Maternal executive function and the family food environment

Maternal executive function and the family food environment

Accepted Manuscript Maternal executive function and the family food environment Katherine W. Bauer, Heidi M. Weeks, Julie C. Lumeng, Alison L. Miller,...

474KB Sizes 0 Downloads 15 Views

Accepted Manuscript Maternal executive function and the family food environment Katherine W. Bauer, Heidi M. Weeks, Julie C. Lumeng, Alison L. Miller, Ashley N. Gearhardt PII:

S0195-6663(18)31562-9

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.02.004

Reference:

APPET 4185

To appear in:

Appetite

Received Date: 22 October 2018 Revised Date:

7 January 2019

Accepted Date: 10 February 2019

Please cite this article as: Bauer K.W., Weeks H.M., Lumeng J.C., Miller A.L. & Gearhardt A.N., Maternal executive function and the family food environment, Appetite (2019), doi: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.02.004. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1

Maternal Executive Function and the Family Food Environment

2 1

Katherine W. Bauer PhD1,2, Heidi M. Weeks PhD , Julie C. Lumeng MD1,2,3, Alison L. Miller

4

PhD2,4, Ashley N. Gearhardt PhD2,5

RI PT

3

5

Affiliations:

6

1. Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, MI.

SC

7

2. Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI

9

3. Division of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI

10 11 12

M AN U

8

4. Department of Health Behavior and Health Education, University of Michigan School of Public Health, Ann Arbor, MI

5. Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI

TE D

13 Corresponding Author:

15

Katherine W. Bauer, PhD

16

Assistant Professor

17

Department of Nutritional Sciences

18

University of Michigan School of Public Health

19

3854 SPH I, 1415 Washington Heights

20

Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2029

21

Phone: (734)763-2546

22

Email: [email protected]

AC C

EP

14

23

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ABSTRACT

25

The family food environment plays an important role in supporting children’s dietary quality,

26

regulating eating behaviors, and promoting a healthy weight status. However, relatively little is

27

known regarding parent-level factors that support or hinder parents’ ability to create health-

28

promoting family food environments. The current study examines whether executive function

29

among mothers, or mothers’ capacity to control their thoughts, emotions, and actions, is

30

associated with qualities of the family food environment that support children’s healthy eating

31

and weight. Cross-sectional data were collected from 492 US-based mothers of 2 to 9-year-old

32

children in August 2017 (Mean maternal age = 34.2 years (SD=6.7), 76.5% White race).

33

Mothers’ difficulties with executive function were measured using the Behavior Rating of

34

Executive Function-Adult Version and family food environment characteristics were measured

35

via psychometrically-sound, self-report surveys. Standardized, linear regression models were

36

used to examine covariate-adjusted associations between mothers’ executive function difficulties

37

and family food environment characteristics, as well as the potential for differences in these

38

associations by family sociodemographic characteristics. Mothers with more executive function

39

difficulties consistently reported less use of recommended food-related parenting practices and

40

less healthful home food environment characteristics including providing frequent family meals,

41

implementing consistent mealtime schedules and structure, and avoiding using food to regulate

42

children’s emotions. No differences in these associations were observed by mothers’ educational

43

attainment, household income-to-needs ratio, or child age. Results suggest that lower executive

44

function may interfere with mothers’ ability to create family food environments that support

45

children’s healthy eating and weight.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

24

46

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

47

Keywords: Executive Function; Parenting; Diet, food, and nutrition

Abbreviations:

50

EF: Executive Function

51

BMI: Body Mass Index

52

MTurk: Amazon Mechanical Turk

53

BRIEF-A: Behavior Rating of Executive Function-Adult Version

54

GEC: Global Executive Composite

55

SD: Standard Deviation

56

SE: Standard Error

M AN U

SC

49

RI PT

48

57 Acknowledgements: N/A

59

Funding: This work was supported by the University of Michigan Momentum Center

60

(www.momentumcenter.org).

63 64 65 66 67

EP

62

AC C

61

TE D

58

68 69

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

BACKGROUND

71

Executive functions (EF) refer to the higher order cognitive processes that allow individuals to

72

control their thoughts, emotions, and actions (Miyake, et al., 2000). Individuals with lower EF

73

are more likely to engage in dysregulated eating behavior (Allom & Mullan, 2014; Dohle, Diel,

74

& Hofmann, 2017), have less healthy dietary intake (Wyckoff, Evans, Manasse, Butryn, &

75

Forman, 2017), and experience obesity more often than individuals with stronger EF (Yang,

76

Shields, Guo, & Liu, 2018). These associations are believed to exist because lower EF interferes

77

with one’s ability to create routines and environments that promote healthy eating. For example,

78

individuals with lower EF may have difficulty avoiding unhealthy food purchases, implementing

79

recipes to prepare home-cooked food, and maintaining regular eating schedules (Dohle, et al.,

80

2017; Hayes, Eichen, Barch, & Wilfley, 2018).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

70

81

While there is substantial evidence suggesting that an individual’s EF impacts their own eating

83

and weight, it has rarely been considered that within families, parents’ EF may impact their

84

children’s eating and weight. The family food environment plays an important role in children’s

85

dietary intake and eating behaviors (Kral & Faith, 2009; Wardle & Carnell, 2009). For example,

86

parents’ use of specific food-parenting practices, such as coercively controlling children’s eating

87

and promoting eating to soothe emotions, may contribute to dysregulated eating behaviors

88

among children (Savage, Fisher, & Birch, 2007; Vaughn, et al., 2016). Further, home food

89

environment characteristics such as availability of healthy food, frequent family meals, and

90

limited consumption of meals prepared outside the home have all been associated with healthier

91

dietary quality among children (Bauer, Neumark-Sztainer, Fulkerson, Hannan, & Story, 2011;

AC C

EP

TE D

82

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

92

Hammons & Fiese, 2011). Deficits in parents’ EF may limit their ability to sustain such healthy

93

home food environments and use positive food parenting practices.

94 A small body of research has examined the role of EF in parents’ general parenting approaches

96

with findings suggesting that mothers with lower EF engage in harsher parenting (Deater-

97

Deckard & Bell, 2017; Monn, Narayan, Kalstabakken, Schubert, & Masten, 2017). Meanwhile,

98

mothers with stronger EF are more likely to demonstrate sensitive, involved parenting and

99

provide developmentally-appropriate supports for their children (Mazursky-Horowitz, et al.,

100

2017). This research lends support for a conceptual framework suggesting that parents’ EF

101

impacts parenting and the home environment, ultimately affecting children’s outcomes

102

(Crandall, Deater-Deckard, & Riley, 2015). However to date, only one study has examined the

103

role of parent EF in the domain of child feeding. Among mothers of infants, Fuglestad, et al.

104

(Fuglestad, et al., 2017) observed that stronger EF was associated with more appropriate

105

responsiveness to babies’ hunger cues at 2 weeks of age, which in turn predicted healthier

106

growth between 2 weeks and 3 months.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

107

RI PT

95

The current study seeks to contribute to this emerging area of research by examining associations

109

between mothers’ EF and characteristics of the family food environment relevant to children’s

110

eating and weight beyond infancy. Further, to increase understanding of whether there are

111

specific families for whom mothers’ EF may be more or less strongly associated with the family

112

food environment, differences in the associations between mother’s EF and family food

113

environment characteristics were examined by family sociodemographic characteristics

114

(mothers’ educational attainment, household income-to-needs ratio, and child age). We

AC C

108

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

hypothesized that mothers with lower EF will be less likely to engage in food parenting practices

116

and report home food environment characteristics that are known to promote children’s healthy

117

eating (e.g., having frequent family meals, limiting access to unhealthy foods). We further

118

hypothesized that in families of lower income, families with mothers with lower educational

119

attainment, and families with younger children, poor maternal EF will be even less likely to

120

report healthy family food environment characteristics. This hypothesis is informed by previous

121

research suggesting that having limited resources and more physically and emotionally

122

dependent children is taxing on mothers’ EF, resulting in more negative parenting outcomes

123

(Deater-Deckard, Wang, Chen, & Bell, 2012; Mokrova, O'Brien, Calkins, & Keane, 2010).

124

Understanding the relations between mothers’ EF and the family food environment can provide

125

insight into why some families have difficulty implementing parenting behaviors for child

126

nutrition promotion and obesity prevention, and ultimately, may help guide the development of

127

more effective family-based child health interventions.

128

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

115

METHODS

130

Study Sample and Data Collection Method

131

Mothers of 2 to 9-year-old children living in the US were recruited to complete an online survey

132

via Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) in August 2017. This relatively wide age range of

133

children was selected because it allowed for examination of potential differences in the relations

134

between maternal EF and the family food environment among families with toddler and

135

preschool-aged children, who often present physical, feeding demands on mothers, versus

136

elementary school-aged children, who may be more independent in feeding but introduce other

137

time demands on families that may affect the family food environment (e.g., extracurricular

AC C

EP

129

6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

activities). MTurk is an increasingly common, as well as cost and time effective, approach to

139

collecting data from parents in studies of children’s eating and weight status (Burrows, et al.,

140

2017; Darling, Sato, van Dulmen, Flessner, & Putt, 2017; Emley, Taylor, & Musher-Eizenman,

141

2017). Recent studies demonstrate that MTurk samples do not differ from study samples

142

obtained by conventional recruitment methods (Mortensen & Hughes, 2018) and may be more

143

socio-demographically diverse than participant samples recruited through other means, such as

144

Facebook or email listservs (Dworkin, Hessel, Gliske, & Rudi, 2016). The study was advertised

145

as a study to understand families’ eating habits. Individuals interested in participating completed

146

a screening survey and were eligible if they reported they were female, 18 years of age or older,

147

had a child between the ages of 2 and 9 years old, were their child’s legal guardian, and lived

148

with their child at least 5 days per week. Of the 1013 individuals who completed the screener,

149

557 were eligible based on the above criteria. Eligible participants were then invited to complete

150

the study survey. If participants had multiple children within the target age range, they were

151

asked to complete the survey with their youngest child in mind. Participants received a $0.05

152

payment for completing the screening survey and a $2.00 payment for completing at least 90%

153

of the study survey. These compensation rates were determined after reviewing the

154

compensation rates of similar MTurk-based studies. Among the individuals who were eligible,

155

95% completed at least 90% of the study survey (N=527). Among these 527 mothers, 6.6%

156

(N=35) were excluded due to infrequent or inconsistent responses on the BRIEF-A measure of

157

EF difficulties, resulting in a final sample of 492. The University of Michigan determined this

158

study was exempt from Institutional Review Board review.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

138

159 160

Measures

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Executive function difficulties. Mothers’ EF difficulties were assessed using the Behavior

162

Rating of Executive Function-Adult Version (BRIEF-A) (Roth, Isquith, & Gioia, 2005), a

163

standardized rating scale used to assess adults’ EF difficulties as manifested in everyday life

164

(Roth, Lance, Isquith, Fischer, & Giancola, 2013). The 75-item measure assesses 9 aspects of

165

EF: 3 core EF processes (response inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility) and 6

166

EF-related skills (emotional control, initiation, planning and organizing, organization of

167

materials, and self-monitoring). Participants rated statements (e.g., “I have trouble changing from

168

one activity or task to another” and “I forget instructions easily”) from 1 (Never) to 3 (Often) and

169

responses were summed to create each sub-scale. The Global Executive Composite (GEC)

170

(Cronbach’s α = 0.97) is a summary measure of the 9 sub-scales, with higher scores indicating a

171

greater degree of EF difficulties. GEC T-scores were calculated in comparison to a normative

172

sample comprised of 1050 self- and 1200 informant reports. The BRIEF-A has been used to

173

assess EF difficulties among healthy and clinical populations (Adler, et al., 2013; Løvstad, et al.,

174

2016; Roth, et al., 2013).

SC

M AN U

TE D

175

RI PT

161

Family food environment. Selection of family food environment measures used in this study

177

was based on evidence-driven hypotheses regarding characteristics known to promote positive

178

nutrition among children that may present a particular challenge for parents with EF deficits. For

179

example, as suggested by the parenting literature (Deater-Deckard, et al., 2012), inhibiting

180

instincts to appease children exhibiting negative emotions with food may be difficult for mothers

181

with lower EF and as suggested by studies demonstrating that cognitive flexibility supports

182

consistent physical activity routines (Kelly & Updegraff, 2017), implementing regular family

183

meals may be difficult for mothers with lower EF.

AC C

EP

176

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

184 Home food environment. Two scales were used to measure the availability of healthy food at

186

home (4 items) and availability of unhealthy food at home (4 items) (Bauer, Hearst, Escoto,

187

Berge, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2012). Mothers reported how often fruits, vegetables, and whole

188

wheat bread were available and/or served with meals in their home (healthy foods) and how

189

often ‘junk food’, salty snacks, candy, and soda pop were available in their home (unhealthy

190

foods). Response options ranged from “Never” (1) to “Always” (4) on a 4-point scale.

191

Cronbach’s α for availability of healthy food = 0.65 and unhealthy food = 0.81. Several qualities

192

of family meals were also assessed including the frequency of family evening meals during the

193

past 7 days (Lytle, et al., 2011), frequency of family evening meals prepared outside the home

194

during the past 7 days (sum of 3 items assessing frequency of meals purchased from fast food

195

restaurants (Lytle, et al., 2011), from another kind of restaurant, and purchased pre-prepared

196

from a store), and the frequency of family evening meals cooked at home during the past 7 days.

197

Response options were on a 5-point scale ranging from “Never” (0) to “7 times” (7). Mothers

198

also reported on mealtime structure (8 items) and mealtime schedule (5 items) using sub-scales

199

of The Feeding Strategies Questionnaire (Berlin, Davies, Silverman, & Rudolph, 2011).

200

Response options for all of these sub-scales were on a 5-point scale that ranged from “Strongly

201

Disagree” (1) to “Strongly Agree” (5). In the current study, Mealtime Structure Cronbach’s α =

202

0.85, and Mealtime Schedule = 0.86.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

203

RI PT

185

204

Food parenting practices. Two food parenting practices, regulating children’s emotions with

205

food and monitoring children’s eating, were measured using the Emotion Regulation Scale (3

206

items) and the Monitoring Scale (4 items) of The Comprehensive Feeding Practices

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Questionnaire (Musher-Eizenman & Holub, 2007). Response options for these scales were on a

208

5-point scale ranging from “Never” (1) to “Always” (5). Cronbach’s α for the Emotion

209

Regulation Scale = 0.82 and 0.87 for the Monitoring Scale in the current sample. The extent to

210

which mothers allow between meal grazing was also measured using a 3-item sub-scale of The

211

Feeding Strategies Questionnaire (Cronbach’s α= 0.88 in the current sample) (Berlin, et al.,

212

2011).

RI PT

207

SC

213

Sociodemographic characteristics and maternal body mass index (BMI). Mothers reported

215

their race/ethnicity, relationship status, educational attainment, total income of their household in

216

the past year before taxes (6 response options ranging from “<$20,000” to “$100,000 or more”),

217

and how many people live in their household. Household income and number of people living in

218

the household were used to calculate income-to-needs ratio as a proportion of the US federal

219

poverty line. Mothers also reported their height in feet and inches and weight in pounds, from

220

which BMI was calculated. Adult height and weight self-reported via online surveys has

221

demonstrated high validity (Lassale, et al., 2013).

TE D

EP

222

M AN U

214

Statistical Analysis

224

Univariate statistics were used to examine the distributions of GEC T-scores, sociodemographic

225

characteristics, and mothers’ BMI. Associations between these variables were examined using

226

ANOVA and Pearson correlations. Measures of the family food environment were standardized

227

to a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1, and separate linear regression models were developed

228

with GEC T-score as the independent variable and each family food environment measure as

229

dependent variables, adjusted for maternal race/ethnicity as a categorical variable and maternal

AC C

223

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

education, household income-to-needs ratio, and maternal BMI as continuous variables. To

231

account for multiple comparisons, a Bonferroni adjustment was implemented and p values <.005

232

used to indicate statistical significance. Effect modification of the relations between maternal EF

233

difficulties and family food environment characteristics by mothers’ educational attainment,

234

household income-to-needs ratio, and child age was examined through the addition of interaction

235

terms including continuous variables to the linear regression models. Given the more limited

236

statistical power of these models, interactions were indicated in cases where the interaction term

237

was significant at p<.05.

SC

RI PT

230

M AN U

238 RESULTS

240

Three-fourths (76.5%) of mothers identified as White, 9.2% Black, 5.7% Hispanic/Latina, and

241

5.5% Asian (Table 1). The majority (53.2%) completed college or had a graduate degree.

242

Mothers’ mean age was 34.2 years (SD=6.7) and mean BMI was 27.3 (SD=7.0). The mean GEC

243

T-score for the sample was 50.7 (SD=11.4) and 13.2% of mothers had a GEC-T score ≥ 65,

244

indicating clinically-elevated EF difficulties (Roth, et al., 2005). GEC T-scores differed across

245

levels of maternal education and were modestly correlated with maternal BMI (r=0.14, p=.002),

246

indicating higher maternal BMI was associated with greater EF difficulties.

AC C

EP

TE D

239

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 1. Maternal Characteristics: Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Associations with Executive Function Difficulties (N=492)1

11.6 (57) 35.2 (173) 53.2 (261)

52.1 (11.5) 52.1 (9.5) 49.5 (11.7)

87.2 (428) 12.8 (63)

50.4 (11.4) 52.7 (11.7)

Test Statistic F 1.29

p-value

3.23

.04

2.30

.13

.27

50.8 (11.3) 48.8 (11.0) 49.9 (11.6) 49.7 (11.9) 56.2 (13.6)

M AN U

76.5 (375) 9.2 (45) 5.7 (28) 5.5 (27) 3.1 (15)

TE D

Race/ethnicity White Black Hispanic/Latina Asian Mixed/other Highest educational attainment Less than high school degree or high school completed Some college Completed college or advanced degree Relationship status Married or in committed relationship Other

SC

Total % (n)

Global Executive Composite T-Score Mean (SD)

RI PT

247

248 249

AC C

EP

Mean (SD) r 2.3 (1.2) --0.08 .08 Household income-to-needs ratio 34.2 (6.7) -0.03 .46 Maternal age (years) 4.8 (2.1) -0.06 .18 Child age (years) 27.3 (7.0) -0.14 .002 Maternal Body Mass Index 1 F statistic from ANOVA, r statistic = Pearson Correlation Coefficient; SD = Standard Deviation; higher GEC T-scores indicate greater executive function difficulties

12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Greater maternal EF difficulties were associated with lower availability of healthy food at home and higher availability of unhealthy food (Table 2). Mothers with greater EF difficulties also reported less frequent family meals, with fewer family meals prepared at home and more

RI PT

frequent family meals prepared outside the home, such as meals purchased from fast food

restaurants, compared to mothers with fewer EF difficulties. Maternal EF difficulties were also negatively correlated with mealtime structure and scheduling; families of mothers with greater

SC

EF difficulties experience less structured meals and less consistent scheduling of meals. Mothers with greater EF difficulties were more likely to report regulating children’s emotions with food

M AN U

and were less likely to monitor children’s eating. The only family food characteristic not associated with mothers’ EF difficulties was allowing between meal grazing.

Table 2. Associations Between Maternal Executive Function Difficulties and Family Food Environment Characteristics.1

TE D

Total Sample Unadjusted Mean (SD)

AC C

EP

β (SE) p-value Home Food Environment Healthy home food availability 3.3 (0.57) -0.25 (0.04) <.001 Unhealthy home food availability 2.3 (0.65) 0.13 (0.05) .004 Frequency of family evening meals 5.2 (1.79) -0.17 (0.05) <.001 Frequency of family evening meals 5.0 (1.57) -0.25 (0.05) <.001 prepared at home Frequency of family evening meals 2.9 (2.22) 0.23 (0.05) <.001 prepared outside home Mealtime structure 3.8 (0.75) -0.30 (0.04) <.001 Mealtime schedule 3.3 (0.88) -0.20 (0.05) <.001 Food Parenting Practices Regulates child emotions with food 1.9 (0.73) 0.23 (0.05) <.001 Monitors child eating 4.0 (0.90) -0.21 (0.05) <.001 Allows between meal grazing 3.3 (0.96) 0.04 (0.05) .38 SD = Standard Deviation, SE = Standard Error; higher GEC T-scores indicate greater executive function difficulties; linear regression adjusted for maternal race/ethnicity (categorical), maternal educational attainment (continuous), household income-to-needs ratio (continuous), and maternal body mass index (continuous)

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Models examining interactions between maternal EF difficulties and maternal educational attainment, household income-to-needs ratio, and child age indicated that there were no

by these sociodemographic characteristics (data not shown).

SC

DISCUSSION

RI PT

differences in the associations between maternal EF difficulties and family food characteristics

In the current study that sought to examine associations between maternal EF and family food

M AN U

characteristics via an MTurk-based survey of US mothers, we effectively and resourceefficiently recruited a study sample whose reported EF difficulties reflected other healthy populations based on GEC T-score mean and variability, but were less than populations with clinical conditions commonly associated with impaired EF including eating disorders and obesity (Ciszewski, Francis, Mendella, Bissada, & Tasca, 2014; Roth, et al., 2005; Rouel, Raman, Hay,

TE D

& Smith, 2016). Consistent with our hypothesis, mothers with greater EF difficulties reported less engagement in several family food environment characteristics that promote children’s

EP

healthy eating. For example, families of mothers with greater EF difficulties reported less frequent family meals, were less likely to have healthy food available at home, and were more

AC C

likely to have unhealthy food available at home. Mothers with greater EF difficulties also were more likely to regulate children’s emotions with food and were less likely to monitor their children’s intake. These family food environment characteristics have been associated with less healthy dietary intake, higher weight status, and/or a greater likelihood of developing maladaptive eating behaviors among children (Bauer, et al., 2011; Hammons & Fiese, 2011; Savage, et al., 2007; Vaughn, et al., 2016). These findings provide some of the first evidence suggesting maternal EF may play an important role in children’s nutrition and weight status.

14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Counter to our hypotheses, the relations between maternal EF difficulties and family food environment characteristics did not differ by family sociodemographic characteristics. That is, lower maternal EF may present a similar barrier to healthy family food environments across

RI PT

diverse types of families.

SC

This study contributes to our growing understanding of how adults’ EF is not only associated with their own dietary intake and weight status (Allom & Mullan, 2014; Dohle, et al., 2017;

M AN U

Wyckoff, et al., 2017; Yang, et al., 2018), but may affect their children’s eating and weight. Building on evidence that as early as infancy, stronger EF among mothers is associated with more responsive feeding (Fuglestad, et al., 2017), the current study suggests that stronger EF may also support mothers’ engagement in parenting practices that impact older children’s eating and establish positive nutrition and eating behaviors throughout the lifespan. Recent evidence

TE D

also demonstrates that children of parents with lower EF are less likely to have optimal bedtime routines (Kitsaras, Goodwin, Allan, Kelly, & Pretty, 2018), another avenue through which parent

EP

EF may impact children’s obesity risk (Anderson, Sacker, Whitaker, & Kelly, 2017). Further, one study has identified that children of parents with greater impulsivity, a component of EF, are

AC C

more likely to experience obesity (Stoklosa, et al., 2018). Together, these findings suggest that families with parents with lower EF may be a population in particular need of nutrition promotion and obesity prevention programming.

While behavior change can be difficult for adults with lower EF (Gettens & Gorin, 2017), several novel treatment approaches are demonstrating promise in improving emotional and behavioral self-regulation among individuals with lower EF to address dysregulated eating and

15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

obesity (Dassen, Houben, Van Breukelen, & Jansen, 2018; Eichen, Matheson, Appleton-Knapp, & Boutelle, 2017; Raman, Hay, Tchanturia, & Smith, 2017). For example, Episodic Future Thinking, where individuals are guided through exercises to visualize positive future events in

RI PT

great detail, has been associated with improvements in impulse control and reductions in

obesogenic eating behaviors (Bromberg, Wiehler, & Peters, 2015; Daniel, Stanton, & Epstein, 2013; Dassen, Jansen, Nederkoorn, & Houben, 2016). Further, Cognitive Remediation Therapy,

SC

which seeks to improve thinking processes and strengthen metacognition, has been shown to improve cognitive flexibility and has led to meaningful improvements in weight status among

M AN U

individuals seeking care for obesity (Allom, Mullan, Smith, Hay, & Raman, 2018). If future studies provide consistent support for the hypothesis that lower EF among parents has a negative impact on children’s nutrition and weight outcomes, integrating intervention strategies such as these into family-based interventions may enhance behavior change among parents with lower

TE D

EF, providing improved opportunities for children to engage in healthier behaviors.

Findings should be considered in light of study limitations. Although use of MTurk to conduct

EP

population-based survey research is growing, it is more difficult than in-person data collection methods to verify participant-provided information. However, electronic and mailed survey-

AC C

based studies are prone to similar limitations and validity checks built into the BRIEF-A identified only a small number of incongruent response patterns. Second, while the BRIEF-A contains two theoretically-distinct indices assessing behavior regulation (the ability to regulate behavior and emotional responses) and metacognition (the ability to self-manage tasks and monitor performance) (Roth, et al., 2005), a high correlation (r=0.81) between the two indices in the current sample led us to rely on a single summary measure of EF difficulties, the GEC.

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Future studies using task-based measures of EF may provide greater insight into whether specific EF processes underlie parent engagement in family food practices. There was also a limited number of measures of child characteristics; for example, children’s heights and weights were

RI PT

not collected due to the relatively low validity of parent report of these measures among parents of children in this age range (Weden, et al., 2013), limiting the opportunity to examine

associations between mothers’ EF and child weight status. Children’s EF was also not measured.

SC

As lower EF among children is associated with obesogenic eating behaviors and obesity (Liang, Matheson, Kaye, & Boutelle, 2014), it would be beneficial to understand how mothers’ EF

M AN U

predicts family and child outcomes independent of child EF, and how mothers’ and children’s EF may interact to impact children’s nutrition and growth. Finally, this study only focused on mothers, despite fathers and other caregivers often having substantial roles in supporting the family food environment, and the study sample was predominantly of White race and had

CONCLUSIONS

TE D

relatively high educational attainment, limiting generalizability to other population groups.

EP

This study provides some of the first evidence that mothers’ EF is associated with elements of the family food environment that support children’s healthy eating and weight status. These

AC C

associations were consistent across families with varying maternal education, household income, and child age. Future research using diverse assessments of parent EF, assessing a wide range of family food environment characteristics, and measuring child-level characteristics including children’s own EF, diet, and BMI, will greatly enhance our understanding of the role of parents’ EF in children’s nutrition and growth. Additionally, as poor parent EF is hypothesized to interfere with even strong intentions to engage in nutrition-promoting parenting practices

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

(Larsen, et al., 2018), it is important to consider the intersections of parent intention, motivation, and EF, and how these factors together support behavior change. This knowledge may guide the development of novel intervention strategies to better promote healthy family food environments

REFERENCES

SC

RI PT

and positive child nutrition and growth, even among families of parents with poor EF.

M AN U

Adler, L. A., Dirks, B., Deas, P. F., Raychaudhuri, A., Dauphin, M. R., Lasser, R. A., & Weisler, R. H. (2013). Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder who report clinically significant impairment in executive function: results from a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

TE D

Allom, V., & Mullan, B. (2014). Individual differences in executive function predict distinct eating behaviours. Appetite, 80, 123-130. Allom, V., Mullan, B., Smith, E., Hay, P., & Raman, J. (2018). Breaking bad habits by

EP

improving executive function in individuals with obesity. BMC Public Health, 18, 505. Anderson, S. E., Sacker, A., Whitaker, R. C., & Kelly, Y. (2017). Self-regulation and household

AC C

routines at age three and obesity at age eleven: longitudinal analysis of the UK Millennium Cohort Study. International Journal of Obesity (2005), 41, 1459-1466.

Bauer, K. W., Hearst, M. O., Escoto, K., Berge, J. M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2012). Parental employment and work-family stress: associations with family food environments. Social Science and Medicine, 75, 496-504.

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Bauer, K. W., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Fulkerson, J. A., Hannan, P. J., & Story, M. (2011). Familial correlates of adolescent girls' physical activity, television use, dietary intake, weight, and body composition. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and

RI PT

Physical Activity, 8, 25.

Berlin, K. S., Davies, W. H., Silverman, A. H., & Rudolph, C. D. (2011). Assessing familybased feeding strategies, strengths, and mealtime structure with the Feeding Strategies

SC

Questionnaire. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 36, 586-595.

Bromberg, U., Wiehler, A., & Peters, J. (2015). Episodic Future Thinking Is Related to

M AN U

Impulsive Decision Making in Healthy Adolescents. Child Development, 86, 1458-1468. Burrows, T., Skinner, J., Joyner, M. A., Palmieri, J., Vaughan, K., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2017). Food addiction in children: Associations with obesity, parental food addiction and feeding practices. Eat Behav, 26, 114-120.

TE D

Ciszewski, S., Francis, K., Mendella, P., Bissada, H., & Tasca, G. A. (2014). Validity and reliability of the behavior rating inventory of executive function - adult version in a clinical sample with eating disorders. Eat Behav, 15, 175-181.

EP

Crandall, A., Deater-Deckard, K., & Riley, A. W. (2015). Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 36,

AC C

105-126.

Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2013). The Future Is Now: Reducing Impulsivity and Energy Intake Using Episodic Future Thinking. Psychological Science, 24, 23392342.

19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Darling, K. E., Sato, A. F., van Dulmen, M., Flessner, C., & Putt, G. (2017). Development of a Measure to Assess Parent Perceptions of Barriers to Child Weight Management. Child Obes.

RI PT

Dassen, F. C. M., Houben, K., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., & Jansen, A. (2018). Gamified working memory training in overweight individuals reduces food intake but not body weight. Appetite, 124, 89-98.

SC

Dassen, F. C. M., Jansen, A., Nederkoorn, C., & Houben, K. (2016). Focus on the future:

Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite, 96, 327-332.

M AN U

Deater-Deckard, K., & Bell, M. A. (2017). Maternal executive function, heart rate, and EEG alpha reactivity interact in the prediction of harsh parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 31, 41-50.

Deater-Deckard, K., Wang, Z., Chen, N., & Bell, M. A. (2012). Maternal executive function,

TE D

harsh parenting, and child conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 53, 1084-1091. Dohle, S., Diel, K., & Hofmann, W. (2017). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating

EP

behavior: A review. Appetite.

Dworkin, J., Hessel, H., Gliske, K., & Rudi, J. H. (2016). A Comparison of Three Online

AC C

Recruitment Strategies for Engaging Parents. Fam Relat, 65, 550-561.

Eichen, D. M., Matheson, B. E., Appleton-Knapp, S. L., & Boutelle, K. N. (2017). Neurocognitive Treatments for Eating Disorders and Obesity. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 19,

62.

Emley, E. A., Taylor, M. B., & Musher-Eizenman, D. R. (2017). Mindful feeding and child dietary health. Eat Behav, 24, 89-94.

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Fuglestad, A. J., Demerath, E. W., Finsaas, M. C., Moore, C. J., Georgieff, M. K., & Carlson, S. M. (2017). Maternal executive function, infant feeding responsiveness and infant growth during the first 3 months. Pediatric Obesity, 12 Suppl 1, 102-110.

RI PT

Gettens, K. M., & Gorin, A. A. (2017). Executive function in weight loss and weight loss

maintenance: a conceptual review and novel neuropsychological model of weight control. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 40, 687-701.

SC

Hammons, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2011). Is frequency of shared family meals related to the nutritional health of children and adolescents? Pediatrics, 127, e1565-1574.

M AN U

Hayes, J. F., Eichen, D. M., Barch, D. M., & Wilfley, D. E. (2018). Executive function in childhood obesity: Promising intervention strategies to optimize treatment outcomes. Appetite, 124, 10-23.

Kelly, S. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2017). Substituting activities mediates the effect of cognitive

669-674.

TE D

flexibility on physical activity: a daily diary study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 40,

Kitsaras, G., Goodwin, M., Allan, J., Kelly, M. P., & Pretty, I. A. (2018). Bedtime routines child

EP

wellbeing & development. BMC Public Health, 18, 386. Kral, T. V., & Faith, M. S. (2009). Influences on child eating and weight development from a

AC C

behavioral genetics perspective. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 596-605.

Larsen, J. K., Hermans, R. C. J., Sleddens, E. F. C., Vink, J. M., Kremers, S. P. J., Ruiter, E. L. M., & Fisher, J. O. (2018). How to bridge the intention-behavior gap in food parenting: Automatic constructs and underlying techniques. Appetite, 123, 191-200.

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Lassale, C., Peneau, S., Touvier, M., Julia, C., Galan, P., Hercberg, S., & Kesse-Guyot, E. (2013). Validity of web-based self-reported weight and height: results of the NutrinetSante study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 15, e152.

RI PT

Liang, J., Matheson, B. E., Kaye, W. H., & Boutelle, K. N. (2014). Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. International Journal of Obesity (2005), 38, 494-506.

SC

Løvstad, M., Sigurdardottir, S., Andersson, S., Grane, V., Moberget, T., Stubberud, J., &

Solbakk, A. (2016). Behavior rating inventory of executive function adult version in

M AN U

patients with neurological and neuropsychiatric conditions: symptom levels and relationship to emotional distress. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22, 682-694.

Lytle, L. A., Hearst, M. O., Fulkerson, J., Murray, D. M., Martinson, B., Klein, E., Pasch, K., &

TE D

Samuelson, A. (2011). Examining the relationships between family meal practices, family stressors, and the weight of youth in the family. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 41, 353-362.

EP

Mazursky-Horowitz, H., Thomas, S. R., Woods, K. E., Chrabaszcz, J. S., Deater-Deckard, K., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2017). Maternal Executive Functioning and Scaffolding in

AC C

Families of Children with and without Parent-Reported ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Mokrova, I., O'Brien, M., Calkins, S., & Keane, S. (2010). Parental ADHD Symptomology and Ineffective Parenting: The Connecting Link of Home Chaos. Parent Sci Pract, 10, 119135.

RI PT

Monn, A. R., Narayan, A. J., Kalstabakken, A. W., Schubert, E. C., & Masten, A. S. (2017). Executive function and parenting in the context of homelessness. Journal of Family Psychology, 31, 61-70.

SC

Mortensen, K., & Hughes, T. L. (2018). Comparing Amazon's Mechanical Turk Platform to Conventional Data Collection Methods in the Health and Medical Research Literature.

M AN U

Journal of General Internal Medicine.

Musher-Eizenman, D., & Holub, S. (2007). Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 960-972.

TE D

Raman, J., Hay, P., Tchanturia, K., & Smith, E. (2017). A randomised controlled trial of manualized cognitive remediation therapy in adult obesity. Appetite. Roth, R. M., Isquith, P. K., & Gioia, G. A. (2005). BRIEF-A: Behavior rating inventory of

Resources.

EP

executive function--adult version: Professional manual: Psychological Assessment

AC C

Roth, R. M., Lance, C. E., Isquith, P. K., Fischer, A. S., & Giancola, P. R. (2013). Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavior rating inventory of executive function-adult version in healthy adults and application to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28, 425-434.

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Rouel, M., Raman, J., Hay, P., & Smith, E. (2016). Validation of the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function - Adult Version (BRIEF-A) in the obese with and without binge eating disorder. Eat Behav, 23, 58-65.

RI PT

Savage, J. S., Fisher, J. O., & Birch, L. L. (2007). Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. Journal of Law, Medicine and Ethics, 35, 22-34.

Stoklosa, M., Shuval, K., Drope, J., Tchernis, R., Pachucki, M., Yaroch, A., & Harding, M.

SC

(2018). The intergenerational transmission of obesity: The role of time preferences and self-control. Economics and Human Biology, 28, 92-106.

M AN U

Vaughn, A. E., Ward, D. S., Fisher, J. O., Faith, M. S., Hughes, S. O., Kremers, S. P., MusherEizenman, D. R., O'Connor, T. M., Patrick, H., & Power, T. G. (2016). Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutrition Reviews.

TE D

Wardle, J., & Carnell, S. (2009). Appetite is a heritable phenotype associated with adiposity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38 Suppl 1, S25-30. Weden, M. M., Brownell, P. B., Rendall, M. S., Lau, C., Fernandes, M., & Nazarov, Z. (2013).

EP

Parent-Reported Height and Weight as Sources of Bias in Survey Estimates of Childhood Obesity. American Journal of Epidemiology, 178, 461-473.

AC C

Wyckoff, E. P., Evans, B. C., Manasse, S. M., Butryn, M. L., & Forman, E. M. (2017). Executive functioning and dietary intake: Neurocognitive correlates of fruit, vegetable, and saturated fat intake in adults with obesity. Appetite, 111, 79-85.

Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Guo, C., & Liu, Y. (2018). Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 225-244.

24