75
Behavioural Processes, 30 (1993) 75-82 0 1993 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved 0376.6357/93/$06,00
BEPROC 00484
Learning to suckle from an artificial within Influence
teat
groups of lambs:
of a knowledgeable
partner
I. Veissier and I. Stefanova ’ INRA, Centre de Clermont-Ferrand
(Accepted
- Theix, Saint-Genk
18 February
Champanelle,
France
1993)
Abstract This work was aimed at finding evidence of observational learning in sheep. The task to be learned was that of suckling milk from a bucket provided with teats. Lambs were reared in groups of either four neonates and an older lamb accustomed to artificial suckling (14 experimental lambs) or five neonates (I 6 controls). One-hour observations were made five times a day. The lambs that learned were grouped and observed for three hours once a week. All experimental lambs learned within three days as compared to nine for the 16 controls. Experimental lambs moved and sniffed or sucked the bucket more often than the controls (2.0 + 1.5 vs. 1.5 + 1 .O% time spent moving, 7.1 + 6.2 vs. 1.6 + 1.8 sniffing/hour, 3.7 + 3.6 vs. 1.7 f 1.7% time spent sucking, P < 0.05). The time to first suckling was not related to these measurements, neither was it related to other behavioural to suckle from the teat-bucket may be a socially transmitted phenomenon, enhancement of investigation.
Key words:
Sheep;
Observational
learning;
Artificial
suckling;
traits. Learning and not only by
Social behaviour
Introduction Observational enables a naive
learning means that observation animal to acquire this task more
of a demonstrator performing a task easily when it is exposed to the same
Correspondence to: I. Veissier, INCA, Centre de Clermont-Ferrand - Theix, 63122 Saint-Gen& Champanelle, France. ’ Present address: Institute of Zootechny and Veterinary Medicine, Zootechny Faculty, Department of Farm Animal Physiology, Stara Zagora, Bulgaria.
76 conditions
(Pallaud
cultural
traits
include
sweet
termite
catching
conspecifics
Lepoivre,
emerge
1985).
distinct
accelerates
1972;
Heyes
1986).
In contrast,
the
acquisition
Dawson,
farm
animals
pigeons
a demonstrator
helps
sensitive
The
horses Baker
normal
behaviour
reported living
(Lynch
(Thorhallsdottir
these
activity
or
(Craig,
1970,
learning,
were
more
activity The
reared
that
of
their
learn;
grouped
This
either
may interfere
factor
white
nor cattle
may be that with
their
are species-accustomed
to
and reduces 1991).
leads
that was
not
to artificial
learning
and consequently
from
suckling.
of others” repertoire
artificial
teats. They
The hypotheses
partner
et of an
of observationdl
would
the knowledgeable
from
grow
behavior
in the individual’s
by a knowledgeable with
beginning
evidence
to nurse
diet
food aversion
to Thorhallsdottir the
similar
to find
close
to determine
food conditions
which
to the
of five had to learn
social relationship
conditions
acquired
According “in
dimed
of a pattern
under
a knowledgeable
to be
learn
more
partner,
the more
partner
would
be
faster.
full
(control from
It was slightly
were visited a lamb
were
separated
from
their
that is 12 to 24 h after birth.
of animals
were inserted
lambs) older
had suckled
or
artificial
was maintdined in the
milk
groups.
milk.
Milk
at about
bucket.
neonates
teat. This
than the neonates
by caretakers
different
A
40 cm above the floor
fed ad libitum
or four
an artificial
from
teats were
of the milk
of hot water
to suckling
able partner. of whether
contact
by 10 cm. Lambs
neonates
dccustomed Animals
visual
a day. Temperature
bucket
lambs
dams
Thereafter
12 to 24 h without receiving milk. Five lambs were randomly x 1 .I 5 m pen. The pens were separated by 1.2 m high walls made
five teats in each pen. The
each other
five
X Ile-de-France to ingest colostrum,
for
prevented
with
least twice second
One
which
that eat a specific
facilitation,
individual
and that lambs
in suckling
to each 2.15
provided from
between
and Methods
were
hdy.
Alderks, to acquire
19861,
1992).
out in sheep
1987)
experiment
accompanied
after they had the opportunity assigned
Observa-
1985;
are of great importance
and Burritt, social
by one present
Animals and procedure Thirty (Romdnov X Limousin) they
partners
et al.,
or not by a lamb accustomed
they would
Materials
social
from
in groups
lambs
that the stronger efficient
models
Provenza
result
is the transmission
Lambs
were accompanied
quickly
Lefebvre,
for
from
and Levine,
for farm animals
rats and pigeons
Social
Chapple
199Ob;
p. 75).
that
1983;
effects
increased
previously.
rapidly;
1976).
to discriminate
situations
paper was carried
Being with
et al.,
et al.,
al. (1990a),
in this
environment.
in this species.
preference
tested
(Calef,
and Crawford,
in it (Veissier,
while
that learning
in rats (Zentall
and
Examples
and the use of tools
behaviours conditions
to explain
species.
life.
experiment
to their
1965)
(Palameta
to experimental
same
It is assumed
neither
use of social cues in these environments laboratory
1979).
et al., 1983;
panel of a box to get food
are more
put forward
of the
not seem to be sufficient
and black to get food in a bucket (Baer to push the front
et al.,
of adaptive
and
does
has been
(Kawai,
in experimental
1990)
observation
watching
process
populations
(McCrew
has been reproduced and
This
wild
in Japanese macaques
in chimpanzees
learning
tasks:
in
potato washing
tional
new
and
that
The
(experimental
but of similar
at 4h20,
9hl0,
14h00,
not was
determined
18h50
was
was replaced
30-40°C
groups
lamb was referred
bucket
and separated
were
lambs)
at
by using with
a lamb
to as the knowledge-
weight. and 23h35.
by touching
a
made of
Estimation
its abdomen.
This
77 provided
a rapid
assessment which
was essential
to remove
lambs that learned
and that
could act as knowledgeable partners for the other lambs of the same group. If the lamb had not suckled, it was bottle fed a maximum of 150 ml of milk. If the abdomen of a lamb looked distended, the lamb was considered to have suckled from the teat-bucket. Such a lamb was taken away from its group as was a lamb that was observed suckling from the teat-bucket. It could be used as a knowledgeable partner for other groups. A lamb that did not suckle from the teat-bucket within four days was removed from the experiment. To maintain five animals in each pen, other newborn lambs in order to replace those that learned to suckle from the were discarded because of their lack of learning. Six 2.15 contained five neonates, except for one control pen that
were introduced into the groups teat-bucket as well as those that x 1 .I 5 m pens were used, each contained six neonates and one
experimental pen, four neonates. A total of 16 control and 14 experimental lambs were used. As the groups of five lambs were not stable, neither the number of neonates accommodated in a pen nor the treatment sizes were multiples of four or five. Afterwards, all the lambs that learned to suckle from the teat-bucket were mixed together in a 4 x 8 m pen. There they were fed milk automatically, and water, hay and concentrates were available. Measurements During the learning period in the small pens, lambs were observed for an hour before the caretakers’ visits by a first experimenter. At 5 min intervals, the position of each lamb was recorded with reference to its general activity (‘resting’ with the chin on the ground, ‘lying’ in the head-up position, ‘standing’ without moving, ‘moving’, ‘sucking’ the bucket or a teat, ‘other’ activities such as social encounters or chewing straw). In addition, social behaviour and sniffing the bucket were recorded by a second experimenter continuously from 8hl5 to 9h15 and from 13hOO to 14hOO on the first five days of the experiment. Social behaviour was classified as follows: sniffing, sucking, aggressive encounters (butts and kicks), sexual encounters (mounting and ritual approaches) and bleating (see Orgeur, 1982, for descriptions). Direct observations where preferred to video records since the spatial relationships and the behavioural patterns could not be assessed with great precision on a videotape unless two cameras per pen were used. Lambs that learned to suckle from the teat-bucket were observed in the large pen for 3 h once a week for three weeks. Their general activities were observed every 5 min. The classes were similar to those described above with the addition of ‘eating’ hay or concentrates and ‘drinking’. Each lamb was weighed at birth, on the first day of suckling from the bucket (called day I> and then once a week for four weeks (days 8, 15, 22 and 29). Data analyses The time needed by a lamb to learn to suckle from the teat-bucket was expressed as the number of caretakers’ visits before the first suckling was detected (maximum = 20). The daily weight gains were estimated from birth to day 1 and then over the following four weeks. For each lamb, the percentage of time spent in any given general activity, the mean synchronization with other lambs, the mean distance from them, the mean frequency of social encounters and the frequency of sniffing the bucket were calculated. To assess the mean synchronization, the percentage of time the lamb is doing the same activity as that of another lamb was first calculated. Then the mean of the percentages obtained for that lamb was calculated. Since the lambs did not stay the same duration of time in the small
78
pens,
the behavioural
it learned
to suckle
Data
distributions
Student’s
were
analysed
using
Skewness
performed
when
the hypotheses
were
variances
used.
between
time
activities.
Fisher
could
Pearson
to
learn
or to
not be rejected,
Spearman
suckle
exact probabilities
(Snedecor
and Cochran,
the group
effect was included
ANOVAs
averaged over the whole
(df = (1,411
pens (n = 6). The
period
(i.e.
before
the teat-bucket).
paired tests were
of homogeneous tests
data of each lamb were from
1967).
and
Results
the
performing
to compare
section
will
number
the
to
the
and and
before
results
and Wilcoxon
assess
proportions
the
relation of
other
in each group
learning
analyses
on significant
ANOVAs distribution
frequencies
of pens for degrees
Mann-Whitney
focus
used
and
As far as the data obtained
tests.
Mann-Whitney
were
teat-bucket
calculated
by using only
Kurtosis
of a gaussian
otherwise
correlations
from were
and
are concerned,
of freedom
in the
on data averaged
by
(P < 0.05).
Results Experimental controls learned
lambs
and three within
learned
to suckle
from
of the latter did not learn
15 caretaker’s
visits,
the
(Fig.
teat-bucket
I).
In
more
quickly
the experimental
that is three days, while
in each control
out of five or six ldmbs
learned
within
three
days (Fisher
exact probability
pens
learned
within
three
days:
vs. O/3,
where
all lambs
caretaker’s and only Ten
visits, one,
either
none
whereas
most
experimental
the task (Fisher During
lambs
or several
learned
close
learned
lambs
learned
for 1 O/4
vs. 3/l
the neonates to the
bucket
3
were were
the
all lambs
pen, only three on proportion
P = 0.10). to suckle to suckle
of
Between
two
but seldom
one
one at a time.
one at a time vs. three out of the 13 controls
exact probability observed
lambs
of the experimental
the 1 -h observations,
the activities
control
3/O
than
pens,
that learned
< 0.05).
rarely
suckling
non-nutritive,
from that
the bucket. is lambs
Most of
sucked
the
neonates
(Ih
100 90
I
60..
70.. 60..
5
0 Fig. 1. Time controls)
3
no caretakers’
controls
12’13’14‘15 16.17 vis its (5 per day) experimental lambs
to learn to suckle from a teat-bucket in groups or four
neonates
and a lamb accustomed
to artificial
of lambs suckling
made
of five
(I 3 experimental
lambs).
79 TABLE
1
Behaviour and weight of lambs in six groups of five neonates (controls) accustomed to artificial suckling (experimental a gaussian distribution
or four neonates and a lamb
lambs). Results from ANOVAs
when the hypotheses of
and of homogeneous variances could not be rejected (df = (I ,4) and (I ,251
before and after learning respectively), and from Mann-Whitney
analyses in other cases (n = 6 groups
and 27 lambs). General activities, frequencies of sniffing and bleating and distances were assessed before learning. The animals suckled
General
activity
were
weighed
before
and after
learning.
The first day when
a lamb
to as dl
is referred
Control
Experimental
lambs
lambs
(n=16)
(n = 14)
Analyses
(% time)
Patterns of activity: resting chin
59.7
f
11.2
54.0
+
10.6
F = 5.26
15.8
*
9.2
14.7
i
7.8
F = 2.03
standing
4.1
*
3.1
4.8
+
5.3
u=4
moving
1.5
*
1.0
2.0
f
1.5
F=11.81*
sucking
1.7
f
1.7
3.7
f
3.6
u=o*
17.1
f
5.6
20.7
f
9.4
u=2
62
f
9
52
f
10
F = 9.39
21.8
+
21.2
41.0
f
58.8
u=2
lying in the head-up
position
other Synchronization Sniffing
between
and bleating
neonates
(per h)
bleating sniffing a neonate
3.21 +
2.06
7.62+
sniffing the bucket
1.6
1.8
7.1
0.1
0.66k
Distance
between
*
neonates
(m)
+
0.55+
Birth weight
(g)
2 640
Daily weight
gain (g)
(n = 13)(2)
k610
2 840
f
6.20
u=2
6.2
F = 20.7(l)
0.1
F = 6.03
f 500
*
F = 0.90
(n = 14)
from birth to dl
70
f
50
60
i
40
F = 0.05
from dl
to d8
60
f
50
240
*
50
F = 1.85
from d8 to dl5
260
*
70
280
i
80
F = 0.28
from dl5
to d22
310
*
50
240
*loo
from d22 to d29
380
f
80
360
f
70
F = 0.69
F = 5.20
*
*: P
after transformation
into square
roots.
(2) Three lambs that did not learn to suckle were discarded.
edge of the controls
bucket
on several
teats, were
partners
synchronised
lambs
was
(respectively correlations between
with
never
sniffing, their
them
drinking. more
than and over
those
were
the bucket
the experimental
sexual
partners.
done.
No
more
from
Therefore,
and were
Sucking
lambs
toward
I). not
between
very
seldom
comparisons
difference
no significant
the
and the
often (Table
lambs occurred
significant
of experimental In addition,
differed the bucket
each other.
encounters
h of observation). not
lambs
or sucking
in between
or
and synchronization
knowledgeable
than
were
the 10
moving
and sniffed
more
aggressive
Experimental
time
each other
not move
parameters
distance
spent
with
did
observed,
these
without
they
3 and 21 times on
and toward
a teat
less synchronised
Knowledgeable less
or
points:
was
and found
each other
correlations
were
80 found
between
able partner
the number
or any other
of caretakers’
After
all lambs
that had learned
suckling
was very
seldom
Thus
differences
be assessed.
in birth lambs
vs. 1.1 f
differences
weight
nor
(daily
1.4,
in growth
weight
and the behaviour
toward
the knowledge-
were
in the large pen,
F = 6.62,
during
a 3 h observation).
and control
lambs
more often than the controls
moved
df = (1,25), lambs
between
(Table
before
times
experimental
experimental
than controls
the teat-bucket
or two
between
rate except
gain from
from one
experimental
between
put on less weight
learning
(about
behaviour
the first week,
(% time = 3.0 k 2.3 Significant
to suckle
observed
in suckling
During
visits
activity.
could
not
P < 0.05). and controls
days
I). The
learning
lambs
were
15 and 22
lambs
detected
where
grew slower
vs. the following
before
week:
neither
experimental than after
t = 15.6,
df = 26,
P < 0.01).
Discussion In natural body
and
conditions,
licking
lamb.
teat-seeking
activity
teat-seeking
is elicited
base of the udder and the position are usually
and
gain
(Vince,
that have already lambs
were
suckled
a knowledgeable physical
learning
1988;
(Renner, and
Also
sucking was found
the
behaved
with
learning
in
probably Most
lambs the former partners.
been helped of the
lambs
may be needed.
suckle
at the
rapidly
in the group.
maturation.
same
Veissier,
Investigation
1992).
In support
According
and the time to learn to suckle
more
than
Moreover,
experimental
the
controls
measurement
is
unlikely learning
of the
a knowledgeable that
artificial
behavioural
once suckling
assumption,
one
from
learned
speed did from
of a quicker traits
of the
teat-bucket
the task
the same control
of the this
the
it.
was
has
partner.
may require
once
from
moving
and the latter
the tendency
partner
lambs
suckling
no
from
did not differ
Thus,
suckling
hypothesis,
learning
of other
of the knowledgeable
fact that several
since
in a help
However,
again,
partners
apart from
them.
a result
of this
lambs. but
partner
the knowledgeable
did in between
to this
involved
can, in turn,
the bucket
groups
Learning
it is unlikely
in
moved
suggests
of items
1992).
in
help learning.
enhanced
by observation
time
Thus, could
surface
In addition,
Le Neindre,
as they
without
lamb to seek
a warm
partner.
near the bucket
earlier
the knowledge-
were
Therefore,
The
the teat-bucket
for another
investigation
the teats
and
in any behavioural
experimental
knowledgeable
stimulus
a knowledgeable partner
can promote
sniffing
frequency.
since
for the lamb.
argue that when
in the effect of the knowledgeable
moving
the experimental
time
lambs
not involved
not vary with
bucket
warm
at the
manage by itself
difficult
from
less
In addition,
as in the case in this experiment.
nibbling
partner
between
experimental
probably
dams,
contact (cattle: Veissier
the
more
no effect of contact with
of the knowledgeable
task even without
relationship
their
never observed
that the mere presence
sniffing
observed
must
her
show
is higher
that for dam-suckling
could
an adequate
(1984)
1964). which
the animal
is probably
One
by orienting
restricted
Williams,
tend to start to suckle
was present.
it provided Vince
from
suckling
the lambs
and
are
and by warmth,
suckling,
different
to suckle
movements
(Alexander
an object
artificial
partner
after a teat. However,
Nevertheless,
weight with
its offspring
whose
For artificial
Hence,
able lamb was suckling,
newborn
less
1984).
experiment,
helps
of ewes
of its head is slightly
a knowledgeable
lambs
probably
Lambs
by contact
not vertical.
In the present when
the dam
the
lambs
learned,
a certain
although group
more
started
learning
to
spread
stage of physiological
stage is achieved,
the experimental
81
lambs could learn the task from the knowledgeable partner and, because the age of the lambs were not exactly the same, they did not learn together. Since learning without a knowledgeable partner requires a longer time, control lambs may have reached the required physiological stage before one of them started to suckle. Afterwards they might have learned from this lamb and thus nearly altogether. Hence, observational learning might have occurred also in groups of lambs without a knowledgeable partner. This would explain the lack of a very clear-cut difference between experimental and control groups. This hypothesis is highly speculative. Further experiments should be designed to assess precisely how learning is spread within a group of naive animals. The strength of the link between an experimental lamb and its partner did not influence the speed of learning. This lack of effect may be a result of age since young sheep do not establish clear-cut preferential relationships (Hafez, 1962, p. 333). Thus, social behaviour per se did not have an effect on learning. Finally, the lambs that learned to feed from a teat-bucket with a knowledgeable partner had a lower growth rate than the controls. This surprising result might have come from the fact that the three controls that did not learn the task were discarded or from a higher intake of dried food in controls. This study suggests that sheep are able to learn through observation. Mere group effects are unlikely to have brought about this result. Observational learning may have occurred in the present experiment because the animals were observed in their home environment without disturbance and small groups were used in contrast to previous experiments on farm species where pairs of animals were placed in an experimental chamber for a short length of time (see references mentioned in Introduction). In this study, animal comfort was maximized in order to exclude environmental sensitivity and maximize the information gathered.
Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to the animal staff for their efforts in designing the experimental devices and care of the lambs. They also thank A. Brelurut for his advice.
References Alderks,
C.E.,
1986.
Observational
percentage of reinforcement. Alexander,
G. and Williams,
Science, 146: Baer, K.L.,
D.,
Learn. Behav., 14: 331-335.
1964.
Maternal facilitation of sucking drive in newborn
lambs.
665-666.
Potter, CD.,
horses. Appl. Anim. Baker, A.E.M.
learning in the pigeon: effects of model’s rate of response and
Anim.
Friend,
T.H.
and Beaver, B.V.,
Ethol., 11: 123-l
and Crawford,
B.H.,
1983.
Observation
effects on learning in
29.
1986.
Observational
learning in horses. Appl. Anim.
Behav. Sci.,
15: 7-13. Chapple, R.S.,
Wodzicka-Tomaszewska,
M. and Lynch, J.J., 1987.
when introduced to wheat. II. Social transmission Appl. Anim. Craig J.V.,
Behav. Sci., 18: 163-l
1981.
Management. Wellington,
Domestic Prentice-Hall
364 pp.
Animal Inc.,
The learning behaviour of sheep
of wheat feeding and the role of the senses.
72. Behaviour:
Causes and Implications
London/Sydney/Toronto/New
for
Animal
Care and
Delhi/Tokyo/Singapore/
a2 Galef, B.G.Jr.,
1976.
Social transmission
learning in vertebrates. York/San
Francisco/London,
Hafez, E.S.E., E.S.E.
Cairns, R.B.,
Hafez (Editor),
of acquired behavior: A discussion
In: Advances in the Study of Behaviour pp. 77-l
Hulet,
of tradition
and social
6. Academic Press
Inc., New
00.
C.V. and Scott, J.P., 1969.
The Behaviour of Domestic Animals.
The behaviour of sheeps and goats. In: Bailliere, Tindall
& Cassell, London, pp.
2966348. Heyes,
C.M. and Dawson,
C.R.,
1990.
A demonstration
bidirectional control. Q.J. Exp. Psychol., Kawai, M., 1965.
Newly-acquired
Koshima islet. Primates, Lynch,
pre-cultural
J.J., Keogh, R.C.,
Edwin, R.L.,
Prod., 36: 175-I
McGrew, W.C.,
Tutin,
P., 1982.
learning in rats using a
behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on
Green,
C.C.
and Mottershead
B.E.,
1983.
Effects of early
acceptance of whole grain wheat by fine-wool
Merino
lambs.
83.
C.E.G. and Baldwin,
ural comparisons of Senegal, Tanzania, Orgeur,
of observational
59-71.
6: I-30.
experience on the post-weaning Anim.
42B:
P.J., 1979.
Chimpanzees,
tools, and termites:
cross-cult-
and Rio muni. Man, 14: 1855214.
OntogenPse du comportement sexuel m8le chez les ovins domestiques (Ovis Aries
L.) Effet de I’environnement
social. Thesis
Palameta, B. and Lefebvrc, L., 1985. what is learned? Z. Anim. Pallaud, B. and Lepoivre,
of the University
The social transmission
of Tours,
France, 108 pp.
of a food-finding technique in pigeons:
Behav., 33: 892-896.
H.,
1985.
Apprentissage d’inversion
spatiale par un groupe de babouins:
dans une situation de discrimination
interactions entre facteurs cognitifs et facteurs sociaux. Cah.
Psychol. Cogn., 5: 527-551. Provenza,
F.D.
and Burritt,
E.A.,
1991.
food aversion in lambs. Appl. Anim. Renner,
M.J., 1988.
Socially
induced diet preference ameliorates
Learning during exploration:
the role of behavioral topography during explo-
ration in determining subsequent adaptive behavior. Int. J. Comp. Psychol., Snedecor,
C.W.
and Cochran, W.G.,
Ames, 593 pp. Thorhallsdottir, A.C.,
Provenza,
F.D.
1967.
Statistical Methods.
and Balph, D.F.,
1990a.
The
A.C.,
Provenza, F.D. and Balph, D.F.,
aversions in sheep. Appl. Anim. Veissier,
I. and Le Neindre, I., Le Neindre,
Appl. Anim. Veissier,
Ability of lambs to learn about novel
Reactivity of Aubrac heifers exposed to a novel environment
P. and Trillat,
1984.
Observational
178:
T.R.
Behav. Sci., 33: 11-I
C., 1989.
5.
Weaning in calves: its effects on social organization.
learning in cattle. Appl. Anim.
Teat seeking or pre-sucking
maternal skin temperature. Zentall,
Behav. Sci., 25: 45-50.
199Ob. Social influences on conditionned food
Behav. Sci., 24: 43-54.
I., 1992.
Vince, M.A.,
Press,
Behav. Sci., 25: 25533.
P., 1992.
alone or in groups of four. Appl. Anim. Veissier,
2: 43-56.
Iowa State University
foods while observing or participating with social models. Appt. Anim. Thorhallsdottir,
conditioned
Behav. Sci., 31: 229-236.
Anim.
and Levine, J.M., 1972.
1220-1221.
Behav. Sci., 35: 235-243.
behaviour in newly-born
lambs:
possible effects of
Behav., 32: 249-254. Observational
learning and social facilitation in the rat. Science,