Neuroanatomy of creative achievement

Neuroanatomy of creative achievement

Journal Pre-proof Neuroanatomy of creative achievement Christopher J. Wertz, Muhammad O. Chohan, Ranee A. Flores, Rex, E. Jung PII: S1053-8119(19)310...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 57 Views

Journal Pre-proof Neuroanatomy of creative achievement Christopher J. Wertz, Muhammad O. Chohan, Ranee A. Flores, Rex, E. Jung PII:

S1053-8119(19)31078-X

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116487

Reference:

YNIMG 116487

To appear in:

NeuroImage

Received Date: 1 August 2019 Revised Date:

12 December 2019

Accepted Date: 20 December 2019

Please cite this article as: Wertz, C.J., Chohan, M.O., Flores, R.A., Jung, R.,.E., Neuroanatomy of creative achievement, NeuroImage (2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116487. This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier Inc.

Neuroanatomy of Creative Achievement

1 2

Christopher J. Wertz1, Muhammad O. Chohan1, Ranee A. Flores1, Rex, E. Jung1

3 4 5

1

University of New Mexico Department of Neurosurgery. Albuquerque, NM USA

6 7 8

Abstract Very few studies have investigated neuroanatomical correlates of “everyday” creative

9

achievement in cohorts of normal subjects. In previous research, we first showed that scores on

10

the Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) were associated with lower cortical thickness

11

within the left lateral orbitofrontal gyrus (LOFG), and increased thickness of the right angular

12

gyrus (AG) (Jung et al., 2010). Newer studies found the CAQ to be associated with decreased

13

volume of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Chen, et al., 2014), and that artistic and

14

scientific creativity was associated with increased and decreased volumes within the executive

15

control network and salience network (Shi et al., 2017). We desired to replicate and extend our

16

previous study in a larger cohort (N=248), comprised of subjects studying and working in

17

science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM). Subjects were youn (Range = 16-32; Mean

18

age = 21.8; s.d. = 3.5) all of whom were administered the CAQ from which we derived artistic

19

and scientific creativity factors. All subjects underwent structural MRI on a 3 Tesla scanner from

20

which cortical thickness, area, and volume measures were obtained using FreeSurfer. Our results

21

showed mostly cortical thinning in relation to total, scientific, and artistic creative achievement

22

encompassing many regions involved in the cognitive control network (CCN) and default mode

23

network (DMN).

24

25

Introduction

26

The science of creative cognition has branched off from broad, behaviorally directed

27

studies initially investigating convergent and divergent thinking, (Sternberg & Lubart, 1999;

28

Torrance, 1976), to more specific studies designed to measure the construct of creativity and how

29

it is manifested in the structure and function of the brain. Currently, creative cognition is broadly

30

measured through three domains, creative potential (an individual’s cognitive ability to create

31

something that is both novel and useful (Jauk, Benedek, & Neubauer, 2014; Stein, 1953),

32

everyday creativity (a regular individual’s ability to create something original in everyday life,

33

(Richards, 2010) and creative achievement (an individual’s real-life creative contribution, such

34

as writing a screenplay for a film (Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005). The creativity field

35

further dissects these constructs, with several domains of assessing creative novelty such as

36

divergent thinking, fluency, imagery, reasoning, and insight. Research consistencies regarding

37

neural correlates within these creative fields remains sparse and inconclusive, largely lacking

38

consensus or replication (Arden, Chavez, Grazioplene, & Jung, 2010; Beaty, Nusbaum, & Silvia,

39

2014). The question remains: what best measures certain people’s proficiency to produce work

40

that is both novel and useful, and how is this manifested in their brain structure and function?

41

Creative achievement driven tasks are a construct of creative cognition that objectively

42

measures individuals’ ability to produce work that is both novel and useful (Simonton, 2012).

43

The Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ; Carson et al., 2005) offers a reliable (0.81) and

44

valid measure of real-life creative productivity across ten specific domains ranging from culinary

45

prowess to scientific innovations. The CAQ is well correlated with measures of divergent

46

thinking such as originality and fluency and, while divergent thinking tests predict creative

47

success (Torrance, 1988), they are limited in their ability to predict real-life applications. While

48

the objective consensus of the CAQ is a considerable strength, one major problem in using the

49

CAQ is that scores are always highly skewed. For example, few people in a normal, non-

50

eminent, sample will have higher scores than the average (of zero). Those few people, however,

51

have accomplished real-life creative contributions of novelty or usefulness as evidenced by

52

patents, published works, and awards. Higher scores on CAQ are therefore eliciting the desired

53

effect in researching creative cognition manifested in creative products in the world recognized

54

by others in terms of their novelty and utility.

55

Recent behavioral research on creative achievement has found it to be moderately

56

correlated with intelligence – as measured by the intelligence quotient (IQ) – and openness

57

(Jauk et al., 2014), as well as artistic and scientific achievement (Kaufman et al., 2016). Another

58

study, found that intentional mind wandering was predictive of greater creative achievement

59

(Agnoli, Vanucci, Pelagatti, & Corazza, 2018). Relatively few studies have been conducted

60

regarding neuroanatomical correlates of creative achievement. Jung et al., first showed that

61

individuals with higher scores on the CAQ had lower cortical thickness within the left lateral

62

orbitofrontal gyrus (LOFG), and increased thickness of the right angular gyrus, (Jung et al.,

63

2010). Subsequent studies demonstrated that the CAQ was associated with increased cortical

64

volume of the superior frontal gyrus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and decreased volume in the

65

dorsal and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), (Chen et al., 2014). Shi et al., further parsed

66

two major domains of the CAQ by extracting artistic and scientific factors (Carson et al., 2005;

67

Kaufman et al., 2016). They found that decreased volume in the supplementary motor area and

68

ACC was associated with artistic creativity, while increased volume in the middle frontal gyrus

69

and inferior occipital gyrus was associated with scientific creativity, (Shi, Cao, Chen, Zhuang, &

70

Qiu, 2017). Functional connectivity studies using CAQ have found networks negatively

71

correlated with medial superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal insula, thalamus

72

(Chen et al., 2014). There is no commonality of findings to report across studies, and differences

73

could be attributed to methodological differences between studies (the last two of which are from

74

the same experimental cohort), including: restriction of creative range, use of different

75

covariates, and unbalanced sex distributions.

76

Our current study was designed to replicate and extend our previous research by looking

77

at CAQ scores in a larger independent healthy young sample across cortical thickness, surface

78

area and volume. We also investigated the artistic creativity and scientific two factor model

79

suggested by (Carson et al., 2005) and explored by (Shi et al., 2017). Previous research has

80

shown both increased and decreased volumes across the salience, executive, default mode

81

network (DMN), and cognitive control network (CCN) in relation to high creativity scores

82

(Beaty, Christensen, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, 2017; Jung, Mead, Carrasco, & Flores, 2013).

83

We, therefore, predict findings to be associated with the CCN and DMN, specifically, increased

84

gray matter in areas of the superior parietal cortex and decreased prefrontal frontal areas with

85

CAQ total. Based on previous work using the CAQ two factor model, we predict similarly

86

decreased gray matter in the ACC with artistic creativity and increased gray matter in the parietal

87

cortex as well as the caudal middle frontal gyrus.

88 89

Methods

90

Two hundred and sixty science, technology, engineering and mathematics (STEM)

91

subjects either working in the field or studying within STEM fields were recruited for the current

92

study. Six subjects were excluded based on missing behavioral data and six subjects were

93

excluded due to incidental MRI findings (e.g., arachnoid cyst), leaving a sample of two hundred

94

and forty-eight.

95

Subjects ranged from (16-32) years of age (21.8 ± 3.5 years), and were well matched by sex (127

96

males, 121 females). They were recruited by postings in various departments and classrooms,

97

around the University of New Mexico, local high schools and various professional STEM related

98

businesses. All subjects signed a consent form approved by the institutional review board of the

99

University of New Mexico prior to participation. Prior to entry into the study, subjects were

100

screened and met no criteria for neurological and psychological disorders that would impact

101

experimental hypotheses (e.g., learning disorders, traumatic brain injury, major depressive

102

disorder). Subjects were also screened for conditions that would prohibit undergoing an MRI

103

scan (e.g., orthodontic braces, claustrophobia). Subjects were compensated $100 for undergoing

104

four hours of cognitive testing, encompassing measures of intelligence, creativity, problem

105

solving, personality, and aptitude, as well as a 30-minute MRI.

106 107

Behavioral Measures

108

All subjects completed the CAQ, a reliable and valid measure of creative achievement across 10

109

domains including visual arts, music, creative writing, dance, drama, architecture, humor,

110

scientific discovery, invention, and culinary arts. In each domain the subject places a checkmark

111

next to a concrete achievement. Each domain has a potential point range of 0-7. Several items if

112

checked were denoted with an asterisk where the subject was required to indicate how many

113

times each achievement occurred (e.g., “I have been published in scientific journals”) and then

114

multiplied by item number. The CAQ total score is tallied by summing across the ten domains. A

115

two-factor model was created based on Carson et al., dividing the ten domains into a science and

116

art total score, (Carson et al., 2005). Creative achievement in the arts represented the sum of

117

scores for drama, humor, music, dance, visual arts, creative writing, and theater/film. Creative

118

achievement in the sciences represented the sum of inventions and scientific discovery.

119

To assess general intelligence, subjects were administered the Wechsler Abbreviated

120

Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II). The WASI battery consists of subtests that measure verbal and

121

non-verbal mental abilities that contribute to general intelligence. We derived a Full-Scale

122

Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) score from performance on the subtests of similarities, matrix

123

reasoning and block design.

124 125

Neuroimaging

126

Structural imaging was obtained at a 3 Tesla Siemens scanner using a 32-channel head coil to

127

obtain a T1 5 echo sagittal MPRAGE sequence [TE = 1.64 ms; 3.5 ms; 5.36 ms; 7.22 ms; 9.08

128

ms; TR = 2530 ms; voxel size = 1.0×1.0×1.0 mm3; FOV = 256 mm; slices = 192; acquisition

129

time = 6:03]. For all scans, each T1 was reviewed for image quality. Cortical reconstruction and

130

volumetric segmentation were performed with the FreeSurfer-v5.3.0 image analysis suite. The

131

methodology for FreeSurfer is described in full in several papers (Fischl et al., 2002, 2004;

132

Ségonne, Pacheco, & Fischl, 2007). Briefly, this process includes automated Talairach

133

transformation, segmentation of the subcortical white matter and deep gray matter volumetric

134

structures, (Fischl et al., 2002, 2004). Segmented data were then parceled into units based on

135

gyral and sulcal structure, resulting in values for cortical thickness, surface area, and volume

136

(Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2004). The results of the automatic segmentations were

137

quality controlled, and any errors were manually corrected. Volume measures are a combination

138

of thickness (a one-dimensional measure) and area (a two dimensional measure) across 33

139

measures per hemisphere (i.e., 66 across the surface of the brain) as well as seven subcortical

140

volumes per hemisphere (i.e., 14 across the brain) including bilateral caudate, putamen, globus

141

pallidus, nucleus accumbens, thalamus, amygdala, and hippocampus (Fischl et al., 2002).

142 143

Statistical Analysis

144

General linear models were used to assess correlations with CAQ total, art total, and science total

145

scores with cortical volume, area, and thickness. This type of analysis was performed by the

146

Query, Design, Estimate, Contrast (QDEC) interface within FreeSurfer. First, each subject’s

147

surface was smoothed using a full-width/half-maximum gaussian kernel of 10 mm. This

148

smoothing was done so that all subjects in this study could be displayed on a common template,

149

which is an average brain in Talairach space. Each design matrix consisted of either CAQ total,

150

art total, or science total raw scores as the independent variable and sex, age, FSIQ and the

151

FreeSurfer calculation of total brain volume categorized as BrainSegVolNotVent as covariates

152

and the slope used was different offset/intercept, different slope (DODS).Correction for multiple

153

comparisons was performed using a Monte Carlo Null-Z simulation method. For these analyses,

154

a total of 10,000 simulations were performed for each comparison, using a threshold of p = 0.05.

155

This is the probability of forming a maximum cluster of that size or larger during the simulation

156

under the null hypothesis and presents the likelihood that the cluster of vertices would have

157

arisen by chance. Multiple regressions were performed on subcortical volume measures, against

158

each factor (CAQ, art, science total scores), controlling for total brain volume, age, sex and

159

FSIQ. We used R version 3.5.3 for Windows for all behavioral statistical analysis.

160 161

162 163

Results CAQ total score, art total, and science total were all highly skewed and therefore any

164

outliers greater than three standard deviations from the mean were excluded by variable.

165

Behavioral characteristics are presented in Table1). Previous studies on creative achievement

166

have specifically found it to be moderately correlated with intelligence. In our sample,

167

CAQ total was weakly correlated with FSIQ (r = 0.06, p = 0.35). As expected, the art factor

168

score was weakly correlated with FSIQ (r = 0.004, p = 0.95) and the science factor was

169

significantly correlated with FSIQ (r = 0.13, p = 0.05). Our sample was selected to sample

170

STEM populations, among whom CAQ-IQ scores are known to be more highly correlated;

171

therefore, controlling for IQ was deemed necessary for accurate interpretation of results. It

172

should be noted, when we ran the same analyses without controlling for IQ, CAQ total did

173

not have any significant associations with cortical structures, artistic creativity findings

174

remained the same, and scientific creativity retained largely the same findings except for

175

right hemisphere postcentral, and precentral regions. The significant associations remained

176

negative, which is consistent with findings when controlling for IQ.

177

Subcortical Analysis

178

A series of multiple regression analyses investigated subcortical volume correlates with CAQ

179

total, art total and sci total. We regressed all subcortical volume measures, against each measure

180

controlling for total brain volume, age, sex and FSIQ. CAQ total score was predicted by a model

181

that included increased left hippocampus and increased posterior corpus callosum volumes (F =

182

3.1, p = 0.006; r2 = 0.07). A model including decreased left amygdala and increased left caudate

183

volume predicted artistic creativity total score (F = 2.24, p = 0.040; r2 = 0.05). Scores on

184

scientific creativity factor were predicted by a model including increased mid-anterior corpus

185

callosum and decreased left thalamus volumes (F = 8.41, p <0.001; r2 = 0.18).

186

Behavioral Demographics

Mean

Std. Dev.

Range

FSIQ

111.6

11.9

80-153

CAQ Total

16.7

14.9

0-74

CAQ Art

9.6

9.8

0-66

CAQ Science

4.3

5.1

0-24

187 188

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participant’s demographics and behavioral measures.

189

Cortical Analysis

190

CAQ Cortical Analysis

191

The results of whole brain cortical analysis with CAQ total score showed significantly decreased volume

192

in the left insula extending into the superior temporal gyrus after multiple comparisons correction (p <

193

0.05). Table 2 and Fig.1 present the results of the statistical maps and linearity. r = -0.114

194

L L Insula Volume

195 196 197

CAQ Volume

198 199 200

-5.00 -2.50 0.00

2.50

5.00

CAQ_Total

201 202 203

Figure 1. Statistical map showing significantly decreased cortical volume with CAQ total scores overlaying the Desikan-Killianey atlas.

Cortical Surface

Max

Size(mm2)

TalX

TalY

TalZ

CWP

Vtxs

Gyrus

Volume

-2.704

1137.90

-36.6

-5.6

-10.3

0.03960

2250

insula

204 205 206 207 208

Table 2. Creative Achievement Questionnaire: Regions surviving Monte Carlo simulation (p < 0.05). Size (mm2) = size of region in square millimeters; TalX = Talairach region X plane; TalY = Talairach region Y plane; TalZ = Talairach region Z plane; CWP cluster-wise probability; Vtxs = number of contiguous vertices.

209

Artistic Creativity Cortical Analysis

210

The results of whole brain cortical analysis with CAQ artistic creativity total score showed significantly

211

decreased thickness in the left superior temporal gyrus extending into the middle temporal gyrus and right

212

superior parietal gyrus, after multiple comparisons correction (p < 0.05). Table 3 and Fig.2 present the

213

results of the statistical maps and linearity.

214 215 216

219 220 221

-5.00 -2.50 0.00

2.50

R Superior Parietal

218

5.00

CAQ Artistic Creativity

222 223 224

r = -0.153

r = -0.102

L Superior Temporal

217

R Thickness

L Thickness

Figure 2. Statistical maps showing significantly decreased cortical thickness with artistic creativity overlaying the Desikan-Killianey atlas.

225 Cortical Surface

Max

Size(mm2)

TalX

TalY

TalZ

CWP

Vtxs

Gyrus

Thickness

-2.871

1583.53

-44.7

5.9

-19.8

0.00580

3233

LH superior temporal

Thickness

-2.883

536.50

24.1

-73.3

32.2

0.02050

769

RH superior parietal

226 227 228 229 230

Table 3. Creative Achievement Factor Artistic Creativity: Regions surviving Monte Carlo simulation (p < 0.05). Size (mm2) = size of region in square millimeters; TalX = Talairach region X plane; TalY = Talairach region Y plane; TalZ = Talairach region Z plane; CWP cluster-wise probability; Vtxs = number of contiguous vertices.

231

Scientific Creativity Cortical Analysis

232

The results of whole brain cortical analysis with CAQ scientific creativity total score showed significantly

233

decreased surface area in the left insula gyrus extending into regions of the superior temporal gyrus,

234

transverse temporal gyrus, and another significant region in the precentral and postcentral gyrus. In the

235

right hemisphere we found significantly decreased area in numerous regions of the brain including left

236

superior frontal gyrus extending into the caudal middle frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus extending

237

into the superior temporal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, and pars-opecularis gyrus extending into the insula and

238

precentral gyrus. We also found a sole positive significant correlation in the right hemisphere postcentral

239

gyrus extending into the superior parietal gyrus with volume, after multiple comparisons correction (p <

240

0.05). Table 4 and Fig. 3 and 4, present the results of the statistical maps and linearity.

R Are L Insula area

242 243 244 245

r = -0.149

r = -0.122 R Superior Frontal area

L 241

246 -5.00 -2.50 0.00

247 248 249

2.50 5.00

CAQ Scientific Creativity

Figure 3. Statistical maps showing significantly decreased cortical surface area with scientific creativity overlaying the Desikan-Killianey atlas.

L

R Postcental Volume

r = 0.196

-5.00 -2.50 0.00 2.50 5.00

CAQ Scientific Creativity 250 251 252

Figure 4. Statistical maps showing significantly increased cortical volume with scientific creativity overlaying the Desikan-Killianey atlas.

Cortical Surface

Max

Size(mm2)

TalX

TalY

TalZ

CWP

Area

-3.871

3138.68

-33.7

-8.8

9

0.00010

7748

LH insula

Area

-3.131

1178.73

-19.3

-21.2

55.6

0.01860

2851

LH precentral

Area

-3.617

1756.45

19.8

25.3

41.1

0.00110

3149

RH superior frontal

Area

-3.228

1583.37

44

-26.7

-3.5

0.00280

3214

RH superior temporal

Area

-3.169

1118.04

48.1

-0.6

9.4

0.03150

2786

RH precentral

Area

-2.624

1225.18

9.1

-31.5

27.8

0.01810

3009

RH isthmus cingulate

Volume

4.248

639.44

16.4

-28.8

69.1

0.00770

1595

RH postcentral

Vtxs

Gyrus

253 254 255 256

Table 4. Creative Achievement Factor Scientific Creativity: Regions surviving Monte Carlo simulation (p < 0.05). Size (mm2) = size of region in square millimeters; TalX = Talairach region X plane; TalY =

257 258

Talairach region Y plane; TalZ = Talairach region Z plane; CWP cluster-wise probability; Vtxs = number of contiguous vertices.

259

Discussion

260

The current study sought to replicate and extend our previous cortical structural findings

261

by exploring the relationship with creative achievement in a normal non-eminent sample. Our

262

results failed to replicate our previous findings of decreased cortical thickness in the left

263

orbitofrontal cortex or increased thickness in the right angular gyrus. We adopted a more

264

complex analysis strategy involving thickness, area, and volume, and instead discovered several

265

new brain regions, where highly creative individuals exhibited cortical structural differences,

266

across a wide network previously implicated in creativity research. Indeed, we found numerous

267

regions to overlap both the DMN (bilateral superior temporal gyri and isthmus cingulate gyrus)

268

and CCN (insula, superior parietal and preSMA). Perhaps more surprisingly, we only found

269

increased volume in relation to scientific creativity in the right postcentral gyrus.

270

The general pattern across morphological studies within the creative achievement

271

tends to show decreased volume (particularly within the DMN) more than increased

272

volumes (Jung et al., 2013). It has been hypothesized that cortical thinning represents greater

273

efficiency and lower levels of plasticity the more proficiently one acquires a particular skill (Jung

274

et al., 2010). However, studies looking at creative potential prominently find increased

275

volume in several areas including the bilateral precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex and

276

the bilateral caudate (Fink et al., 2014; Jauk, Neubauer, Dunst, Fink, & Benedek, 2015).

277

Our results showed decreased volume to be associated with higher CAQ total scores in the left

278

insula extending into the superior temporal gyrus, and increased volume in the left hippocampus

279

and posterior corpus callosum. The left hemisphere regions associated with CAQ, including the

280

insula and superior temporal gyrus have been shown to overlap with the DMN and salience

281

networks respectively (Beaty et al., 2017), and are implicated in fMRI studies looking at verbal

282

creativity (Boccia, Piccardi, Palermo, Nori, & Palmiero, 2015). Left insular activation was found

283

during a brainstorming task in a study looking at neuro-correlates of creative writing (Shah et al.,

284

2013). The left superior temporal lobe has been found to be active during imagination type tasks

285

(Huang et al., 2013), and decreased volume has been correlated with high imagination ability,

286

(Jung, Flores, & Hunter, 2016). Finally, research has demonstrated a role for the hippocampus in

287

creativity, as patients with lesions to the hippocampus are found to have lower scores on

288

measures of divergent thinking (Duff, Kurczek, Rubin, Cohen, & Tranel, 2013). Our finding of

289

increased volume in relation to creative achievement in the posterior corpus callosum is in

290

contrast to other studies finding an inverse relationship in size with higher divergent thinking

291

scores (Gansler et al., 2011).

292

Within the artistic creativity measure we found an inverse relationship with thickness in

293

the left superior temporal gyrus extending into the middle temporal gyrus and right superior

294

parietal gyrus. As previously noted, the left superior-middle temporal gyrus is involved in verbal

295

creativity and imagination. The right superior parietal lobule is involved in mentally re-ordering

296

the sequence of visual stimuli, (Gansler et al., 2011). Zhu et al., found the bilateral superior

297

parietal cortex to be inversely associated with verbal and visual creativity (Zhu et al., 2017). We

298

also observed higher artistic creativity in relation to decreased left amygdala volume and

299

increased volume in the left caudate. The amygdala has been implicated in artistic creativity, is

300

associated with spontaneous and emotional functioning, and has been implicated in numerous

301

musical creativity studies (Bashwiner, Wertz, Flores, & Jung, 2016; McPherson, Barrett, Lopez-

302

Gonzalez, Jiradejvong, & Limb, 2016). Finally, increased left hemispheric activation in the left

303

caudate nucleus was observed in expert creative writers in contrast to non-experts (Erhard,

304

Kessler, Neumann, Ortheil, & Lotze, 2014). Within scientific creativity, we found several inverse relationships with surface area and

305 306

high scientific creative scores. We found significantly decreased surface area in the left insula

307

and precentral gyrus. Left precentral gyrus is activated during visuo-spatial creativity tasks

308

(Boccia et al., 2015). The insula is involved in general and fluid intelligence (Haier, Jung, Yeo,

309

Head, & Alkire, 2004; Woolgar et al., 2010). The right hemisphere findings revealed several

310

negative correlations in the superior frontal gyrus, which has also been found with higher

311

divergent thinking performance anticorrelated within the superior frontal regions. However,

312

other studies have found increased volume in right superior frontal gyrus to be correlated with

313

creativity and cognitive flexibility (Kühn et al., 2014). Scientific creative achievement is

314

hypothesized to rely more heavily on convergent thinking processes in the pursuit of solving one

315

specific goal. Indeed, many of our findings overlap with previous work with convergent

316

thinking. For example, Jung-Beeman et al., 2004 found that solving problems through a modified

317

form of Mednick’s test activated the right anterior superior temporal gyrus (Jung-Beeman et al.,

318

2004).

319

The literature regarding the neuroscience of creativity is in a nascent stage, characterized

320

by disparate and divergent findings across laboratories. Previous research within the

321

neuroscience of intelligence has found similar dissimilarities to be associated with such

322

seemingly subtle effects as those associated with analysis techniques and covariates chosen

323

(Martínez et al., 2017). Thus, it is not surprising that we have found results different from those

324

found previously, as such differences can be caused by different analysis packages, sample

325

characteristics, and covariates applied. Similarly, our sample focused on STEM subjects, while

326

previous samples looked at more broadly distributed subject cohorts. Finally, the interplay

327

between intelligence (or IQ) and creativity, is an important one, with the potential interaction

328

providing potential differences between studies that either use IQ as a covariate and those that do

329

not. A special issue has recently been published in Neuropsychologia with several perspectives

330

regarding the “common ground and differences” between these two cognitive constructs

331

(Benedek, Jung, & Vartanian, 2018). There are only a handful of studies in existence regarding

332

structural correlates regarding the CAQ, and further research in this area will be critical to

333

establish convergence over time, as has been evident in creative neuroscience more broadly

334

(Jung et al., 2013). However, as currently comprised, the literature regarding structural correlates

335

of the CAQ now shows both increased and decreased volume in relation to higher creative

336

achievement (Chen et al., 2014; Jung et al., 2010; Shi et al., 2017).

337

Previous studies in creativity neuroscience have implicated broad regions overlapping

338

significantly with the DMN (Bashwiner et al., 2016; Beaty, Benedek, Kaufman, & Silvia, 2015;

339

Kühn et al., 2014) and the CCN (Chrysikou, Weber, & Thompson-Schill, 2014; Li, Yang, Zhang,

340

Li, & Qiu, 2016). The current findings appear to conform with the hypothesis regarding dynamic

341

interplay between DMN and CCN (mediated by the SN) as our results implicated several areas

342

across these broad networks. Thus, these two specific subdomains of creativity require a

343

dynamic interconnection of the DMN and CCN in order to achieve success in the creative arts

344

and sciences.

345

There are several limitations to the current study. Our sample was restricted to a healthy

346

normal population and was focused on individuals expressing interest in scientifically inclined

347

students and professionals. While the CAQ is a valid and reliable measure of creative

348

achievement, our sample, comprised of mostly college students, have had fewer opportunities to

349

express their potential due to their relatively young age and low professional status. The CAQ is

350

always skewed in its distribution, with occasional extreme outliers. We chose to remove such

351

outliers (+3 s.d.) although these individuals could represent true creative accomplishment.

352

Removing high achievers therefore could have impacted our results. We re-ran the analysis

353

with those 6 outliers included in the sample and found significantly decreased surface area

354

in the posterior cingulate gyrus. However, to have 6 subjects drive the overall relationship

355

would not, in our opinion, represent the balance of the findings across the remaining 248

356

subjects; therefore, we ultimately decided to remove them from the analysis. Future studies

357

focused on such extreme outliers could be helpful in understanding brain organization in

358

extremely high creative achievement. Finally, we failed to replicate the findings of both our

359

previous study and that of another group, although we have provided several methodological

360

reasons why the current results might differ. Future research will be necessary to support the

361

relationships observed between brain structure and creative achievement. We would encourage

362

researchers to use methods designed to parse cortical thickness, area, and volume (e.g.,

363

FreeSurfer) as opposed to voxel intensities, the former of which, arguably, gets closer to

364

biological variables of interest. We would further encourage researchers to include age, sex, total

365

brain volume, and intellectual functioning (e.g., IQ). This last variable is particularly important

366

to include as a covariate, given its significant relationship to variables of interest to creativity

367

research (e.g., divergent thinking, CAQ), as well as established brain-IQ relationships which

368

could potentially confound the variable of interest (R.E. Jung & Haier, 2007).

369 370 371 372 373 374

References Agnoli, S., Vanucci, M., Pelagatti, C., & Corazza, G. E. (2018). Exploring the link between mind wandering, mindfulness, and creativity: A multidimensional approach. Creativity Research

375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420

Journal, 30(1), 41–53. Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., & Jung, R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: a psychometric view. Behavioural Brain Research, 214(2), 143–156. Bashwiner, D. M., Wertz, C. J., Flores, R. A., & Jung, R. E. (2016). Musical creativity “revealed” in brain structure: interplay between motor, default mode, and limbic networks. Scientific Reports, 6, 20482. Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, S. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964. Beaty, R. E., Christensen, A. P., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). Creative constraints: Brain activity and network dynamics underlying semantic interference during idea production. Neuroimage, 148, 189–196. Beaty, R. E., Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2014). Does insight problem solving predict realworld creativity? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(3), 287. Benedek, M., Jung, R. E., & Vartanian, O. (2018). The neural bases of creativity and intelligence: common ground and differences. Neuropsychologia. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.006 Boccia, M., Piccardi, L., Palermo, L., Nori, R., & Palmiero, M. (2015). Where do bright ideas occur in our brain? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies of domain-specific creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1195. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17(1), 37–50. Chen, Q., Yang, W., Li, W., Wei, D., Li, H., Lei, Q., … Qiu, J. (2014). Association of creative achievement with cognitive flexibility by a combined voxel-based morphometry and resting-state functional connectivity study. Neuroimage, 102, 474–483. Chrysikou, E. G., Weber, M. J., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). A matched filter hypothesis for cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 62, 341–355. Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., … Hyman, B. T. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage, 31(3), 968–980. Duff, M. C., Kurczek, J., Rubin, R., Cohen, N. J., & Tranel, D. (2013). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts creative thinking. Hippocampus, 23(12), 1143–1149. Erhard, K., Kessler, F., Neumann, N., Ortheil, H.-J., & Lotze, M. (2014). Professional training in creative writing is associated with enhanced fronto-striatal activity in a literary text continuation task. Neuroimage, 100, 15–23. Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, E., Benedek, M., Weber, B., … Weiss, E. M. (2014). Gray matter density in relation to different facets of verbal creativity. Brain Structure and Function, 219(4), 1263–1269. Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., … Klaveness, S. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. Fischl, B., Van Der Kouwe, A., Destrieux, C., Halgren, E., Ségonne, F., Salat, D. H., … Kennedy, D. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 11–22. Gansler, D. A., Moore, D. W., Susmaras, T. M., Jerram, M. W., Sousa, J., & Heilman, K. M. (2011). Cortical morphology of visual creativity. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2527–2532. Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., & Alkire, M. T. (2004). Structural brain variation

421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466

and general intelligence. Neuroimage, 23(1), 425–433. Huang, P., Qiu, L., Shen, L., Zhang, Y., Song, Z., Qi, Z., … Xie, P. (2013). Evidence for a left‐ over‐right inhibitory mechanism during figural creative thinking in healthy nonartists. Human Brain Mapping, 34(10), 2724–2732. Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). The road to creative achievement: A latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. European Journal of Personality, 28(1), 95–105. Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Dunst, B., Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2015). Gray matter correlates of creative potential: A latent variable voxel-based morphometry study. NeuroImage, 111, 312–320. Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., … Kounios, J. (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology, 2(4), e97. Jung, R.E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(2). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07001185 Jung, Rex E, Flores, R. A., & Hunter, D. (2016). A new measure of imagination ability: Anatomical brain imaging correlates. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 496. Jung, Rex E, Segall, J. M., Jeremy Bockholt, H., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 31(3), 398–409. Jung, Rex Eugene, Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. (2013). The structure of creative cognition in the human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 330. Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J. B., Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2016). Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality, 84(2), 248–258. Kühn, S., Ritter, S. M., Müller, B. C. N., Van Baaren, R. B., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). The importance of the default mode network in creativity—a structural MRI study. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 48(2), 152–163. Li, W., Yang, J., Zhang, Q., Li, G., & Qiu, J. (2016). The Association between resting functional connectivity and visual creativity. Scientific Reports, 6, 25395. Martínez, K., Janssen, J., Pineda-Pardo, J. Á., Carmona, S., Román, F. J., Alemán-Gómez, Y., … Santarnecchi, E. (2017). Individual differences in the dominance of interhemispheric connections predict cognitive ability beyond sex and brain size. NeuroImage, 155, 234–244. McPherson, M. J., Barrett, F. S., Lopez-Gonzalez, M., Jiradejvong, P., & Limb, C. J. (2016). Emotional intent modulates the neural substrates of creativity: an fMRI study of emotionally targeted improvisation in Jazz musicians. Scientific Reports, 6, 18460. Richards, R. (2010). Everyday creativity. The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, 189–215. Ségonne, F., Pacheco, J., & Fischl, B. (2007). Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 26(4), 518–529. Shah, C., Erhard, K., Ortheil, H., Kaza, E., Kessler, C., & Lotze, M. (2013). Neural correlates of creative writing: an fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 34(5), 1088–1101. Shi, B., Cao, X., Chen, Q., Zhuang, K., & Qiu, J. (2017). Different brain structures associated with artistic and scientific creativity: a voxel-based morphometry study. Scientific Reports, 7, 42911. Simonton, D. K. (2012). Quantifying creativity: can measures span the spectrum? Dialogues in

467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482

Clinical Neuroscience, 14(1), 100. Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. The Journal of Psychology, 36(2), 311–322. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. Handbook of Creativity, 1, 3–15. Torrance, E. P. (1976). Creativity testing in education. Creative Child & Adult Quarterly. Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing. The Nature of Creativity: Contemporary Psychological Perspectives, 43. Woolgar, A., Parr, A., Cusack, R., Thompson, R., Nimmo-Smith, I., Torralva, T., … Duncan, J. (2010). Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(33), 14899–14902. Zhu, W., Chen, Q., Xia, L., Beaty, R. E., Yang, W., Tian, F., … Chen, X. (2017). Common and distinct brain networks underlying verbal and visual creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 38(4), 2094–2111.