Brain Research, 150 (1978) 85-101 © Elsevier/North-HollandBiomedicalPress
85
IMPAIRED SYNAPTIC POTENTIATION PROCESSES IN THE HIPPOCAMPUS OF AGED, MEMORY-DEFICIENT RATS
PHILIP W. LANDFIELD,JAMES L. MCGAUGHand GARY LYNCH Department of Psychobiology, University of California, Irvine, Carl]: 92717 (U.S.A.)
(Accepted November 10th, 1977)
SUMMARY A series of neurophysiological experiments was performed on the Schaffercommissural system of the hippocampus of aged and young anesthetized Fischer rats. The aged Fischer rats were previously found to exhibit retention performance deficits. No obvious differences were found between aged and young animals in amplitude, latency, stimulation threshold, or wave forms of typical synaptic responses when these were elicited by control (0.3 Hz) stimulation pulses. Further, the temporal curves of facilitation during a paired-pulse series were not different in aged and young animals. However, aged and young synapses showed consistently different responses during repetitive stimulation. Synapses of aged animals were deficient in frequency potentiation processes during 12 Hz stimulation; and the aged animals exhibited a delayed rise of post-tetanic synaptic potentiation following a 5 sec, 100 Hz stimulation train. Moreover, aged synapses 'exhausted' more rapidly during continuous 4 Hz stimulation. Throughout these studies a biphasic pattern of potentiation was observed during repetitive stimulation (brief potentiation, depression, renewed potentiation). Aged animals were deficient primarily in development of the second phase of potentiation. This pattern suggests an age-related impairment of some secondary process of potentiation, leading to an increased tendency to synaptic depression during and after stimulation. The possibility that the impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity may be related to reported deficient behavioral plasticity in the aged animals is considered.
INTRODUCTION Many histological and chemical correlates of brain aging have been reported in the literature10,1~,13,z0,45,47,49-51,55-57,a0-6L In contrast to the large number of reported neuroanatomical and neurochemical correlates of brain aging, however, there are almost no data available on the fundamental neurophysiology of the aging brain.
86 Although a few basic neurophysiological studies of aging have been conducted, nearly all of these have employed spinal or peripheral systems 7,4s,63,64. A number of studies have examined age effects on the EEG and evoked potentials of animals and humans. These have indicated, in general, a reduced amplitude and frequency of synchronous waves and a reduced amplitude of evoked responses with age 5,1s,:19,44. Moreover, there is some evidence, as well as considerable speculation, that certain aspects ol synchronous EEG waves, including amplitude, may be correlated with complex memory and information processes (see review in ref. 30). Nevertheless, EEG studies of aging, while extremely valuable for correlative and diagnostic purposes, do not provide much information on possible cellular mechanisms of dysfunction. At the other end of the age continuum, fundamental studies of hippocampal neurophysiology have provided important insights into the functional significance of the various anatomical patterns seen during development 46. To date, however, neurophysiological methods have not been used to aid our understanding of the functional consequences of anatomical and chemical patterns seen in the brain during aging. This lack of basic neurophysiological information regarding the aged brain seems to be a major obstacle to determining which, if any, of the brain's chemical and anatomical agechanges are directly relevant tofimctional decline, as has been noted elsewhere ~. In an attempt to obtain this information, we have recently conducted a series of concomitant in vivo and in vitro studies on the hippocampal neurophysiology of aged and young Fischer rats. The in vivo studies are reported here, and studies using in vitro hippocampal preparations are reported in a separate paper 3"). Preliminary data from these studies have been described in abstract form 33. The aged Fischer rats used in the present study were recently found by Gold and McGaugh zz to exhibit memory retention performance deficits. Another group has also found similar retention impairment in aged Fischer rats 45. These deficits seem to be analogous in temporal pattern to those in aged humans H,53 and in aged monkeys '~1 (i.e. the aged animals show normal acquisition, but retention performance decays rapidly over time). Moreover, the animals exhibit a distribution of brain deterioration 34 similar to changes seen for human cortex lz and for human hippocampus 4°,57. Thus, these Fischer animals appear to be reasonable subjects, not only for the study of the general neurophysiology of brain aging, but perhaps for the study of age-related mechanisms of changes in information storage as well. The hippocampus was used in these investigations for a number of reasons. First, the distinctly laminated anatomical structure of the hippocampus makes it an excellent model cortical system4,8,23,2~.36,~7; second, the basic neurophysiology of the hippocampus is comparatively well understood as a consequence of extensive and thorough intracellular and extracellular analyses of the various major pathways 1-3,14,21,'~7,3~, 46,58; third, in humans 40 and in animals 12,34,47, the hippocampus is one of the structures affected earliest and most consistently by the histopathology of aging; therefore, any general brainwide physiological deficits that develop with age might well be detectable in hippocampus before they can be seen in other structures; fourth, in humans at least, the hippocampal region clearly seems to be linked to 'recent' memory processes 4'~, and human recent memory is consistently affected by aging well before the development of
87 'senility'Sa; fifth, hippocampal synapses exhibit one of the longest duration forms of post-tetanic potentiation9,15, and one of the most robust forms of frequency potentiation 3 so far observed for any vertebrate synapse. This latter point seems relevant since investigators have long speculated about a possible role of synaptic potentiation processes in behavioral plasticity (e.g. ref. 17). In the studies reported below, a number of experiments were conducted sequentially on each animal. This procedure may affect the absolute values of data obtained in the later experiments because of the influence of the earlier stimulation. However, this procedure also allows for the collection of a great deal of relative data, on which aged and young can be compared under a wide variety of conditions. Since the aged animals are extremely expensive, and difficult to raise in germ-free conditions, it therefore seems justified, so long as the treatment sequence is the same for all groups at comparable points in the same sequence. Instances in which the earlier experiments appear to have affected the later values have been noted in the text. METHODS The experiments were performed using inbred, male, albino Fischer rats, barrierreared by Charles River Co. under an agreement with the National Institute on Aging. Some of the animals had first been used briefly in a series of behavioral experiments in which the aged Fischer rats have been found to show retention deficits ~2. Until the animals were used for physiological experiments, they were maintained in an isolated clean room apart from the other animals, and their cages were changed only by handlers in clean coats and gloves. Young-mature animals were 4-7 months old, and aged animals were 25-27 months old when used. Data were obtained from 11 aged and 13 young animals. Of these, 9 young and 9 aged animals were carried throughout the entire sequence of 4 experimental series (see below), and approximately half of each group was then used for antidromic studies. Additionally, 2 aged and 4 young animals were employed solely for antidromic control or localization studies. Aged animals showing any overt signs of pathology (e.g. tumors, weight loss, infection) were excluded from study. Animals were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of urethane (l g/5 ml/kg) and were occasionally given supplemental doses of 0.1-0.2 g/kg when needed. Local anesthetics (procaine) were standardly applied to wound edges. The animal was placed in a heavy stereotaxic instrument with the skull flat; the skull was surgically exposed, and holes were drilled in the skull with a small dental drill at appropriate coordinates (see below). The same basic series of experiments was performed on the Schaffer collateral system of each animal. This system also includes commissural fibers which travel with the Schaffer collaterals in stratum radiatum 2a (for purposes of convenience, however, these fibers will generally be referred to here as the Schaffer collateral system). Standard electrophysiological recording and stimulating equipment and procedures were employed, and responses were displayed on a Tektronix storage oscilloscope for amplitude measurements. The indifferent electrode was grounded to the stereotaxic frame.
88 Electrode placements were made based upon both physiological criteria and stereotaxic coordinates. All electrodes were inserted using micromanipulators (Kopf) with minimal calibrated increments of 10 #m. The recording micropipette (tip diameter: 1-3 #m; impedance: 3-8 M ~ ; 2.5 M NaC1) was vertically lowered into CA1 of the left hippocampus at the following coordinates: 3.5-4.0 mm posterior to bregma: 2.0 mm lateral to the midline. At approximately 2.0-2.2 mm below the brain surface, large single unit spikes were recorded from multiple cells in the pyramidal cell soma layer CAl. These spikes generally showed typical pyramidal cell patterns of discharge (i.e. large amplitude, double spike discharges, with spikes separated by several msec) TM 46. Single unit spikes are primarily only recorded from the cell layers within the hippocampus with these methods. The pipette tip remained in the CAI cell layer while the stimulating electrode (bipolar; 2 twisted strands of 120/~m enamel-coated stainless steel wire, evenly cut at the tips; 20-30 kf~ DC resistance) was lowered into the Schaffer collaterals at the following coordinates: 3.5-4.0 mm posterior to bregma, 4.0 mm lateral to the midline, and angled 15° towards the midline. Beginning 2 mm below the surface, the typical Schaffer collateral synaptic response was generally observed. This response has been extensively analyzed in detail by Fujita and Sakata zl and by Andersen and his associates 1, and only a brief description will be given here. In the cell layer a slow positive potential is seen with an onset latency of 2-4 msec. This is similar to the response observed in other hippocampai fields35. If the stimulation voltage is sufficiently intense, a large negative population spike is seen riding on the slow positive wave, and this is immediately followed by a larger slow positive wave (Fig. 1). The initial slow wave seems to be the extracellular summated population EPSP; the sharp negative population spike has been shown to be an 'envelope' of summated single unit spikes, and the subsequent large positive wave appears to be a summated extracellular IPSP, which is triggered by the cell firing (and recurrent inhibition) associated with the population spike 1,2,27. The EPSP reverses polarity and becomes negative as the micropipette is lowered approximately 200 #m beyond the layer of the pyramidal somata into the apical dendrites1,21, 35, as is shown in Fig. 1. The most likely explanation of this reversal is that the cell somata act as a 'source' for the active 'sink' on the dendrites where the Schaffer collaterals terminate1, 2~,32. Once a CA1 response was obtained which met our criteria for Schaffer collateral activation (onset latency under 3.2 msec, polarity reversal of the EPSP when the pipette was lowered), the recording electrode was returned to the cell layer and was maintained there for the duration of the experiments. The threshold for eliciting the population spike was initially determined during 0.3 Hz stimulation (general range: 300-800 #A). The voltage and current (across a 1 kf~ series resistance) were simultaneously monitored in most instances. Current was generally stable, but occasional small fluctuations were compensated for by very minor voltage adjustments. The voltage was then lowered to approximately 50 ~ of the threshold value in order to conduct a paired-pulse facilitation series. The lower voltage was necessary because when a population spike is elicited, the spike response to the second stimulation pulse is often inhibited for 30-100 msec, a finding observed even when microelectrode stimulation of the Schaffer collaterals is employed2L This inhibition appeared
89
CAI Rec.
fffer m.
Fig. 1. Schematic illustration of representative electrode placements, as determined by histology, physiological criteria and stereotaxic coordinates (see text). Above, the typical Schaffer collateral response is shown. Upper trace: Schaffer collateral response as recorded in the CA1 pyramidal cell layer (dotted lines in figure); lower trace: Schaffer collateral response as recorded in the CA1 apical dendrites, 200 /zm below the pyramidal cell layer. Note reversal of the extracellular EPSP component. For additional details see text.
to be less p r o n o u n c e d in in vitro hippocampal slice preparations3L Below spike threshold, only the slow population E P S P is elicited (positive in cell layer, negative in apical dendrites), and the inhibition period is much reduced. Spike-induced inhibition is presumably due to recurrent inhibition 1,2,~7. Using this voltage level, responses were measured during 0.3 Hz stimulation. At this stimulation frequency responses are highly stable, and the response amplitude at
90 0.3 Hz was taken as the control value. The stimulator was then set in the paired-pulse mode and the interval between the first and second pulses was systematically varied between 20 and 200 msec. The amplitude of the second response was measured at 20. 30, 40, 60, 80, 100, 120, 140, 160, 180 and 200 msec intervals (positive peak-to-baseline). At each interval, two sets of paired pulses were given at 0.3 Hz, and the average of the two responses to the two second pulses was taken as the measured value at that interval. Very little variation of responses occurred at a given duration. Following the paired-pulse series, the voltage was raised to approximately 20'J,, above the level needed to elicit a fully developed population spike. In some cases, inputoutput relations were briefly studied. Control values were then obtained at 0.3 Hz stimulation. (Measurements of spike amplitude were always made from the origin of the spike on the EPSP to the negative peak of the spike.) Frequency and post-tetanic potentiation series were all conducted at this stimulation intensity. Frequency potentiation (growth of a response during repetitive stimulation) was measured by stimulating at 6 Hz for 5 sec. After a I min interval, during which 0.3 Hz control stimulation was administered, a second series was performed, stimulating at 6 Hz for 15 sec. After a 2 rain interval of control stimulation, a third series was also conducted, stimulating at 12 Hz for 5 sec, and after a 1 min interval of control stimulation, a fourth series at 12 Hz for 15 sec was performed. With the exception of the last series, responses of both groups to 0.3 Hz following each series were similar to control levels. The last two synaptic responses at the end of each of the four series were displayed on the storage oscilloscope, and the average of the two responses (which varied very little) was taken as the frequency potentiation value (in comparison to initial control values) for that series. Although many components of the response exhibit potentiation, the greatest change is found in population spike amplitude 35,52. Therefore, in the Schaffer collateral system, amplitude of the population spike was used as the basic measure during the frequency potentiation, post-tetanic potentiation and 'exhaustion' studies. A 5 min interval was allowed between the last frequency potentiation series and the start of the PTP series. During this interval, 0.3 Hz stimulation was given. For animals that did not show spike values at control levels during this interval, voltage was adjusted slightly to produce the initial control values. Five minutes after the last frequency potentiation series, post-tetanic potentiation (PTP - - growth of response to control pulses following termination of repetitive stimulation) was studied by administering a 100 Hz stimulation train for 5 sec. (This terminology of'post-tetanic potentiation' is strictly descriptive; it implies nothing regarding the pre- or postsynaptic mechanistic basis of the phenomenon.) This intense (100 Hz) stimulus was employed in order to overwhelm any effects of the prior 12 Hz stimulation series. The amplitude of the population spike to 0.3 Hz pulses was then measured at 10 sec, 2 min, 5 rain, 15 rain and 30 min following the 100 Hz train. At each measurement interval, the average amplitude of three responses was taken as the value for that interval. The three responses typically varied very little except at the 10 sec interval, at which point the first response was generally extremely large, with the second and third responses decreasing very rapidly, often to the point of total depression. Studies of'synaptic exhaustion' were initiated following the 30 rain measurements
91 of PTP. A new control value for the population spike was obtained during 0.3 Hz stimulation, due to the prior influence of tetanizing stimulation. These values were obtained at the 30 min PTP period at which point the two groups were not significantly different (Fig. 4); therefore, in terms of the initial pre-frequency potentiation control values, the groups were still roughly comparable. Exhaustion studies were carried out by administering continuous stimulation at 4 Hz for 5 min. Averages of two synaptic responses were obtained at 15, 30, 45, 60, 90, 120, 240 and 300 sec after the onset of 4 Hz stimulation. No interruption of stimulation occurred during these measurements. An average of two measurements was sufficient since these responses are highly stable over short periods of time (e.g. 5 sec), even though they may be changing substantially over longer periods. In several animals, lowering the stimulation electrode induced an electrographic seizure and subsequent depression of spontaneous slow wave amplitudes and single unit activity. In these cases, which occurred with approximately equal frequency in aged and young animals, an interval of 15-20 min was allowed before continuing. By that time, slow wave amplitudes and spontaneous unit spikes were fully recovered. In 7 aged and 7 young animals, antidromic control experiments were performed. In these, the pipette was maintained in the CA1 pyramidal layer, but the stimulation electrode was aimed at regions of the alveus in which run CA1 output fibers1,zS. What appeared to be an antidromic response (i.e. 0.5 msec latency, negative spike not preceded by a slow wave, and followed by a positive slow wave) could often be elicited using these placements. Experiments with repetitive stimulation similar to those carried out orthodromically were performed with these responses. At the completion of an experiment, approximately half of the animals were perfused intracardially with saline and 10 ~ formalin, and the brains were removed and stored in formalin. They were subsequently frozen, sectioned (35/zm) and stained (cresyl violet) for verification of electrode placements. Data analysis was performed by converting all responses to percent of the initial control values (obtained prior to the facilitation, frequency potentiation, and exhaustion series). Data were analyzed using both parametric and non-parametric statistics. However, tests showed that in most cases the assumption of homogeneity of variance had to be rejected, and a normal distribution could also not be assumed, since many aged animals exhibited response depression to zero. Therefore, statistical inferences were based largely on distribution-free statistics. However, it should be noted that parametric analyses of variance and individual group comparisons showed highly similar results, Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare aged and young animals at various time points, and Wilcoxon matched-pairs, signed-ranks tests for paired samples were used to determine whether a change within a group was significantlydifferent from baseline 54. All reported effects are significant at least at the P < 0.05 level, two-tailed, and group differences are represented in the figures by asterisks. In brief, then, both the aged and young groups were carried through precisely the same sequence of 4 experiments (facilitation, FP, PTP, exhaustion). At the start of each experiment, the two groups were not significantly different from each other during 0.3 Hz stimulation (in terms of percentage changes from original control values). In
92 only one experiment-- exhaustion - - did each group begin significantly above its initial control levels (as a consequence of the prior PTP). Nevertheless, the two groups did not differ significantly from one another. A new control value to 0.3 Hz stimulation was taken prior to the exhaustion series. RESULTS
No significant differences were observed between young and aged animals in the following measures: threshold to elicit a population spike, amplitudes of population spikes at threshold or at various supra-threshold voltages, latency of response onset, stimulation voltage needed to elicit a 600/~V population EPSP, and wave forms of the responses. Additionally, there did not appear to be major differences in spontaneous single unit activity or amplitudes of unit spikes, although these were not systematically analyzed. Of course, there may well be small differences in threshold, input-output functions, spontaneous activity, etc., which might be found by more extensive and precise analyses. No differences were observed in the time curves of paired-pulse facilitation, nor in the temporal course of decay or in the maximum points of facilitation (Fig. 2). However, a non-significant tendency for the amplitudes of the second EPSPs to be larger at briefer paired-pulse intervals was observed in young animals. Significant differences were found, however, in nearly all experiments employing repetitive stimulation. During frequency potentiation (FP) both aged and young animals exhibited initial spike potentiation, often associated with the development of double or triple population spikes. In young animals, frequency potentiation tended to Paired-Pulse
Facilitation young- mature
oi
17o 160
150 o
140 130
~ 12o
~. I10
o
2b ~o,;o
~'o
8'0
In1'erpulse
I~o Interval
,~;o
,,io
,~o
i~o
2~o
(msec)
Fig. 2. Paired-pulse facilitation curves from aged and young animals. The amplitude of the second response textraceUular EPSP) of the pair is plotted as a per cent of the control response (ordinate) and as a function of the interval between the first and second pulses (abscissa). The first response of the pair generally varied little from the control (0.3 Hz) level. No significant differences were observed at any time point. For both groups the maximum facilitation was obtained in the Schaffer collaterals at approximately 20-40 msec intervals. The aged animals, however, appear to exhibit a tendency to a slower rise time of facilitation than do younger animals. The course of temporal decay seems to be very similar. Means ~ S.E.M.
93 Frequency Potentiation 190 180 170 m 0
160 150
C: 0
14.0 130
0 ,,t.¢-
120 I10
12.
I00
c 0
90
young-mature
80
o/" aged
70 60-
ol
5'see 6 Hz
i
IS see
()
i
5 sec 12 Hz
15'sec
Fig. 3. Frequency potentiation in aged and young animals as a function of both duration of stimulation (5 sec vs 15 sec) and frequency of stimulation (6 Hz vs 12 Hz). The ordinate plots the amplitude of the population spike as a per cent of stable control responses obtained while stimulating at 0.3 Hz. The difference between aged and young increases with increasing duration and increasing frequency, apparently due to a biphasic pattern of potentiation (see text). Asterisks represent significant differences between groups. Means i S.E.M. increase with increasing stimulation duration (at least within these parameters); while in older animals, an increased duration of stimulation was associated with decreasing population spike amplitudes. With higher frequencies (e.g. 12 Hz vs 6 Hz), the population spike decrease in aged animals occurred more rapidly. Thus, as can be seen in Fig. 3, the age-related impairment of frequency potentiation mechanisms appears to be both frequency- and duration-dependent. A biphasic pattern of response was often observed during the frequency potentiation series, particularly among young animals: the spike showed initial potentiation, then some depression, usually followed by a renewed and vigorous potentiation. This 'second phase' of potentiation appears to be the basis for the higher values of FP found in young animals at 15 sec as opposed to 5 sec. Moreover, this pattern seems to suggest the presence of some sort of delayed 'mobilization' process at hippocampal synapses; this process is seemingly activated approximately 5-15 sec after the onset of 6-12 Hz stimulation. As seen in Fig. 4, 100 Hz stimulation for 5 sec elicited a large post-tetanic potentiation (PTP) in young animals. This PTP was also somewhat biphasic, but with a d!fferent time course. Immediately after stimulation termination, a large potentiation was observed, which rapidly decreased to a depressed or only slightly potentiated level by 10 sec poststimulation. This initial large PTP was extremely transitory and difficult to measure accurately, and is not shown in Fig. 4. During the early depressed phase that
94 26O
Posttetanic Potentiation
250 24O 25O
-~ zzo z,o 0 0
200
'~ ~9o "~
180
u
170
13-
160
-
¢-
150
-
-
140
-
150
-
120
-
II0
-
young-mature aged
I00
i
i
i
I0 sec I 5rnin 2rnin
,
i
15min
3 0 min
Poststimulation
Fig. 4. Post-tetanic potentiation of response amplitudes as a per cent of control responses (ordinate). Responses were measured at various intervals following a 5 sec, 100 Hz stimulation train (abscissa). Both control responses and post-100 Hz stimulation responses were obtained during 0.3 Hz stimulation. A large early potentiation in both groups was observed immediately following 100 Hz stimulation, but this rapidly decayed to a depressed stage between 10 sec and 1 min. Because of the rapid decay, this large early potentiation apparently lasting less than 10 sec is not seen in the graph. The only significant differences (asterisks) between aged and young were found in the Schaffer collateral system at 2 and 5 min post-100 Hz stimulation, and appear to reflect a more rapid recovery in the young animals from the depression that followed the early potentiation. Mean _~ S.E.M.
followed initial PTP, the Schaffer collateral population spike sometimes disappeared. The population spike, however, generally recovered most of its potentiated level by 2 min poststimulation in y o u n g e r animals. Spike recovery or growth tended to continue between 5 and 15 min, at which time it usually became asymptotic. Other investigators have noted that potentiated hippocampal region responses generally remain elevated at asymptotic levels for hours or days 9,15,~2, and we have observed similar results (numerous unpublished observations). Aged animals also exhibited an initial large PTP, followed by depression at 10 sec. However, in aged animals the recovery o f P T P appears to be considerably delayed. Significant differences in P T P in the Schaffer collateral system between aged and y o u n g animals were f o u n d only during the early phases (Fig. 4). The P T P of the aged animals generally rose to the level of P T P in y o u n g animals by 15-30 min, although the largest degrees of P T P seen in some y o u n g animals (e.g. 60(0700 ~ ) were not attained by any aged animals (e.g. see means and S.E.M.s in Fig. 4). Thus, studies with larger groups might well indicate that the eventual levels o f P T P attained by aged animals are also different f r o m y o u n g animals.
95 Fig. 5 shows that continuous 4 Hz stimulation also induced significant differences in the synaptic responses of aged and young animals, beginning approximately 45-60 sec after stimulation onset. Continuous 4 Hz stimulation depressed the Schaffer collateral population spike of most aged animals within 5 min. In younger animals, the response was generally stable for 5 rain of 4 Hz stimulation. In this study also, a biphasic pattern was often seen. Following an initial potentiation to 4 Hz, an initial small decreasing phase was often observed, followed by a recovery and then stabilization. A similar depression and an attempted recovery were also seen in a number of aged animals, but this recovery was usually abortive, leading to further depression, or to stabilization at lower levels (Fig. 5). Thus, there appears to be a greater tendency to 'exhaustion' or 'fatigue' at aged synapses during continuous stimulation. (No initial frequency potentiation is seen in Fig. 5 because the control spike amplitude was measured from an already potentiated level, following the PTP series.) In the recent literature, it has been reported that monosynaptic 'habituation' occurs at a hippocampal synapse 5s and neural habituation has been extensively studied as a process conceptually different from exhaustion 5a. Therefore, attempts to differentiate these processes were made. At the conclusion of the 5 min measurements, controls
]
Exhaustion
_~ young-mature ~; aged
/
120-I
0 0
[
\
•
o ,~ 3'0 4'5 e'0
9'0
8070-
~
60
n eo
40
:~
2o-
30-
I00
,~o
,~o Seconds of Stimulation
a~o
360
Fig. 5. Effects of continuous 4 Hz stimulation on the amplitude of the Schaffer collateral population spike. After approximately 45-60 sec of stimulation, the amplitude of responses at aged synapses generally decreased rapidly. After a depression phase, a small recovery developed in aged animals, which, however, typically failed. In a number of young animals exhibiting a similar brief depression phase, a recovery also occurred. However, in young animals, the recovery was generally sustained thereafter for the duration of stimulation. The Schaffer collateral population spike was depressed essentially to zero by 5 min in many aged animals (asterisks reflect significant differences). Frequency potentiation is not seen during the early phases of 4 Hz stimulation in young animals because control responses for this 'exhaustion' series were obtained following the post-tetanic potentiation series and, therefore, reflect an already potentiated response (see text for procedures). Means 4- S.E.M.
96 for 'dishabituation' were run in aged animals. Such procedures are generally viewed to be acceptable criteria for differentiating exhaustion from neural habituation.~S..~L Varying the frequency of stimulation did not produce 'dishabituation' (recovery) nor did increasing or decreasing the stimulation intensity. At very high intensities, the spike could sometimes be elicited again, but often at significantly reduced amplitudes in comparison to control levels. Thus, this depression phenomenon appears to be due t~ fatigue processes, rather than to habituation-like phenomena. The spike amplitudes exhibited substantially greater variance among young than among aged animals in most of the experiments, primarily because of extremely large potentiation in some young animals. Moreover, for similar reasons, variability often changed substantially during stimulation. These effects are shown in Figs, 3, 4 and 5. in young animals, repetitive stimulation often elicited double or triple population spikes to each stimulation pulse during frequency potentiation, as noted by other investigators1,3, ~'~, with spikes separated by 4-8 msec intervals. This occurred typically during large frequency potentiation, and was also seen briefly in many aged animals, prior to the onset of spike depression. Potentiation of CA 1 responses has been described in several papers, and the patterns seen here were similar to prior reports, in cases of depression, the spike disappeared or was reduced, the extracellular 'IPSP' was proportionately reduced, and the 'EPSP' appeared basically unchanged in wave shape. Eatency and rise-time were not systematically studied following repetitive stimulation. Control experiments with antidromic activation of CAI by stimulation of the alveusl, '~5 induced no age-related differences in CA1 responses during many minutes of alvear stimulation at frequencies up to 30 Hz, nor was consistent potentiation or depression seen during many minutes of such stimulation. At higher frequencies, (e.g. 30 Hz) the antidromic response decreased up to a third in amplitude, possibly because of increased recurrent inhibition 2,27. However, responses were stable at this level and no differences were seen between aged and young animals: in no case was complete spike depression observed, which did, however, occur often in aged animals at frequencies above 10 Hz using orthodromic stimulation of the Schaffer collaterals. It is possible that the fiber properties of the Schaffer system, which was activated orthodromically, are different from those of the alveus, which was used for antidromic control studies. However, both systems are myelinated, and in other studies, performed in vitro ~2, similar age-related orthodromic effects were obtained, but antidromic sti mulation of both alvear and Schaffer fibers produced no age differences. Thus, these effects seem to be limited to orthodromic activation. Histological analysis showed that in all animals examined, the Schaffer collateral stimulation electrode was in the CA3 cell layers or in the dendritic region in which the Schaffer collaterals travel. The CA 1 recording pipettes were also found to be correctly oriented in all animals studied. The pipette tips could not be localized with these methods, but our physiological criteria for localization in the cell layers and dendrites closely agreed with findings from other studies in which extensive physiological-anatomical correlations have been performed in the CAI field 1,~1, and also agreed very well with stereotaxic coordinates ~8. No age-related differences in the histological localization of the electrodes were observed, and the variation appeared to cover the same anterior-posterior coordinates in both groups.
97 DISCUSSION The results reported here indicate that the hippocampal physiology of aged animals differs from that of young animals primarily when the synapses are 'challenged' by repetitive stimulation. Synaptic responses of aged animals to control (0.3 Hz) stimulation appeared similar to those of young-mature animals with regard to latency, wave-form, thresholds or response amplitudes at 20 ~ above spike thresholds. More detailed studies of basic physiological responses to single-pulse stimulation are planned for the future to determine whether small age differences in threshold, conduction velocity, or input-output functions are present. However, it seems clear that aged and young hippocampal synapses are considerably more different during repetitive stimulation than during what is essentially single-pulse (i.e. 0.3 Hz) control stimulation. Thus, it seems to be the neural responses which are generally elicited by repetitive stimulation (e.g. potentiation, depression) that are most changed in the aged hippocampus; this seems of some interest since these responses have often been speculatively linked to behavioral plasticity. The observed age-related effects of repetitive stimulation are, as noted, based upon sequential studies. Thus, even though the two age groups were subjected to the same sequences, it is of course possible that the earlier stimulation affected the aged animals more than it did the young. These results, therefore, indicate an age-related effect of the overall sequence of repetitive stimulation, but cannot be conclusively linked to any single experimental paradigm. Nevertheless, the groups were comparable at the slart of each experiment, and the aged animals exhibited normal early frequency potentiation and normal responses during the early phases of the exhaustion experiment. This suggests that prior experiments may not have greatly influenced the results of the later ones; moreover, it should perhaps be noted that we have obtained similar results, with regard to frequency and post-tetanic (or long-term) potentiation in in vitro studies of the hippocampal slice in which only one stimulus train was administered32. The CA1 field of young animals in the present study appeared to show synaptic response patterns similar to those reported for rabbits 1, cats 21, and other strains or species of rodentsa4,39,~. However, the percentage increase in spike amplitude during potentiation was somewhat less in the present study than is generally reported 1,35,3.9,52. This can probably be accounted for by the substantially larger control level of spike amplitude used in this study (e.g., Fig. 1 vs Fig. 6 in ref. 52). This factor can greatly influence percentage spike changesaS,5~. As described in the Results section, potentiation mechanisms at hippocampal synapses appear to involve a biphasic process, both during and after stimulation. The time course of the development of the second phase seems to depend upon stimulation frequency, and seemingly suggests the presence of a delayed, secondary process of potentiation which may be different from that elicited by the first pulses. It seems conceivable that the particularly robust potentiation seen in hippocampus is at least partially due to this secondary, delayed potentiation process. The aged animals, moreover, primarily appear deficient in the development of this second phase. An understanding of the mechanistic significance of this second phase of hippocampal potentiation, however, seems likely to require considerable further research.
98 The apparent failure of some aspects of synaptic potentiation in aged animals. however, may not be due solely to a deterioration of potentiation mechanisms per se. That is, the initial levels of frequency potentiation during repetitive stimulation (Fig. 3) and the eventual levels of posttetanic potentiation attained (Fig. 4) by aged animals~ were not significantly different from levels seen in young animals. These findings could therefore suggest that the mechanisms of potentiation of aged synapses are not greatly impaired, but that instead, the aged synapses may be more sensitive to depressive effects of repetitive stimulation. At many synapses, stimulation induces two independent processes of potentiation and depression e6,29. Both processes also appear to occur at hippocampal synapses (see discussion in ref. 35). Nevertheless, it seems far from certain that these processes are essentially the same in hippocampus as in other systems. The extremely long-lasting, nearly semi-permanent potentiation seen in hippocampus is different from the potent iation in most systems studied, which generally decays in minutes or hours. Moreover, recent findings suggest that low-frequency hippocampal depression may involve some postsynaptic aspects 3s,39, whereas depression seems to be presynaptic in most other systems studied. However, the impaired potentiation or depression seen in the present study in aged animals is more pronounced at higher frequencies (Fig. 3), and therefore appears different from the low-frequency depression seen in young animals 3s,39. In some ways, then, the age-related deficits observed here appear more analogous to studies in other systems involving presynaptic processes 26,'~9. Although substantially more research will clearly be required to establish the preor postsynaptic basis of the age-related synaptic deficit reported here, there does seem to be a reasonable possibility that the deficit may involve presynaptic transmitter processes, since depletion of synaptic vesicles (and depression) is also increased by increased frequency and/or duration of stimulation in several vertebrate synaptic systems z4,43. Quantal analyses of various defined synaptic systems have suggested that facilitation and potentiation may involve transmitter mobilization whereas depression may involve depletion of the releasable transmitter pooF 6,'~9,6'~. Other data strongly suggest the possibility that transmitter functions may be deficient in aged animals. A decreased metabolism of catecholamines as well as reduced catecholamine levels in some regions, has been reported in the brains of aged animals 20. 55. Alterations in cholinergic and in putative amino acid transmitter metabolism have also been noted6°,6L Moreover, a decreased frequency of spontaneous miniature end plate potentials has been reported at neuromuscular junctions of aged animats 63. A possible impairment of axonal transport in aged brains has also been proposed :'7, which could lead to decreased vesicle populations. There is additionally, a clear degeneration of synaptic elements in aged brains 1°,51,57,61. Thus, at least to this point, the present physiological data seem consistent with neurochemical and morphological evidence indicating some form of impairment of transmitter or other synaptic functions in the brains of aged animals. Impaired synaptic potentiation in the hippocampus of aged rats, regardless of the cellular mechanisms involved, could conceivably influence the efficiency of in-
99 f o r m a t i o n processing o r storage. W e have suggested elsewhere 32,a3 t h a t i m p a i r e d h i p p o c a m p a l s y n a p t i c p o t e n t i a t i o n m a y be responsible for the ' s h o r t - t e r m ' m e m o r y deficits o b s e r v e d in these aging animals. I n h u m a n s the r e l a t i o n s h i p o f the h i p p o c a m p a l regions to ' s h o r t - t e r m ' m e m o r y processes a p p e a r s s u p p o r t e d by considerable data. A l t h o u g h the functions o f the h i p p o c a m p u s in n o n - h u m a n m a m m a l s are controversial, recent findings have shown t h a t e x p e r i m e n t a l driving o f the characteristic h i p p o c a m p a l t h e t a r h y t h m , utilizing septal s t i m u l a t i o n at 7.7 Hz, can i m p r o v e a p p a r e n t p o s t - t r i a l m e m o r y processes in rats 30,31. Nevertheless, the view t h a t we have p r o p o s e d - - t h a t the i m p a i r e d p o t e n t i a t i o n is a significant f a c t o r in the a g e - d e p e n d e n t m e m o r y decline in F i s c h e r rats - - is a highly tentative h y p o t h e s i s which we suggest p r i m a r i l y to p r o v i d e a clear basis on which to test this possibility. Hopefully, this m a y also facilitate the general analysis o f age-related decline o f b r a i n function. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS These e x p e r i m e n t s were s u p p o r t e d by R e s e a r c h G r a n t s A G 00341 to P.L., A G 00538 a n d B M S 7202237 to G . L . , a n d A G 00469 to J.M. W e wish to t h a n k Lisa Sandles a n d T h o m a s W o h l s t a d t e r for valuable technical assistance.
REFERENCES 1 Andersen, P., Organization of hippocampal neurons and their interconnections. In R. L. Isaacson and K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The Hippocampus, Plenum Press, New York, 1975. 2 Andersen, P., Eccles, J. C. and Loyning, Y., Pathway of postsynaptic inhibitory synapses on hippocampal pyramids, J. Neurophysiol., 27 (1964) 592-607. 3 Andersen, P. and Lomo, T., Control of hippocampal output by afferent volley frequency. In W. R. Adey and T. Tokizane (Eds.), Structure and Function of the L imbic System, Progr. Brain Res., VoL 27, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1967, pp. 400-412. 4 Altman, J. and Bayer, S., Postnatal development of the hippocampal dentate gyrus under normal and experimental conditions. In R. L. Isaacson and K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The Hippocampus, Plenum Press, New York, 1975. 5 Beck, E. C., Dustman, R. E. and Schenkenberg, T., Life span changes in the electrical activity of the human brain as reflected in the cerebral evoked response. In J. M. Ordy and K. R. Brizzee (Eds.), Neurobiology of Aging, Plenum Press, New York, 1975. 6 Birren, J. E., Toward an interdisciplinary approach. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging and the Brain, Plenum Press, New York, 1972. 7 Birren, J. E. and Wall, P. D., Age changes in conduction velocity, refractory period, number of fibers, connective tissue space and blood vessels in sciatic nerve of rats, J. comp. Neurok, 104 (1956) 1-16. 8 Blackstad, T. W., Commissural connections of the hippocampal region in the rat, with special reference to their mode of termination, J. comp. NeuroL, 105 (1956) 417-538. 9 Bliss, T. V. P. and Lomo, T., Long lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in dentate area of the anesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path, J. Physiol. (Lond.), 232 (1973) 331-356. 10 Bondareff, W. and Geinisman, Y., Loss of synapses in the dentate gyrus of the senescent rat, Amer. J. Anat., 145 (1976) 129-136. 11 Botwinick, J., Geropsychology, Ann. Rev. Psychol., 21 (1970) 239-272. 12 Brizzee, K. R., Kaack, B. and Klara, P., Lipofuscin: intra- and extraneuronal accumulation and regional distribution. In J. M. Ordy and K. R. Brizzee (Eds.), Neurobiology of Aging, Plenum Press, New York, 1975.
100 13 Brody, H., Aging in the vertebrate brain. In M. Rockstein and M. Sussman (Eds.), Deve&pme~t and Aging in the Central Nervous System, Academic Press, New York, 1973. 14 Deadwyler, S. A., West, J. R., Cotman, C. W. and Lynch, G. S., A neurophysiological analysis of the commissural projections to the dentate gyrus of the rat, J. NeurophysioL, 38 (1975) 167 184. 15 Douglas, R. M. and Goddard, G. V., Long-term potentiation of the perforant path-granule cell synapse in the rat hippocampus, Brain Research, 86 (1975) 205-215. 16 Dunwiddie, T. and Lynch, G., Long-term potentiation and depression of synaptic responses in the hippocampus: localization and frequency dependence, J. Neurophysiol., in press. 17 Eccles, J. C., The Neurophysiological Basis of Mind, Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1953. 18 Eleftheriou, B. E., Zolovick, A. J. and Elias, M. F., Electroencephalographic changes with age in male mice, Gerontologia, 21 (1975) 21 30. 19 Feinberg; 1., Koresko, R. L. and Heller, N., EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and pathological aging in man, J. psychiat. Res., 5 (1967) 107-144. 20 Finch, C. E., Catecholamine metabolism in the brains of aging male mice, Brahl Research, 52 (1973) 261-276. 21 Fujita, Y. and Sakata, H., Electrophysiological properties of CA1 and CA2 apical dendrites of rabbit hippocampus, J. Neurophysiol., 25 (1962) 209-222. 22 Gold, P. E. and McGaugh, J. L., Changes in learning and memory during aging. In J. M. Ordy and K. R. Brizzee (Eds.), Neurobiology of Aging, Plenum Press, New York, 1975. 23 Gottlieb, D. I. and Cowan, W. M., Autoradiographic studies of the commissural and ipsilateral association connections of the hippocampus and dentate gyrus of the rat. 1. The commissural connections, J. comfl. Neurol., 149 (1973) 393-422. 24 Heuser, J. E., arid Reese, T. S., Evidence for recycling of synaptic vesicle membrane during transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction, J. Cell Biol., 57 (1973) 315-344. 25 Hjorth-Simonaen, A., Some intrinsic connections of the hippocampus in the rat: an experimental analysis, J. comp. Neurol., 147 (1973) 145-162. 26 Hubbard, J. I., Repetitive stimulation at the neuromuscular junction and the mobilization of transmitter, J. Physiol. (Lond.), 169 (1963) 641-662. 27 Kandel, E. R., Spencer, W. A. and Brinley, F. J., Electrophysiology of hippocampal neurons. 1. Sequential invasion and synaptic organization, J. Neurophysiol., 24 (1961) 225-242. 28 K6nig, J. F. R. and Klippel, R. A., The Rat Brain. A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Forebrain and Lower Parts of the Brain Stem, Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, 1963. 29 Kuno, M., Quantum aspects of central and ganglionic synaptic transmission in vertebrates, Physiol. Rev., 51 (1971) 647-678. 30 Landfield, P. W., Synchronous EEG rhythms: their nature and their possible functions in memory, information transmission, and behavior. In W. H. Gispen (Ed.), Molecular and Functional Neurobiology, Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1976. 31 Landfield, P. W., Different effects of posttrial driving or blocking of the theta rhythm on avoidance learning in rats, Physiol. Behav., 18 (1977) 439M45. 32 Landfield, P. W. and Lynch, G., Impaired monosynaptic potentiation in in vitro hippocampal slices from aged, memory-deficient rats, J. Gerontol., 32 (1977) 523-533. 33 Landfield, P. W., McGaugh, J. L. and Lynch, G., Impaired synaptic potentiation in the hippocampus of aged, retention-deficient rats, Neurosci. Abstr., 2 (1976) 312. 34 Landfield, P. W., Rose, G., Sandles, L., Wohlstadter, T. and Lynch, G., Patterns of astroglial hypertrophy and neuronal degeneration in the hippocampus of aged, memory-deficient rats, ./. Gerontot., 32 (1977) 3-12. 35 Lomo, T., Potentiation of monosynaptic EPSPs in the perforant path dentate granule cell synapse, Exp. Brain Res., 19 (1971) 46-63. 36 Lorente de N6, R., Studies on the structure of the cerebral cortex. 1I. Continuation of the study of the ammonic system, J. Psychol. NeuroL (Lpz.), 46 (1934) 113-177. 37 Lynch, G. and Cotman, C., The hippocampus as a model for studying anatomical plasticity in the adult brain. In R. L. lsaacson and K. H. Pribram (Eds.), The Hippocampus, Plenum Press, New York, 1975. 38 Lynch, G., Gribkoff, V. K. and Deadwyler, S. A., Long term potentiation is accompanied by a reduction in dendrite responsiveness to glutamic acid, Nature (Lond.), 263 (1976) 151-153. 39 Lynch, G., Dunwiddie, T. and Gribkoff, V., Heterosynaptic depression: a postsynaptic correlate of long-term potentiation, Nature (Lond.), 266 (1977) 737-739. 40 Malamud, N., Neuropathology of organic brain syndrome associated with aging. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging and the Brain, Plenum Press, New York, 1972.
101 41 Medin, D. L., O'Neil, P., Smeltz, E. and Davis, R. T., Age differences in retention of concurrent discrimination problems in monkeys, J. Gerontol., 28 (1973) 63-67. 42 Milner, B., Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In K. H. Pribram and D. E. Broadbent (Eds.), Biology of Memory, Academic Press, New York, 1970. 43 Model, P. G., Highstein, S. M. and Bennett, M. V. L., Depletion of vesicles and fatigue of transmission at a vertebrate central synapse, Brain Research, 98 (1975) 209-228. 44 Obrist, W. D., Cerebral physiology of the aged: Influence of circulatory disorders. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging and the Brain, Plenum Press, New York, 1972. 45 Ordy, J. M., Brizzee, K. R. and Hansche, W. J., Life-span changes in short-term memory in relation to cell loss in the cortex of the rat determined by an automated, quantitative image-analyzing system, Neurosci. Abstr., 2 (1976) 177. 46 Purpura, D. P., Prelevic, S. and Santini, M., Postsynaptic potentials and spike variations in the feline hippocampus during postnatal ontogenesis, Exp. NeuroL, 22 (1968) 408-522. 47 Reichel, W., Hollander, J., Clark, J. H. and Strehler, B. L., Lipofuscin pigment accumulation as a function of age and distribution in rodent brain, J. Gerontol., 23 (1968) 71-78. 48 Retzlaff, E. and Fontaine, J., Functional and structural changes in motor neurons with age. In A. T. Welford and J. E. Birren (Eds.), Behavior, Aging, andthe Nervous System, Charles C. Thomas, Springfield, 1965. 49 Roth, G. S. and Adelman, R. C., Age-related changes in hormone binding by target cells and tissues : Possible role in altered adaptive responsiveness, Exp. Gerontol., 10 (1975) 1-1 l. 50 Samorajski, T. and Ordy, J. M., Neurochemistry of aging. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging andthe Brain, Plenum Press, New York, 1972. 51 Scheibel, M. E., Lindsay, R. D., Tomiyasu, V. and Scheibel, A. B., Progressive dendritic changes in aging human cortex, Exp. Neurol., 47 (1975) 393403. 52 Schwartzkroin, P. A. and Wester, K., Long-lasting facilitation of a synaptic potential following tetanization in the in vitro hippocampal slice, Brain Research, 89 (1975) 107-119. 53 Shakow, D., Dolkert, M. B. and Goldman, R., The memory function in psychoses in the aged, Dis. Nervous System, 2 (1941) 4348. 54 Siegel, S., Nonparametric Statistics jbr the Behavioral Sciences, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1956. 55 Simpkins, J. W., Mueller, G. P., Huang, H. H. and Meites, J., Evidence for depressed catecholamine and enhanced serotonin metabolism in aging male rats: possible relation to gonadotropin secretion, Endocrinology, 100 (1977) 1672--1678. 56 Strehler, B. L. and Barrows, C. H., Senescence: cell biological aspects of aging. In O. A. Scheide and J. de Villis (Eds.), Cell Differentiation, Van Nostrand, Reinhold, New York, 1970. 57 Terry, R. D. and Wisniewski, H. M., Ultrastructure of senile dementia and ofexperimental analogs. In C. M. Gaitz (Ed.), Aging and the Brain, Plenum Press, New York, 1972. 58 Teyler, T. J. and Alger B. E., Monosynaptic habituation in the vertebrate forebrain: the dentate gyrus examined in vitro, Brain Research, 115 (1976) 413424. 59 Thompson, R. F. and Glanzman, D. L., Neural and behavioral mechanisms of habituation and sensitization. In R. N. Leaton and T. Tighe (Eds.), Habituation: Perspectives from Child Development, Animal Behavior, and Neurophysiology, Plenum Press, New York, 1976. 60 Timiras, P. S., DevelopmentalPhysiology and Aging, Macmillan, New York, 1972. 61 Vaughan, D. W., Age-related deterioration of pyramidal cell basal dendrites in rat auditory cortex, J. comp. Neurol., 171 (1977) 501-516. 62 Vernadakis, A., Neuronal-glial interactions during development and aging, Fed. Proc., 34 (1975) 89-95. 63 Vyskocil, F. and Gutmann, E., Spontaneous transmitter release from nerve endings and contractile properties in the soleus and diaphragm muscles of senile rats. Experientia (Basel), 28 (1972) 280281. 64 Wayner, M. J. and Emmers, R., Spinal synaptic delay in young and aged rats, Amer. J. Physiol., 194 (1958) 403405. 65 Wilson, D. F. and Skirboll, L. R., Basis for posttetanic potentiation at the mammalian neuromuscular junction, Amer. J. Physiol., 227 (1974) 92-95. 66 Zimmer, J., lpsilateral afferents to the commissural zone of the fascia dentata, demonstrated in decommissurated rats by silver impregnation, J. comp. Neurol., 142 (1971) 393416.