Accepted Manuscript Muscle-selective disinhibition of corticomotor representations using a motor imagerybased brain-computer interface Mitsuaki Takemi, Maeda Tsuyoshi, Yoshihisa Masakado, Hartwig Roman Siebner, Junichi Ushiba PII:
S1053-8119(18)30768-7
DOI:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.070
Reference:
YNIMG 15231
To appear in:
NeuroImage
Received Date: 24 January 2018 Revised Date:
14 August 2018
Accepted Date: 28 August 2018
Please cite this article as: Takemi, M., Tsuyoshi, M., Masakado, Y., Siebner, H.R., Ushiba, J., Muscleselective disinhibition of corticomotor representations using a motor imagery-based brain-computer interface, NeuroImage (2018), doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.070. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1 2
Muscle-selective disinhibition of corticomotor representations using a motor imagery-based brain-computer interface
3
Mitsuaki Takemi1, 2, Tsuyoshi Maeda1, Yoshihisa Masakado3, Hartwig Roman Siebner2, 4,
5
Junichi Ushiba5, 6*
RI PT
4
6 7
1
8
Technology, Keio University, Kanagawa, Japan
9
2
School of Fundamental Science and Technology, Graduate School of Science and
10
Hvidovre, Hvidovre, Denmark
11
3
12
Japan
13
4
14
Copenhagen, Denmark
15
5
16
University, Kanagawa, Japan
17
6
18
Japan
M AN U
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Tokai University School of Medicine, Kanagawa,
Department of Neurology, Copenhagen University Hospital Bispebjerg,
TE D
Department of Biosciences and Informatics, Faculty of Science and Technology, Keio
Keio Research Institute for Pure and Applied Sciences (KiPAS), Keio University, Kanagawa,
EP
19
SC
Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance, Copenhagen University Hospital
*Corresponding author: Junichi Ushiba, Ph.D.
21
3-14-1 Hiyoshi, Kohoku-ku, Yokohama, Kanagawa, Japan
22
Tel/Fax: +81-45-563-1141; E-mail:
[email protected]
AC C
20
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Abstract
2
Bridging between brain activity and machine control, brain-computer interface (BCI)
3
can be employed to activate distributed neural circuits implicated in a specific aspect
4
of motor control. Using a motor imagery-based BCI paradigm, we previously found a
5
disinhibition within the primary motor cortex contralateral to the imagined
6
movement, as evidenced by event-related desynchronization (ERD) of oscillatory
7
cortical activity. Yet it is unclear whether this BCI approach does selectively
8
facilitate corticomotor representations targeted by the imagery. To address this
9
question, we used brain state-dependent transcranial magnetic stimulation while
10
participants performed kinesthetic motor imagery of wrist movements with their right
11
hand and received online visual feedback of the ERD. Single and paired-pulse
12
magnetic stimulation were given to the left primary motor cortex at a low or high
13
level of ERD to assess intracortical excitability. While intracortical facilitation
14
showed no modulation by ERD, short-latency intracortical inhibition was reduced the
15
higher the ERD. Intracortical disinhibition was only found in the agonist muscle
16
targeted by motor imagery at high ERD level, but not in the antagonist muscle.
17
Single pulse motor-evoked potential was also increased the higher the ERD. However,
18
at high ERD level, this facilitatory effect on overall corticospinal excitability was not
19
selective to the agonist muscle. Analogous results were found in two independent
20
experiments, in which participants either performed kinesthetic motor imagery of
21
wrist extension or flexion. Our results showed that motor imagery-based BCI can
22
selectively disinhibit the corticomotor output to the agonist muscle, enabling
23
effector-specific training in patients with motor paralysis.
24
Keywords: electroencephalogram (EEG), event-related desynchronization (ERD),
25
short-latency intracortical inhibition (SICI), sensorimotor rhythm (SMR), state-dependent,
26
transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1. Introduction
2
Brain-computer interface (BCI) links brain activity with machine control. BCI-based
3
paradigms are widely used to engage neural pathways associated with specific motor
4
tasks, especially in patients who are unable to generate overt movement due to severe
5
paralysis. BCI can help them to regulate motor-related brain activity (Mukaino et al.,
6
2014; Ono et al., 2014; Pichiorri et al., 2015; Soekadar et al., 2015). During BCI
7
training, patients are instructed to imagine hand movements. They concurrently receive
8
online feedback of their motor-related brain activity via visual or somatosensory
9
stimuli. Repeated use of such BCI is believed to strengthen residual neural networks
10
involved in motor control and facilitate motor recovery through error-based learning
11
and several neuroplasticity mechanisms, such as Hebbian-like plasticity and
12
use-dependent plasticity (Belardinelli et al., 2017; Kraus et al., 2016a; Pichiorri et al.,
13
2011; Sitaram et al., 2012; Soekadar et al., 2014; Vukelić and Gharabaghi, 2015;
14
Young et al., 2014).
15
Most BCI paradigms using motor imagery for rehabilitation purposes in post-stroke
16
hemiparesis exploit sensorimotor event-related desynchronization (ERD) as online
17
readouts of motor cortical activity (Mukaino et al., 2014; Ono et al., 2014; Pichiorri et
18
al.,
19
electroencephalography (EEG) and refers to the amplitude decrease of sensorimotor
20
oscillations in the mu (7–13 Hz) and beta (14–26 Hz) frequency bands. The use of ERD
21
as online EEG marker of cortical activity can be explained by the fact that the
22
physiological characteristics of ERD have been studied intensively over the last
23
decades. Sensorimotor mu ERD evoked by the imagery of voluntary hand movements
24
is known to show similar spatial profiles to the ERD associated with the preparation of
25
voluntary hand movements (Pfurtscheller and Neuper, 1997). Yuan et al. (2010) found
26
that
27
blood-oxygen-level-dependent signals in functional magnetic resonance imaging
28
during motor imagery. Furthermore, sensorimotor mu ERD during voluntary inhibition
29
of motor memory retrieval was significantly larger in dystonia patients whose motor
TE D
Young
et
al.,
2014).
ERD
can
be
readily
recorded
with
AC C
EP
2015;
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
both
mu
and
beta
ERDs
co-localize
and
co-vary
with
the
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
system has shown to be hyper-excitable relative to healthy subjects (Hummel et al.,
31
2002). A recent study demonstrated high test-retest reliability of the magnitude of beta
32
ERD induced by wrist movements across multiple sessions (Espenhahn et al., 2016).
33
Together, these studies qualify ERD during motor tasks as a reliable biomarker
34
representing motor cortical activities.
35
In line with these findings, we have shown that the magnitude of mu/beta ERD during
36
the use of a motor imagery-based BCI reflects the excitability of primary motor cortex
37
(M1) (Takemi et al., 2013) and spinal motoneurons (Takemi et al., 2015). Hence, the
38
use of motor imagery-based BCI can increase the excitability of corticospinal output
39
engaged for imagined movements. Robust increases in the corticospinal excitability
40
(CSE) have also been reported in relation to beta ERD magnitudes after BCI
41
interventions (Kraus et al., 2016a). Moreover, we recently demonstrated that the
42
association between sensorimotor mu ERD during a BCI control and the excitability of
43
ipsilateral M1 varies depending on whether the users imagine distal hand or proximal
44
arm movements (Hasegawa et al., 2017). Yet it remains to be clarified whether the
45
increase in induced M1 excitability in relation to ERD magnitude is confined to the
46
muscle representation engaged by the imagery task. Like an increase in CSE is specific
47
to the muscles involved in motor imagery (Fadiga et al., 1999; Hashimoto and
48
Rothwell, 1999; Ikai et al., 1996; Levin et al., 2004; Tremblay et al., 2001), the
49
BCI-induced excitability increase should be confined to the agonist muscle without
50
affecting the excitability of antagonist muscle. However, since somatotopy in ERD
51
patterns during arm/hand movements are highly overlapping (Miller et al., 2007), the
52
users of motor imagery-based BCI may elevate ERD by engaging imagery of incorrect
53
or unspecific movements. If this were the case, the BCI-induced excitability increase
54
could be expressed in the agonist and antagonist muscle representations.
55
To test these hypotheses, we used BCI-inspired brain state-dependent brain stimulation,
56
in which transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is given depending on the current
57
state of sensorimotor EEG oscillations. In the experiment, participants were asked to
58
perform kinesthetic motor imagery of wrist movements (i.e., flexion or extension) and
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
30
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
they received online visual feedback of the amount of ERD during the motor imagery.
60
Single-pulse or paired-pulse TMS was given over the M1 contralateral to the imagined
61
movement and triggered by the extent of ERD. Single-pulses were applied to assess
62
CSE, and paired-pulses were applied to probe GABA A-mediated short-latency
63
intracortical inhibition (SICI) (Ziemann et al., 1996) and NMDA-mediated intracortical
64
facilitation (ICF) (Ziemann et al., 1998). Motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) were
65
recorded with surface electromyography (EMG) from wrist flexor and extensor
66
muscles. This procedure enabled us to test whether motor imagery-based BCI
67
selectively facilitated corticomotor excitability in the imagined muscle movement (i.e.,
68
agonist) relative to the non-imagined movement (i.e., antagonist) and the association
69
between the amount of BCI-induced ERD increase and alterations in cortical
70
excitability.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
59
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2. Materials and Methods
72
2.1 Participants
73
24 volunteers were recruited. 10 participated in experiment 1 (aged 21.8 ± 1.5 years; six men,
74
four women). The results of experiment 1 prompted a follow-up experiment (experiment 2).
75
We screened additional 14 volunteers of whom 10 volunteers participated in experiment 2
76
(aged 22.4 ± 0.8 years; eight men, two women). The screening procedure is described in detail
77
in the subsequent section. All participants were right-handed and had normal vision
78
(according to self-reports), with no history of brain disorders or other health problems. All
79
participants were naïve to the purpose of the experiment and the procedures. The purpose and
80
experimental procedure were explained to the participants, before written informed consent
81
was obtained. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of Keio University
82
(IRB approved number: 23-16, 24-23) and performed in accordance with the
83
Declaration of Helsinki.
84
2.2 TMS and EMG
85
Surface EMG was recorded from the right extensor carpi radialis (ECR) and flexor carpi
86
radialis (FCR) muscles using two pairs of bipolar Ag/AgCl electrodes ( φ = 10 mm). The
87
cathode electrodes were placed over the muscle belly, while the anode electrodes were placed
88
20 mm distal from the cathode electrodes. Impedance for all channels was maintained below
89
20 kΩ throughout the experiment. EMG signals were band-pass filtered (5–2000 Hz with 2nd
90
order Butterworth) with a 50-Hz notch to avoid power-line noise contamination, digitized at 5
91
kHz using a biosignal amplifier (Neuropack MEB-9200; Nihon Koden, Tokyo, Japan), and
92
monitored throughout the experiment. The EMG data of each trial were stored on a computer
93
from 50 ms before the TMS pulse to 150 ms after the pulse.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
71
94
TMS was delivered using two interconnected single-pulse magnetic stimulators
95
(Magstim BiStim 2 ; Magstim, Whiteland, UK) producing a monophasic current
96
waveform in a 70-mm figure-of-eight coil. In experiment 1, we identified the optimal
97
coil position at which a single-pulse TMS evoked a MEP response in the ECR muscle
98
with the lowest stimulus intensity, referred to as the motor hotspot. At this position, the
99
coil was oriented approximately 45° to the sagittal plane. The resting motor threshold
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(RMT) was defined as the lowest stimulator output eliciting an MEP in the relaxed
101
ECR of >50 µV peak-to-peak in 5 out of 10 trials (Rossini et al., 1994). The motor
102
hotspot was marked with ink over the scalp to ensure an exact repositioning of the coil
103
throughout the experiment. In experiment 2, the motor hotspot and RMT for the FCR
104
muscle were first identified, using the same procedure as for experiment 1. We
105
subsequently tested the RMT of the ECR muscle by applying TMS to the FCR hotspot
106
in 14 volunteers in order to confirm that comparable MEP amplitudes can be elicited in
107
the FCR and ECR muscles. The difference in RMTs of the ECR and FCR muscles was
108
≤1% of the maximum stimulator output in 10 of the 14 participants. Participants with
109
matched RMTs were included in experiment 2.
SC
RI PT
100
Single-pulse TMS was set at an intensity of 120% of the RMT. In paired-pulse TMS
111
paradigm, a subthreshold conditioning stimulus was set at 80% of the RMT, and was
112
delivered through the same magnetic coil at 2, 3, 10 or 15 ms prior to the suprathreshold test
113
stimulus adjusted to 120% of the RMT (Vaalto et al., 2011; Takemi et al., 2013). The
114
stimulus intensity remained constant throughout the experiment for each participant.
115
2.3 EEG measurement and analyses
116
EEG was recorded with five Ag/AgCl electrodes ( φ = 10 mm) placed at C3 and 30 mm
117
anterior, posterior, medial and lateral to C3 to cover the sensorimotor hand area. Ground and
118
reference electrodes were placed at the forehead and left earlobe, respectively. Impedance for
119
all channels was maintained below 5 kΩ throughout the experiment. EEG signals were
120
band-pass filtered (0.1–100 Hz with 4th order Butterworth) with a 50 Hz notch, and digitized
121
at 512 Hz using a biosignal amplifier (g.USBamp; g.tec medical engineering, Graz, Austria).
122
The EEG data was transmitted to the workspace of MATLAB 2011a (The Mathworks, Natick,
123
MA) for online analysis and offline storage.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
110
124
The EEG signal from C3 was then re-referenced by the four neighboring
125
electrodes. This derivation, referred to as a Laplacian, is a known spatial filter that
126
emphasizes sensorimotor oscillations originating immediately below the electrodes
127
(McFarland et al., 1997). The Laplacian EEG data were segmented into successive
128
512-point (1000 ms) windows with 480-point overlapping. Fast Fourier transformation
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 129
with a Hanning window was applied in each segment to estimate the power spectrum
130
density. ERD was calculated at each segment with a time resolution of 62.5 ms and a
131
frequency resolution of 1 Hz, according to the following calculation:
ERD( f ,t) =
R( f ) − A( f ,t) R( f )
RI PT
132
A denotes the power spectrum density of the EEG, at time, t, and frequency, f. R is the
134
mean power spectrum of the baseline period, defined as the 3-s resting interval between
135
4–1 s prior to the onset of motor tasks. A large positive value indicates a large decrease in
136
the EEG power spectrum compared with a baseline period. Note that ERD value at time t
137
was delayed by 500 ms on average due to FFT over 1000 ms window with symmetrical
138
Hanning function.
139
2.4 Experimental protocol
140
The experiment consisted of three conditions in the following order: screening, resting, and
141
BCI condition (Fig. 1a). All conditions were performed on the same day with the same
142
EEG and EMG electrode settings. The participants sat in a comfortable armchair and
143
placed their hand, palm side down, on the armrest. A 20-inch computer monitor was
144
placed 60 cm in front of the participant’s eyes. In the screening condition (Fig. 1a, top), the
145
subjects repeatedly performed sustained right wrist extension (experiment 1) or flexion
146
(experiment 2). Each trial started with the presentation of the word ‘Rest’ in the center of the
147
monitor. Six seconds later, the word ‘Ready’ was presented for 1 s. Thereafter a vertical line
148
was displayed in the center of the monitor, which prompted participants to perform 5-s of
149
sustained wrist extension or wrist flexion with their right hand. A single trial lasted 12 s and
150
was repeated 30 times.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
133
151
ERD is typically observed over the sensorimotor cortex contralateral to the
152
imagery hand, but the most reactive frequency band displaying ERD slightly varies
153
between participants (Pfurtscheller et al., 2006; Takemi et al., 2013, 2015). Thus the
154
3-Hz width frequency band displaying the largest ERD was determined within mu and
155
beta frequencies (7–26 Hz) in each participant by using his/her EEG signals recorded
156
in the screening condition that may reflect cortical activity during voluntary wrist
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
movement. The frequency band displaying the largest ERD was calculated immediately
158
after the screening condition and used for online ERD calculation in the BCI condition.
159
The resting condition (Fig. 1a, middle), which served as a control for the BCI condition,
160
was always conducted before the BCI condition. Similar to the screening condition,
161
each trial started with the presentation of the word ‘Rest’ for 6 s, before the word ‘Ready’ was
162
subsequently displayed for 1 s. The monitor then displayed the vertical line and a horizontal
163
bar that moved randomly for 5 s while participants remained at rest. In 80% of trials, one
164
single or paired-pulse TMS (with inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) at 2, 3, 10, and 15 ms) was
165
randomly applied to the motor hotspot while the horizontal bar was moving to avoid
166
habituation with repeated stimulation. The experimenter continuously monitored EMG
167
activities of the target muscles to ensure that the participants maintained complete relaxation.
168
Trials contaminated by more than ±25 µV of background EMG activities were discarded
169
online. A total of 50 MEPs without any background EMG activities (10 MEPs for each pulse
170
configuration) were collected.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
157
The BCI condition was conducted using the same time scheme as the screening session
172
(Fig. 1a, bottom). Instead of performing the actual movement, participants remained their
173
right hand (palm side down) on the armrest and performed kinesthetic motor imagery of the
174
sustained wrist extension in experiment 1 and of the sustained wrist flexion in experiment 2
175
exerting maximum mental effort. Besides, we did not explicitly give instruction regarding
176
amplitude and force of movements. The length of the horizontal bar displayed on the monitor
177
was updated every 62.5 ms according to the ERD magnitude during the motor imagery
178
period. Thus the participants received online visual feedback of the ERD (Fig. 1b). The
179
center of the axis represented no ERD, without any amplitude changes in the EEG from the
180
baseline period. The position of the bar shifted to the right when the ERD value was positive;
181
the bar moved to the left when ERD value was negative. Before the BCI condition,
182
participants were informed that successful motor imagery (i.e., positive ERD) would shift the
183
bar to the right edge of the monitor, while unsuccessful motor imagery (i.e., negative ERD)
184
would shift the bar to the left edge.
AC C
EP
TE D
171
185
The BCI condition consisted of two blocks. In one block, single-pulse TMS or
186
paired-pulse TMS at short ISI (2 or 3 ms) or long ISI (10 or 15 ms) was given to the motor
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
hotspot at the first instance when ERD exceeded 5% during the motor imagery period. In the
188
other block, TMS was triggered at the first instance that ERD exceeded 15% during the motor
189
imagery period. To test SICI, we chose the short ISI, which showed stronger inhibition (2 or 3
190
ms) in the resting condition. Similarly, for ICF we chose the long ISI, which showed stronger
191
facilitation (10 or 15 ms) in the resting condition. The order of the blocks and single and
192
paired-pulse TMS within a single block were pseudo-randomized and counter-balanced
193
between participants to minimize the accumulative effect of TMS in conjunction with ERD
194
(Kraus et al., 2016b). Trials contaminated by more than ±25 µV of background EMG activity
195
were discarded online and 30 MEPs without any background EMG activities were collected
196
in each block (10 MEPs for each pulse configuration). The BCI condition was paused every
197
15 min to give the participants a short break and avoid mental fatigue. In the breaks, we asked
198
participants whether they wanted to drink, readjust their seat position, and put eye drops in
199
order to avoid swallowing, neck muscle contraction and eye blinks, which induce large
200
artifacts in EEG, as much as possible. The TMS coil was then positioned to the motor hotspot
201
and the remaining trials were resumed. We confirmed that the RMT of the agonist muscle did
202
not change before and after any suspension.
203
2.5 MEP analysis
204
Peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes were measured offline. We analyzed the data in the resting and
205
BCI conditions separately. For the resting condition data, MEP amplitudes were averaged for
206
each TMS protocol (i.e., single-pulse and paired-pulses with short ISI (2 or 3 ms) and long ISI
207
(10 or 15 ms)) and muscle (i.e., FCR and ECR). To ensure that paired-pulse TMS with
208
different ISIs induced intracortical inhibition and facilitation, the mean MEP amplitude in the
209
resting condition was analyzed using a two-way ANOVA, with the TMS protocol and muscle
210
as within-subject factors. Here, SICI was evaluated using the mean MEP amplitude provoked
211
with a short ISI that showed smaller MEP. Likewise, ICF was evaluated using the mean MEP
212
amplitude with a long ISI showing larger MEP.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
187
213
Peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes in the BCI condition were averaged for each TMS
214
protocol, muscle and ERD magnitude (5%ERD and 15%ERD). To quantify SICI and ICF the
215
mean MEP amplitude by paired-pulse TMS was expressed as a percentage of the mean MEP
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
amplitude by single-pulse TMS. Mean single-pulse MEP amplitude in the BCI condition was
217
subtracted from the mean amplitude obtained in the resting condition. Also, mean SICI and
218
ICF strength in the BCI condition was subtracted from the mean strength obtained in the
219
resting condition. These subtracted values were then statistically analyzed using ANOVA and
220
bootstrap test. To test whether motor imagery-based BCI modulated different aspect of
221
intra-M1 excitability, an ANOVA was separately applied to the single-pulse MEP, SICI and
222
ICF. ERD magnitude and muscle were set as within-subject factors, while the imagery task
223
(wrist flexion or extension) was set as the between-subject factor. The normal distribution of
224
the data was confirmed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Post-hoc pairwise
225
comparisons were performed using the Sidak procedure to correct for multiple
226
comparisons. A bootstrap test was performed to examine whether a distribution of TMS
227
measures obtained during imagery of wrist movement was significantly different from
228
those in the resting condition. For each TMS measure, data were randomly sampled
229
with replacement and the bootstrapped mean was calculated 5000 times. A histogram
230
of the bootstrapped means was then used to test significance with a Bonferroni-Holm
231
correction. The type I error was set to 0.05. IBM SPSS Statistics, version 22 for
232
Windows, was used for ANOVA and post-hoc pairwise comparisons. Bootstrap test was
233
performed using MATLAB 2014a.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
216
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3. Results
235
3.1 Electroencephalogram
236
In the screening session, all participants showed ERD over the sensorimotor cortex
237
contralateral to the hand executed wrist movement. The median and interquartile range of
238
experiment 1 were 12.6 and 9.2–13.9% (during 5-s of sustained right wrist extension),
239
respectively, and 14.9 and 11.0–19.2% in experiment 2 (during 5-s of sustained right wrist
240
flexion), respectively. The median and interquartile range of the frequency band showing
241
strongest ERD were 13 and 8–15 Hz in experiment 1 and 14 and 9–15 Hz in experiment 2,
242
respectively. The ERD characteristics in the screening session were not different between
243
experiments 1 and 2, based on Mann-Whitney test (ERD magnitude: p = 0.38; the frequency
244
band showing strongest ERD: p = 0.59).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
234
Since ERD is a relative measure and depends on the baseline power, we also compared
246
the Laplacian EEG power for the baseline period (4–1 s prior to the onset of motor
247
imagery in the BCI condition and 1–0 s before TMS was triggered in the resting condition)
248
in the trials where TMS was applied among conditions. The baseline EEG power at the 3-Hz
249
frequency band displaying the largest ERD, defined in the screening session, was averaged
250
over the trials, time, and frequencies. Friedman’s test revealed that the grand average of the
251
baseline EEG power was not different between conditions (rest, 5%ERD, 15%ERD) in both
252
experiment 1 ( χ 2 (2) = 2.6, p = 0.27) and 2 ( χ 2 (2) = 0.8, p = 0.67). Considering that the
253
baseline power was calculated with EEG signals more than 2 s later from the offset of
254
motor imagery, the baseline power was not contaminated by post-movement oscillatory
255
rebound.
256
3.2 MEP, SICI, and ICF in the resting condition
257
Figure 3 summarizes the group results. Data of a single subject are shown in figure 2 (left
258
column). Paired-pulse TMS at short ISI induced inhibition of the MEP (i.e., SICI), while
259
paired-pulse TMS at long ISI facilitated the MEP amplitude relative to single-pulse
260
stimulation (i.e., ICF). For the data acquired in experiment 1, ANOVA demonstrated an
261
interaction between the ISI and muscle (F(2,18) = 3.82, p = 0.042). For the ECR muscle,
262
post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed smaller MEP amplitudes at short ISI (i.e., SICI
AC C
EP
TE D
245
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
condition) relative to single-pulse TMS and paired-pulse TMS at long ISI (p < 0.001).
264
The FCR muscle showed only a trend difference towards smaller MEP amplitudes in
265
the SICI condition relative to single-pulse TMS (p = 0.061) and the ICF protocol (p =
266
0.057). In addition, MEP amplitudes of the ECR muscle tended to be larger than MEP
267
amplitudes of the FCR muscle for single-pulse TMS (p = 0.059) and the ICF protocol (p
268
= 0.069).
RI PT
263
The strength of SICI is known to be weaker when the test MEP amplitude is smaller
270
(Sanger et al., 2001). Given the trend showing differences in the MEP amplitudes of the ECR
271
and FCR muscles in experiment 1, this may complicate the comparison of SICI in the two
272
muscles with respect to the ERD magnitude. This prompted us to perform experiment 2, in
273
which we adjusted the mean test MEP amplitudes of the two muscles at rest. We only
274
enrolled those 10 subjects who showed no relevant difference in the RMTs between
275
ECR and FCR muscles (≤1% of the maximum stimulator output). In experiment 2, an
276
ANOVA using mean MEP amplitude as dependent variable demonstrated no interaction
277
between the TMS protocol and muscle (F(2,18) = 0.91, p = 0.42), suggesting matched MEP
278
amplitudes in the two muscles. A main effect of TMS protocol was confirmed (F(2,18) = 43.8,
279
p < 0.001). A post-hoc test revealed that the MEP provoked by the SICI protocol was
280
smaller than the MEP provoked by the single-pulse TMS and ICF protocol (p < 0.001).
281
In addition, the MEP of ICF was larger than the single-pulse MEP (p < 0.01).
282
3.3 MEP, SICI, and ICF in the BCI condition
283
Figure 4 summarizes the group results, and data from a single participant are shown in figure
284
2 (middle and right column). Motor imagery-based BCI increased the unconditioned MEP
285
amplitude evoked by single-pulse TMS. The increase in MEP depended on the magnitude of
286
ERD and was not limited to the agonist muscle engaged by motor imagery task (Fig. 4a).
287
Accordingly, ANOVA revealed a main effect of ERD (F(1,18) = 5.09, p = 0.037) and an
288
interaction between muscle and imagery task (F(1,18) = 9.76, p = 0.006). No other interactions
289
were found (p > 0.25). Post-hoc analysis revealed that single-pulse MEPs had larger mean
290
amplitudes at 15%ERD than 5%ERD (p = 0.037). During imagery of wrist flexion,
291
single-pulse MEPs of the FCR muscle were larger than the MEPs of the ECR muscle (p =
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
269
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
0.016). In addition, bootstrap tests revealed an increase in single-pulse MEP during motor
293
imagery relative to the resting condition regardless of the ERD magnitude, which mostly
294
occurred in the agonist muscle. During imagery of the wrist flexion, significant
295
differences were found in the FCR muscle at 5%ERD and 15%ERD (p < 0.05). During
296
imagery of wrist extension, significant differences were found in the ECR muscle at
297
5%ERD, and in the ECR and FCR muscles at 15%ERD (p < 0.05).
RI PT
292
In contrast to the unconditioned MEPs, the effects of motor imagery-based BCI on SICI
299
were specific to the muscle engaged by the imagery of wrist movement (Fig. 4b). This was
300
reflected by a three-way interaction among ERD, muscle, and imagery task (F(1,18) = 22.3, p
301
< 0.001), showing that both the amount of ERD and the imagery task influenced the relative
302
strength of SICI in a muscle-specific fashion. Post-hoc pair-wise comparisons provided
303
consistent results for the two imagery tasks. Attenuation of SICI scaled with the magnitude of
304
ERD in the agonist muscle that was targeted by the motor imagery task (p < 0.001). Moreover,
305
there was a significant difference of SICI between the agonist and antagonist muscle at
306
15%ERD (p = 0.001, by imagery of wrist extension; p = 0.002, by imagery of wrist flexion).
307
A bootstrap test for SICI strength during motor imagery relative to the resting
308
condition revealed that, regardless of the imagery task, SICI was only significantly reduced
309
in the agonist muscle at 15%ERD (p < 0.05).
TE D
M AN U
SC
298
In contrast to SICI, motor imagery-based BCI had no effect on the magnitude of ICF
311
(Fig. 4c). The respective ANOVA yielded no interaction between any factors (F(1,18) = 0.46−
312
1.65, p = 0.22−0.51). Further, there was no main effect for ERD (F(1,18) = 0.44, p = 0.51),
313
muscle (F(1,18) = 0.16, p = 0.70), and imagery task (F(1,18) = 0.47, p = 0.50).
314
3.4 Relationship between task compliance and SICI modulation in the BCI condition
315
Cortical excitability can be influenced not only by ERD magnitude but also by the degree of
316
task compliance (de Lange et al., 2008). Thus, we calculated the percentage of task success,
317
as the ratio of TMS applied trials to total number of motor imagery trials for each participant.
318
A paired t-test demonstrated a difference in the task success rate between the two conditions
319
in experiment 1 (p < 0.001: 54 ± 6% and 30 ± 4% at 5%ERD and 15%ERD, respectively) and
320
experiment 2 (p < 0.001: 57 ± 7% and 30 ± 6% at 5%ERD and 15%ERD, respectively).
AC C
EP
310
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
However, there was no correlation between SICI strength in the agonist muscle, which was
322
reduced with a higher ERD, and the task success rates in those conditions (Experiment 1: r(18)
323
= -0.22, p = 0.34; Experiment 2: r(18) = -0.26, p = 0.27). This result indicated that the
324
reduction of SICI by the use of motor imagery-based BCI was not associated with task
325
difficulty.
RI PT
321
We also concerned that the duration of kinesthetic motor imagery (i.e., time since
327
participants started to perform motor imagery until TMS pulse was applied) affected the
328
amount of SICI reduction. Thus, using a paired t-test, we compared the duration of
329
kinesthetic motor imagery before TMS applied in the 5%ERD and 15%ERD conditions. The
330
results were as follows: 1.6 ± 0.1 s at 5%ERD and 3.0 ± 0.3 s at 15%ERD in
331
experiment 1 (p < 0.001); and 1.6 ± 0.2 s at 5%ERD and 3.1 ± 0.2 s at 15%ERD in
332
experiment 2 (p < 0.001). However, there was no correlation between the SICI strength
333
in the agonist muscle and duration of kinesthetic motor imagery in the BCI condition
334
(Experiment 1: r(18) = 0.31, p = 0.18; Experiment 2: r(18) = 0.34, p = 0.14). The present
335
results are compatible with the notion that the attenuation of SICI strength in the agonist
336
muscle was rather associated with ERD magnitude than the duration of kinesthetic
337
motor imagery. Yet a non-significant correlation is not sufficient to prove the lack of
338
causal relationship between the variables.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
326
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
4. Discussion
340
In the present study, we tested whether the increase of M1 excitability by motor
341
imagery-based BCI is confined to the muscle representation engaged by the imagery
342
task. The findings can be summarized as follows. First, motor imagery-based BCI
343
increased the CSE, which was measured by single-pulse MEP. This increase was
344
associated with the ERD magnitude but not confined to the agonist muscle at high ERD
345
level. Second, although ICF showed no modulation by ERD, SICI was reduced when
346
the BCI required a high ERD level. This intracortical disinhibition was only found in
347
the agonist muscle targeted by motor imagery without affecting the excitability of the
348
antagonist muscle. Third, two experiments in which participants either performed
349
kinesthetic motor imagery of wrist extension or flexion yielded analogous results,
350
providing internal replication.
351
4.1 Changes in corticomotor excitability, evaluated with EEG-triggered TMS
352
At both levels of ERD, motor imagery-based BCI increased corticomotor excitability
353
as probed by single-pulse TMS. The facilitatory effect of our ERD-based BCI approach
354
was most prominently expressed in the muscle targeted by motor imagery, but also was
355
present in the antagonist muscle at the high ERD level. In agreement with the present
356
results, the M1 excitability profiles of an agonist muscle have been shown to increase
357
during a period of a voluntary movement (Hoshiyama et al., 1997; Reynolds and Ashby,
358
1999). We postulated that the BCI-induced increase in the corticomotor excitability
359
was mediated by analogous representations as the corresponding motor execution. Our
360
results also demonstrated larger mean MEP amplitudes at the 15%ERD relative to the
361
5%ERD. These results are compatible with the notion that the CSE of hand/arm
362
muscles may inversely scale with the synchronization of sensorimotor oscillations
363
(Hummel et al., 2002).
364
Contrary to M1 excitability profiles just before voluntary wrist movement (Hoshiyama
365
et al., 1997; Reynolds and Ashby, 1999), the BCI-induced increase in the CSE was not
366
confined to the agonist muscle of imagined movement, especially at the high ERD level.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
339
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Although this may be surprising, a previous study evaluating CSE during motor
368
imagery actually demonstrated facilitation of the antagonist muscle MEP (Kasai et al.,
369
1997). Others revealed the inhibition of the antagonist MEP in 18 of the 28 tested
370
muscles during motor imagery (Ikai et al., 1996), but the majority of these changes
371
were not statistically significant. These observations led us to suggest that motor
372
imagery-based BCI have dynamic effects on the M1 excitability, which are similar but
373
not identical to those seen during motor execution. Note that the fixed order of the
374
conditions (resting and BCI) might have introduced order effects, as a TMS study
375
found single pulse TMS repeated every 2.5-5.5 s induced cumulative increases in the
376
CSE (Pellicciari et al., 2016). However, we expected such order effects to be stronger
377
if the BCI condition would be followed by measurements at rest. Therefore, we decided
378
against counter-balancing the two conditions.
379
We also found modulation of SICI by motor imagery-based BCI. SICI was
380
significantly reduced only at the high level of ERD. Moreover, contrary to the results
381
of the single-pulse MEP, BCI-associated disinhibition was confined to the agonist
382
muscle representation regardless of the amount of ERD. SICI was significantly
383
attenuated only at 15%ERD compared to the resting condition, unlike single-pulse
384
MEP, which was facilitated at 5%ERD. These discrepancies between MEP and SICI
385
results suggested a distinct role of MEP increase and SICI decrease in a motor
386
imagery-based BCI task. As a local intracortical phenomenon, SICI would be well
387
placed to modulate the relationship between adjacent intracortical representations via
388
changes in horizontal connections (Reis et al., 2008). Conversely, the increase in MEP
389
is a direct phenotype of corticospinal outputs, which are affected by inputs from
390
non-primary motor areas as well as spinal excitability. Thus, BCI-induced SICI
391
modulation may be not to directly drive an increase in the CSE, but rather serve to
392
focus it appropriately to the task. Furthermore, a previous study investigating the
393
precise timing of SICI changes in a simple reaction time protocol revealed that the
394
reduction of SICI occurred later than the increase of single-pulse MEP (Nikolova
395
et al., 2006). Augmentation of the MEP amplitude was initiated approximately 120
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
367
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ms before the reaction onset, though the inhibition was abolished less than 60 ms
397
prior to the onset. This relationship of early-MEP and late-SICI modulation in a
398
reaction time task was similar to our findings (Fig. 5). In our motor imagery-based
399
BCI task, an increase in the MEP was initiated with a weaker ERD than the
400
reduction of SICI. Considering that the ERD gradually increases toward the
401
movement onset (Deiber et al., 2012), voluntary movements and a BCI control may
402
share a specific aspect of motor control. A motor imagery-based BCI would thus
403
allow to primarily modulate corticomotor activity close to the changes
404
accompanying the actual movements. Indeed, corroborating physiological findings
405
have demonstrated that BCI may bridge the gap between motor imagery and actual
406
movements (Bauer et al., 2015).
407
Our results demonstrated that a motor imagery-based BCI exploiting ERD induces
408
deactivation of inhibitory M1 neurons innervating corticomotor outputs to the agonist
409
muscle. However, we still cannot determine the extent to which the observed SICI
410
changes were related merely to motor imagery. In this study, TMS pulse was given at a
411
certain ERD magnitude, of which the participant was aware due to the visual feedback.
412
Thus, the increased M1 excitability might be related to the imagery task and/or the
413
awareness of successful trials. Yet no correlation between the amount of SICI and task
414
success rates across participants suggested that motor imagery rather than
415
neurofeedback context caused modulation of motor cortex excitability. This
416
interpretation is supported by a recent study in which motor imagery with and without
417
neurofeedback induced significant modulation of ERD magnitude (Darvishi et al.,
418
2017). Our results also proved the possibility that neither task difficulty nor duration of
419
motor imagery modulated M1 excitability. Given that the motor imagery of hand
420
movements at different force levels did not alter both MEP amplitudes (Park and Li,
421
2011) and ERD magnitudes (Murphy et al., 2016; Zaepffel et al., 2013), the
422
neuromodulatory effects by the use of the present BCI approach may stem from brain
423
activities related to motor imagery rather than to task compliance. However, future
424
studies that compare the effects of motor imagery with and without online visual
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
396
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
feedback of ongoing ERD are needed to clarify whether ERD-based BCI immediately
426
boosts corticomotor excitability relative to motor imagery alone.
427
4.2 How do sensorimotor EEG oscillations reflect TMS responses?
428
The present results demonstrated a negative relationship between the amount of ERD
429
and GABA A-mediated SICI in the agonist muscle of motor imagery task, but not in the
430
antagonist muscle. However, considering that EEG signal reflects a spatial sum of
431
neuronal activity, one may wonder how the disinhibition of the agonist muscle
432
representation by kinesthetic motor imagery was selectively reflected in the amount of
433
ERD. A key factor generating sensorimotor oscillations is a negative feedback loop
434
composed by excitatory thalamo-cortical relay nucleus and inhibitory thalamic
435
reticular nucleus (Steriade and Llinás, 1988). This negative feedback loop synchronizes
436
the firing of multiple cortical neurons. This generates EEG oscillations, with a voltage
437
that consists of a spatial sum of postsynaptic potentials. The amplitude is particularly
438
large when the loop is stable, such as at rest. However, the down-regulation of
439
inhibitory neural activity leads to the loop divergent. This results in desynchronization
440
of neural activities and decrease in the sum of postsynaptic potentials (Pfurtscheller
441
and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Suffczynski et al., 2001). Thus, suppression of the
442
sensorimotor oscillations could be regarded as a neural marker reflecting deactivation
443
of inhibitory neurons. The present results showed that motor imagery focally
444
attenuated the activity of inhibitory circuits in M1 in a dynamic manner. Given the
445
results, we assumed that the loop involving the cortical neurons projecting to the
446
agonist muscle was selectively divergent during motor imagery. This assumption is
447
supported by TMS studies in humans, which indicated that kinesthetic motor imagery
448
has dynamic and focal effects on the excitability of the agonist M1 area (Fadiga et al.,
449
1999; Hashimoto and Rothwell, 1999; Ikai et al., 1996; Levin et al., 2004; Tremblay et
450
al., 2001). Altogether, it suggests that partial desynchronization of the neuronal
451
populations innervating agonist muscle by the selective disinhibition might lead a
452
decrease in the sum of postsynaptic potentials, resulting in the attenuation of
453
sensorimotor EEG oscillations, even when the remaining neurons were in the loop and
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
425
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT synchronized.
455
4.3 Practical implication for the clinical use of BCI training
456
Several mechanisms have been hypothesized for neuroplasticity that facilitates motor
457
recovery, which might occur through the continued use of motor imagery-based BCI.
458
For example, somatosensory feedback triggered by ERD of ipsilesional M1 is believed
459
to induce Hebbian-like plasticity by which co-activation of the M1 and periphery
460
fosters the ipsilesional sensorimotor loop (Belardinelli et al., 2017; Kraus et al., 2016a;
461
Soekadar et al., 2014). Real-time visual feedback of the ERD strength allows the users
462
to voluntarily and repeatedly regulate the ipsilesional M1 excitability based on their
463
intention, which could promote use-dependent plasticity (Young et al., 2014). Note that
464
in either case a premise underlying neuroplasticity through BCI training is that ERD
465
magnitude during motor imagery reflects focal increase of the M1 excitability to the
466
targeted muscle of imagined movements.
467
In the current study, single-pulse TMS of M1 revealed that motor imagery-based BCI
468
can dynamically modulate CSE of the agonist muscle depending on ERD magnitude in
469
the healthy participants who were naïve to motor imagery task. This result suggests
470
people can perform motor imagery of targeted muscle contraction at a first instance
471
being the user of the BCI. However, since the BCI-induced increase in the CSE was not
472
confined to the agonist muscle at high ERD level, muscle-specific plasticity of
473
ERD-based BCI may be caused by other mechanisms. Our study points to a
474
muscle-selective effect on SICI as possible candidate mechanism. Motor imagery
475
caused a muscle-specific intracortical disinhibition of the agonist muscle but not the
476
antagonist muscle at a high level of ERD. Weakening the excitability of intracortical
477
inhibitory circuits concurrently with motor training is known to enhance synaptic
478
plasticity and foster the learning process (Ziemann and Siebner, 2008). The use of BCI
479
may acutely increase CSE not focally to the target muscles, but repeated use of such
480
BCI will aid to induce plasticity mostly in the M1 of the target muscle representation
481
by the selective disinhibition. Moreover, the somatosensory feedback (e.g., electrical
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
454
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
stimulation to the targeted muscle of motor imagery and robotic orthosis that executes
483
imagined movement) contingent upon the occurrence of ERD allows sensorimotor
484
co-activation to be restricted to the targeted corticomuscular region (Kraus et al.,
485
2016a). This should help selectively reinforce the sensorimotor representations in a
486
BCI task (Ono et al., 2014; Soekadar et al., 2014). Adaptive modification of the ERD
487
threshold at which somatosensory feedback is given is known to foster an increase in
488
ERD throughout the intervention (Naros et al., 2016). This could further down-regulate
489
SICI in the brain, resulting in enhancement of motor improvement. However, to our
490
knowledge, no one has examined effects of the BCI training on cortical representations
491
of functionally reciprocal muscles. Yet, based on a previous study, we inferred that
492
cortical plasticity induced by motor imagery-based BCI is effector-specific and reflects
493
the muscular pattern required to overtly perform the imagined action (Pichiorri et al.,
494
2011).
495
4.4 Limitations
496
We found that the down-regulation of SICI in relation to ERD magnitude was only
497
prominent in the agonist muscle without affecting the SICI of the antagonist muscle.
498
While SICI reflects intracortical GABA A inhibition, other types of GABA-mediated
499
inhibition, such as long-interval intracortical inhibition (LICI) and late cortical
500
disinhibition (LCD), might also be concurrently modulated by the use of motor
501
imagery-based BCI (Chong and Stinear, 2017). LICI and LCD are thought to reflect the
502
degree of postsynaptic GABAB inhibition, and presynaptic GABAB disinhibition,
503
respectively, which should be both relevant to the facilitation of cortical plasticity
504
following motor training (Di Pino et al., 2014). Therefore, future studies are warranted
505
to test how GABAB related intracortical inhibition scales with ERD magnitude.
506
In the present study, the frequency band of ERD testing relationship with MEP, SICI,
507
and ICF has a bias towards mu frequency band. This is because ERD magnitude
508
accompanying actual wrist movements (evaluated in the screening session) was larger
509
in the mu frequency band than the beta band for most participants. It still remains to be
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
482
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 510
determined which frequency band should be used for ERD-based BCI in post-stroke
511
hemiplegia
512
Ramos-Murguialday et al., 2013), beta ERD (Belardinelli et al., 2017), and both mu
513
and beta ERD (Mukaino et al., 2014; Pichiorri et al., 2015) have all shown significant
514
improvements of motor function in post-stroke patients. To tackle this question, future
515
studies need to systematically compare the effects on motor recovery that can be
516
achieved with motor imagery-based BCIs informed by mu- and beta-ERD. In our
517
previous study, we found that ERD magnitude in the frequency band showing largest
518
ERD during voluntary movements, but neither in the fixed mu (7−13 Hz) nor beta
519
(14−26 Hz) band, reflects spinal excitability (Takemi et al., 2015). This finding
520
suggests that individual adjustments of the target frequency band for BCI may be most
521
suited to promote functional recovery.
BCIs
operated
by mu
ERD
(Ono
et
al.,
2014;
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
rehabilitation.
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
5. Conclusions
523
In the present study, we tested whether the increase of M1 excitability by motor
524
imagery-based BCI was confined to the muscle representation engaged by motor
525
imagery task. We found motor imagery-based BCI has distinct effects on CSE and
526
intracortical inhibition depending on ERD magnitudes. The increase of CSE, measured
527
by single-pulse MEP, was associated with the ERD magnitude, but not confined to the
528
agonist muscle when the BCI task required a high level of ERD. SICI was significantly
529
reduced only at the high level of ERD, and this intracortical disinhibition was only
530
found in the agonist muscle without affecting the SICI of the antagonist muscle
531
regardless of the amount of ERD. These electrophysiological results were confirmed in
532
two independent experiments, in which participants either performed kinesthetic motor
533
imagery of wrist extension or flexion. This meant that motor imagery-based BCI
534
selectively deactivates inhibitory interneurons in the M1 representation innervating agonist
535
muscle. This finding is important as it implies that cortical plasticity occurring through
536
repeated use of the BCI may be rendered effector-specific.
537
Acknowledgements
538
We thank Sayoko Ishii for her technical support. This work was supported by a KAKENHI
539
“Non-linear Neuro-Oscillology” (15H05880) from Japan Society for the Promotion of
540
Science (JSPS) to JU and a Novo Nordisk Foundation Interdisciplinary Synergy Program
541
“BaSiCs” (NNF14OC0011413) to HRS. MT was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for JSPS
542
Fellows (16J02485).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
522
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 543
References
544
Bauer, R., Fels, M., Vukelić, M., Ziemann, U., Gharabaghi, A., 2015. Bridging the gap
545
between motor imagery and motor execution with a brain-robot interface.
546
Neuroimage 108, 319–327. Belardinelli, P., Laer, L., Ortiz, E., Braun, C., Gharabaghi, A., 2017. Plasticity of
RI PT
547 548
premotor cortico-muscular coherence in severely impaired stroke patients with
549
hand paralysis. NeuroImage: Clinical 14, 726–733.
551 552
Chong, B.W.X., Stinear, C.M., 2017. Modulation of motor cortex inhibition during motor imagery. J Neurophysiol 117, 1776–1784.
SC
550
Darvishi, S., Gharabaghi, A., Boulay, C.B., Ridding, M.C., Abbott, D., Baumert, M., 2017. Proprioceptive Feedback Facilitates Motor Imagery-Related Operant
554
Learning of Sensorimotor β-Band Modulation. Front Neurosci 11, 60.
555
doi:10.3389/fnins.2017.00060
556
M AN U
553
Deiber, M.P., Sallard, E., Ludwig, C., Ghezzi, C., Barral, J., Ibañez, V., 2012. EEG alpha activity reflects motor preparation rather than the mode of action selection.
558
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 6, 59. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00059
559
TE D
557
Di Pino, G., Pellegrino, G., Assenza, G., Capone, F., Ferreri, F., Formica, D., Ranieri, F., Tombini, M., Ziemann, U., Rothwell, J.C., Di Lazzaro, V., 2014. Modulation of
561
brain plasticity in stroke: a novel model for neurorehabilitation. Nat Rev Neurol 10,
562
597–608.
Espenhahn, S., de Berker, A.O., van Wijk, B.C.M., Rossiter, H.E., Ward, N.S., 2016.
AC C
563
EP
560
564
Movement-related beta oscillations show high intra-individual reliability.
565
Neuroimage 147, 175–185.
566
Fadiga, L., Buccino, G., Craighero, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Pavesi, G., 1999.
567
Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by motor imagery: a magnetic
568
stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 37, 147–158.
569
Hasegawa, K., Kasuga, S., Takasaki, K., Mizuno, K., Liu, M., Ushiba, J., 2017.
570
Ipsilateral EEG mu rhythm reflects the excitability of uncrossed pathways
571
projecting to shoulder muscles. J Neuroeng Rehabil 14, 85.
572
doi:10.1186/s12984-017-0294-2
25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 573 574 575
Hashimoto, R., Rothwell, J.C., 1999. Dynamic changes in corticospinal excitability during motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 125, 75–81. Hoshiyama, M., Kakigi, R., Koyama, S., Takeshima, Y., Watanabe, S., Shimojo, M., 1997. Temporal changes of pyramidal tract activities after decision of movement: a
577
study using transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex in humans.
578
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 105, 255–261.
579
RI PT
576
Hummel, F.C., Andres, F., Altenmüller, E., Dichgans, J., Gerloff, C., 2002. Inhibitory control of acquired motor programmes in the human brain. Brain 125, 404–420.
581
Ikai, T., Findley, T.W., Izumi, S., Hanayama, K., Kim, H., Daum, M.C., Andrews, J.F.,
SC
580
Diamond, B.J., 1996. Reciprocal inhibition in the forearm during voluntary
583
contraction and thinking about movement. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol 36,
584
295–304.
M AN U
582
585
Kasai, T., Kawai, S., Kawanishi, M., Yahagi, S., 1997. Evidence for facilitation of
586
motor evoked potentials (MEPs) induced by motor imagery. Brain Res 744,
587
147–150.
Kraus, D., Naros, G., Bauer, R., Leão, M.T., Ziemann, U., Gharabaghi, A., 2016a.
TE D
588 589
Brain-robot interface driven plasticity: Distributed modulation of corticospinal
590
excitability. Neuroimage 125, 522–532.
Kraus, D., Naros, G., Bauer, R., Khademi, F., Leão, M.T., Ziemann, U., Gharabaghi, A.,
592
2016b. Brain State-Dependent Transcranial Magnetic Closed-Loop Stimulation
593
Controlled by Sensorimotor Desynchronization Induces Robust Increase of
594
Corticospinal Excitability. Brain Stimul 9, 415–424.
AC C
EP
591
595
de Lange, F.P., Roelofs, K., Toni, I., 2008. Motor imagery: a window into the
596
mechanisms and alterations of the motor system. Cortex 44, 494–506.
597
Levin, O., Steyvers, M., Wenderoth, N., Li, Y., Swinnen, S.P., 2004. Dynamical
598
changes in corticospinal excitability during imagery of unimanual and bimanual
599
wrist movements in humans: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neurosci
600
Lett 359, 185–189.
601 602
McFarland, D.J., McCane, L.M., David, S.V., Wolpaw, J.R., 1997. Spatial filter selection for EEG-based communication. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 603
103, 386–394.
604
Miller, K.J., Leuthardt, E.C., Schalk, G., Rao, R.P.N., Anderson, N.R., Moran, D.W.,
605
Miller, J.W., Ojemann, J.G., 2007. Spectral changes in cortical surface potentials
606
during motor movement. J Neurosci 27, 2424–2432. Mukaino, M., Ono, T., Shindo, K., Fujiwara, T., Ota, T., Kimura, A., Liu, M., Ushiba,
608
J., 2014. Efficacy of brain-computer interface-driven neuromuscular electrical
609
stimulation for chronic paresis after stroke. J Rehabil Med 46, 378–382.
610
Murphy, B.A., Miller, J.P., Gunalan, K., Ajiboye, A.B., 2016. Contributions of
RI PT
607
Subsurface Cortical Modulations to Discrimination of Executed and Imagined
612
Grasp Forces through Stereoelectroencephalography. PLoS ONE 11, e0150359.
613
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0150359
M AN U
614
SC
611
Naros, G., Naros, I., Grimm, F., Ziemann, U., Gharabaghi, A., 2016. Reinforcement
615
learning of self-regulated sensorimotor β-oscillations improves motor performance.
616
Neuroimage 134, 142–152.
617
Nikolova, M., Pondev, N., Christova, L., Wolf, W., Kossev, A.R., 2006. Motor cortex excitability changes preceding voluntary muscle activity in simple reaction time
619
task. Eur J Appl Physiol 98, 212–219.
620
TE D
618
Ono, T., Shindo, K., Kawashima, K., Ota, N., Ito, M., Ota, T., Mukaino, M., Fujiwara, T., Kimura, A., Liu, M., Ushiba, J., 2014. Brain-computer interface with
622
somatosensory feedback improves functional recovery from severe hemiplegia due
623
to chronic stroke. Front Neuroeng 7, 19. doi:10.3389/fneng.2014.00019
625 626
AC C
624
EP
621
Park, W.H., Li, S., 2011. No graded responses of finger muscles to TMS during motor imagery of isometric finger forces. Neurosci Lett 494, 255–259. Pellicciari, M.C., Miniussi, C., Ferrari, C., Koch, G., Bortoletto, M., 2016. Ongoing
627
cumulative effects of single TMS pulses on corticospinal excitability: An intra- and
628
inter-block investigation. Clin Neurophysiol 127, 621–628.
629
Pfurtscheller, G., Brunner, C., Schlögl, A., Lopes da Silva, F.H., 2006. Mu rhythm
630
(de)synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery
631
tasks. Neuroimage 31, 153–159.
632
Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H., 1999. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization
27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 633 634 635 636
and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin Neurophysiol 110, 1842–1857. Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., 1997. Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans. Neurosci Lett 239, 65–68. Pichiorri, F., De Vico Fallani, F., Cincotti, F., Babiloni, F., Molinari, M., Kleih, S.C., Neuper, C., Kübler, A., Mattia, D., 2011. Sensorimotor rhythm-based
638
brain-computer interface training: the impact on motor cortical responsiveness. J
639
Neural Eng 8, 025020. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/8/2/025020
640
RI PT
637
Pichiorri, F., Morone, G., Petti, M., Toppi, J., Pisotta, I., Molinari, M., Paolucci, S., Inghilleri, M., Astolfi, L., Cincotti, F., Mattia, D., 2015. Brain-computer interface
642
boosts motor imagery practice during stroke recovery. Ann Neurol 77, 851–865. Ramos-Murguialday, A., Broetz, D., Rea, M., Läer, L., Yilmaz, O., Brasil, F.L.,
M AN U
643
SC
641
644
Liberati, G., Curado, M.R., Garcia-Cossio, E., Vyziotis, A., Cho, W., Agostini, M.,
645
Soares, E., Soekadar, S., Caria, A., Cohen, L.G., Birbaumer, N., 2013.
646
Brain-machine interface in chronic stroke rehabilitation: a controlled study. Ann
647
Neurol 74, 100–108.
Reis, J., Swayne, O.B., Vandermeeren, Y., Camus, M., Dimyan, M.A., Harris-Love, M.,
TE D
648
Perez, M.A., Ragert, P., Rothwell, J.C., Cohen, L.G., 2008. Contribution of
650
transcranial magnetic stimulation to the understanding of cortical mechanisms
651
involved in motor control. J Physiol (Lond) 586, 325–351.
653
Reynolds, C., Ashby, P., 1999. Inhibition in the human motor cortex is reduced just before a voluntary contraction. Neurology 53, 730–735.
AC C
652
EP
649
654
Rossini, P.M., Barker, A.T., Berardelli, A., Caramia, M.D., Caruso, G., Cracco, R.Q.,
655
Dimitrijević, M.R., Hallett, M., Katayama, Y., Lücking, C.H., 1994. Non-invasive
656
electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic
657
principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN
658
committee. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 91, 79–92.
659 660 661 662
Sanger, T.D., Garg, R.R., Chen, R., 2001. Interactions between two different inhibitory systems in the human motor cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 530, 307–317. Sitaram, R., Veit, R., Stevens, B., Caria, A., Gerloff, C., Birbaumer, N., Hummel, F., 2012. Acquired control of ventral premotor cortex activity by feedback training: an
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 663
exploratory real-time FMRI and TMS study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 26,
664
256–265.
667 668 669 670 671
interfaces in neurorehabilitation of stroke. Neurobiol Dis 83, 172–179. Soekadar, S.R., Witkowski, M., Birbaumer, N., Cohen, L.G., 2015. Enhancing Hebbian
RI PT
666
Soekadar, S.R., Birbaumer, N., Slutzky, M.W., Cohen, L.G., 2014. Brain-machine
Learning to Control Brain Oscillatory Activity. Cereb Cortex 25, 2409–2415. Steriade, M., Llinás, R.R., 1988. The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiol Rev 68, 649–742.
Suffczynski, P., Kalitzin, S., Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H., 2001.
SC
665
Computational model of thalamo-cortical networks: dynamical control of alpha
673
rhythms in relation to focal attention. Int J Psychophysiol 43, 25–40.
674
M AN U
672
Takemi, M., Masakado, Y., Liu, M., Ushiba, J., 2015. Sensorimotor event-related
675
desynchronization represents the excitability of human spinal motoneurons.
676
Neuroscience 297, 58–67.
677
Takemi, M., Masakado, Y., Liu, M., Ushiba, J., 2013. Event-related desynchronization reflects downregulation of intracortical inhibition in human primary motor cortex. J
679
Neurophysiol 110, 1158–1166.
TE D
678
Tremblay, F., Tremblay, L.E., Colcer, D.E., 2001. Modulation of corticospinal
681
excitability during imagined knee movements. J Rehabil Med 33, 230–234.
682
EP
680
Vaalto, S., Säisänen, L., Könönen, M., Julkunen, P., Hukkanen, T., Määttä, S., Karhu, J., 2011. Corticospinal output and cortical excitation-inhibition balance in distal
684
hand muscle representations in nonprimary motor area. Hum Brain Mapp 32,
685
1692–1703.
AC C
683
686
Vukelić, M., Gharabaghi, A., 2015. Self-regulation of circumscribed brain activity
687
modulates spatially selective and frequency specific connectivity of distributed
688
resting state networks. Front Behav Neurosci 9, 181.
689
doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00181
690
Young, B.M., Nigogosyan, Z., Walton, L.M., Song, J., Nair, V.A., Grogan, S.W., Tyler,
691
M.E., Edwards, D.F., Caldera, K., Sattin, J.A., Williams, J.C., Prabhakaran, V.,
692
2014. Changes in functional brain organization and behavioral correlations after
29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 693
rehabilitative therapy using a brain-computer interface. Front Neuroeng 7, 26.
694
doi:10.3389/fneng.2014.00026
695
Yuan, H., Liu, T., Szarkowski, R., Rios, C., Ashe, J., He, B., 2010. Negative covariation between task-related responses in alpha/beta-band activity and BOLD
697
in human sensorimotor cortex: an EEG and fMRI study of motor imagery and
698
movements. Neuroimage 49, 2596–2606.
699
RI PT
696
Zaepffel, M., Trachel, R., Kilavik, B.E., Brochier, T., 2013. Modulations of EEG beta power during planning and execution of grasping movements. PLoS ONE 8,
701
e60060. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060060
703
Ziemann, U., Chen, R., Cohen, L.G., Hallett, M., 1998. Dextromethorphan decreases the excitability of the human motor cortex. Neurology 51, 1320–1324.
M AN U
702
SC
700
704
Ziemann, U., Lönnecker, S., Steinhoff, B.J., Paulus, W., 1996. Effects of antiepileptic
705
drugs on motor cortex excitability in humans: a transcranial magnetic stimulation
706
study. Ann Neurol 40, 367–378.
TE D
709
homeostatic metaplasticity. Brain Stimul 1, 60–66.
EP
708
Ziemann, U., Siebner, H.R., 2008. Modifying motor learning through gating and
AC C
707
30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure captions
711
Figure 1. (a) Experimental paradigm of the screening, resting, and brain-computer interface
712
(BCI) conditions. (b) Experimental setup for the BCI condition. Five electroencephalogram
713
(EEG) electrodes were placed around the right-hand motor area. The event-related
714
desynchronization (ERD) was calculated from EEG signals in online. Transcranial magnetic
715
stimulation (TMS) was applied when the ERD exceeded a predefined threshold during wrist
716
motor imagery. The motor-evoked potential (MEP) was recorded from both flexor carpi
717
radialis (FCR) and extensor carpi radialis (ECR) muscles. Participants received visual
718
feedback of the ERD magnitude, forming the BCI system.
719
Figure 2. MEP waveforms induced by single-pulse and paired-pulse TMS in the resting
720
and the BCI conditions. MEP data shown here were obtained from a single participant
721
of Experiment 1 so the participant performed imagery of wrist extension in the BCI
722
condition. Thin grey lines represent ten MEP traces overlaid per condition, while the
723
thick black line is average of the ten MEP traces. The filled triangles and vertical lines
724
below indicate timing of the test stimulus. The open triangles and vertical lines below
725
indicate the timing of the conditioning stimulus. Herein, the mean single-pulse MEP
726
amplitude was increased in the agonist muscle during motor imagery (ECR Rest: 0.85
727
mV, 5%ERD: 1.21 mV, 15%ERD: 1.30 mV; FCR Rest: 0.51 mV, 5%ERD: 0.59 mV,
728
15%ERD: 0.64 mV). Short-latency intracortical inhibition (SICI), which quantified by
729
the ratio between the mean conditioned MEP and the mean unconditioned MEP, was
730
attenuated as the ERD increased in the agonist muscle (ECR Rest: 30.7 %, 5%ERD:
731
40.3 %, 15%ERD: 78.0 %; FCR Rest: 67.0 %; 5%ERD: 63.5 %, 15%ERD: 58.4 %).
732
Intracortical facilitation (ICF) was not different among conditions in both muscles
733
(ECR Rest: 144.6 %, 5%ERD: 139.3 %, 15%ERD: 132.0 %; FCR Rest: 128.0 %,
734
5%ERD: 117.8 %, 15%ERD: 129.9 %).
735
Figure 3. Mean MEP amplitudes provoked by single and paired-pulse TMS in the resting
736
condition of experiment 1 (a) and experiment 2 (b). The SICI results consisted of paired-pulse
737
TMS at the short inter-stimulus interval (ISI; 2 or 3 ms) that showed smaller MEP amplitude.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
710
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Likewise, the ICF results consisted of paired-pulse TMS at the long ISI (10 or 15 ms), which
739
showed larger MEPs. Paired-pulse results demonstrated inhibition by the short ISI protocol
740
and facilitation by the long ISI protocol. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean. **
741
p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
742
Figure 4. Association between the ERD magnitude and TMS measures (the single-pulse MEP
743
amplitude, SICI and ICF) in the BCI condition of experiment 1 (a) and experiment 2 (b).
744
Increase in single-pulse MEP and reduction of SICI was both associated with the amount of
745
ERD. However, the increase in the MEP was not confined to the agonist muscle at the
746
high ERD level. Conversely, the disinhibition was selectively observed in the agonist
747
muscle. ICF showed no modulation by motor imagery. Data shows the mean values for all
748
participants. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; # p <
749
0.05, different from a corresponding resting condition.
750
Figure 5. Changes in the excitability of agonist M1 representation by actual voluntary
751
movement and motor imagery-based BCI task. (a) According to Nikolova et al. (2006), the
752
MEP increased from approximately 120 ms prior to the onset of muscle activity; then
753
a reduction of SICI occurred. It was speculated that the relatively late reduction in
754
SICI allowed increase in the excitability to be better focused, enabling the
755
generation of appropriate motor commands. (b) In the motor imagery-based BCI
756
task, an increase of MEP was initiated with a weaker ERD than a reduction of SICI.
757
This relationship was similar to that seen with voluntary movement. A motor
758
imagery-based BCI may enable to induce corticomotor activity close to the
759
changes accompanying actual movements, especially when a sufficient ERD was
760
induced.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
738
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT