Behavioural Processes, 28 (1993) 0 1993
181-I
181
88
ElsevierScience Publishers B.V. All rights reserved 0376-6357/93/$06.00
BEPROC 00453
Lateral forebrain
lesions affect pecking accuracy in the pigeon Ralf Jiger
Allgemeine
Psychologie,
UniversitSt
Konstanz,
Konstanz,
FRG
(Accepted 28 September 1992)
Abstract
Pigeons were trained to preferentially
peck the centermost element of a concentric key
array. When they had learned the task to a lateral telencephalon. The ablation caused proportionally more pecks being directed at analysis this result was interpreted as being
criterion the subjects received lesions of the an unexpected performance improvement, the central key element. However, on closer the effect of a mandibular gape control loss
rather than being due to an actual peck localization enhancement.
Key words:
Pigeon; Telencephalic
lesion; Pecking; Response localization;
Gape control
Introduction
The organization and control of pecking of birds has lately attracted increasing attention as a simple motor act controlled by neuronal structures that might be accessible to a thorough analysis (Delius, 1985; Siemann, 1992; Zeigler et al., 1980; Zweers, 1982). One of the brain regions involved in the control of pecking behavior is the lateral pole of the telencephalon, which is known to receive visual and trigeminal input (Ritchie, 1979; Wild et al., 1985). Older experiments performed with chickens indicated visual impairments after lesions of this area and showed a marked deficit in grain uptake (Salzen et al., 1975). Visual impairments
Correspondence
were confirmed
in a better-controlled
to: R. Jkiger, Allgemeine Psychologie, UniversiGt
study on pigeons demonstrating
Konstanz,
D 7750
Konstanz,
FRC.
182
the incidence of intensity, hue and shape discrimination deficits after ablations of the said forebrain region (Delius et al., 1984). Furthermore, in a recent study, it was shown that after equivalent ablations, pigeons need significantly longer than control birds to find and eat a given number of grains dispersed within a defined quantity of grit. The same birds, however, also revealed difficulties in simply grasping and eating a fixed number of easily visible grains. There was no postoperative recovery with the grain/grit discrimination but the performance on the grain-alone task improved somewhat with experience and time, though never to preoperative levels (Jager, 1991). Based on these differing recovery courses it was suggested that the lesions had separately produced both a visual and a visuomotor impairment of feeding behaviour. The first effect could be easily linked with the findings of the earlier mentioned visual deficit studies. On the other hand the precise nature of the pecking accuracy impairment was less certain. Some incidental observations, however, supported the hypothesis that it entailed a diminished ability to direct pecks at targets. The experiment we now report was thus designed to directly measure the peck location accuracy of pigeons before and after lateral forebrain lesions.
Materials
and Methods
Subjects Ten adult pigeons (Columba /ivia) of local homing stock participated in this experiment. Birds were deprived to 80% of their normal body weight and housed individually under an alternating 12-h light/dark schedule. All animals were trained to peck a horizontally mounted piezoelement for the delivery of a few grains in a trough next to it.
Apparatus The experimental chamber (34 x 34 X 34 cm) was illuminated by a shaded house light and incorporated a specially designed panel bearing a horizontally mounted compound key (Fig. 1). The key (24 mm overall diameter) consisted of four independent concentric annuli (each 2 mm wide) separated by 0.5 mm gaps and surrounding a circular center element (4 mm diameter). Each element was mounted on a spring leaf and could be activated separately. Depression of an element closed an individual electric contact. A light bulb above the key signalled the enabling of the contacts. Whenever the contact of one element was closed it caused the disabling of all key contacts and the extinction of the signal light. Mixed grain could be offered with a solenoid-operated hopper. Event sequencing and data recording was controlled by modular digital equipment.
Behavioral
procedures
Trials within the peck localization training began with the signalled enabling of the key
183
signal light for ksy
--
-
Fig. 1. Experimental chamber with food hopper, signal light, and horizontally
elements.
Initially
the
pigeons
were
hopper-rewarded
(3
s> for
key elements
mounted key elements.
pecking
any of the
key
elements. Later they were rewarded for pecks directed at the inner elements but punished with time out (house light off, 15 s> for pecks at the outermost ring element. Trials were separated by intervals (3 s) during which the house light was on but the signal light was off. As training proceeded, additional outer rings were programmed to yield punishment, the aim being to get subjects to peck the central element only. Whenever the pigeons had achieved 90% correct responses in two successive sessions they proceeded to the next constraint stage. Each of the daily sessions consisted of 50 trials. Ten final stage sessions served as a preoperative baseline and XI sessions served for postoperative assessment. Correct and incorrect responses to the various key components were counted separately. Latencies from trial onset to response were measured.
Surgery and histology On the basis of the performance during the final training stages the pigeons were divided into two matched groups, each containing five birds. The subjects of the lesion group sustained aspiration ablations of the lateral telencephalon and the control birds underwent sham surgery, all under anesthesia. The pigeons were allowed three days recovery before postoperative testing started. When testing was completed the birds were perfused intracardially, and the brains cut on a freezing microtome. The extent of the lesions was examined with a microscope and transferred onto drawings of a pigeon brain atlas (Karten and Hodos, 1967). The detailed surgical and histological procedure is described elsewhere (Delius et al., 1984).
sessions Fig.
2.
Mean
percent
pertorlndnc-e
of
and
experimentdl
dnd
postoperative
control
5e~sions
(C)p:
bircls
in
precJperdtive
bdseline
sesbions
Surgery).
Results
Behavioral
d&a
PigecJib
birds Iwo
I.lt-&wwt
successive
key. The first
sessions
remaining
surrounding
that in several tJhe spring
st’SSi(Jns
demanding
eight birds
instances
90%
only
hut all
~~~cd~izd~iOl1trdiiling.
peck
but without
or more
managed withotut
the key elements,
baseline
birds.
matched
control
sessions
Perfot-mance
were
during
and experimental
I>aiiy pre- dnd postoperative
percentages The
Of
responses
/~iOWeVc’r,
ever meeting on the center
to reach the penultimate
ever achieving particularly
h\YJ
element
of
of Ihe
stage (center
the criterion.
the center
(mly
the criterion
and
It was remarkable
element,
wete
ripped
ofi
leabe mountings.
Preoperative
65%
60
the final stage of training
element)
ittdividual way.
dtJ()llt
tnanaged to enter
of the groups
preoperative
correct
(Fig.
of both groups the
dnd redched a level of 75-850/;,
In contrast,
the control subjects.
birds eventually Andlysis
stage reached
to divide sessions
POskJperdtiVe
of cWrect
provetnent
experimental
served responses
were
the subjects
were
run
by the into the
in the sdme
transformed
into mean
2).
Postoperatively
of dbout 7O’i/oc,orrec‘t responses,
run at the lerminal
sesGot)s
groups. scores
performance
responses.
necessarily these
but still (its
pige(JIX
c,orrect responses
showed
a delayed
exhibited
varidn(.e
cut pigeons
lesiotled
(Kit-k,
oscillated showed within
1’368)
45%
and
an immedidte
in)-
the first
improvement
a greater variability
between
three sessions.
to an dverage level in performance
of the lot-dlizdtitrn
scores
than
revedkd
185
Fig. 3. Diagrams of the lateral telencephalic lesions in the experimental group (for abbreviations
see
text).
no preoperative difference between experiments and controls (F(1/8) = 0.02; P > O.OS>, while postoperatively the differences were significant (F(1/8) = 6.22; P < 0.05). Analyses of variance of the latency data did not reveal any significant differences between the groups.
Anatomy The lateral parts of the neostriatum (N), the lateral corticoid area (CDL), the area temporo-parieto occipitalis (TPO), and the frontolateral edge of the ventral hyperstriatum (HV) were removed bilaterally in all birds (Fig. 3). In four birds, lesions of the left hemisphere were slightly larger than that of the right hemisphere including the outermost lateral pole of the ectostriatum (E). Four birds showed unilateral damage of the tractus dorsoarchistriatalis (DA). One bird had bilateral damage to this tract. In three birds, the outermost edge of the paleostriatum augmentatum (PA) was removed unilaterally. The tractus frontoarchistriatalis (FA) was damaged bilaterally in four pigeons and unilaterally in one pigeon.
Discussion Pecking for food requires accurate coordination of head, neck and beak movements in order to achieve a precise on-target peck localization (Zeigler et al., 1980; Zweers, 1982). Pigeons (and chickens: Salzen et al., 1975) with lateral telencephalic lesions show
186
decreased interpreted
efficiency in the ingestion as reflecting a visuomotor
of easily detectable grains. This has been tentatively deficit affecting peck localization (Jager, 1991). The
present experiment represented an attempt to verify this hypothesis. The expectation was that lateral forebrain lesioned birds would have more difficulties in exclusively directing pecks at a small operant target than control birds. However, the actual results were surprising. The first observation was that, although birds quickly learned to peck keys, generally they had considerable difficulties in concentrating their pecks exclusively on the central elements. Only two birds were partially successful in restricting their pecks to the middle 4 mm diameter disc element, the other eight only managing to partly focus them on a 6.5 mm diameter area. This relatively poor accuracy did not improve with extended training. Pigeons are known to be able to successfully target more than 90% of their pecks at seeds markedly smaller than 4 mm in normal foraging situations (Zeigler et al., 19X), but also when pecking small features displayed on operant targets they manage far better localization performances than those achieved in the present experiment (Jenkins and Sainsbury, 1970). Even more remarkable, however, was the finding that after surgery the lateral telencephalon lesioned subjects showed both a rapid and significant increase in the proportion of pecks exclusively localized on the center element as well as a less variable performance by the sham operated birds. Both the preoperative failure in acquiring the localization task and the apparent improvement shown by the lesioned subjects can be simply accounted for by considering certain features of the pecking response topography. Food-reinforced operant pecks are similar in their dynamic geometry to ingestive pecks (Jenkins and Moore, 1973). Both kinds of pecks include a grasping component involving the opening of the mandibles into a gape with approximately the size of food items serving as reinforcers (LaMon and Zeigler, 1984). Pigeons may, however, also adjust gape amplitude (Schall and Delius, 1991). The key elements used in partially smaller than the grains offered as reward. suggests that the pigeons were attempting to grasp
with respect to salient key features the present experiment were of sizes The key ripping that was observed the key components, including the
center disc. The resulting gape was presumably sufficiently large to cause frequent contact of one or the other mandible tip with key elements adjacent to the intended target and thus interfered with our requirements for peck localization, Attempts to condition animals to emit responses that interfere with their natural habits often result in learning failures (Breland and Breland, 1961). Our subjects were expected to learn to produce operant pecks with a necessarily almost closed beak, a response that can be assumed incompatible with the response normal for the context. This would explain the problems the birds had with fulfilling the criteria set by the experimenter. This account is completed by supposing that the enhancement after ablation does not reflect a genuine visuomotor improvement in peck targeting. Rather the lateral telencephalic lesions are assumed to cause a disturbance in the gape size adjustment mechanism. Indeed these lesions impinge upon a region in the caudal neostriatum which has been shown to be a component of a trigeminal sensorimotor circuit (Wild et al., 1985). Birds with lesions in the motoric end of this circuit have been reported to have gape adjustment problems (Levine and Zeigler, 1983). Pigeons with lateral telencephalic lesions have been found to often leave behind the larger sized grains of their normal forage mixture (lager, 1991). In concluding, the results are very clearly localization
accuracy
is disrupted
by lateral
in contradiction telencephalic
with the hypothesis lesions (JBger, 1991).
that peck Recent
187
evidence
indicates that this component
of the peck control mechanism
is not part of the
pigeon’s telencephalon C&iger et al., 1992). The most likely explanation for the deficits that these lesions cause with the uptake of easily visible large grains is that the preparation of grasping with an adequate gape adjustment is impaired by the disturbance of a component of the somatomotor control of pecking. Nevertheless the apparent improvement in the targeting of a motor act after a lesion is a remarkable finding.
Acknowledgments This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, including a postdoctoral grant to the author. I am grateful to Profs. J.D. Delius (Konstanz) and H.P. Zeigler (New York) for carefully revising the manuscript, S. Wagner (Bochum) for technical assistance, and G. Keim (Bochum)
and B. Bayless (New York) for photographic work.
References Breland, K. and Breland, M., 1961. The misbehavior of organisms. Am. Psychol., 16: 681-684 Delius, J.D., 1985. al. (Editors), Delius, I.D.,
The peck of the pigeon: free for all. In: C.F. Lowe, M. Richelle, D.E. Blackman et
Behaviour Analysis and Contemporary
Jsger, R. and Friesel, M., 1984.
Psychology. London, Erlbaum,
pp. 53-81
Lateral telencephalic lesions affect visual discriminations
in pigeons. Behav. Brain Res., 11: 249-258 Jager, R.,
1991.
Visuomotor
feeding perturbations
after lateral telencephalic
lesions
in pigeons.
Behav. Brain Res., 45: 65-69 Jhger, R., Arends, J.J.A., Schall, U. and Zeigler,
H.P.,
1992.
The visual forebrain and eating in pigeons
(Columba livia). Brain, Behav. Evol., 39: 153-168 Jenkins,
H.
and Moore,
reinforcers. Jenkins,
B.,
1973.
The
form
of the autoshaped
response
with
food or water
J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 20: 197-207
H.M
and Sainsbury,
R.S.,
Analysis.
New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, W.,
D.F.
Discrimination
or negative trials.
Karten, H.J. and Hodos,
In:
1970.
positive
1967.
Mostofsky
learning with
(Editor),
Attention:
the distinctive Contemporary
feature on Theory
and
pp. 239-273
A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Pigeon. Johns Hopkins
Press, Baltimore/Maryland Kirk,
R., 1968.
Experimental
Design:
Procedures
for the Behavioural
Sciences.
Brooks/Cole,
Bel-
mont, California LaMon, B. and Zeigler,
H.P.,
1984.
conditioned and consumatory Levine, R.R. and Zeigler,
H.P.,
Grasping in the pigeon (Columba livia): stimulus
responses. Anim.
1983.
control during
Learn. Behav., 12: 223-231
Extratelencephalic pathways and feeding behavior in the pigeon
(Columba livia). Brain Behav. Evol., 19: 56-92 Ritchie, T.L.C.,
1979.
lntratelencephalic connections and their relationship to the archistriatum
pigeon (Columba livia). Thesis Salzen,
E.A.,
Parker,
D.M.
Ph.D.,
University
and Williamson,
in the
of Virginia, Charlottesville
A.J., 1975.
A forebrain
lesion preventing imprinting
in
domestic chicks. Exp. Brain Res., 24: 145-157 Schall,
U. and Delius,
J.D.,
1991.
Grasping in the pigeon: Control
feedback mediated by the nucleus basalis. Physiol. Siemann,
M. and Delius,
J.D.,
1992.
Wild, J.M., Arends, J.J.A. and Zeigler, in the pigeon (Columba 441-464
livia):
through sound and vibration
Behav., 50: 983-988
Variability of forage pecking in pigeons. Ethology, 92: 29950. H.P.,
1985.
a trigeminal
Telencephalic connections of the trigeminal system sensory
motor
circuit.
J. Comp.
Neurol.,
234:
188 Zeigler,
H.P.,
Levitt, P.W.
patterns, stereotypy, Zeigler,
H.P.,
livia):
and Levine, R.R.,
and stimulus
1980.
Miller, M. and Levine, R.R., 1975.
neurosensory
control
Eating in the pigeon (Columba livia): movement
control. J. Comp.Physiol. Trigeminal
of the consumatory
Psychol.,
94: 783-794
nerve and eating in the pigeon (Columba
responses.
J. Comp.
Physiol.
854-858 Zweers,
G.A., 1982.
Pecking of the pigeon (Columba livia L.). Behaviour,
81 : 175-230
Psychol.,
89: