Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329 – 336
Original articles
Measurement of self-reported pain intensity in children and adolescents Anna Huguet a,⁎, Jennifer N. Stinson b , Patrick J. McGrath a b
a IWK Health Centre and Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada Department of Anesthesia and Pain Medicine, The Hospital for Sick Children, and Lawrence S. Bloomberg Faculty of Nursing, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Received 24 February 2009; received in revised form 11 June 2009; accepted 11 June 2009
Abstract Acute and chronic pain is a common experience in children and youth. A thorough assessment is fundamental to understand this experience and to assess and monitor treatment responses. The intensity of pain is the parameter most commonly assessed. In this article, we describe the different methods employed to assess pediatric pain intensity and review well-validated and commonly used self-report measures of pain. This review is based on the recent systematic reviews conducted for the Pediatric Initiative on Methods, Measurement, and Pain Assessment in Clinical Trials Consensus Group and the Society of Pediatric Psychology. Amongst the several types of pediatric pain measures, self-report, when available, is regarded as the primary
source of information about pain intensity, to be complemented by observation and knowledge of the context. There is a large number of self-report measures of pediatric pain intensity; and there is some agreement that professionals in the clinical and research practice should assess pain intensity using the Pieces of Hurt Tool, the Faces Pain Scale, the Oucher, or Visual Analogue Scales because these measures have shown to have sound psychometric properties and clinical utility. Despite the increased number of age-appropriate self-report measures of pediatric pain intensity over the last years, we report several research gaps and priorities of future research. © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Pain intensity; Pediatric pain; Measurement; Self-reports; Psychometric properties; Clinical utility
Introduction Pain is defined as “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage” by the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP [1] p. 210). Such a definition parallels, to some extent, the widely accepted and used biopsychosocial model of pain [2]. It goes beyond the biomedical perspective which dominated the field years ago. Pain is much more than a simple, straightforward, sensory experience. Pain results from the interaction of multiple factors, physical, as well as emotional, cognitive, behavioral, and contextual [3]. Pain is unpleasant but necessary. Pain has biological value, it can be a sign that something dangerous is occurring ⁎ Corresponding author. IWK Health Centre, 5850/5980 University Ave., Room K8508 PO box 9700, Halifax, Canada NS B3K 6R8. E-mail address:
[email protected] (A. Huguet). 0022-3999/09/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.06.003
in the body. That is, pain can warn us of actual or impeding tissue damage and motivate the individual to escape or avoid further harm. Indeed, acute pain is defined by the IASP as pain that usually has an identifiable relationship to injury or disease and it has a recent onset and probable limited duration [1]. However, there are times when the pain experience is unnecessary, it happens when pain has lost its value as a signal of danger. This is true of most painful medical procedures and is typical in chronic pain problems. Chronic pain is defined by the IASP as pain that persists beyond normally expected healing [1]; however, it might also occur without a physical injury [4]. It is experienced as recurrent (i.e., repeated) or continuous (i.e., persistent) in nature [4]. Pain, either acute or chronic, is a very common experience in children and youth. Many children are exposed to acute painful procedures (e.g., needles for immunization and blood sampling, and surgery) with immunizations being the most frequently performed procedure in pediatric setting
330
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
[5]. On the other hand, the prevalence of chronic pain is estimated to range from 15% to 30% with headache and abdominal pain the most common recurrent pain problems (e.g., Ref. [6] and [7]). Both acute and chronic pain are often under-recognized and not treated appropriately, which may lead to both short and long-term negative consequences [8]. Accurate pain assessment using reliable and valid measures is the cornerstone of effective treatment. The purpose of this article is to (a) provide an overview of the measurement of pain intensity in children and youth, (b) outline the approaches to assessment of pediatric pain, (c) describe single item self-report measures with well-established reliability and validity for clinical and research use, and (d) discuss future areas for research. Pain intensity When assessing pediatric pain (acute and chronic) there are multiple dimensions that can be assessed. These dimensions include: (a) sensory (e.g., intensity, word descriptors, duration, location, and frequency), (b) affective/cognitive (pain unpleasantness), and (c) impact of pain in aspects of every day life (physical, social, emotional, and role functioning). While it is important to assess each of these domains, the most commonly used parameter in clinical and research practices is the measurement of the intensity of pain or how much it hurts [9]. Moreover, pain intensity has been recently suggested as being a primary outcome domain to be used in pediatric pain clinical trials by the Pediatric Initiative on Methods, Measurement, and Pain Assessment in Clinical Trials Consensus Group (PedIMMPACT [10]). Several systematic reviews of pediatric pain measures have been published over the past few years [11–13]. These reviews were commissioned by two independent working groups: Ped-IMMPACT group and the Society of Pediatric Psychology (SPP). In general terms, both groups summarize and describe the most relevant measures to assess the key domains in the field of pediatric pain based on empirical evidence as well as experts' opinions. The Ped-IMMPACT group was an initiative that included academic researchers, government funding and regulatory agencies, and the pharmaceutical industry. The aim of this group was to determine which domains and measures should be used in clinical trials for pediatric pain. This consensus group commissioned two systematic reviews of pain intensity measures for children 3–18 years of age in line with their recommendations to use pain intensity as a primary outcome measures in pediatric clinical trials. These two systematic reviews aimed to identify self-report pain intensity [12] and observational measures of pain intensity [13] with wellestablished psychometric properties that could be recommended for use in clinical trials. On the other hand, the SPP commissioned reviews of measures used by pediatric psychologists working as scientist-practitioners to assess
several areas of interest, including pain. The SPP review of pediatric pain measures, unlike the previous systematic reviews commissioned by Ped-IMMPACT, did not only focus on identifying pain intensity measures, but also measures intended for assessing other pain-related domains. The goal of this review was to identify measures with wellestablished psychometric properties that could be used in clinical practice. The results derived from these three reviews are relevant as they focused on pain intensity but they approached the reviews from different perspectives (research versus clinical practice). The Ped-IMMPACT was focused more heavily on reliability and validity of pain intensity measures, whereas the SPP recommendations were more focused on clinical utility. Clinical utility refers to the applicability of the measure within clinical context [14]. Second, the two groups used different methodologies to review and develop recommendations about pain intensity measures. On one hand, the Ped-IMMPACT group used a two-stage process. In the first phase they conducted a Delphi Survey and held a 2-day consensus conference regarding the core domains to be assessed in clinical pain trials. The second phase was the commissioning of two independent systematic reviews to identify reliable and valid pain intensity measures; one on self-report measure and the other on behavioral observation measures for children 3–18 years of age (Refs. [12 and 13] respectively). On the other hand, the SPP results were based on the Society Pediatric Pain Assessment Task Force recommendations, a survey of members of the Society Pediatric Pain listserv, and a search of literature. Despite these methodological differences in selecting scales for review, both groups used the same criteria to evaluate the quality of measures included in the reviews. These criteria combine appropriate demonstration of the psychometric properties of the measure with more practical considerations (i.e., accurate and precise presentation of the measure) in an attempt to operationalize evidence-based assessment. These criteria were suggested by Cohen et al. [15]. Finally, PedIMMPACT excluded from their reviews measures designed and tested exclusively for children younger than three years, and Cohen et al. [15] did not. Table 1 lists all single item pain intensity measures identified by both groups with some descriptive information. However, we will describe in more detail those measures that receive ratings of “well established” by both working groups at this time. For a review of well-validated disease-specific pain intensity measures, see Ref. [11]. Type of pain intensity measures Before describing pain intensity measures with wellestablished psychometric properties recommended by both groups it is important to mention that there are three main approaches to the assessment of intensity of pain in children: physiological, behavioral, and self-report (for other previous extended reviews, see Refs. [48,49]). There are a number of
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
physiological measures of pain, for example: heart and respiratory rate, blood pressure, and oxygen saturation. Despite the lack of response bias and the apparent objectivity, no physiological measures have been shown to
Table 1 Details of single item self-report measures of pediatric pain intensity identified by both working groups Evidence of reliability and validity Name of the scale (acronym)
Ped-IMMPACT Stinson et al. [12]
Analogue Chromatic Continuous Scale [16] Body Outline [17] Children's Anxiety and Pain Scales [18] Children's Global Rating Scale [19] Colour Analogue Scale [20] Eland Colour Tool [21] Facial Expression Scale [22] Faces Scale [23] Faces Scale [24] Faces Scale [25] Faces Scale [26] Faces Pain Scale and Faces Pain Scale-Revised (FPS, FPS-R) a [27,28] Finger Span [29] Graphic Numerical Rating Scale [30] Glasses Rating Scale [30] Linear analogue pain scale [31] Multiple Size Poker Chip Tool [32] Numerical Rating Scale Thermometer [33] The Oucher-Photographic and Numerical Rating Scale a [34] Pain Ladder [35] Pieces of Hurt Tool a [36] Scheffield Children's Hospital Pain Tool [37] Smiley Analogue Scale [38] Sydney Animated Facial Expression Scale [39] 4-point Verbal Descriptor Scale [40] 4-point Verbal Descriptor Scale [39,41] Verbal Numerical Rating Scale [42] VAS a [43] VAS of Faces [44] Visual Analogue Toy [46] Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale [45] 5-point Word Graphic Rating Scale [47] 6-point Word Graphic Rating Scale [30]
++
Cohen et al. [11]
++ ++ – +++
+++
– + + +++
+ +++ -
be very useful. Many physical measures are not specific to pain intensity, in that they simply cannot discriminate well between the responses to pain and other forms of stress in the body, and vary according to extraneous factors. Furthermore, these physiological indices habituate over short periods of time and are thus not appropriate for the measurement of acute pain that may last over several days (e.g., postoperative) or chronic pain. Behavioral measures of pain are based on the observation of nonverbal clues about specific types of distress behaviors (e.g., vocalization, facial expression, and body movement) that have been associated with pain to estimate the intensity of the child's pain. Although these measures provide very valuable information, they are not very appropriate for assessing recurrent and chronic pain because they are vulnerable to habituation, that is, behavioral signs of chronic pain tend to dissipate as time passes, so it is difficult to observe reliably these behavioral signs in cases of chronic pain.. Behavioral and physiological measures are most commonly used in preverbal (infants) and young children younger than 3 years of age. They can be used together in composite measures such as the Premature Infant Pain Profile or separately. For a review of observational measures in children 3–18 years old, see Ref. [13], and for infants, see Ref. [50]. Self-report measures of pain are preferred over other types of pain measures for use with children capable of verbal communication. There are several advantages in using self-report measures. Pain is a subjective experience and selfreport measures ask for the individual to articulate their pain experience themselves. Moreover, they have high clinical utility since most of them are convenient to use in everyday clinical practice (e.g., easy to use, fast administration). However, self-report measures also have limitations. First,
+++
+++
+ – + + – +++ ++ +++
331
+++
++
+ +
Rating based on the criteria developed by Cohen et al. [15] to apply to
Notes to Table 1: Rating based on the criteria developed by Cohen et al. [15] to apply to evaluate assessment instruments in pediatric psychology. +++, for excellent evidence on psychometric testing (well-established assessment measure). Detailed information indicating good validity and reliability in at least 1 peer-reviewed article. ++, for good evidence on psychometric testing (approaching wellestablished assessment measure). Validity and reliability information either presented in vague terms (e.g. no statistics presented) or only moderate values presented in at least two peer-reviewed articles which might be presented by the same investigator or research team. +, for some evidence on psychometric testing (promising assessment measure). Validity and reliability information either presented in vague terms (e.g., no statistics presented) or only moderate values presented in at least one peer-reviewed article. -, for minimal psychometric testing. – no research on psychometric testing at all. a The four well-established tools can be obtained from the following sources: the FPS-R can be found at: http://painsourcebook.ca/docs/pps92. html; the Oucher can be ordered at: http://www.oucher.org/the_scales.html; the instructions on Pieces of Hurt Tool can be found at http://www. painresearch.utah.edu/cancerpain/attachb7.html; a Visual Analogue Scale can be found at: http://cme.medscape.com/content/2003/00/44/96/449675/ 449675_fig.html.
332
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
they are dependent on the child's social, cognitive and communicative competence such as his/her ability to match items, to place items in a correct series, and to listen to the instructions of the person administering the measure while looking at materials [51,52]. Second, child's reports are also influenced by their context (whom is asking the question, setting). Therefore, it is possible that children may respond in a biased fashion (e.g., minimize their pain for fear of getting needle). Finally, they may be subject to recall bias when they are used to ask children to recall there pain over prolong periods (weeks to months) [53]. Self-report measures Stinson et al. [12] identified a total of 34 unidimensional self-report measures of pain intensity for children aged between 3 and 18 years old; six of these measures met the criteria of being a “well-established measure” based on the assessment criteria developed by Cohen et al. [15]. Cohen et al. [11] reviewed a total of 8 self-report measures which are commonly used to assess pain intensity in children: 4 unidimensional measures and 4 multidimensional measures. They assigned a rating of “well-established measure” for five of these measures reviewed. Both groups agreed that the Pieces of Hurt Tool, the Faces Pain Scale-Revised, the Oucher, and the Visual Analogue Scale were the best measures available for clinical and research practice. Pieces of Hurt Tool (often referred to as the Poker Chip Tool) This tool uses four red poker chips. The chips are aligned horizontally in front of the child on the bedside table or any other firm surface. The chips are depicted as equal to pieces of hurt. One chip represents “a little bit of hurt,” and four chips represent “the most hurt the child could ever have.” In other words, the first chip means just a little hurt, the second chip means a little more hurt, the third chip means more hurt, and the fourth chip means the most hurt. The child is asked how many pieces of hurt he/she has and the number of chips selected is recorded. This tool was initially developed and tested to use with children aged between 4–6 years old [36]. However, after this initial work this tool has been used for a wider age range (i.e., for 3–18 years old) [54]. The Pieces of Hurt Tool has been established to have sound psychometric properties. Test-retest correlations have showed score stability over short period of time in cases of acute pain [45] and [55]. Scores on Pieces of Hurt Tool have also correlated with many other indicators of pain intensity such as the Faces Pain Scale, behavioral reactions to painful stimulus, observers' global judgments, the Oucher, visual analogue scale, and the Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale [35,36,39,54–56]. Despite these results in favor of the convergent construct validity of the Pieces of Hurt Tool, by comparing distribution of scores on several self-reports of
pain intensity, Goodenough et al. [39] suggested that scores on Pieces of Hurt Tool could be inflated when younger children between 4 and 6 years old rated short sharp (needle) pain. Finally, the Pieces of Hurt Tool has also shown to be sensitive to expected changes in pain levels after surgery [55] and administration of analgesics [56] and [39]. Furthermore, the Pieces of Hurt Tool has been translated and adapted to other languages including Spanish, Thai [22], and Arab [54]. Although this tool is widely recognized as an appropriate to be used for preschool–age children, asking “how many pieces of hurt” presupposes that the child is able to count and estimate quantity and younger children might not have acquired these cognitive skills yet. Therefore, we should further assess the appropriateness of the measure when used for younger children. Faces pain scale-revised The Faces Pain Scale-Revised [28] (www.painsourcebook.ca) uses facial expressions to assess pain intensity. The child is asked to select the face that best reflects the intensity of the pain he/she has from a series of faces depicting different levels of pain intensity in a horizontal orientation. This measure was adapted from the Faces Pain Scale (FPS) [27]. The FPS was revised in order to make it consistent with the 0-10 common metric [57] Specifically, the FPS-R is based on six faces instead of seven faces that were the number of faces used in its original version so that a numeric value from 0 to 10 (0–2–4–6–8–10) can be assigned to each face. The end points are explained as “no pain” and “very much pain.” The FPS or FPS-R has several advantages over other existing faces scales (see reviews [18,49,58]). First, it has no smiling and/or tearful faces, which is relevant considering that scales that use a series of faces with expressions from smiling faces (no pain) to tearful (very much pain) can confound the affective component (distress) and the sensory component of pain (pain intensity) [58,59]. Second, it has the advantage of being suitable for use with the most widely used scoring metric (0–10) [28]. Third, besides having a true 0 point, the intervals on the scale are equal [60]. Finally, the instructions have been translated into more than 32 languages (http://painsourcebook.ca). The psychometric properties of this scale have been widely explored. The FPS-R has shown to be reliable and valid for use in normal and clinical pediatric populations aged between 4 and 16 years old. Stability reliability has been evaluated using test-retest technique with different time interval. For example, Miró and Huguet [61] reported stability coefficients for a month retesting period with a schoolchildren sample to be appropriate. Participating school children were asked to imagine themselves in several hypothetical painful situations and estimate pain intensity. Perrot et al. [62] also reported stability coefficients rather high for one and two days retesting period with a clinical sample who had undergone surgery. The FPS-R has also shown to have moderate to good convergent construct
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
validity when FPS-R is compared with other well-established self-report pain intensity measures such as visual analogue scales, the Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale (e.g., Refs. [28,63,64]) and with other well-established observational measures such as Children's Hospital of Eastern Ontario Pain Scale [65]. The correlation between FPS-R and the Facial Affective Scale, which assess the affective dimension of pain, has also been shown to be sufficiently low as to provide some evidence of the discriminant construct validity of the FPS-R [61]. The criterion-related validity of the FPS-R has also been supported using as a criterion for example affective state [61]. Moreover, the FPS-R has also shown favorable sensitivity in identifying changes in pain intensity for example, after the administration of an anesthetic during a painful procedure [66]. The FPS-R has high clinically utility as it is easy to administer, is available free of charge (www. painsourcebook.ca) and readily photocopied. Furthermore, faces scales are preferred by both children and nurses over other self-report measures such as VAS (e.g., Ref. [45]). The Oucher-Photographic And Numerical Rating Scale There are two vertical pain scales on the Oucher: (a) a numerical rating scale of 0–10 for older children and (b) a color photographic scale of child's faces with different pain expressions for younger children. Therefore, this measure can be used with children from 3 to 12 years of age. The Oucher permits older children (from 8 to 12 years old) to rate their pain intensity by picking the number from 0 (“no hurt”) to 10 (“the biggest hurt you could ever have”) that is like the hurt that they are having when they are assessed. The Faces Pain scale also permits younger children (from three to seven years old) to rate their pain intensity by matching it to one of the six photographs of faces depicting increasing levels of pain. A real child (3 years of age) is depicted in the photographs and the child's face in the bottom photograph shows no hurt and in the top photograph shows the biggest hurt you could ever have. The original Oucher was initially developed to be used for Caucasian children; however, other ethnic versions of the photographic scale of faces have been developed [67]. Specifically, under the assumption that verbal pain expression may be affected by cultural context (e.g., Ref. [68]), they have used photographs of faces of Caucasian, Hispanic, and African-American children to use this scale for Caucasian, Hispanic, and African-American child population. However, this contrasts with some preliminary evidence in the literature against significant differences in pain expression by ethnicity [69]. There is evidence to support the psychometric properties of the numerical and photographic scales of the Oucher versions. For example, Beyer and Aradine [34] provided evidence supporting the content validity of the photographic scale of the original Caucasian version in children aged between 3 and 7 years recruited from daycare and hospital
333
settings. Overall the participating children were able to discriminate the facial expressions of the photographs. Participating children were able to sequence quite well the six photographs from no hurt to the biggest hurt, although the youngest children displayed a poorer performance. However, as the authors acknowledged the task performance might not only depend upon age, it can be effected by a range of other influences. The African-American and Hispanic versions have also evidence of content validity [70]. The test-retest reliability of the Oucher versions has also been studied by several teams. For example, Luffy and Grove [71] showed high test-retest reliability of the African-American version when children with sickle cell anemia disease were asked to recall and rate the pain intensity of painful procedure/treatment in a minimum of 15 min between the test and retest. Several other groups of researchers have also demonstrated the construct validity of the Oucher using similar methods. Strong positive correlations were found between the Oucher score and other pain intensity measures for children including well-established tools such as Pieces of Hurt Tool, Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale, and Visual Analogue Scale and through a weak correlation between the Oucher score and fear measures (for validity testing of the Caucasian version; see, e.g., Refs. [56,72], and for validity testing of the African American and Hispanic versions, see, e.g., Refs. [71,73]). Several authors have also demonstrated that the Oucher is able to detect change in pain intensity levels following the administration of analgesics [72,73]. However, the Oucher requires purchase or costly color printing and requires disinfecting between use with patients in a hospital setting, which may diminish its clinical utility in some settings. Visual Analogue Scale The Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) is usually a horizontal line, 100 mm in length, anchored at each end by nonstandardized verbal descriptors which represent extreme limits of pain intensity (e.g., upper scale anchors: “most pain” [20], “pain as bad as it could be” [33]). The child is asked to place a mark on the line at a point which they feel that represents the intensity of their pain. The VAS is scored by measuring from no pain anchor to the point selected on the scale. Due to the conceptual complexity required to understand VAS, this measure is recommended for children 8 years of age and older. The VAS has also been presented as a vertical scale [74] and, less often, as a chromatic scale (e.g., Color Analogue Scale, [20]). The psychometric properties of the VAS have been the most widely explored. Several authors have showed the reliability and validity of the VAS by means of test-retest reliability, construct validity, and criterion-related validity. For instance, in terms of reliability, VAS scores demonstrate to be stable over a 2-week period [75]. In terms of construct validity, VAS scores correlate well with other measures of pain intensity such as the standardized color analog scale and
334
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
the Faces Pain Scale and Oucher (e.g., [39,56,76,77]). And, in terms of criterion-related validity, the VAS scores also correlate with clinical characteristics typically associated with pain [43,55,78]. The VAS is also sensitive to changes in pain levels during postoperative periods [77] or following administration of analgesics [77,79,80]. However, importantly, there are inconsistent findings in the literature regarding the youngest age at which a child is able to use accurately the VAS. While some authors find the VAS to be an appropriate measure for use by children aged five years and older [33,75], others only advise to use VAS for children aged seven years old onwards [56,81]. As recommended by Shields et al. [81] clinicians and researchers should be cognizant of child's cognitive abilities which have been found to better predict the successful performance in the use of the VAS than chronological age. There is also controversy surrounds whether the VAS score is ratio or ordinal (see, e.g., [75]). In terms of clinical utility, the Coloured Analogue Scale has to be purchased and cleaned as an appropriate infection control precaution in hospital; however, the article VAS is readily photocopied and free for use. Conclusions Particular attention has been paid to the area of pediatric pain assessment over the last years. Several events have contributed to elevate the attention towards the pediatric pain assessment among health care professionals. Among the most important events we must highlight: overcoming several popular beliefs and myths surrounding pediatric pain (e.g., children, especially infants do not feel pain the way adults do, pain builds character), adding pain as the 5th vital sign by the American Pain Society, the declaration of 2000-2010 as the Decade of Pain Control and Research made by the US Congress [82]; and recent advances in our knowledge of pediatric pain including knowledge of the developmental neurobiology of pain processing (e.g., Ref. [83]), knowledge of negative effects that unrecognized and untreated pain can have on the child (e.g., Ref. [84]), and increased availability of evidence-based guidelines for the management of pain in children (e.g., Ref. [85]). All these multiple factors have contributed to the proliferation of ageappropriate measurements of pain intensity. In this chapter, we have described the best evaluated and commonly used self-report measures of pediatric pain intensity based on the work done by Ped-IMMPACT group and the SPP. In order to assess pain intensity in acute and chronic pain clinical trials, Ped-IMMPACT recommended the use of the Pieces of Hurt Tool for children aged between 3 and 4 years, the Faces Pain Scale-Revised for children aged between 4 and 12 years, and the Visual Analogue Scale for children 8 years of age upwards. However, Stinson et al. [12] also acknowledged the Oucher and Wong-Baker FACES Pain Rating Scale as measures that have well-established evidence of reliability and validity. On the other hand, Cohen et al. [11], on
behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology recommended to professionals the use the Pieces of Hurt Tool for children between 4 and 7 years old, the Faces Pain Scale-Revised for children aged between 4 and 16 years old, the Visual Analogue Scale for children 3 years of age upwards, and the Oucher for children aged between 3 and 12 years old as single item self-reports. Therefore, in general, the comparison of reviews conducted by both groups lead to similar results with only a few discrepancies which are due to the different approach and methodology they used. Although it is beyond the scope of this review to exhaustively discuss future research areas, among areas which merit attention for clinical research on self-report pain intensity measures are the following ones (see Ref. [86] for a further discussion of these issues): ⁎ To integrate the utilization of well-established pain assessment tools in the clinical practices in a consistent manner, paying special attention to how pain scores changes over time within individuals. ⁎ To enhance prospective records of pain intensity by creative applications of these well-established existing tools such as the newly developed handheld electronic pain diaries used for recurrent or chronic pain (see Ref. [87,88]). ⁎ To demonstrate the impact of pain intensity assessment in the practice setting. ⁎ To provide culturally appropriate tools, that is, to adapt and test the existing well-established tools in other cultures. ⁎ To evaluate the psychometric properties of existing measures which have not been rated as well-established yet. For example, the numerical rating scale (NRS) is the most commonly used self-report measure in the clinical practice despite the lack of existing evidence on its reliability, validity and ability to detect change [89]. The NRS is administered by asking the child to verbally estimate his/her pain level on a 0-10 pain scale, with 0 representing no pain and 10 representing the worst pain. The NRS is administered verbally without any printed or physical materials that enhances its clinical utility. Several researchers are working on studies which will provide evidence for the validity and reliability of the -point (0–10) numerical rating scale [90]. ⁎ To integrate knowledge of the context when we assess pain intensity to make sense of the pain intensity scores. ⁎ To design and validate screening methods which allow professionals identify those children who are able to provide meaningful self-reports and those who are not (see, e.g., Ref. [91]), and to investigate whether those who are not able to provide meaningful self-reports can be trained. ⁎ To learn more about the ability of children with developmental and communicative impairments to use self-reports to guide the work with these children [92].
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
Acknowledgments The authors express their appreciation to Dr. Carl von Baeyer for his comments and suggestions on last versions of this article. Anna Huguet's work is supported by a postdoctoral grant from the Pain in Child Health: A CIHR Strategic Training Initiative in Health Research. Jennifer Stinson's work is supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research PostDoctoral Fellowship and Career Enhancement Award from Canadian Child Health Clinician Scientist Program. Dr. McGrath's work is supported by a Canada Research Chair. References [1] Merskey H, Bogduk N. Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. 2nd ed. Seattle (Wash): IASP Press, 1994. [2] Turk DC, Meichenbaum D, Genest M. Pain and behavioral medicine. A cognitive-behavioral perspective. New York (NY): Guilford Press, 1983. [3] Craig KD, Korol CT, Pillai RR. Challenges of judging pain in vulnerable infants. Clin Perinatol 2002;29:445–57. [4] American Pain Society. Pediatric chronic pain. Glenview (Ill): American Pain Society, 2001. [5] Schechter NL, Zempsky WT, Cohen LL, et al. Pain reduction during pediatric immunizations: evidence-based review and recommendations. Pediatircs 2007;119:e1184–98. [6] Perquin CW, Hazebroek-Kampschreur AAJM, Hunfeld JAM, et al. Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience. Pain 2000;87: 51–8. [7] Huguet A, Miró J. The severity of chronic pediatric pain: an epidemiological study. J Pain 2008;9:226–36. [8] Mitchell A, Boss BJ. Adverse effects of pain on the nervous systems of newborns and young children: a review of the literature. J Neurosci Nurs 2002;34:228–36. [9] Eccleston C, Morley S, Williams A, et al. Systematic review of randomised controlled trials of psychological therapy for chronic pain in children and adolescents, with a subset meta-analysis of pain relief. Pain 2002;99:157–65. [10] McGrath PJ, Walco G, Turk DC, et al. Core outcome domains and measures for pediatric acute and chronic/recurrent pain clinical trials: PedIMMPACT recommendations. J Pain 2008;9:771–83. [11] Cohen LL, Lemanek K, Blount RL, et al. Evidence-based assessment of pediatric pain. J Pediatr Psychol 2008;33:939–55. [12] Stinson JN, Kavanagh T, Yamada J, et al. Systematic review of the psychometric properties, interpretability and feasibility of self-report pain intensity measures for use in clinical trials in children and adolescents. Pain 2006;125:143–5. [13] von Baeyer C, Spagrud LJ. Systematic review of observational (behavioral) measures of pain for children and adolescents aged 3 to 18 years. Pain 2007;127:140–50. [14] Johnston CC. Psychometric issues in the measurement of pain. In: Finley GA, McGrath PJ, editors. Measurement of pain in infants and children. Seattle: IASP Press, 1998. p. 5–21. [15] Cohen LL, La Greca AM, Blount RL, et al. Introduction to special issue: evidence-based assessment in pediatric psychology. J Pediatr Psychol 2008;33:911–5. [16] Grossi E, Borghi C, Cerchiara E, et al. Analogue continuous chromatic scale (ACCS): a new method for pain assessment. Clin Exp Rheumatol 1983;1:337–40. [17] O'Donnell PJ, Curley H. Validation of a nonverbal instrument for pain location descriptions in children. Percept Mot Skills 1985;60: 1010.
335
[18] Kuttner L, LePage T. Faces scales for the assessment of pediatric pain. A critical review. Can J Behav Sci 1989;21:198–209. [19] Carpenter P. New method for measuring young children's self-report of fear and pain. J Pain Symptom Manage 1990;5:233–40. [20] McGrath PA, Seifert CE, Speechley KN, et al. A new analogue scale for assessing children's pain: an initial validation study. Pain 1996;64: 435–43. [21] Eland J. Minimizing pain associated with prekindergarten intramuscular injections. Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs 1981;5:361–72. [22] Suraseranivongse S, Montapaneewat T, Monon J, et al. Crossvalidation of a self-report scale for postoperative pain in school-aged children. J Med Assoc Thai 2005;88:412–8. [23] LeBaron L, Zeltzer L. Assessment of acute pain and anxiety in children and adolescents by self-reports, observer reports, and a behaviour checklist. J Consult Clin Psychol 1984;52:729–38. [24] Douthit J. Patient care guidelines: psychological assessment and management of pediatric pain. J Emerg Nurs 1990;16:168–70. [25] Maunuksela EL, Olkkola KT, Korpela R. Measurement of pain in children with self-reporting and behavioral assessment. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1987;42:137–41. [26] Tree-Trakarn T, Pirayavaraporn S, Lertakyamanee J. Topical analgesia for relief of post-circumcision. Anesthesiology 1987;67:395–9. [27] Bieri D, Reeve R, Champion G, et al. The faces pain scale for the selfassessment of the severity of pain experienced by children: development, initial validation, and preliminary investigation for ratio scale properties. Pain 1990;41:139–50. [28] Hicks CL, von Baeyer CL, Spafford PA, et al. The Faces Pain ScaleRevised: toward a common metric in pediatric pain measurement. Pain 2001;93:173–83. [29] Franzen OG, Ahlquist ML. The intensive aspect of information processing in the intradental A-delta system in man—a psychophysiological analysis of sharp dental pain. Behav Brain Res 1989;33: 1–11. [30] Whaley L, Wong D. Nursing care of infants and children. 3rd ed. St. Louis: The C.V. Mosby Company, 1987. p. 1070. [31] Broadmann L, Rice L, Hannallah R. Evaluation of an objective pain scale for infants and children. Reg Anesth 1988;13:45. [32] St-Laurent-Gagnon T, Bernard-Bonnin AC, Villeneuve E. Pain evaluation in preschool children and by their parents. Acta Paediatr 1999;88:422–7. [33] Szyfelbein S, Osgood P, Carr D. The assessment of pain and plasma Bendorphin immunoactivity in burned children. Pain 1985;22:173–82. [34] Beyer J, Aradine C. Content validity of an instrument to measure young children's perceptions of the intensity of their pain. J Pediatr Nurs 1986;1:386–95. [35] Hester N, Foster R, Kristensen K. Measurement of pain in children: generalizability and validity of the pain ladder and Pieces of Hurt tool. Adv Pain Res Ther 1990;15:79–84. [36] Hester N. The preoperational child's reaction to immunization. Nurs Res 1979;28:250–5. [37] Goddard J, Pickup S. Postoperative pain in children. Anaesthesia 1996; 51:588–90. [38] Pothmann R. Comparison of the visual analog scale (VAS) and a smiley analog scale (SAS) for the evaluation of pain in children. Adv Pain Res Ther 1990;15:95–9. [39] Goodenough B, Addicoat L, Champion GD, et al. Pain in 4- to 6-yearold children receiving intramuscular injections: a comparison of the Faces Pain Scale with other self-report and behavioral measures. Clin J Pain 1997;13:60–73. [40] Bernston L, Svensson E. Pain assessment in children with juvenile chronic arthritis: a matter of scaling and rater. Acta Paediatr 2001;90: 1131–6. [41] Goodenough B, Piira T, von Baeyer CL, et al. Comparing six self report measures of pain intensity in children. The Suffering Child 2005;8:1–25, www.usask.ca/childpain/research/6scales/6scales.pdf. [42] Vetter T. Pediatric patient-controlled analgesia with morphine versus meperidine. J Pain Symptom Manage 1992;7:204–8.
336
A. Huguet et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 68 (2010) 329–336
[43] Scott P, Ansell B, Huskisson E. Measurement of pain in juvenile chronic polyarthritis. Ann Rheum Dis 1977;36:186–7. [44] Barretto EP, Ferreira E, Pordeus IA. Evaluation of toothache severity in children using a visual analogue scale of faces. Pediatr Dent 2004;26: 485–91. [45] Wong D, Baker C. Pain in children: comparison of assessment scales. Pediatr Nurs 1988;14:9–17. [46] White J, Stow P. Rationale and experience with visual analogue toys. Anaesthesia 1985;40:601–3. [47] Tesler MD, Savedra MC, Holzemer WL, et al. The word graphic rating scale as a measure of children's and adolescents' pain intensity. Res Nurs Health 1991;14:361–71. [48] Finley GA, McGrath PJ. Measurement of pain in infants and children. Seatlle: IASP Press, 1998. [49] McGrath PA, Gillespie J. Pain assessment in children and adolescents. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, 2001. p. 97–119. [50] Duhn LJ, Medves JM. A systematic integrative review of infant pain assessment tools. Adv Neonatal Care 2004;4:126–40. [51] Besenski LJ, Forsyth SJ, von Baeyer CL. Screening young children for their ability to use self-report pain scales. Pediatric Pain Letter 2007;9: 1–6, www.childpain.org/ppl. [52] von Baeyer CL, Forsyth SJ, Stanford EA, et al. Response biases in preschool children's ratings of pain in hypothetical situations. Eur J Pain 2009;13:209–13. [53] Gill LJ, Shand PAX, Fuggle P, et al. Pain assessment for children with sickle cell disease: improved validity of diary keeping versus interview ratings. Br J Health Psychol 1997;2:131–40. [54] Gharaibeh M, Abu-Saad H. Cultural validation of pediatric pain assessment tools: Jordanian perspective. J Transcult Nurs 2002;13:12–8. [55] Beyer J, Aradine C. Patterns of pediatric pain intensity: a methodological investigation of a self-report scale. Clin J Pain 1987;3:130–41. [56] Beyer J, Aradine C. The convergent and discriminant validity of a selfreport measure of pain intensity for children. Children's Health Care 1988;16:274–82. [57] von Baeyer CL, Hicks CL. Support for a common metric for pediatric pain intensity scales. Pain Res Manag 2000;5:157–60. [58] Champion GD, Goodenough B, von Baeyer C, et al. Measurement of pain by self-report. In: Finley GA, McGrath PJ, editors. Measurement of pain in infants and children. Seattle (Wash): IASP Press, 1998. p. 123–60. [59] Chambers CT, Craig KD. An intrusive impact of anchors in children's faces pain scales. Pain 1998;78:27–37. [60] Spagrud LJ, Piira T, von Baeyer CL. Children's self-report of pain intensity: the Faces Pain Scale-Revised. Am J Nurs 2003;103:62–4. [61] Miró J, Huguet A. Evaluation of reliability, validity, and preference for a pediatric pain intensity scale: the Catalan version of the Faces pain scale-revised. Pain 2004;111:59–64. [62] Perrott DA, Goodenough B, Champion GD. Children's ratings of the intensity and unpleasantness of post-operative pain using facial expression scales. Eur J Pain 2004;8:119–27. [63] Migdal M, Chudzynska-Pomianowska E, Vause E, et al. Rapid, needlefree delivery of lidocaine for reducing the pain of venipuncture among pediatric patients. Pediatrics 2005;115:e393–8. [64] Newman CJ, Lolekha R, Limkittikul K, et al. A comparison of pain scales in Thai children. Arch Dis Child 2005;90:269–70. [65] Cassidy KL, Reid GJ, McGrath PJ, et al. Watch needle, watch TV: an audiovisual distraction in preschool immunization. Pain Med 2002;3: 108–18. [66] Taddio A, Soin HK, Schuh S, et al. Liposomal lidocaine to improve procedural success rates and reduce procedural pain among children: a randomized controlled trial. Can Med Assoc J 2005;172:1691–5. [67] Beyer J, Denyes M, Villarruel A. The creation, validation, and continuing development of the Oucher: a measure of pain intensity in children. J Pediatr Nurs 1992;7:335–46. [68] Adams JA. Methodological study of pain assessment in Anglo and Hispanic children with cancer. In: Tyler DC, Krane EJ, editors. Advances
[69]
[70] [71]
[72]
[73]
[74] [75]
[76]
[77] [78]
[79]
[80] [81]
[82] [83] [84]
[85] [86] [87]
[88]
[89] [90]
[91]
[92]
in pain research and therapy, Vol. 15. New York: Raven Press, 1990. p. 43–51. Schiavenato M, Byers JF, Scovanner P, et al. Neonatal pain facial expression: evaluating the primal face of pain. Pain 2008;138: 460–71. Villarruel A, Denyes M. Pain assessment in children: theoretical and empirical validity. ANS Adv Nurs Sci 1991;14:32–41. Luffy R, Grove SK. Examining the validity, reliability, and preference of three pediatric pain measurement tools in African-American children. Pediatr Nurs 2003;29:54–9. Aradine C, Beyer J, Tompkins J. Children's pain perceptions before and after analgesia: A study of instrument construct validity. J Pediatr Nurs 1987;3:1–23. Beyer JE, Knott CB. Construct validity estimation for the AfricanAmerican and Hispanic versions of the Oucher Scale. J Pediatr Nurs 1998;13:20–31. Beyer JE, Knapp TR. Methodological issues in the measurement of children's pain. Child Health Care 1986;14:233–41. McGrath P, de Veber L, Hearn M. Multidimensional pain assessment in children. Paper presented at the Advances in pain research and therapy: proceedings from the 4th world congress on pain. Seattle, WA, 1985. Bailey B, Bergeron S, Gravel J, et al. Comparison of four pain scales in children with acute abdominal pain in a pediatric emergency department. Ann Emerg Med 2007;50:379–83. Tyler D, Tu A, Douthit J, et al. Toward validation of pain measurement tools for children: a pilot study. Pain 1993;52:301–9. Schanberg L, Keefe F, Lefebvre J, et al. Social context of pain in children with juvenile primary fibromyalgia syndrome: parental pain history and family environment. Clin J Pain 1998;14:107–15. Romsing J, Moller-Sonnergaard J, Hertel S, et al. Postoperative pain in children: comparison between ratings of children and nurses. J Pain Symptom Manage 1998;11:42–6. Abu-Saad H, Holzemer W. Measuring children's self-assessment of pain. Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs 1981;5:337–49. Shields B, Palermo T, Powers J, et al. Predictors of a child's ability to use a visual analogue scale. Child Care Health Dev 2003;29: 281–90. Nelson R. Decade of pain control and research gets into gear in USA. Lancet 2003;362:1129. Fitzgerald M, Beggs S. The neurobiology of pain: developmental aspects. Neuroscientist 2001;7:246–57. Taddio A, Katz J, Ilersich AL, et al. Effect of neonatal circumcision on pain response during subsequent routine vaccination. Lancet 1997;349: 599–603. Schechter NL. From the ouchless place to comfort central: the evolution of a concept. Pediatrics 2008;122(Suppl 3):S154–60. von Baeyer CL. Children's self-report of pain intensity: what we know, where we are headed. Pain Res Manag 2009;14:39–45. Palermo TM, Valenzuela D, Stork PP. A randomized trial of electronic versus paper pain diaries in children: impact on compliance, accuracy, and acceptability. Pain 2004;107:213–9. Stinson JN, Petroz GC, Tait G, et al. e-Ouch: usability testing of an electronic chronic pain diary for adolescents with arthritis. Clin J Pain 2006;22:295–305. Jacob E, Puntillo KA. A survey of nursing practice in the assessment and management of pain in children. Pediatr Nurs 1999;25:278–86. von Baeyer CL, Spagrud LJ, McCormick JC, et al. Three new datasets supporting use of the Numerical Rating Scale (NRS-11) for children's self-reports of pain intensity. Pain 2009;143:223–7. McGrath PA. Pain assessment in children—a practical approach. In: Tyler DC, Krane EJ, editors. Advances in pain research therapy, Vol. 15. New York: Raven Press, 1990. p. 5–30. Fanurik D, Koh JL, Harrison RD, et al. Pain assessment in children with cognitive impairment. An exploration of self-report skills. Clin Nurs Res 1998;7:103–19.