Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation

Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation

Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevi...

380KB Sizes 0 Downloads 38 Views

Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph

Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation Liron Jacobson a, Adi Ezra a, Uri Berger b, Michal Lavidor a,b,c,⇑ a

The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel Department of Psychology, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel c Department of Psychology, University of Hull, UK b

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Accepted 19 September 2011 Available online 12 October 2011 Keywords: ADHD Behavioral inhibition EEG tDCS Theta rIFG Response inhibition Cognitive control Electroencephalography

h i g h l i g h t s  EEG recordings were compared following anodal and sham right inferior frontal gyrus-tDCS.  A significant and selective diminution of theta band was evident in the rIFG area.  rIFG stimulation improved behavioral inhibition as well as its electrophysiological correlates.

a b s t r a c t Objective: Studies have mainly documented behavioral changes induced by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), but recently cortical modulations of tDCS have also been investigated. Our previous work revealed behavioral inhibition modulation by anodal tDCS over the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG); however, the electrophysiological correlates underlying this stimulation montage have yet to be established. The current work aimed to evaluate the distribution of neuronal oscillations changes following anodal tDCS over rIFG coupled with cathodal tDCS over left orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) using spectral power analysis. Methods: Healthy subjects underwent sham and real tDCS (15 min, 1.5 mA, anodal rIFG; cathodal lOFC) stimulation conditions in a single-blind, placebo-controlled cross-over trial. Following tDCS session, resting EEG recordings were collected during 15 min. Results: Analysis showed a significant and selective diminution of the power of theta band. The theta diminution was observed in the rIFG area (represented the anode electrode), and was not found in the lOFC area (represented the cathode electrode). A significant effect was observed only in the theta but not in other bands. Conclusions: These results are the first demonstration of modulating oscillatory activity as measured by EEG with tDCS over rIFG in general, and documenting theta band reduction with this montage in particular. Significance: Our results may explain the improvement in behavioral inhibition reported in our previous work, and although this study was conducted with healthy subjects, the findings suggest that tDCS may also modulate electrophysiological changes among ADHD patients, where decreasing theta activity is the target of neuro-feedback methods aimed to improve cognitive control. Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Investigating the effect of a weak electrical current passing through the human’s scalp is now common. In order to understand its effects, researchers have investigated both the behavioral and electrophysiological changes induced by stimulation. Recording electrophysiological changes is of major importance as it enables ⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan 52900, Israel. Tel.: +972 3 5318171; fax: +972 3 5352184. E-mail address: [email protected] (M. Lavidor).

the understanding of the direct effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the human brain. The combination of electrophysiological and behavioral data may be the basis for the use of tDCS as a therapeutic tool for psychiatric disorders characterized by abnormalities in these electrophysiological and behavioral parameters. Electrophysiological changes in EEG oscillations following tDCS have been previously documented. The first to conduct such a study applied the tDCS electrodes over motor areas; Ardolino et al. (2005) reported that cathodal stimulation of the motor cortex increased the power of delta and theta rhythms, while Polania et al.

1388-2457/$36.00 Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.016

980

L. Jacobson et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

(2011) reported that following anodal stimulation over the primary motor cortex (M1), functional connectivity patterns significantly increased within the premotor, motor, and sensory motor areas of the stimulated hemisphere during motor activity. Using other imaging methods, Alon et al. (2011) reported that tDCS decreased connectivity in M1, suggesting down regulation of the motor cortex resting state after tDCS. Additionally, Lang et al. (2005) reported an increase in regional cerebral blood flow after tDCS as assessed by positron emission tomography (PET), and Baudewig et al. (2001) reported that cathodal tDCS over the sensorimotor area resulted in a global decrease of the mean number of activated pixels as assessed by blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) MRI. Electrophysiological changes following non-motor areas were also documented; Keeser et al. (2011) and Zaehle et al. (2011) have stimulated the left DLPFC and reported a significant reduction of mean current densities (sLORETA) in the delta band following anodal stimulation and changes in both theta and alpha bands activity, respectively. Keeping in mind the importance of combining data of behavioral and electrophysiological changes induced by tDCS, and in order to understand deeply the characteristic and the potential of tDCS, we followed our previous work (Jacobson et al., in press). We previously documented an improvement of behavioral inhibition following anodal stimulation over the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) and cathodal over left orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) as assessed by the stop signal task (SST), and here we conducted EEG recordings following this stimulation montage and compared it to sham tDCS. Behavioral inhibition is a key component found in several psychiatric disorders, where the difficulty to inhibit responses is considered to be the primary deficit and the major hallmark of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Barkley, 1997; Niedermeyer, 1998, 2001). There is evidence that the electrophysiological activity associated with behavioral inhibition is theta band activity. Lansbergen et al. (2007) reported that lower power of theta band was associated with a higher probability to inhibit responses. ADHD patients, marked by impaired behavioral inhibition, have been characterized by more slow wave (mainly delta and theta bands) and less fast wave activity (mainly in the alpha and beta bands) than control groups (Capute et al., 1968; Chabot and Serfontein, 1996; Clarke et al., 1998, 2001, 2002). Pharmacological approaches aimed to diminish ADHD deficits (methylphenidate; MPH) were observed to decrease the power of theta band (Clarke et al., 2003; Loo et al., 1999; Swartwood et al., 1998; Weinbruch et al., 2005). An additional approach aimed to treat ADHD is NeuroFeedback training (NF; i.e., EEG biofeedback), a method based on controlling central nervous system activity by using real-time displays of EEG, where in order to treat ADHD, the focus is decreasing the power of theta band and increasing the power of beta band (Gevensleben et al., 2009). The effects of tDCS when applied over prefrontal regions on a variety of executive functions have been broadly investigated. The tDCS literature involved behavioral inhibition (Beeli et al., 2008a; Jacobson et al., in press), planning (Dockery et al., 2009), and risk taking diminution (Beeli et al., 2008b; Fecteau et al., 2007a,b). Considering the above literature, we conclude that tDCS should be put forward as a promising new tool to improve executive function deficits; therefore investigation of the electrophysiology correlates underlying tDCS effects in a variety of executive functions is necessary and timely. The theoretical aim of the current study was to investigate the electrophysiological changes induced by anodal tDCS over rIFG. The practical implication of this investigation is the ability to test whether tDCS may serve as a therapeutic tool for behavioral inhibition based disorders such as ADHD. We previously showed that anodal tDCS over rIFG can improve healthy subjects’ ability

to inhibit responses (Jacobson et al., in press) and we currently tested for changes in the oscillatory activity linked to the behavioral changes induced by this montage of tDCS. Since it has already been suggested by Lansbergen et al. (2007) that lower theta band is associated with a higher probability to inhibit responses, and based on the above ADHD literature, our main hypothesis was that the main stimulation effect would be a reduction in the power of theta band in the rIFG area. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants The population was composed of 11 healthy participants (6 women, 5 men; mean age, 26.27 ± 4.15 years; age range 23–37 years), who volunteered to take part in the experiment in return for payment. Subjects were informed about all aspects of the experiments and all gave informed consent before taking part in the study, which was approved by the Bar Ilan Ethics committee. All of the subjects were right handed and none of them suffered from any neurological or psychiatric disorder, or had metallic implants/implanted electrical devices. All subjects stated they are not taking medication on a regular basis and they did not take any medication prior the experimental session. 2.2. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) Since our previous work showed that cathodal stimulation over rIFG did not modify behavioral inhibition as assessed by the stop signal task, in this study we compared two stimulation conditions: anodal over rIFG coupled with cathodal over lOFC, to sham tDCS. A direct current of 1.5 mA was induced by two saline-soaked surface sponge electrodes (25 cm2) and delivered for 15 min by a batterydriven, constant-current stimulator (Rolf Schneider Electronics, Germany). The same stimulation protocol employed in previous studies (Ross et al., 2010) was well tolerated, and a bigger intensity of 2 mA was found to be safe for healthy participants (Iyer et al., 2005). In the real tDCS condition (anodal), the anode electrode was positioned over the region of interest – the rIFG, and the cathode electrode served as a reference electrode, and was placed over the left orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC). Localization was established using the 10–20 EEG technique; the rIFG was identified as the crossing point between T4-Fz and F8-Cz, based on Monti et al. (2008) who localized the homologue IFG between T3-Fz and F7-Cz. The lOFC electrode was positioned above the left eyebrow (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000). In the sham condition, the current was applied for 30 s and then turned off automatically. Because most subjects feel an itching sensation only initially during tDCS, this procedure prevents awareness of the stimulation conditions (Gandiga et al., 2006; Nitsche et al., 2008). Stimulation sessions were counterbalanced and were administered at about a 1 week interval between them. 2.3. Data acquisition EEGs (ASA ANT system) were recorded for 15 min in a resting state, about 5 min after finishing the stimulation sessions (anodal, sham). This experimental design was chosen based on two top priority issues: causing less discomfort to the participants and collecting the most refined EEG data. EEGs were recorded against an average of two reference electrodes (right and left ear lobes) in the following 27 positions: Fp1, Fpz, Fp2, AF7, AF3, AFz, AF4, AF8, F7, F5, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, F6, F8, FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8 and Cz, using Ag/AgCl electrodes, electrode impedance was monitored throughout the experiment to be less than 5 kO.

L. Jacobson et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

Data was acquired at sampling rate of 2048 Hz and was filtered online with an anti-aliasing filter; a low pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 555 Hz, after acquisition data was down-sampled to 256 Hz for further analysis. Recording was conducted in a soundattenuated room where subjects sat in a comfortable reclining chair. During the EEG recording, subjects were instructed to keep their eyes closed and remain calm and alert. After the recording, participants were asked if they were alert during recording.

2.4. Data analysis and statistics Data analysis was performed offline using EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), the MATLAB 7 (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) toolbox for electrophysiological research and custom written scripts. The two raw EEG data sets (anodal, sham) for each participant were split into 1 s segments (900 segments in total). Before analysis, manual artifact detection was conducted by an unbiased analyzer over all the EEG channels; all EEG segments containing eye or muscle artifacts were excluded, an artifact in one channel lead to the exclusion of the whole 1 s segment. A fast Fourier transform (FFT) was computed on the raw EEG data, 1 s segments, multiplied by a Hamming window moving half a second at a time, which yielded the absolute power spectra for each segment. The segments were then averaged for each subject in each condition (anodal, sham), and in order to evaluate any induced changes in cortical activity, the mean spectral amplitude was obtained, in four frequency ranges: theta band frequency (4– 7 Hz), alpha band frequency (8–12 Hz), beta band frequency (13– 30 Hz) and gamma band frequency (31–45 Hz). Since our region of interest cannot be specified by a single electrode, we divided the channels into clusters, such that two of them will represent the two regions of interest, two will represent the two contralateral areas and the other clusters will be grouped based on proximity and symmetry; the 27 channels were divided into seven clusters (Fig. 1). The region of interest was the rIFG area, over which the anode electrode was positioned which is represented by the following four channels: FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8. The cathode electrode which was positioned over the lOFC is represented by the following four channels: F7, AF7, FP1, FPz. The two contralateral clusters were: [FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1; represent the contralateral area to the rIFG]; [Fp2, AF8, F8; represent the contralateral

Fig. 1. Illustration of the 27 recorded channels located over the frontal half of the scalp. The seven different colors refer to the seven different clusters of which the 27 channels were divided to. The 7 clusters were: [FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8]; [F7, AF7, FP1, FPz]; [AF3, F5, F3, F1]; [FT7, FC5, FC3, FC1]; [AFz, Fz, FCz, Cz]; [Fp2, AF8, F8]; [AF4, F2, F4, F6].

981

area to the lOFC], and the additional three clusters were: [AF3, F5, F3, F1]; [AFz, Fz, FCz, Cz]; [AF4, F2, F4, F6]. To analyze the difference between the two stimulated regions, i.e., the rIFG (anode electrode) and the lOFC (cathode electrode), we compared the channels that represent the anode electrode [FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8] to those represent the cathode electrode [F7, AF7, FP1, FPz] for each band by repeated measures ANOVA. To do that, we calculated a measurement that represents the percent change (the difference between sham to anodal divided by sham) for each channel and band, and then summed the results twice, for the two sets of four channels represent the anode and the cathode electrodes. To analyze whether effects were consistent with all four channels represent the tDCS electrodes, or perhaps derived from a single channel, an additional repeated measures ANOVA was conducted between the two tDCS conditions and the four channels for the recognized, selective and significant effect found in the initial analysis. To investigate the selectivity of the effect we conducted further t-maps for each of the clusters, for each of the four analyzed bands. We averaged the activity for each bands, each of the seven clusters and for the two tDCS conditions and conducted t-tests (corrected for multiple comparisons between anodal and sham stimulation conditions for each band and cluster.

3. Results There was no significant difference between the numbers of epochs in the anodal (659.727 ± 143.862) and sham (670.545 ± 136.400) stimulation conditions (t (11) = .276, p = .789; mean ± STD), which enables a comparison between the two tDCS conditions. The repeated measures ANOVA for the percent change [(sham-anodal)  100/sham] yielded a significant interaction (F (3, 30) = 3.378, p = .031) between band (theta, alpha, beta, gamma) and area (anode-rIFG area, cathode-lOFC area), and insignificant main effects (F (3, 30) = .640, p = .565; F (1, 10) = .294, p = .600 respectively). To define the source of interaction, post-hoc comparisons were run for each of the bands (theta, alpha, beta, gamma). The difference between the two areas (anode-rIFG area, cathode-lOFC area) for the power of theta band was significant (t (10) = 2.306, p = .044), whereas no significant differences were evident in any of the other bands (alpha: t (10) = 1.695, p = .121; beta: t (10) = .152, p = .882; gamma: t (10) = .818, p = .432). The significant effect of the power of theta band in the rIFG area was due to a reduction in the power following anodal condition compared to sham, the effect was consistent for all participants and the average (±SEM) percent change was about 27.5% (±5.88; Fig. 2a). In order to confirm that the significant effect of theta band was consistent with each of the four channels that represent the rIFG area (where the anode electrode was positioned), a repeated measures ANOVA was conducted between the two tDCS conditions (anodal, sham) and each of the four channels that represent the rIFG area (FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8). Significant main effects were yielded for tDCS conditions (F (1, 10) = 10.597, p = .009), and for channels (F (3, 8) = 25.458, p < .001), whereas the interaction was not significant (F (3, 8) = 2.914, p = .101), which confirm the consistency of the tDCS effect over the four channels in the power of theta band (Fig. 2b). T-maps were conducted to confirm that the reduction of the power of theta band was selective to the rIFG area. Analysis revealed a significant difference between anodal and sham stimulation conditions in the power of theta band in the rIFG area (t (10) = 3.37, p = .007), while no additional significant difference was found for the additional clusters, nor for the additional bands (Fig. 3).

982

L. Jacobson et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

Fig. 2. (a) The difference between the power recorded following anodal and sham stimulation conditions presented as percent change [(sham-anodal)  100/sham; mean ± SEM]; The differences are plotted for each band (theta, alpha, beta, gamma) and for two clusters representing the rIFG area, where the anode electrode was positioned (dark gray), and the lOFC, where the cahotde electrode was positioned (light gray). ⁄Indicates p < .05. (b) The power (mean ± SEM) of theta band for each of the four electrodes represent the rIFG area (FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8). ⁄Indicates p < .05, ⁄⁄indicates p < .01.

Fig. 3. T-maps representing the difference in the power for each of the four analyzed bands (theta, alpha, beta, gamma) between anodal and sham stimulation conditions. Red colors represent significant difference (p < .05); black color stands for non recorded regions.

4. Discussion In the present study we applied tDCS (anodal over rIFG; cathodal over lOFC) and recorded EEG oscillation following real stimulation and sham. Corroborating our a priori hypothesis, we demonstrated that this stimulation montage led to a significant and selective reduction among all subjects in the power of theta band in the rIFG area, as no such change was evident in the other recorded areas and none of the other analyzed bands were significantly affected by the stimulation. Additionally the effect was consistent with the four channels that represent the rIFG. The current study used a stimulation protocol that had been proved to be successful in improving behavioral inhibition (Jacobson et al., in press), and here revealed its electrophysiological correlates, which are assumed to derive from the rIFG area, as suggested by Asada et al. (1999) and Ishii et al. (1999). They suggested

that the theta rhythm may derive from prefrontal regions; however due to EEG characteristics we can only refer to electrode locations and not to brain locations. The integration of the current electrophysiology study with our previous work (Jacobson et al., in press) provides evidence that anodal tDCS over the rIFG is able to affect behavioral inhibition as well as to modulate cortical activity; i.e., the electrophysiological correlates of behavioral inhibition, and it appears that the changes in the ability to inhibit responses achieved by tDCS may account for a reduction in the power of theta band. The present study is the first to demonstrate a significant and selective reduction in the power of theta band following anodal stimulation over the rIFG compared to sham stimulation. The neuronal effect of tDCS has been investigated in a few animal-based studies. For example, Bindman et al. (1964) reported that when a positive stimulation (i.e., anode) is applied, a depolarization of the resting membrane potential occurs, which increases

L. Jacobson et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

neuronal excitability and therefore increases spontaneous cell firing. Inversely, when a negative stimulation (i.e., cathode) is applied, a hyperpolarization occurs, which decreases neuronal excitability and decreases spontaneous cell firing. This effect was afterwards replicated by Nitsche and Paulus (2000), who applied the tDCS electrodes over human motor cortex. The effects of a single session of tDCS last for a few minutes after stimulation and the assumption is that after-effects following stimulation are enabled by modulation of LTP/LTD mechanisms (Nitsche et al., 2003). To extend these data, there is still a need to use brain imaging techniques to better determine the cortical changes induced by stimulation. Although there is considerable literature on the ability of tDCS to modulate behavior when applied over the cortex, the associated changes in brain activity following stimulation have yet to be fully established. The current study contributes to the growing list of electrophysiological documented changes induced by tDCS applied over the cortex. tDCS over motor areas has been found to: induce changes in functional connectivity patterns (Alon et al., 2011; Polania et al., 2011) and in delta and theta rhythms (Ardolino et al., 2005), promote an increase in regional cerebral blood flow (Lang et al., 2005), and cathodal tDCS over sensorimotor area has resulted in a global decrease of the mean number of activated pixels (Baudewig et al., 2001). tDCS over DLPFC has been found to induce changes in current densities of delta band (Keeser et al., 2011) and in theta and alpha bands (Zaehle et al., 2011). The novelty of the current study is in the broad perspective provided by the combination of the current electrophysiology study, and our previous behavioral study applied the same montage of tDCS. Previously we demonstrated behavioral inhibition modulation (Jacobson et al., in press), and here we demonstrated an electrophysiology modulation which may be the basis underpinning the previous behavioral findings. The decrease in the power of theta band which is associated with a better ability to inhibit responses following anodal stimulation over the rIFG is consistent with the literature about ADHD, a common psychiatric disorder which is primarily characterized by a behavioral inhibition deficit (Barkley, 1997). People with ADHD, compared to control groups, have been characterized as having increased levels of theta band activity (Clarke et al., 1998, 2001; Capute et al., 1968; Chabot and Serfontein, 1996). The standard pharmacological treatment – MPH – has been found to decrease the power of theta band among ADHD patients (Clarke et al., 2003; Loo et al., 1999; Swartwood et al., 1998). Similarly, Neurofeedback Training, which is designed to decrease attention and impulsivity deficits among ADHD patients, attempts to increase beta band activity and decrease theta band activity (Gevensleben et al., 2009). Although this study was conducted with healthy subjects it may suggest that tDCS may serve as a tool to modulate theta power also in ADHD patients. The ability to modulate the power of theta band, according to the above literature, may be used as a therapeutic tool to treat impulsive based disorders such as ADHD. As up to now there is no published tDCS study conducted with ADHD patients, the current study may prompt such research. Further research is also needed to verify the long lasting after effects of tDCS as a therapeutic tool to improve behavioral inhibition in healthy and eventually impaired populations. Several limitations of the current study must be acknowledged. Only some of the possible montages of prefrontal regions were tested; in addition, it would be worthwhile to conduct EEG recordings during stimulation as well and not only off-line protocols. Nevertheless, these findings have significant theoretical and practical implications. At the theoretical level, the results suggest that cortical oscillatory changes are induced by tDCS, which may account for the observed behavioral changes. They also have practical implications for the development of interventions to benefit

983

individuals with various types of attentional deficits. For the participants in the current study who were in their twenties and thirties, the effect of stimulation, while significant, was relatively modest. It is likely that older adults or people with attentional impairments seen in conditions such as ADHD would show greater effects. We hope that in future studies the potential of tDCS with attention – challenged populations such as ADHD would be investigated. 5. Conclusion Anodal tDCS over the rIFG decreased the power of theta band in the rIFG area selectively among healthy participants. These results may suggest that the use of tDCS would be beneficial as a therapeutic tool for improving behavioral inhibition deficits in populations such as ADHD. Acknowledgments The study was supported by Grant No. 100/10 from the Israel Science Foundation. M. Lavidor is also supported by an ERC startup grant. References Alon G, Roys SR, Gullapalli RP, Greenspanc JD. Non-invasive electrical stimulation of the brain (ESB) modifies the resting-state network connectivity of the primary motor cortex: A proof of concept fMRI study. Brain res 2011;1403:37–44. Ardolino G, Bossi B, Barbier S, Priori A. Non-synaptic mechanisms underlie the aftereffects of cathodal transcutaneous direct current stimulation of the human brain. J Physiol 2005;568(2):653–663. Asada H, Fukuda Y, Tsunoda S, Yamaguchi M, Tonoike M. Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neurosci Lett 1999;274:29–32. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997;121:65–94. Baudewig J, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Frahm J. Regional modulation of BOLD MRI responses to human sensorimotor activation by transcranial direct current stimulation. Magn Reson Med 2001;45:196–201. Beeli G, Casutt G, Baumgartner T, Jancke L. Modulating presence and impulsiveness by external stimulation of the brain. Behav Brain Funct 2008a;4:33. Beeli G, Koeneke S, Gasser K, Janck L. Brain stimulation modulates driving behavior. Behav Brain Funct 2008b;4:34. Bindman LJ, Lippold OCJ, Redfearn JWT. The action of brief polarizing currents on the cerebral cortex of the rat 1(during current flow and 2) in the production of long-lasting after-effects. J Physiol 1964;172:369–82. Capute A, Niedermeyer E, Richardson F. The electroencephalogram in children with minimal cerebral dysfunction. Pediatrics 1968;41:1104–14. Chabot R, Serfontein G. Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996;40:951–63. Clarke A, Barry R, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. EEG analysis in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Res 1998;81:19–29. Clarke AR, Barry R, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. EEG differences in two subtypes of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychophysiology 2001;38:212–21. Clarke AR, Barry R, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. EEG analysis of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbid reading disabilities. J Learn Disabil 2002;35:276–85. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Brown CR, Croft RJ. Effects of stimulant medications on the EEG of children with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder predominantly inattentive type. Int J Psychophysiol 2003;47:129–37. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics. J Neurosci Methods 2004;134:9–21. Dockery CA, Hueckel-Weng R, Birbaumer N, Plewnia C. Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. J Neurosci 2009;29:7271–7. Fecteau S, Knoch D, Fregni F, Sultani N, Boggio P, Pascual-Leone A. Diminishing risktaking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. J Neurosci 2007a;27:12500–5. Fecteau S, Pascual-Leone A, Zald DH, Liguori P, Theoret H, Boggio P, et al. Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making. J Neurosci 2007b;27:6212–8. Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. J Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:845–50. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Vogel C, Schlamp D, Kratz O, et al. Is neurofeedback an efficious treatment for ADHD? a randomized controlled clinical trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009;50:780–9.

984

L. Jacobson et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 979–984

Ishii R, Shinosaki K, Ukai S, Inoue T, Ishihara T, Yoshimine T, et al. Medial prefrontal cortex generates frontal midline theta rhythm. Neuroreport 1999;10:675–9. Iyer MB, Mattu U, Grafman J, Lomarev M, Sato S, Wassermann EM. Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals. Neurology 2005;64:872–5. Jacobson L, Javitt DC, Lavidor M. Activation of inhibition: Diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the Inferior Frontal Gyrus. J Cogn Neurosci 2011 Nov;23(11):3380–7. Keeser D, Padberg F, Reisinger E, Pogarell O, Kirsch V, Palm U, et al. Prefrontal direct current stimulation modulates resting EEG and event-related potentials in healthy subjects: a standardized low resolution tomography sLORETA study. Neuroimage 2011;55:644–57. Lang N, Siebner HR, Ward NS, Lee L, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN, Frackowiak RS. How does transcranial DC stimulation of the primary motor cortex alter regional neuronal activity in the human brain? Eur J Neurosci 2005;22:495–504. Lansbergen MM, Schutter DJLG, Kenemans JL. Subjective impulsivity and baseline EEG in relation to stopping performance. Brain Res 2007;1148:161–9. Loo SK, Teale PD, Reite ML. EEG correlates of methylphenidate response among children with ADHD: a preliminary report. Biol Psychiatry 1999;45:1657–60. Monti A, Cogiamanian F, Marceglia S, Ferrucci R, Mrakic-Sposta S, Vergari M, et al. Improved naming after transcranial direct current stimulation in aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:451–3. Niedermeyer E. Frontal lobe disinhibition, Rett syndrome and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Electroencephalogr 2001;32:20–3.

Niedermeyer E. Frontal lobe functions and dysfunctions. Clin Electroencephalogr 1998;29:79–90. Nitsche MA, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Priori A, Lang N, Antal A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul 2008;1:206–23. Nitsche MA, Fricke K, Henschke U, Schlitterlau A, Liebetanz D, Lang N, et al. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol 2003;553:293–301. Nitsche MA, Paulus WJ. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. Physiology 2000;527:633–9. Polania R, Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Modulating functional connectivity patterns and topological functional organization of the human brain with transcranial direct current stimulation. Hum Brain Mapp 2011;32:1236–49. Ross L, McCoy D, Wolk DA, Coslett B, Olson IR. Improved proper name recall by electrical stimulation of the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 2010;48:3671–4. Swartwood MO, Swartwood JN, Lubar JF, Timmermann DL, Zimmerman AW, Muenchen RA. Methylphenidate effects on EEG behavior, and performance in boys with ADHD. Pediatr Neurol 1998;18:244–50. Weinbruch C, Paul I, Bauer S, Kivelitz H. The influence of methylphenidate on the power spectrum of ADHD children–an MEG study. BMC Psychiatry 2005;5:29. Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne JD, Jancke L, Herrmann CS. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance. Combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci 2011;12:2.