Journal Pre-proof Moral Motivation and the Basal Forebrain Roland Zahn, Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza, Jorge Moll
PII:
S0149-7634(19)30077-6
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.022
Reference:
NBR 3587
To appear in:
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
Received Date:
28 January 2019
Revised Date:
24 October 2019
Accepted Date:
28 October 2019
Please cite this article as: Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Moral Motivation and the Basal Forebrain, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.022
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier.
1
Moral Motivation and the Basal Forebrain Roland Zahn a,b , Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza a,c *, Jorge Moll a
a
Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience Unit, D’Or Institute for Research
& Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. b
Centre for Affective Disorders, Department of Psychological Medicine,
ro of
Division of Academic Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, United Kingdom. c
The Federal University of the State of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro,
lP
* Corresponding author
re
-p
Brazil.
Dr Jorge Moll, MD PhD
ur na
Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience Unit D’Or Institute for Research & Education (IDOR) Rua Diniz Cordeiro, 30
22281-100 Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Jo
e-mail:
[email protected].
Highlights Moral motivations drive humans to serve the needs of others and society.
2
The basal forebrain is important for enabling moral motivation.
This includes the septo-hypothalamic region, implicated in kinship bonding.
And also includes the connected subgenual frontal cortex.
Implications for prosocial behaviour and mental health are discussed.
ro of
Abstract
Moral motivations drive humans to sacrifice selfish needs to serve the needs of others and internalized sociocultural norms. Over the past two decades,
-p
several brain regions have been associated with different aspects of moral
re
cognition and behaviour. Only more recently, however, investigations have highlighted the importance of the basal forebrain for moral motivation. This
lP
includes the septo-hypothalamic region, implicated in kinship bonding across mammal species, and the closely connected subgenual frontal cortex.
ur na
Understanding the neuroanatomy of moral motivation and its impairments will be fundamental for future research aiming to promote prosocial behaviour and mental health.
Jo
Keywords:
moral emotions; Brodmann’s area 25; social cognition; septal area; attachment; hypothalamus; subgenual cingulate cortex; anterior cingulate cortex
3
1. Introduction In a time of complex and rapidly changing social identities, there is an urgent need to understand the roots of the sheer altruism of voluntary acts, as well as the ordinary manifestations of moral behaviour in everyday life. Significant advances have been made in identifying the key neural systems
ro of
responsible for moral cognition and behaviour in humans in general since our review over a decade ago(Moll et al., 2005). Despite the rapidly
expanding field of moral cognitive neuroscience(Fumagalli and Priori,
-p
2012), only recently new research has started to unveil the brain systems underpinning moral motivations (being motivated to e.g. “help an orphan
re
child”), that are important for driving moral behaviour (e.g. “donating to a
lP
charity that benefits orphan children”, Box 1). In keeping with our view of moral cognition and emotion as inextricably linked psychologically and anatomically(Moll et al., 2005), we will argue below that moral motivations
ur na
require both cognitive components which specify the goal of one’s action (e.g. “alleviating a child’s hunger”) as well as basic emotional elements (e.g. “a caring/attached emotional state”) which provide the motivational force
Jo
allowing us to overcome competing selfish motivations (e.g. “buying something for oneself”). This is in keeping with psychological models of motivation that postulate closely connected, but distinct roles of (1) goal representations and (2) the magnitude (i.e. force) of their subjective emotional value(Wigfield, 1994). Moral motivations promote both costly helping behaviours and moral punishment. These motivations are fundamental for driving moral
4 behaviour in the absence of external punishment and reward contingencies, such as norm enforcement, reputation and reciprocity concerns(Gintis et al., 2008; Nowak and Sigmund, 2005). Impaired moral motivations are typically illustrated by the behaviour of psychopathic individuals, who can play the moral rules when self-interest is at stake but fail to do so otherwise (Cleckley, 1976) , a similar pattern of behaviour is observed in healthy people with psychopathic traits (Lockwood et al., 2017).
ro of
Over the last decade, novel evidence on the neural basis of moral motivations has emerged, and as we will show in this review, has
consistently implicated a set of basal forebrain structures – namely the
-p
septo-hypothalamic and subgenual frontal areas. These structures are closely connected anatomically (Box 2). In this review, we will integrate this recent
re
evidence for the first time to elucidate the role of these regions in moral motivation, for which we only had scarce evidence in our review over a
lP
decade ago(Moll et al., 2008). We will discuss the contribution of the subcortical septo-hypothalamic area to kinship bonding across species (Moll
ur na
et al., 2012; Preston, 2013; Stack et al., 2002) and the role of the subgenual area in more complex aspects of moral motivation such as social agency, and the value of social outcomes (Moll et al., 2008; Moll et al., 2006). Crucially, these brain regions are functionally and anatomically interlocked
Jo
with those responsible for general reinforcement and social cognition (Moll et al., 2005; Preston, 2013). Moreover, recent studies addressing moral emotions and motivation bear direct relevance for a better understanding of neuropsychiatric disorders, with potential clinical applications (Decety et al., 2013; Green et al., 2012; Harrison et al., 2012).
5 After defining moral motivation, we will review the evidence for impaired moral behaviour as observed in patient lesion studies. This is because, in contrast to functional neuroimaging in healthy individuals, lesion studies are able to identify brain regions that are necessary for a specific function(Price et al., 1999). We will then review functional neuroimaging studies in healthy individuals with direct relevance for the understanding of the role of the basal forebrain in moral motivations.
ro of
Finally, we will discuss implications for the fields of neuropsychiatry and psychological wellbeing research.
-p
2. What are moral motivations?
Morality can be operationally defined as the sets of customs and values
re
that are embraced by a sociocultural group to guide social behaviour(Moll et
lP
al., 2005). This definition, however, does not specify the motivations that drive these actions(Moll et al., 2008). Only when considering the motivations behind an action, are we able to delineate the cognitive and
ur na
emotional components that are specific for moral behaviour as compared with social behaviour in general. Thus, moral motivation can be more sharply defined as being driven by the needs of other people, including kin,
Jo
or society(Zahn et al., 2011b). This definition allows for individual and cultural variability but excludes other adaptive types of social motivations (e.g., abiding by social norms to avoid reputation loss, helping others to obtain social recognition or to uphold any form of self-regarding preferences). We will use the term “prosocial” behaviour as an umbrella term to describe socially adaptive behaviours when their true motivation cannot be determined.
6 The term “moral motivation” was given prominence by eighteenth century philosophers of the Scottish Enlightenment (see Box 1, (Zahn et al., 2011b)). They highlighted the central importance of moral sentiments for providing moral motivation. Modern psychologists speak of “moral emotions” rather than sentiments and generally agree on the importance of guilt and compassion/sympathy (Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al., 2007). Stressing the subjective and complex nature of moral sentiments which
ro of
include cognitive ingredients such as causal attributions, we will refer to them as feelings. We may also use the term emotions synonymously with feelings or sentiments.
-p
As mentioned above, we conceive of moral motivation as consisting of both the motivational force and the associated goal. This definition
re
highlights two key components of moral motivation: the motivational force driving behaviour and the social knowledge about the needs of others and
lP
about sociocultural norms(Moll et al., 2008; Zahn et al., 2007), which
ur na
defines the goals entailed in moral motivations.
3. The neural architecture of moral motivations In this section, we will first review studies of patients with brain lesions and impaired moral behaviour before summarising evidence from fMRI in
Jo
healthy volunteers and in psychiatric conditions. We will focus on the involvement of basal forebrain and the partly overlapping ventromedial frontal cortex (FC) subregions, in particular on the septal, hypothalamic, and subgenual regions, which have been more consistently implicated in studies of moral decisions and feelings. We emphasize, however, that it is beyond the scope of this review to systematically report all the brain regions
7 implicated in studies relevant to moral motivation and kinship bonding, or to appraise all the studies reporting activations in the basal forebrain. Among others, two isocortical brain regions that have been implicated in abstract conceptual and sequential/prospective aspects of moral goals – the superior anterior temporal cortex (ATL) and the medial frontopolar cortex, respectively – will also be briefly discussed due to converging evidence from
3.1. Brain lesions and impaired moral behaviour
ro of
lesion and fMRI studies for their importance in moral motivations.
By demonstrating which brain regions are necessary for moral and
-p
prosocial behaviour, lesion studies provide important insights, even if they
relate to less confined anatomical territories such as the ventral frontal areas
re
which contain but are not limited to the subgenual region. The 19 th century physician Leonore Welt provided the first review of patients who exhibited
lP
changes in moral behaviour as a result of a focal brain injury(Welt, 1888),(Zahn et al., 2011a). In her article, she presented the case of a furrier
ur na
who underwent a drastic change in character after a head injury with predominant damage to the right frontal lobe. Based on the location of the injury found at autopsy in her case as well as in 12 others that she collected from the literature with similar changes in moral character, Welt concluded
Jo
that damage to the right medial orbital region was the common denominator in all cases(Zahn et al., 2015). In the 1980s, Eslinger & Damasio stimulated new interest in the neuroanatomy of impaired prosocial and moral conduct by describing EVR, a patient with a ventromedial FC injury resulting from a tumour resection(Eslinger and Damasio, 1985). A few years later, a neurodegenerative disease originally described by Pick in
8 the late 19th century became recognised as a form of early-onset dementia (frontotemporal dementia, FTD) that could reliably be diagnosed before death based on its clinical features alone(Snowden et al., 2001). Patients with FTD consistently displayed impaired prosocial behaviour(Bozeat et al., 2000), which was independently associated with atrophy of right ATL and ventromedial FC, including the subgenual region(Liu et al., 2004). Atrophy of the subgenual region was later shown to be associated with loss of
ro of
empathy(Rankin et al., 2006), affiliative feelings and interpersonal warmth(Sollberger et al., 2009). More recent studies have provided evidence that empathic concern is primarily impaired in FTD(Baez et al., 2014) and
-p
associated with posterior medial orbitofrontal atrophy (Baez et al., 2016)
close to the subgenual cortex. Loss of guilt was also reported by caregivers
re
(Koenigs et al., 2007), and associated with decreased prosocial decisions in economic games(Krajbich et al., 2009) in patients with ventromedial FC
lP
lesions that included subgenual sectors of the orbitofrontal cortex. Converging evidence from fMRI and patients with FTD linked the
ur na
ventromedial FC to the anticipation of negative consequences of social behaviour(Grossman et al., 2010). Less evidence is available on the behavioural effects of the subcortical parts of the basal forebrain in humans, but hypothalamic lesions were associated with aggressive
Jo
behaviour(Haugh and Markesbery, 1983; Weissenberger et al., 2001), although endocrine disturbances may have played a role. Further studies have begun to probe moral motivations experimentally
in FTD whilst controlling for social conceptual knowledge and viceversa(Zahn et al., 2009b),(Moll et al., 2011). These studies indicate that loss of simple interpersonal moral motivations and loss of abstract conceptual
9 social knowledge (e.g., what it means to act “tactfully”) are partly dissociable (Box 3). Septal damage in FTD was associated with diminished guilt and pity, but not embarrassment in an experimental task, whilst frontopolar damage was associated with impaired embarrassment in addition to guilt and pity(Moll et al., 2011). This showed that septal damage was associated with impairments of those moral feelings that entail empathic concern for other people, whilst frontopolar cortical damage was
ro of
associated with prosocial feelings more generally, including embarrassment which is primarily related to upholding one’s social reputation rather than
concern for others(Eisenberg, 2000). The experimental task controlled for
-p
other negative emotions such as anger and disgust, whose impairment was associated with amygdala and dorsomedial FC damage(Moll et al., 2011),
re
although it is outside of the scope of this review to consider studies of disgust and anger. These selective patterns occurred despite comparable
lP
degrees of social knowledge being required to understand the stimuli used in the task. In contrast to these associations of different moral feelings with
ur na
different frontal-subcortical lesion patterns, another study showed that FTD patients with right ATL damage displayed selective impairments of abstract conceptual social relative to non-social conceptual knowledge(Zahn et al., 2009b). These lesion studies confirmed earlier fMRI evidence of
Jo
partly dissociable representations of abstract conceptual social knowledge in the right superior ATL(Zahn et al., 2007) and different moral feelings in frontal-subcortical regions(Zahn et al., 2009c), which can independently contribute to impaired prosocial behaviour(Krajbich et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2004). These results are further consistent with the hypothesis that frontal and subcortical basal forebrain regions are necessary for core aspects of
10 moral motivations, whereas ATL regions are relevant for representing conceptual aspects of abstract moral values which can serve as goals of complex societal value-based moral motivations (Box 3). This interpretation is based on the hypothesis that moral motivations in direct interpersonal interactions such as those driven by the perception of a sad facial expression of someone in need(Moll et al., 2005) for example, do not necessarily require more complex moral goal representations such as
ro of
abstract conceptual goals (ATL). Taken together, lesions in the hypothalamus, the septal region and ventromedial parts of the frontal cortex, especially those including the
-p
subgenual cortex (BA25) and the more anterior subgenual cingulate and subgenual orbitofrontal areas, led to changes in moral behaviour which
re
would be compatible with their proposed critical role for moral motivation. Lesions to other cortical brain regions which were shown to represent goals
lP
of socio-moral behaviour, such as the long-term consequences (frontopolar cortex, (Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al., 2017)) and conceptual
ur na
quality of social behaviour (right superior ATL, (Zahn et al., 2009b)) led to changes in moral behaviour as well (Zahn et al., 2017) in keeping with the notion that motivations rely both on the motivational force as well as the goal representation (for further lesion evidence on the role of isocortical
Jo
frontal regions in moral motivation, see Box 4). In contrast, lesions to the occipital and parietal cortices, posterior
inferior temporal, as well as posterior left dorsolateral, posterior dorsomedial frontal and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices are usually not clearly associated with observable inappropriate social behaviour(Moll et al., 2005). This does not preclude an important role of these regions in socio-
11 emotional functions more generally which is beyond the scope of this review. For example, the dorsal anterior cingulate region is activated for empathic simulation (Lockwood, 2016), such as sharing others’ pain(Lamm et al., 2011), with evidence for gyral versus sulcal functional subdivisions(Apps et al., 2016). Patients with anterior cerebral artery stroke regularly including the more posterior portions of the dorsal anterior cingulate develop the syndrome of “akinetic mutism” with a general lack of
ro of
spontaneous activity and speech(Nagaratnam et al., 2004), but little evidence of selective social impairments. It is also important to note that
most ventromedial frontal subregions are necessary for prosocial behaviour,
-p
such as strategic cooperation, more generally (Melloni et al., 2016) rather than selective for moral motivation.
re
Schadenfreude and envy were outside of the scope of this review as they are usually considered as social rather than moral feelings (Jankowski and Takahashi,
lP
2014). A recent study in patients with neurodegenerative disorders, however, specifically examined these emotions in moral situations (Santamaria-Garcia et al.,
ur na
2017). Interestingly, Schadenfreude in moral situations was higher in patients with frontopolar and angular gyrus atrophy and envy in moral situations was more pronounced in patients with precuneus atrophy. Whilst interesting, it is difficult to ascertain which cognitive components of these experiences led to the described
Jo
associations.
3.2. Moral vs. self-interested choices It is well established that patients showing impaired prosocial behaviour are often able to serve their own immediate self-interests (Eslinger and Damasio, 1985). This indicates the existence of at least partly
12 separable neural correlates of moral and selfish motivations. The studies discussed so far, however, have not made this direct comparison, which is necessary to draw conclusions about selectivity. Studies of human cooperation in economic games have paved the way for investigating the neural basis of complex altruistic behaviour (Figure 2). Using an economic game format, a charity donation paradigm was employed to determine areas of selective activation for altruistic decisions, compared
ro of
with decisions associated with pure monetary gain for oneself(Moll et al., 2006). The septal and subgenual cortex and the anterior medial
orbitofrontal/frontopolar cortex showed selectivity for altruistic decisions,
-p
the latter area being more active during costly choices. Moreover, septal activations were also detected when pairs of participants relied on
re
unconditional trust in their decisions to cooperate, in contrast to ventral tegmental area activity during strategic cooperation (i.e., expectancy of
lP
mutual gains from cooperation)(Krueger et al., 2007a). The engagement of the septal area in donation behaviour has also been confirmed in more
ur na
recent studies(Harbaugh et al., 2007; Hsu et al., 2008). Importantly, septal activity was observed when healthy subjects observed sad faces when taking a compassionate perspective(Kim et al., 2009), and septal responses specifically predicted daily helping in another fMRI study(Morelli et al.,
Jo
2014).
Another neuroeconomic fMRI study demonstrated that activity in septal and anterior subgenual orbitofrontal areas tracked the levels of reported guilt associated with dishonouring trust(Chang et al., 2011). The anterior subgenual orbitofrontal region was also shown to respond to voluntary contributions in a public goods game more strongly when identical
13 outcomes were interpreted as emerging from a generous intent than when they were not (Cooper et al., 2010). Furthermore, activation in the subgenual orbitofrontal region was shown for upholding a principle of social equality in costly decisions(Zaki and Mitchell, 2011). The selectivity of subgenual areas for altruistic compared with selfish decisions was further corroborated in that individuals with higher empathic concern displayed stronger subgenual cingulate activation when deciding to
ro of
sacrifice their money to prevent others from receiving an electric shock(FeldmanHall et al., 2012) (FeldmanHall et al., 2015). Moreover,
anterior subgenual cingulate activity tracked the amounts donated in a
-p
charitable donation task(Hare et al., 2010) and correlated with individual differences in aversion to third-party harm(Wiech et al., 2013). A meta-
re
analysis comparing selfish and vicarious reward fMRI studies found, however, that right anterior subgenual cingulate activations (BA24) were
lP
both found consistently in selfish and vicarious reward studies vs. low level baseline conditions(Morelli et al., 2015). This meta-analysis did not identify
ur na
more posterior subgenual, septal or hypothalamic activations consistently for vicarious rewards as one might have expected on the basis of our review of the literature. This is not surprising, however, given that quantitative imaging meta-analyses rely on peak coordinates reported to be significant in
Jo
the cited studies and this will bias the results against basal forebrain areas which are only reliably detected using optimized fMRI parameters to improve the otherwise high rate of signal dropout due to the vicinity of the skull and sinuses(Wastling and Barker, 2015) and by including specific a priori regions of interest to improve the statistical power of detecting activations in small brain regions. This could explain why a more recent
14 meta-analysis, which modelled signal dropout, was able to detect selective activations in the subgenual cortex (BA25) for altruistic vs. selfish decisions (Cutler and Campbell-Meiklejohn, 2019). Another difference between both meta-analyses is that Morelli et al. included vicarious rewards in a nonmoral context, whereas Cutler and Campbell-Meiklejohn specifically investigated differences between moral and selfish motivations. More recent studies have investigated moral motivation-related brain
ro of
responses using computational models. Prosocial prediction errors were specifically encoded within the posterior subgenual cortex and adjacent septal area employing reinforcement learning models(Lockwood et al.,
-p
2016). In contrast, non-overlapping responses in the ventral striatum
tracked both selfish and prosocial reward prediction errors(Lockwood et al.,
re
2016). Interestingly, individuals prone to empathise had a higher learning rate in the prosocial condition and higher subgenual activity for prosocial
lP
vs. self-reward prediction errors. Another computational modelling study measured care and distress levels to narratives of human suffering (Ashar et
ur na
al., 2017), which predicted subsequent charitable donation. Empathic care was preferentially associated with septal activity. Whilst these computational modelling-based studies are in agreement with the proposed role of basal forebrain areas in moral motivation, a
Jo
recent study failed to show selective brain activations for altruistic choices to lessen others’ pain by sacrificing monetary reward vs. selfish choices (Crockett et al., 2017). Instead a large cluster of ventromedial frontal activation, strongest in the pregenual area, was common to selfish and altruistic choices; the cluster spread into the subgenual frontal area. This lack of selective activation, however, may well be due to using cluster-based
15 inference at the whole brain level, thus decreasing the likelihood of detecting activations in small basal forebrain and subgenual subregions. Another recent study failing to support the selective importance of ventromedial frontal areas for altruistic choices was conducted in Vietnam Veterans with penetrating head injuries(Moll et al., 2018). The study, however, had limited value in assessing these regions, because the number of veterans with such lesions was very low. Further limitations of this study
ro of
were that MRI scans could not be carried out due to metal injuries, and so reliance on CT scans limited the precision of lesion mapping. In addition, time post-lesion of around 40 years was exceptionally long, which meant
-p
that compensation as well as ageing-related and neurodegenerative changes were likely to have added variability to CT-detectable lesion-function
Jo
ur na
lP
re
associations.
16 3.3. Imaging the experience of moral feelings The investigation of the neural correlates of subjective experiences of moral feelings is important for our understanding of moral motivation. This is because, the previous section showed that while neuroeconomic paradigms can be used to infer neural correlates of moral motivations as long as appropriate control conditions are used, they usually fail to reveal the qualities of underlying emotional experience. For example, one may
ro of
decide to sacrifice money to benefit a social cause based on either guilt avoidance or on the positive feelings derived from such action (e.g. “warm glow”; (Andreoni, 1990)).
-p
In this section, we review research on moral feelings with a focus on
guilt and pity/compassion, given their consistent associations with altruistic
re
behaviour. We conceive of moral feelings as the subjective experience of moral motivations. Experimentally, however, fMRI studies of moral feelings
lP
often suffer from not being able to directly show which of the activated brain regions are related to motivating moral behaviour and which regions
ur na
are correlates of the experience without being relevant for motivating behaviour. This is why fMRI studies of moral feelings can only be interpreted in conjunction with the evidence provided in the previous sections.
Jo
The anticipation of guilt is important in preventing us from immoral
actions and to motivate reparative action(Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al., 2007). Empathic concern—a core component of empathy—is closely related to pity, compassion and sympathy, which promote altruistic helping(Weng et al., 2015). Such feelings extend beyond perceiving, sharing or simulating other's emotions, requiring an extra step of feeling for the other
17 person(Decety et al., 2012). Frontopolar activations emerge as most reproducible for both guilt(Moll et al., 2007; Zahn et al., 2009c),(Basile et al., 2011; Kedia et al., 2008; Morey et al., 2012; Seara-Cardoso et al., 2016; Takahashi et al., 2004) and compassion(Immordino-Yang et al., 2009),(Moll et al., 2007),(Fehse et al., 2015; Kedia et al., 2008) compared against equally unpleasant and complex emotions, such as indignation towards others. In addition, guilt was reproducibly associated with activations of the subgenual
ro of
cingulate cortex (extending posteriorly to the adjacent septal area and the more anterior pregenual cingulate area in several studies) when compared with other complex negative emotions(Zahn et al., 2009a),(Green et al.,
-p
2012; Zahn et al., 2009c),(Basile et al., 2011; Morey et al., 2012). Septal and/or subgenual cingulate activations for guilt were found in several
re
studies, however, only when modelling individual differences in guilt proneness and empathic concern(Zahn et al., 2009a),(Green et al., 2012;
lP
Zahn et al., 2009c). Interestingly, a study on counterintuitive utilitarian judgments found that reduced anterior subgenual cingulate cortex activity
ur na
undergirded an increased inclination to accept harmful actions towards a third party, suggesting reduced empathic concern instead of increased explicit moral deliberation(Wiech et al., 2013). Despite these reproducible associations of subgenual cingulate and
Jo
septal activations with individual differences in guilt-proneness and empathic concern, two recent systematic reviews of fMRI studies probing guilt(Gifuni et al., 2016) (Bastin et al., 2016) have failed to detect these regions. Because these reviews did not base their conclusions on studies controlling for individual differences in the experience of guilt-evoking stimuli, and on those studies using optimised fMRI sequences for ventral
18 frontal regions, it is not surprising that subgenual cingulate/septal activations were not given prominence and this will be important in future systematic reviews.
3.4. Social attachment and moral motivation Being able to feel attached to others is a basic ingredient for moral feelings such as compassion and guilt (Moll and Schulkin, 2009; Moll et al.,
ro of
2011) (Preston, 2013). Specific brain structures have been critically implicated in social attachment mechanisms in non-human animals,
including pair bonding and bonding between mother and offspring(Insel
-p
and Young, 2001; Stack et al., 2002; Swain et al., 2012). These brain
structures include the preoptic-anterior hypothalamic area, septal nuclei and
re
associated basal forebrain regions (Stack et al., 2002). These areas host abundant receptors for neuropeptides (oxytocin, vasopressin), monoamines
lP
(dopamine) and opioids that have been shown to influence attachmentrelated behaviours (Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Insel and Young,
ur na
2001; Stack et al., 2002). Septal damage in rodents disrupts maternal caregiving (Febo et al., 2005).
Human fMRI investigations of maternal and romantic love reported
remarkably overlapping activations in these regions(Aron et al., 2005;
Jo
Bartels and Zeki, 2004; Swain et al., 2007), and oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and prosocial temperament were shown to be associated with individual differences in hypothalamic volume and function(Tost et al., 2010). Until recently, an open question was whether brain activation associated with attachment-related experiences could be dissociated from that associated with general positive or negative emotions. A recent study
19 employed passive presentation of social narratives involving kin (i.e., associated with affiliative states) or not involving kinship(Moll et al., 2012). The results confirmed the prediction that the septo-hypothalamic region was selectively activated for affiliative emotional states, while controlling for positive or negative emotional valence and for the context of agency. Interestingly, subgenual cingulate cortex activation was only detected when modelling individual differences in how strongly participants perceived their
ro of
own families as a distinctive social group in affiliative social scenarios(Rusch et al., 2014). A recent study confirmed the role of
subgenual frontal areas in distinguishing between in- and outgroups by
-p
showing that the subgenual cortex was selectively activated for efforts
benefitting anonymous fellow fans of one’s soccer club compared with
re
playing to benefit non-fans(Bortolini et al., 2017).
Basal forebrain regions were also found to be activated by witnessing the
lP
delivery of rewards to similar others (“vicarious rewards”(Mobbs et al., 2009)). In this study, watching another player, that one could identify with,
ur na
receive rewards led to activation of the ventral striatum and adjoining septo-hypothalamic area. Interestingly, when correlating with the perceived degree of similarity of shared values (a more complex construct), higher
Jo
activity was observed in the subgenual frontal cortex.
3.5. Summary of evidence on the basal forebrain’s role Taken together, this body of evidence suggests a specific neural basis
for moral motivations and associated feelings, which drive altruistic choices towards other individuals or groups including kin. The emerging picture indicates that the septo-hypothalamic and subgenual frontal areas may play
20 distinct but interlocking roles in moral motivation.
The septo-hypothalamic region, given its anatomical organisation as well as the evidence discussed above, appears to be more relevant for representing basic components of moral motivation such as interpersonal affiliation and social bonding.
In contrast, the subgenual frontal areas are likely to represent more dynamic and complex aspects of moral motivation, such as
ro of
social agency, and the value of social outcomes, as well as perceived group belongingness (see also Figure 1).
-p
4. Moral motivation in psychiatry: emerging applications
The application of insights on the neural architecture of moral
re
motivation to the study of psychiatric disorders is still in its infancy(Zahn et al., 2015). These insights are likely to become increasingly important in
lP
understanding individual differences in vulnerability to specific psychiatric disorders. Here, our focus is on psychopathy and major depressive disorder
ur na
(MDD) as two examples of conditions associated with moral motivation disturbances.
Psychopathy has classically been described as a disorder of moral motivation, being specifically associated with an inability to experience guilt
Jo
and pity, with a spared ability to understand and manipulate others for selfish purposes(Cleckley, 1976). Callousness in patients with psychopathy(Hare, 2003) correlates with diminished grey matter volume in the frontopolar and subgenual orbitofrontal areas(de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2008). Another study revealed that psychopaths show diminished subgenual cingulate activation to moral violation-depicting pictures(Harenski and
21 Hamann, 2006) and during empathy for pain(Decety et al., 2013). An association of neurodevelopmental changes in the septum pellucidum with antisocial behaviour and psychopathy was reported(Raine et al., 2010), in keeping with a purported role of the septal nuclei for moral behaviour. Psychopathy has also been reproducibly associated with abnormal fMRI activation (Harenski and Hamann, 2006; Kiehl et al., 2004) and structure of the right ATL(Gregory S and et al., 2012; Muller et al., 2008) and
ro of
frontopolar cortex(Gregory S and et al., 2012), as well as the right uncinate fasciculus(Craig et al., 2009) connecting anterior temporal and subgenual areas. This suggests, that psychopathy is not only associated with basal
-p
forebrain dysfunction, but also with isocortical representations of more complex moral goals.
re
MDD is associated with overgeneralised forms of empathy-based interpersonal guilt(O'Connor et al., 2002) and self-contempt/disgust(Green
lP
et al., 2013), which remain detectable on remission of symptoms(Green et al., 2013). This indicates that self-blaming emotional biases are a trait
ur na
vulnerability factor for MDD as postulated by cognitive models(Abramson et al., 1978). The importance of subgenual FC dysfunction in MDD has been well established(Price and Drevets, 2010; Ressler and Mayberg, 2007). Accordingly, overgeneralization of guilt (i.e. feeling guilty for everything
Jo
and thus hating oneself as a result) correlated with functional connectivity changes between a subgenual cingulate region and the right superior ATL whilst patients with remitted MDD experienced self-blame in the scanner(Green et al., 2012), and predicted risk of subsequent recurrence(Lythe et al., 2015). Another study of moral motivations in MDD employed a charity donation paradigm in remitted patients during fMRI to
22 show that subgenual cingulate activation was higher whilst sacrificing money to donate compared with control participants(Pulcu et al., 2014), despite no overall increase in donations made during the experiment. Further studies of the neural correlates of moral feelings and altruistic behaviour in psychopathy and MDD are needed to confirm and extend these findings. Promoting prosocial behaviours is a topic of wide interest and concern.
ro of
There is some initial evidence pointing to ways on how to boost moral motivation and its neural underpinnings using psychological and
neurotechnological approaches. For example, a recent controlled study
-p
using mindfulness meditation demonstrated that activity in the septal area was selectively associated with increased cooperation in the Ultimatum
re
game. Increased cooperation was also associated with increased functional connectivity between the septal area and insula (Kirk et al., 2016).
lP
Technological advances in real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback also promise exciting opportunities for testing neuroscience-informed models
ur na
and for translating them into novel treatments(Kim and Birbaumer, 2014; Sulzer et al., 2013). fMRI neurofeedback provides individuals access to information about their current brain activity that is usually outside of their awareness. A recent study has demonstrated that healthy participants can
Jo
voluntarily modulate neural correlates of complex moral feelings by increasing activation of septo-hypothalamic and frontopolar areas whilst experiencing affection(Moll et al., 2014). The possibility of enhancing human capacities has become a hot topic of debate within the scientific, educational and social policy communities during the last two decades(Hyman, 2011).
23
5. Future perspectives and conclusion Research on moral cognitive neuroscience during the last decade has led to a striking advance in our understanding of human social behaviour, bringing to the neurobiological arena a topic that belonged exclusively to the humanities and the psychological sciences. We have discussed evidence indicating that the subgenual frontal and septo-hypothalamic area are
ro of
currently the best candidates for hosting subregions with selective involvement in moral vs. selfish motivations. Future studies are needed to
address the question of a plausible anterior-posterior gradient in complexity
-p
within the septal and subgenual frontal continuum analogous to a similar proposal for hedonic reward value representations in the medial
re
orbitofrontal cortex (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). It also remains to be investigated whether the anterior subgenual cingulate regions code for
lP
causal social agency due to their closer connection with pregenual anterior cingulate representations shown to correlate with subjective feelings of
ur na
motor agency (Marchesotti et al., 2017) and emerging evidence for functional subdivisions between pregenual and subgenual areas in social learning(Lockwood and Wittmann, 2018). A primate pharmacological inactivation study further supports functional subdivisions between the
Jo
subgenual cortex (BA25) and more anterior subgenual cingulate areas (BA32) as measured by autonomic nervous system responses to aversive stimuli (Wallis et al., 2017). Inactivation of BA25 reduced autonomic nervous system correlates of aversive stimuli, whereas BA32 inactivation led to overgeneralised responses. This would be compatible with more complex and contextualised information being represented in anterior subgenual
24 cingulate areas which guard against overgeneralisation. Future studies are needed to probe whether subregions within the subgenual cortex (BA25) represent attachment-related values of social outcomes analogous to the proposed role of the medial orbitofrontal cortex in representing hedonic reward value (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). An exciting future of applying novel neuroscience insights to an improved pathophysiological understanding and treatment of psychiatric disorders and societal
ro of
functioning lies ahead.
Acknowledgements
-p
RZ was partly funded by the UK MRC (G09023042) and US Brain & Behavior
Research Foundation (24715). ROS and JM were funded by IDOR. The authors
re
are thankful for discussions with Ivanei Bramati, Fernanda Tovar-Moll, Leonardo
Jo
ur na
lP
Fontenelle, Yotam Heineberg and João Ascenso.
25
References Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., Teasdale, J.D., 1978. LEARNED HELPLESSNESS IN HUMANS - CRITIQUE AND REFORMULATION. J Abn. Psychol. 87, 49-74. Allison, T., Puce, A., McCarthy, G., 2000. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 267-278. Andreoni, J., 1990. Impure altruism and donations to public good: a theory of warm glow giving. The Economic Journal 100, 464-477. Apps, Matthew A.J., Rushworth, Matthew F.S., Chang, Steve W.C., 2016. The Anterior Cingulate Gyrus and Social Cognition: Tracking the Motivation of Others. Neuron 90, 692-707.
ro of
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D.J., Strong, G., Li, H., Brown, L.L., 2005. Reward, Motivation, and Emotion Systems Associated With Early-Stage Intense Romantic Love. J Neurophysiol 94, 327-337.
Ashar, Y.K., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Dimidjian, S., Wager, T.D., 2017. Empathic Care and Distress: Predictive Brain Markers and Dissociable Brain Systems. Neuron 94, 1263-1273 e1264.
re
-p
Bado, P., Engel, A., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Paiva, F.F., Basilio, R., Sato, J.R., Tovar-Moll, F., Moll, J., 2014. Functional dissociation of ventral frontal and dorsomedial default mode network components during resting state and emotional autobiographical recall. Hum Brain Mapp 35, 3302-3313.
lP
Baez, S., Manes, F., Huepe, D., Torralva, T., Fiorentino, N., Richter, F., Huepe-Artigas, D., Ferrari, J., Montañes, P., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Vigliecca, N.S., Decety, J., Ibanez, A., 2014. Primary empathy deficits in frontotemporal dementia. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 6.
ur na
Baez, S., Morales, J.P., Slachevsky, A., Torralva, T., Matus, C., Manes, F., Ibanez, A., 2016. Orbitofrontal and limbic signatures of empathic concern and intentional harm in the behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex 75, 20-32. Bartels, A., Zeki, S., 2004. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 21, 1155-1166. Basile, B., Mancini, F., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Bozzali, M., 2011. Deontological and Altruistic Guilt: Evidence for Distinct Neurobiological Substrates. Hum Brain Mapp 32, 229-239.
Jo
Bastin, C., Harrison, B.J., Davey, C.G., Moll, J., Whittle, S., 2016. Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71, 455-471. Bishop, J.D., 1996. Moral motivation and the development of Francis Hutcheson's philosophy. Journal of the History of Ideas 57, 277-295. Bortolini, T., Bado, P., Hoefle, S., Engel, A., Zahn, R., de Oliveira Souza, R., Dreher, J.C., Moll, J., 2017. Neural bases of ingroup altruistic motivation in soccer fans. Sci Rep 7, 16122. Bozeat, S., Gregory, C.A., Ralph, M.A., Hodges, J.R., 2000. Which neuropsychiatric and behavioural features distinguish frontal and temporal variants of
26 frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer's disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 69, 178-186. Casebeer, W.D., 2003. Moral cognition and its neural constituents. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 840-846. Chang, L.J., Smith, A., Dufwenberg, M., Sanfey, A.G., 2011. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion. Neuron 70, 560-572. Cleckley, H.M., 1976. The mask of sanity, 5th ed. Mosby, St. Louis, MO. Cooper, J.C., Kreps, T.A., Wiebe, T., Pirkl, T., Knutson, B., 2010. When Giving Is Good: Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Activation for Others' Intentions. Neuron 67, 511-521.
ro of
Coricelli, G., Critchley, H.D., Joffily, M., O'Doherty, J.P., Sirigu, A., Dolan, R.J., 2005. Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nature Neuroscience 8, 1255-1262. Craig, M.C., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, E., Kanaan, R., Picchioni, M., McGuire, P.K., Fahy, T., Murphy, D.G., 2009. Altered connections on the road to psychopathy. Mol Psychiatry 14, 946-953, 907.
-p
Crockett, M.J., Siegel, J.Z., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Dayan, P., Dolan, R.J., 2017. Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of value. Nature Neuroscience 20, 879-+.
re
Cutler, J., Campbell-Meiklejohn, D., 2019. A comparative fMRI meta-analysis of altruistic and strategic decisions to give. NeuroImage 184, 227-241.
lP
de Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R.D., Bramati, I.E., Garrido, G.J., Azevedo Ignácio, F., Tovar-Moll, F., Moll, J., 2008. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage 40, 1202-1213. Decety, J., Grezes, J., 2006. The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Res 1079, 4-14.
ur na
Decety, J., Norman, G.J., Berntson, G.G., Cacioppo, J.T., 2012. A neurobehavioral evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy. Prog Neurobiol 98, 38-48. Decety, J., Skelly, L.R., Kiehl, K.A., 2013. Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA psychiatry 70, 638-645.
Jo
Delgado, M.R., Frank, R.H., Phelps, E.A., 2005. Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nat Neurosci 8, 1611-1618. Depue, R.A., Morrone-Strupinsky, J.V., 2005. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. The Behavioral and brain sciences 28, 313-350; discussion 350-395. Eisenberg, N., 2000. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu Rev Psychol 51, 665-697. Eslinger, P.J., Damasio, A.R., 1985. Severe Disturbance of Higher Cognition after Bilateral Frontal-Lobe Ablation - Patient EVR. Neurology 35, 1731-1741.
27 Febo, M., Numan, M., Ferris, C.F., 2005. Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother-pup bonding during suckling. Journal of Neuroscience 25, 11637-11644. Fehse, K., Silveira, S., Elvers, K., Blautzik, J., 2015. Compassion, guilt and innocence: An fMRI study of responses to victims who are responsible for their fate. Social Neuroscience 10, 243-252. FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Evans, D., Mobbs, D., 2015. Empathic concern drives costly altruism. Neuroimage 105, 347-356. FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Thompson, R., Evans, D., Schweizer, S., Mobbs, D., 2012. Differential neural circuitry and self-interest in real vs hypothetical moral decisions. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 7, 743-751.
ro of
Fumagalli, M., Priori, A., 2012. Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality. Brain 135, 2006-2021. Gifuni, A.J., Kendal, A., Jollant, F., 2016. Neural mapping of guilt: a quantitative metaanalysis of functional imaging studies. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 1-15. Gintis, H., Henrich, J., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., Fehr, E., 2008. Strong reciprocity and the roots of human morality. Social Justice Research 21, 241-253.
-p
Green, S., Moll, J., Deakin, J.F., Hulleman, J., Zahn, R., 2013. Proneness to Decreased Negative Emotions in Major Depressive Disorder when Blaming Others rather than Oneself. Psychopathology 46, 34-44.
re
Green, S., Ralph, M.A.L., Moll, J., Deakin, J.F.W., Zahn, R., 2012. Guilt-Selective Functional Disconnection of Anterior Temporal and Subgenual Cortices in Major Depressive Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69, 1014-1021.
lP
Gregory S, f.D.S.A., et al., 2012. The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters: a structural mri investigation of antisocial male violent offenders. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 962-972.
ur na
Grossman, M., Eslinger, P.J., Troiani, V., Anderson, C., Avants, B., Gee, J.C., McMillan, C., Massimo, L., Khan, A., Antani, S., 2010. The role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in social decisions: converging evidence from fMRI and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropsychologia 48, 3505-3512. Harbaugh, W.T., Mayr, U., Burghart, D.R., 2007. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316, 16221625.
Jo
Hare, R.D., 2003. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd ed. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto. Hare, T.A., Camerer, C.F., Knoepfle, D.T., Rangel, A., 2010. Value Computations in Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Charitable Decision Making Incorporate Input from Regions Involved in Social Cognition. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 583-590. Hare, T.A., Camerer, C.F., Rangel, A., 2009. Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmPFC Valuation System. Science 324, 646-648. Harenski, C.L., Hamann, S., 2006. Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage 30, 313-324.
28 Harris, S., 2011. The moral landscape: How science can determine human values. . Simon and Schuster. Harrison, B.J., Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Hernandez-Ribas, R., Lopez-Sola, M., Ortiz, H., Alonso, P., Deus, J., Menchon, J.M., Real, E., Segalas, C., ContrerasRodriguez, O., Blanco-Hinojo, L., Cardoner, N., 2012. Neural correlates of moral sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69, 741-749. Haugh, R.M., Markesbery, W.R., 1983. Hypothalamic astrocytoma. Syndrome of hyperphagia, obesity, and disturbances of behavior and endocrine and autonomic function. Arch Neurol 40, 560-563.
ro of
Hayashi, T., Ko, J.H., Strafella, A.P., Dagher, A., 2013. Dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex interactions during self-control of cigarette craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110, 4422-4427.
Hsu, M., Anen, C., Quartz, S.R., 2008. The right and the good: distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency. Science 320, 1092-1095.
Hyman, S.E., 2011. Cognitive enhancement: promises and perils. Neuron 69, 595-598.
-p
Immordino-Yang, M.H., McColl, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A., 2009. Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 8021-8026.
re
Insel, T.R., Young, L.J., 2001. The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2, 129-136.
lP
Jankowski, K.F., Takahashi, H., 2014. Cognitive neuroscience of social emotions and implications for psychopathology: Examining embarrassment, guilt, envy, and schadenfreude. Psychiat Clin Neuros 68, 319-336. Kahane, G., 2012. On the Wrong Track: Process and Content in Moral Psychology. Mind Lang 27, 519-545.
ur na
Kant, I., 1786. Grundlegung zur Metaphysik der Sitten, 2nd ed. Johann Friedrich Hartknoch, Riga. Karim, A.A., Schneider, M., Lotze, M., Veit, R., Sauseng, P., Braun, C., Birbaumer, N., 2010. The Truth about Lying: Inhibition of the Anterior Prefrontal Cortex Improves Deceptive Behavior. Cerebral Cortex 20, 205-213.
Jo
Kedia, G., Berthoz, S., Wessa, M., Hilton, D., Martinot, J.L., 2008. An agent harms a victim: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study on specific moral emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 20, 1788-1798. Kiehl, K.A., Smith, A.M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B.B., Hare, R.D., Liddle, P.F., 2004. Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry research 130, 297-312. Kim, J.-W., Kim, S.-E., Kim, J.-J., Jeong, B., Park, C.-H., Son, A.R., Song, J.E., Ki, S.W., 2009. Compassionate attitude towards others’ suffering activates the mesolimbic neural system. Neuropsychologia 47, 2073-2081. Kim, S., Birbaumer, N., 2014. Real-time functional MRI neurofeedback: a tool for psychiatry. Curr. Opin. Psychiatr. 27, 332-336.
29 Kirk, U., Gu, X., Sharp, C., Hula, A., Fonagy, P., Montague, P.R., 2016. Mindfulness training increases cooperative decision making in economic exchanges: Evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage 138, 274-283. Klingler, J., Gloor, P., 1960. The connections of the amygdala and of the anterior temporal cortex in the human brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 115, 333-369. Knoch, D., Schneider, F., Schunk, D., Hohmann, M., Fehr, E., 2009. Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 20895-20899. Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., Damasio, A., 2007. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature 446, 908-911.
ro of
Krajbich, I., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N.L., Camerer, C.F., 2009. Economic Games Quantify Diminished Sense of Guilt in Patients with Damage to the Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 2188-2192.
-p
Kringelbach, M.L., Rolls, E.T., 2004. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol 72, 341-372.
re
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, R., Strenziok, M., Heinecke, A., Grafman, J., 2007a. Neural correlates of trust. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 20084-20089.
lP
Krueger, F., Moll, J., Zahn, R., Heinecke, A., Grafman, J., 2007b. Event frequency modulates the processing of daily life activities in human medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 17, 2346-2353. Lamb, R.B., 1974. Adam Smith's system: sympathy not self-interest. Journal of the History of Ideas 35, 671-682.
ur na
Lambon Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., 2008. Generalization and differentiation in semantic memory: insights from semantic dementia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124, 61-76. Lamm, C., Decety, J., Singer, T., 2011. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage 54, 2492-2502.
Jo
Liu, W., Miller, B.L., Kramer, J.H., Rankin, K., Wyss-Coray, C., Gearhart, R., Phengrasamy, L., Weiner, M., Rosen, H.J., 2004. Behavioral disorders in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 62, 742748. Lockwood, P.L., 2016. The anatomy of empathy: Vicarious experience and disorders of social cognition. Behavioural brain research 311, 255-266. Lockwood, P.L., Apps, M.A.J., Valton, V., Viding, E., Roiser, J.P., 2016. Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 9763-9768.
30 Lockwood, P.L., Hamonet, M., Zhang, S.H., Ratnavel, A., Salmony, F.U., Husain, M., Apps, M.A.J., 2017. Prosocial apathy for helping others when effort is required. Nat Hum Behav 1. Lockwood, P.L., Wittmann, M.K., 2018. Ventral anterior cingulate cortex and social decision-making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 92, 187-191. Loup, F., Tribollet, E., Duboisdauphin, M., Dreifuss, J.J., 1991. Localization of HighAffinity Binding-Sites for Oxytocin and Vasopressin in the Human Brain - an Autoradiographic Study. Brain Res 555, 220-232. Lythe, K.E., Moll, J., Gethin, J.A., Workman, C.I., Green, S., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Deakin, J.F., Zahn, R., 2015. Self-blame-Selective Hyperconnectivity Between Anterior Temporal and Subgenual Cortices and Prediction of Recurrent Depressive Episodes. JAMA psychiatry 72, 1119-1126.
ro of
Marchesotti, S., Martuzzi, R., Schurger, A., Blefari, M.L., del Millán, J.R., Bleuler, H., Blanke, O., 2017. Cortical and subcortical mechanisms of brain-machine interfaces. Hum Brain Mapp 38, 2971-2989.
-p
Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., de la Fuente, L., Forno, G., Birba, A., García-Cordero, I., Serrano, C., Plastino, A., Slachevsky, A., Huepe, D., Sigman, M., Manes, F., García, A.M., Sedeño, L., Ibáñez, A., 2016. Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 1-19.
re
Mobbs, D., Yu, R.J., Meyer, M., Passamonti, L., Seymour, B., Calder, A.J., Schweizer, S., Frith, C.D., Dalgleish, T., 2009. A Key Role for Similarity in Vicarious Reward. Science 324, 900-900.
lP
Moll, J., Bado, P., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Lima, D.O., Paiva, F.F., Sato, J.R., Tovar-Moll, F., Zahn, R., 2012. A Neural Signature of Affiliative Emotion in the Human Septohypothalamic Area. J.Neurosci. 32, 12499-12505.
ur na
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Basilio, R., Bramati, I.E., Gordon, B., Rodriguez-Nieto, G., Zahn, R., Krueger, F., Grafman, J., 2018. Altruistic decisions following penetrating traumatic brain injury. Brain 141, 1558-1569. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Garrido, G.J., Bramati, I.E., Caparelli-Daquer, E.M.A., Paiva, M.M.F., Zahn, R., Grafman, J., 2007. The self as a moral agent: linking the neural bases of social agency and moral sensitivity. Social Neuroscience 2, 336-352.
Jo
Moll, J., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Zahn, R., 2008. The Neural Basis of Moral Cognition: Sentiments, Concepts, and Values. Ann.NYAcad.Sci. 1124, 161-180. Moll, J., Krueger, F., Zahn, R., Pardini, M., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Grafman, J., 2006. Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103, 15623-15628. Moll, J., Schulkin, J., 2009. Social attachment and aversion in human moral cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 456-465. Moll, J., Weingartner, J.H., Bado, P., Basilio, R., Sato, J.R., Melo, B.R., Bramati, I.E., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Zahn, R., 2014. Voluntary enhancement of neural signatures of affiliative emotion using FMRI neurofeedback. PLoS One 9, e97343.
31 Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Krueger, F., Tura, B., Cavanagh, A.L., Grafman, J., 2011. Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 54, 1735-1742. Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., Grafman, J., 2005. Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 799-809. Morelli, S.A., Rameson, L.T., Lieberman, M.D., 2014. The neural components of empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 9, 39-47. Morelli, S.A., Sacchet, M.D., Zaki, J., 2015. Common and distinct neural correlates of personal and vicarious reward: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage 112, 244-253.
ro of
Morey, R.A., McCarthy, G., Selgrade, E.S., Seth, S., Nasser, J.D., LaBar, K.S., 2012. Neural systems for guilt from actions affecting self versus others. NeuroImage 60, 683-692.
-p
Muller, J.L., Ganssbauer, S., Sommer, M., Dohnel, K., Weber, T., Schmidt-Wilcke, T., Hajak, G., 2008. Gray matter changes in right superior temporal gyrus in criminal psychopaths. Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Psychiatry research 163, 213-222. Nagaratnam, N., Nagaratnam, K., Ng, K., Diu, P., 2004. Akinetic mutism following stroke. J Clin Neurosci 11, 25-30.
re
Nauta, W.H., Domesick, V., 1981/1993. Ramifications of the Limbic System, Neuroanatomy. Birkhäuser Boston, pp. 329-352.
lP
Nowak, M.A., Sigmund, K., 2005. Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437, 12911298. O'Connor, L.E., Berry, J.W., Weiss, J., Gilbert, P., 2002. Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 71, 19-27.
ur na
Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., Rogers, T.T., 2007. Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 976-987. Preston, S.D., 2013. The Origins of Altruism in Offspring Care. Psychol Bull 139, 1305-1341.
Jo
Prevost, C., Pessiglione, M., Metereau, E., Clery-Melin, M.L., Dreher, J.C., 2010. Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 14080-14090. Price, C.J., Mummery, C.J., Moore, C.J., Frakowiak, R.S., Friston, K.J., 1999. Delineating necessary and sufficient neural systems with functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients. J Cogn Neurosci 11, 371-382. Price, J.L., Drevets, W.C., 2010. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 192-216. Pulcu, E., Zahn, R., Moll, J., Trotter, P.D., Thomas, E.J., Juhasz, G., Deakin, J.F., Anderson, I.M., Sahakian, B.J., Elliott, R., 2014. Enhanced subgenual cingulate response to altruistic decisions in remitted major depressive disorder. NeuroImage. Clinical 4, 701-710.
32 Raine, A., Lee, L., Yang, Y., Colletti, P., 2010. Neurodevelopmental marker for limbic maldevelopment in antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry 197, 186-192. Rankin, K.P., Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Allison, S.C., Stanley, C.M., Glenn, S., Weiner, M.W., Miller, B.L., 2006. Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative disease. Brain 129, 2945-2956. Ressler, K.J., Mayberg, H.S., 2007. Targeting abnormal neural circuits in mood and anxiety disorders: from the laboratory to the clinic. Nature Neuroscience 10, 1116-1124. Ribas, G.C., 2007. Neuroanatomical basis of behavior: history and recent contributions. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 29, 63-71.
ro of
Rohan, M.J., 2000. A rose by any name? The values construct. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 4, 255-277. Ross, L.A., Olson, I.R., 2010. Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes. NeuroImage 49, 3452-3462.
Ruff, C.C., Ugazio, G., Fehr, E., 2013. Changing Social Norm Compliance with Noninvasive Brain Stimulation. Science 342, 482-484.
-p
Rusch, N., Bado, P., Zahn, R., Bramati, I.E., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2014. You and your kin: Neural signatures of family-based group perception in the subgenual cortex. Soc Neurosci 9, 326-331.
lP
re
Santamaria-Garcia, H., Baez, S., Reyes, P., Santamaria-Garcia, J.A., SantacruzEscudero, J.M., Matallana, D., Arevalo, A., Sigman, M., Garcia, A.M., Ibanez, A., 2017. A lesion model of envy and Schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration. Brain 140, 3357-3377. Saper, C.B., 2011. Diffuse Cortical Projection Systems: Anatomical Organization and Role in Cortical Function, Comprehensive Physiology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
ur na
Schwartz, S.H., Bilsky, W., 1987. Toward a Universal Psychological Structure of Human-Values. J Pers Soc Psychol 53, 550-562. Seara-Cardoso, A., Sebastian, C.L., McCrory, E., Foulkes, L., Buon, M., Roiser, J.P., Viding, E., 2016. Anticipation of guilt for everyday moral transgressions: The role of the anterior insula and the influence of interpersonal psychopathic traits. Scientific Reports 6.
Jo
Shima, K., Isoda, M., Mushiake, H., Tanji, J., 2007. Categorization of behavioural sequences in the prefrontal cortex. Nature 445, 315-318. Simmons, W.K., Reddish, M., Bellgowan, P.S.F., Martin, A., 2009. The Selectivity and Functional Connectivity of the Anterior Temporal Lobes. Cereb. Cortex, bhp149. Skipper, L.M., Ross, L.A., Olson, I.R., 2011. Sensory and semantic category subdivisions within the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 49, 34193429. Smith, D.V., Hayden, B.Y., Truong, T.K., Song, A.W., Platt, M.L., Huettel, S.A., 2010. Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 2490-2495.
33 Snowden, J.S., Bathgate, D., Varma, A., Blackshaw, A., Gibbons, Z.C., Neary, D., 2001. Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 70, 323-332. Sollberger, M., Stanley, C.M., Wilson, S.M., Gyurak, A., Beckman, V., Growdon, M., Jang, J., Weiner, M.W., Miller, B.L., Rankin, K.P., 2009. Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 47, 28122827. Stack, E.C., Balakrishnan, R., Numan, M.J., Numan, M., 2002. A functional neuroanatomical investigation of the role of the medial preoptic area in neural circuits regulating maternal behavior. Behavioural brain research 131, 17-36. Stellar, E., 1954/1994. The physiology of motivation. Psychol Rev 101, 301-311.
ro of
Sulzer, J., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Weiskopf, N., Birbaumer, N., Blefari, M.L., Bruehl, A.B., Cohen, L.G., deCharms, R.C., Gassert, R., Goebel, R., Herwig, U., LaConte, S., Linden, D., Luft, A., Seifritz, E., Sitaram, R., 2013. Real-time fMRI neurofeedback: Progress and challenges. Neuroimage 76, 386-399. Swain, J.E., Konrath, S., Brown, S.L., Finegood, E.D., Akce, L.B., Dayton, C.J., Ho, S.S., 2012. Parenting and Beyond: Common Neurocircuits Underlying Parental and Altruistic Caregiving. Parent.-Sci. Pract. 12, 115-123.
re
-p
Swain, J.E., Lorberbaum, J.P., Kose, S., Strathearn, L., 2007. Brain basis of early parent-infant interactions: psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 48, 262-287.
lP
Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Matsuda, T., Asai, K., Okubo, Y., 2004. Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 967-974. Tangney, J.P., Stuewig, J., Mashek, D.J., 2007. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology 58, 345-372.
ur na
Tassy, S., Oullier, O., Duclos, Y., Coulon, O., Mancini, J., Deruelle, C., Attarian, S., Felician, O., Wicker, B., 2012. Disrupting the right prefrontal cortex alters moral judgement. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 7, 282-288.
Jo
Tost, H., Kolachana, B., Hakimi, S., Lemaitre, H., Verchinski, B.A., Mattay, V.S., Weinberger, D.R., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., 2010. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 13936-13941. Wallis, C.U., Cardinal, R.N., Alexander, L., Roberts, A.C., Clarke, H.F., 2017. Opposing roles of primate areas 25 and 32 and their putative rodent homologs in the regulation of negative emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114, E4075E4084. Walter, H., 2001. Neurophilosophy of Free Will: From Libertarian Illusions to a Concept of Natural Autonomy MIT Press, Boston. Wastling, S.J., Barker, G.J., 2015. Designing hyperbolic secant excitation pulses to reduce signal dropout in gradient-echo echo-planar imaging. Magnetic resonance in medicine 74, 661-672.
34 Weissenberger, A.A., Dell, M.L., Liow, K., Theodore, W., Frattali, C.M., Hernandez, D., Zametkin, A.J., 2001. Aggression and psychiatric comorbidity in children with hypothalamic hamartomas and their unaffected siblings. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40, 696-703. Welt, L., 1888. Über Charakterveränderungen des Menschen. Dtsch Arch Klin Med 42, 339-390. Weng, H.Y., Fox, A.S., Hessenthaler, H.C., Stodola, D.E., Davidson, R.J., 2015. The Role of Compassion in Altruistic Helping and Punishment Behavior. PLOS ONE 10, e0143794. Wiech, K., Kahane, G., Shackel, N., Farias, M., Savulescu, J., Tracey, I., 2013. Cold or calculating? Reduced activity in the subgenual cingulate cortex reflects decreased emotional aversion to harming in counterintuitive utilitarian judgment. Cognition 126, 364-372.
ro of
Wigfield, A., 1994. Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: A developmental perspective. Educational Psychology Review 6, 49-78. Wood, J.N., Grafman, J., 2003. Human prefrontal cortex: Processing and representational perspectives. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 139-147.
-p
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I., Garrido, G., Moll, J., 2009a. Subgenual cingulate activity reflects individual differences in empathic concern. Neurosci.Lett. 457, 107-110.
re
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2011a. The Neuroanatomical Basis of Moral Cognition and Emotion, From DNA to Social Cognition. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., pp. 123-138.
lP
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2011b. The neuroscience of moral cognition and emotion, The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience. Oxford University Press, pp. 477-490.
ur na
Zahn, R., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2015. The Neural Foundation of Morality, in: Wright, J.D. (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Elsevier, pp. 606-618. Zahn, R., Green, S., Beaumont, H., Burns, A., Moll, J., Caine, D., Gerhard, A., Hoffman, P., Shaw, B., Grafman, J., Lambon Ralph, M.A., 2017. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration and social behaviour: dissociation between the knowledge of its consequences and its conceptual meaning. Cortex.
Jo
Zahn, R., Moll, J., Iyengar, V., Huey, E.D., Tierney, M., Krueger, F., Grafman, J., 2009b. Social conceptual impairments in frontotemporal lobar degeneration with right anterior temporal hypometabolism. Brain 132, 604-616. Zahn, R., Moll, J., Krueger, F., Huey, E.D., Garrido, G., Grafman, J., 2007. Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 6430-6435. Zahn, R., Moll, J., Paiva, M., Garrido, G., Krueger, F., Huey, E.D., Grafman, J., 2009c. The Neural Basis of Human Social Values: Evidence from Functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 19, 276-283.
35 Zaki, J., Mitchell, J.P., 2011. Equitable decision making is associated with neural markers of intrinsic value. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 19761-19766. Zhu, L.S., Jenkins, A.C., Set, E., Scabini, D., Knight, R.T., Chiu, P.H., King-Casas, B., Hsu, M., 2014. Damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects tradeoffs between honesty and self-interest. Nature Neuroscience 17, 1319-1321.
Boxes Box 1| The history of the idea of moral motivations
ro of
Following the Aristotelian tradition, the perspective of virtue ethics highlighted the importance of attaining virtues by training oneself to act according to their
prescriptions(Casebeer, 2003). In the 18th century, philosophers of the Scottish Enlightenment – Francis Hutcheson, his successor Adam Smith, and David
-p
Hume(Zahn et al., 2011b) – highlighted the central importance of moral sentiments
for moral behaviour. “Sympathy”, defined as “man's capacity for fellow feeling with
re
others”, was considered the primary moral sentiment by Adam Smith(Lamb, 1974).
lP
Hutcheson postulated that “benevolence” motivates virtuous actions and thereby provides “moral motivations”(Bishop, 1996). Modern psychologists speak of “moral emotions” rather than “moral sentiments” with some variation as to which
ur na
emotions are considered moral(Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al., 2007). The German philosopher Immanuel Kant, who was a contemporary of Hume, distinguished the ability to judge what is morally right and wrong (“principium diiudicationis”) from the motivation (“principium motivationis”) to act accordingly(Kant, 1786) , He
Jo
opposed the view that moral actions could be defined by the experienced moral sentiments, which were thought to originate from the external senses. He claimed instead that true moral actions are motivated directly by respect (“Achtung”) for the moral law, which is self-generated and an act of free will(Kant, 1786). Kant further pointed out that respect for the moral law is the impression of a moral value that opposes our selfishness. Moral actions are those guided by a principle
36 that the individual wants to become a general law. Thus, moral motivations as defined by the opposing schools of moral philosophy are either the respect for moral rules (Kant) or moral sentiments (Hutcheson and Smith). As previously pointed out(Zahn et al., 2011a), there is an often overlooked agreement across schools in that those motivations that enable us to overcome self-interest are key to understanding moral behaviour. Neuroscience and psychology can produce evidence and theories about the structure and dynamics of subjective experiences and behavioural expressions of moral motivations, such as “respect for moral
ro of
principles” or “feelings of guilt”, and their neural underpinnings. Beyond the realm of pure empirical science, however, lies the contemporary philosophical debate
among supporters of versions of deontology, sentimentalism, consequentialism and
-p
virtue theory and their implications for free will, moral realism and moral
Jo
ur na
lP
re
philosophy in general (Casebeer, 2003),(Harris, 2011; Kahane, 2012; Walter, 2001).
37 Box 2 | Anatomy of the basal forebrain The basal forebrain (BF) is a heterogeneous collection of structures and pathways lying underneath the intercommissural plane(Klingler and Gloor, 1960). We here briefly review and summarise the complex anatomy of this region. The medial BF comprises the more anteriorly located subgenual cingulate cortex (subgenual parts of areas 24, 32 and, 33), the posterior subgenual cortex (area 25), Broca’s paraolfactory area, preoptic area, and ventromedial hypothalamus. These areas contain the highest concentrations of receptors for the “affiliative neuropeptides”
ro of
oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain(Loup et al., 1991) and comprise a core node of the human “default mode” network(Bado et al., 2014). Laterally, the BF encompasses the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum, the amygdala and the
-p
basal nucleus of Meynert(Nauta and Domesick, 1981/1993). The medial and lateral BF structures are heavily interconnected by the medial forebrain bundle, Broca’s
re
diagonal band (ventral amygdalofugal pathway), stria terminalis (dorsal amygdalofugal pathway), and stria medullaris(Ribas, 2007). Together with the
lP
median-paramedian structures of the upper brainstem tegmentum, they provide massive direct extrathalamic projections to the entire cerebral cortex(Saper, 2011).
ur na
The orbitofrontal, subgenual, and paraolfactory areas, anterior insula and temporal pole (TP) are given anatomic unity by the uncinate fascicle, representing endpoints of massive ascending extrathalamic and thalamic projections. Figure Box 2. Parasagittal and coronal sections to illustrate the BF structures related to the visceroendocrine representation of the internal milieu and motivational
Jo
states. (A) Subgenual: structures underneath the genu (“knee”) of the corpus callosum at this level are represented by the posterior subgenual cortex (area 25), originally referred to as “subgenual area” by Brodmann, and the more anterior subgenual cingulate cortex (subgenual parts of areas 24, 32, and 33). The subgenual cingulate cortex is bordered ventrally by the subgenual sectors of the orbitofrontal cortex ( [OFC], area 11) including the subgenual sector of the paracingulate cortex
38 which some atlases consider to be part of area 32 (B) Precommissural: the medial septal nuclei, which make up the surface of the ventromedial wall of the cerebral hemisphere at this level, are represented by the paraterminal and paraolfactory gyri (Broca’s paraolfactory area). Note that area 25 has also been described as extending more posteriorly(Price and Drevets, 2010) into the region here identified as paraolfactory area. (C) Commissural: the anterior commissure [AC] pierces the basal ganglia creating a subcommissural sector (nucleus accumbens [NAcc] and ventral pallidum [VP]), which is laterally coextensive with the basal nucleus of Meynert and
ro of
the ansa peduncularis [AP]; the AP connects the amygdala [Amyg] with the
Jo
ur na
lP
re
-p
ventromedial hypothalamus (not shown).
ur na
lP
re
-p
ro of
39
Jo
Box 3 | The neural basis of socio-moral values Abstract social concepts such as “honour” and “honesty” are used to describe social and moral values which operate at a higher level of abstraction than simple social attitudes towards a person or specific action(Rohan, 2000). Social psychologists have defined social values as trans-situational goals which motivate and guide our behaviour(Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987). FMRI studies have shown that
40 the ATLs, especially within their right superior sectors(Skipper et al., 2011),(Zahn et al., 2007), are selectively more activated when considering social concepts than they are for non-social concepts(Ross and Olson, 2010; Zahn et al., 2015; Zahn et al., 2007),(Simmons et al., 2009),(Skipper et al., 2011). Furthermore, right superior ATL activation increased with the richness of detail with which social concepts describe social behaviour(Zahn et al., 2007) in keeping with the role of the ATLs in representing differentiated conceptual knowledge(Lambon Ralph and Patterson, 2008),(Patterson et al., 2007). Activation in the right superior ATL was reproduced
ro of
when making emotional rather than semantic judgments about social values(Zahn et al., 2009c). Interestingly, activation in this region was independent of the emotional (valence: positive vs. negative(Zahn et al., 2007),(Zahn et al., 2011a; Zahn et al.,
-p
2009c), moral sentiment: guilt vs. pride vs. gratitude vs. indignation(Zahn et al.,
2009c)) and agency (self vs. other) context, supporting the hypothesis that the ATL
re
represents the context-independent (i.e. trans-situational) meaning of social values and thus goals entailed in moral motivations, whereas fronto-subcortical regions
lP
represent cognitive and emotional ingredients specific for particular moral
Jo
ur na
sentiments(Moll et al., 2005).
41 Box 4 | Lesions of frontal lobe subregions and moral motivation Altruistic giving is a prototypic behaviour involving moral motivation. Recent studies using non-invasive “virtual lesion” methods that employ brain stimulation techniques (transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)) and lesion mapping have provided important hints on the brain regions that are necessary for moral behaviours and choices(Tassy et al., 2012). TMS or tDCS studies using two-person economic games show that the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is necessary for making choices that maximise one’s
ro of
reputation(Knoch et al., 2009) and for social norm compliance under punishment
threats(Ruff et al., 2013), findings that are consistent with the suggested role of this region in reducing subjective values associated with the pursuit of immediate self-
-p
interest(Hare et al., 2009). Decisions to uphold social norms in these experiments
do not necessarily rely on moral motivation, because these decisions could also be
re
driven by self-regarding motivations, such as avoidance of punishment, preserving one’s social reputation and anger resulting from one’s self-interest being
lP
challenged. By contrast, interference with medial frontopolar cortex function using tDCS reduced guilt and increased deceitful behaviour, facilitating lying in mock
ur na
crime interrogations(Karim et al., 2010). A recent study(Zhu et al., 2014) showed that damage to the (mostly left) DLPFC, but not to the orbitofrontal cortex, impaired honesty concerns. The economic decision task enabled teasing apart honesty concerns from altruistic preferences per se. Whereas all groups were equally altruistic in sharing money with anonymous participants in a dictator-type task,
Jo
DLPFC lesion patients were less concerned about honesty and thus indulged more in sending “false messages”. These results highlight the roles of the DLPFC and medial orbitofrontal cortex in prosocial behaviour―i.e., abiding to the value of honesty, and are corroborated by their functional interplay in self-control(Hare et al., 2009; Hayashi et al., 2013) and value computations(Prevost et al., 2010). In patients with FTD, reduced resting glucose metabolism in the septal region and
42 frontopolar cortex predicted impairments of guilt and compassion on a moral sentiment task. Impairment of “other-critical” feelings (anger/indignation and disgust), in contrast, was associated with abnormal amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal (DMPFC) metabolism(Moll et al., 2011). Taken together, these studies point to the importance of frontopolar and ventromedial frontal regions (subgenual cingulate and adjacent medial orbitofrontal cortex) in motivating positive prosocial behaviours, and indicate that the DLPFC and lateral orbitofrontal cortices are key for responses that enforce honesty, self-reputation concerns, self-control and third-
ro of
party punishment(Coricelli et al., 2005; Delgado et al., 2005). The frontopolar
contribution to moral motivation is most likely related to its representation of longterm consequences of social behaviour (Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al.,
Jo
ur na
lP
re
-p
2017) and thus its importance for representing goals of moral motivations.
43
Figure legends Figure 1 The neural architecture of human moral motivation Panel a| Brain regions underpinning moral cognition and general motivation. Neural structures important to moral motivation include the septal/preoptic hypothalamic area (Septal/hypoth) and subgenual frontal cortex (SGC). The frontopolar and anterior superior temporal cortices enable more complex moral
ro of
goals. These are closely integrated with reward and behavioural reinforcement systems, including the ventral tegmental area (VTA), ventral striatum (VStr), medial forebrain bundle (MFB), raphe nuclei and the amygdalae (Amyg). These regions are further integrated with the anterior and posterior medial (ant and post mOFC) and
-p
lateral (lat OFC) sectors of the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral (DLPFC) prefrontal cortices, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), superior anterior temporal
re
cortex (Sup ATL) and posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and temporoparietal junction (TPJ).
lP
b| The event-feature-emotion complex (EFEC) model(Moll et al., 2005) postulates that moral cognition, emotion and motivation emerge from network integration via
ur na
temporal binding. Neural components having core roles for moral motivation: (1) basal forebrain, especially the septal/hypothalamic area, implicated in basic (i.e. context-independent) affiliative states(Moll et al., 2012; Moll et al., 2005; Stellar, 1954/1994), and subgenual frontal cortex [SGC], involved in more complex
Jo
context-dependent aspects such as social agency and responsibility attribution(Moll et al., 2008),(Rusch et al., 2014); (2) anterior prefrontal / frontopolar cortex [FPC], which stores event and action sequences(Krueger et al., 2007b; Shima et al., 2007; Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al., 2017) enabling long-term goal representations; (3) anterior temporal cortex (especially the right Sup ATL), representing abstract social conceptual knowledge(Zahn et al., 2007), e.g. “generous” or “rude” qualities of behaviour(Zahn et al., 2009b)); and (4) the
44 pSTS/TPJ, encoding perceptual social features such as emotional face and body posture(Allison et al., 2000) and mental models of socially-relevant
-p
ro of
perspectives(Decety and Grezes, 2006).
re
Figure 2
Functional imaging the basal forebrain in moral motivation
lP
Recent fMRI studies have implicated basal forebrain regions in social decisionmaking, altruistic choices and prosocial emotions. a| Septo-hypothalamic activity
ur na
(arrow) during fMRI neurofeedback-training of affectionate/tender feelings(Moll et al., 2014). b| Cooperation increased septo-hypothalamic activity (circled), as trust developed in a repeated interaction economic task(Krueger et al., 2007a). c| Septal area fMRI responses evoked when seeing empathy-eliciting pictures depicting other’s pain, anxiety or happiness. Activation predicted self-reported scores of real-
Jo
life helping behaviours, as illustrated by the scatterplot (pain vs. neutral contrast)(Morelli et al., 2014). d| Subgenual cingulate cortex fMRI responses to kinship-associated narratives tracked individual differences in “family entitativity”, i.e., perceiving own family as a cohesive social group(Rusch et al., 2014). e| Individuals higher in empathic concern showed increased subgenual cingulate responses to guilt/compassion-evoking scenarios(Zahn et al., 2009a). f| Functional
45 MRI responses of the subgenual cingulate cortex / septal area during charitable donations (altruistic decisions) (Moll et al., 2006) . g|Subgenual cortex fMRI activation selective for prosocial vs. selfish prediction errors which was higher in empathic individuals(Lockwood et al., 2016). h| Increased insula, SMA, and right DLPFC activity (yellow) when matching monetary expectations of the partner in a trust game. Activity in subgenual cortex (not shown) and in septal area / nucleus accumbens (NAcc, in blue), when participants returned less than expected, generating feelings of guilt(Chang et al., 2011). i| Scatterplot shows the relationship
ro of
between counterfactual guilt sensitivity and septal area / NAcc responses(Chang et al., 2011).
-p
Panel a|, adapted from (Moll et al., 2014), panel b| adapted from (Krueger et al.,
2007a), panel c| adapted from (Morelli et al., 2014), panel d| adapted from (Rusch
re
et al., 2014), panel e| adapted from (Zahn et al., 2009a), panel (f) adapted from
(Moll et al., 2006) , all with permission. Panel g| reproduced from (Smith et al.,
Jo
ur na
permission.
lP
2010), with permission. Panels (h) and (i) adapted from (Chang et al., 2011), with
ro of
-p
re
lP
ur na
Jo
46