightly lowered.

Avoid

With the body turned, moving away from the human/vehicle by walking or running on all four limbs towards an area where no other stimulus is present.

Chase

Using all four limbs to rapidly move behind or beside the human/vehicle with the eyes and head fixated on the target.

Follow

With all four limbs, moving in the same direction behind a human/vehicle and continuing along the same route.

Olfaction

With the head pointed towards the object, extending the nose and drawing air in when moving the nostrils over the surface of the human/vehicle.

Play

With the head and body oriented towards the human, wagging the tail in a back and forth motion while jumping, licking, sniffing and leaning on the human. The individual may bend its upper thoracic body towards the ground and raise the rump above the head while wagging the tail in front of the human. Vocalization may occur with rapid and directionless running.

Bark

Using the throat to create a sharp noise towards a visual or auditory stimulus. This may be associated with the flicking of the tail from side to side.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

Interaction

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

2 b) Human-initiated interactions Description

Call

Saying the community dog’s name towards the dog in a normal speaking tone.

Feed

Placing bowls of food (meat, rice, beans etc.) onto the side of the street or pouring food onto the corner of the sidewalk. The person may also place food in his or her hand and extend it out towards the dog.

Kick

With force, hitting the foot quickly towards the dog’s head, body or limbs.

Scold

Speaking to the dog in a loud and harsh tone.

Show affection (hug, pet, kiss)

Using the arms to embrace the dog’s head, limbs and body while using the hands to stroke the dog’s fur. Simultaneously, the person may use his or her lips to briefly touch the dog’s head, limbs or body.

Scare

With intention to surprise the dog, making sudden movements in front of or rapidly approaching the dog.

Talk

With a soft tone, speaking to the dog as if in conversation.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

Interaction

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

B)

C)

D)

M AN U

SC

RI PT

A)

F)

AC C

EP

G)

TE D

E)

Figure 1. Community dog subjects (n=7) observed in Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil, from June to August, 2014: A) Juli, B) Mindinga, C), Moranguinha, D) Pretinha, E) Negão, F) Tigrão and G) Pitoco.

a) 80

RI PT

60

40

20

0 follow olfaction

b) 80

60

bark

TE D

40

20

0

play

M AN U

chase

EP

Dog-initiated interactions to vehicles (median # of interactions per 6h observation period)

approach avoid

SC

Dog-initiated interactions to humans (median # of interactions per 6h observation period)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

avoid

AC C

approach

chase

follow

olfaction

bark

Figure 2. Comparison between a) median number of dog-initiated interactions (n=7) to humans and b) median number of dog-initiated interactions (n=7) to vehicles, per 6 hour observation period over 18 days in regions of Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Figure 3. Total number of a) dog-initiated human interactions and b) dog-initiated vehicle interactions, observed of each dog (n=7; Ju = Juli, Mi = Mindinga, Mo = Moranguinha, Ne = Negão, Pi = Pitoco, Pr = Pretinha, Ti = Tigrão) across 18 observation days in Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil.

a) 25

RI PT

20 15

SC

10 5 0 call

feed

kick

b) 25

play

scold

scare

talk

scold

scare

talk

TE D

20 15

EP

10

5 0

AC C

Community member-initiated interactions to dogs (median # of interactions per 6h observation period)

affection

M AN U

Stranger-initiated interactions to dogs (median # of interactions per 6h observation period)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

affection

call

feed

kick

play

Figure 4. Comparison between a) median number of stranger-initiated interactions to dogs (n=7) and b) median number of community member-initiated interactions to dogs(n=7), per 6 hour observation period over 18 days in regions of Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Figure 5. Total number of a) stranger-initiated dog interactions and b)community memberinitiated dog interactions, observed of each dog (n=7; Ju = Juli, Mi = Mindinga, Mo = Moranguinha, Ne = Negão, Pi = Pitoco, Pr = Pretinha, Ti = Tigrão) across 18 observation days in Campo Largo, Paraná, Brazil.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Human-animal interactions of community dogs in Campo Largo, Brazil: a descriptive study Y.K. Eugenia Kwok1, Marina A.G. von Keyserlingk*1, Gisele Sprea2 and Carla Forte Maiolino Molento3

RI PT

*Corresponding author: [email protected]

Highlights:

We introduce and describe the community dog program of Campo Largo, Brazil



The relative frequency of dog-initiated interactions towards vehicles was much lower than those directed towards humans



Community members and strangers interacted similarly and mostly in a positive manner with community dogs



Only strangers kicked dogs



Behaviors varied across dogs, particularly regarding chasing and avoiding vehicles

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC