INFANT
BEHAVIOR
AND
DEVELOPMENT
15,
155-l
77 (1992)
Neurobehavioral Assessment in Rhesus Monkey Neonates (Macacc~ muluffc~): Developmental Changes, Behavioral Stability, and Early Experience L. SCHNEIDER
MARY University
of Wisconsin-Madison
J. SUOMI
STEPHEN
National
Institute of Child Health and Human
Development
This prospective study documented developmental changes, individuol stability, and the effects of early experience on the neurobehoviorol repertoire of nursery-reared rhesus monkey neonates (Mococa mulatto) tested repeatedly across the first month of life. Thirty-six infants were tested three times weekly on o substantially modified version of the Neonatal Behavior01 Assessment Stole. The Infants were reared under several conditions in which their exposure to antmate ond inonimate obiects varied. Eleven infants were reared with only o cloth, 8 were reared with an upright cloth-covered surrogate, 7 were reored with on upright movable cloth-covered surrogate, and 10 were reored with an upright movable cloth-covered surrogote ond regular exposure to peers and novel toys. Infants reared with o movable surrogate demonstrated superior motor maturotion in comparison to those reared with only o cloth or cloth-covered surrogate. However, infonts reared with o movable surrogate OS well OS exposure to peers and novel toys showed not only greater motor maturotion than cloth-reored infants but olso greater responsiveness on orientation items and lower ratings of fearfulness when compared to infants in the three other conditions. Furthermore, the dato indicate that behovtorol stobility of individual differences con be demonstrated in indivrduols undergoing ropid developmental change regordless of rearing conditions.
Mococa
The
author
mulotfo neuromotor nursery
gratefully
rhesus maturity rearina
acknowledges
monkey
neonatal temperoment stabilitv individuol
the
assistance
enrichment behavior visual orienting differences
of Sara
McClurg. and Kirstcn Oleson in the data collection, Christopher Fcrguson for comments on a draft of this manuscript, and Joyce of the manuscript. This rcscarch was supported by the Graduate
Rimm.
Elizabeth
Roughton.
Suzy
Cot, Susan Clarke, and Sherri Zander for help with preparation School and the Department of
Therapeutic Science at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the Maternal and Child Health Training Grant No. Y 102. Corrcspondencc and rcqucsts for reprints should be sent to Mary L. Schneider, 2175 Medical Science
Center,
1300
University
Avcnuc.
Madison,
WI
53706. 155
156
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
There are a variety of reasons for studying neonatal behavior in nonhuman primates. These range from defining differences and similarities between human and nonhuman primates (Hallock, Worobey, & Self, 1989) to investigation of disorders common in human neonates and infants such as fetal alcohol syndrome (Clarren & Bowden, 1982), lead exposure (Levin, Schneider, Ferguson, Schantz, & Bowman, 1988), low birth weight (Gunderson, Grant-Webster, & Sackett, 1989), prenatal stress (Schneider, in press), and drug exposure (Golub, Sassenrath, & Chapman, 1981; Phillips & Lockard, 1985). In addition, the study of neonatal behavior in nonhuman primate infants can provide an avenue for investigating the effects of early environmental influences upon infant development under controlled conditions (Champoux, DiGregorio, Schneider, & Suomi, 1990; Schneider, Kraemer. & Suomi, 1991). Moreover, the experimental control possible in nonhuman primate studies enables the assessment of how such environmental influences may differentially affect certain infants based on their inherent behavioral style or temperament (Schneider, Moore, Suomi, & Champoux, 1991). Despite the extensive literature describing the effects of early experience on the behavioral development in nonhuman primates (see Mitchell, 1970, and Sackett, 1968, for reviews), to date there are only a few published reports on observations made during the neonatal period (Champoux et al., 1990; Schneider, Moore, et al., in press). Moreover, there are limited normative data pertaining to nonhuman primate neonatal development. The neonatal data that do exist are restricted largely to descriptions of motor development, for example, the dates of appearance and/or disappearance of reflexes (Caste11 & Sackett, 1973; Ehrlich, 1974; King, Fobes, & Fobes, 1974; Mowbray & Cadell, 1962; Sackett, Gunderson, & Baldwin, 1982). Using standard test protocols, investigators have also studied species variation in motor development, particularly in those infant reflexes that facilitate maternal contact (see Rogers, 1988, for a review). The predominant theme in behavioral research with human neonates is that human infants differ on temperamental characteristics (Kagan, 1982; Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981; Thomas, Chess, & Birch, 1968) and that these inherent temperamental qualities predict the kind of interaction the infant is likely to elicit from the environment (Brazelton, 1973). The Brazelton Newborn Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) has been the most commonly used instrument for research with human neonates. Two studies have compared the behavior of chimpanzees and human neonates on the NBAS. Hallock et al. (1989) reported that, whereas human infants scored higher on an orientation cluster and lower in motor maturity relative to the chimpanzee infants, newborn chimpanzees were remarkably similar to human neonates in their behavioral responses on the NBAS. Similarly, Bard, Platzman, Lester, and Suomi (1992) reported striking similarities in early orientation ability between human neonates and nursery-reared chimpanzees.
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
IN RHESUS
157
To provide detailed data on a wide range of neonatal behaviors, we developed an assessment instrument based on the NBAS to capture the characteristics and capabilities of the newborn rhesus monkey. This instrument was designed not only to specify the infants’ current behavioral responses pertaining to temperament, interactive, and neuromotor functioning but also to be used longitudinally to indicate emerging developmental trends. In this study, we report observations of the neurobehavioral repertoire of nursery-reared infants from birth through the first month of age who were reared under a number of conditions in which their exposure to animate and inanimate objects varied. We had three specific goals: (a) to describe changes in the emerging developmental functions, (b) to assess the behavioral stability of individual differences across time, and (c) to examine whether early measures varied as a function of environmental experience. METHOD
Subjects The subjects were 36 infant rhesus monkeys (Macaca mufutfa), 22 males and 14 females, who were tested from birth through the first month of age. All were vaginally delivered and healthy at birth. Birth weights for males ranged from 420 to 600 gms (M = 498, SE = 11.6); for females, the range was 415 to 605 gms (M = 510, SE = 14.3). Gestational age for males was 155 to 179 days (M = 169, SE = 1.5); for females, the range was 146 to 175 days (M = 169, SE = 2.1). Gestational age was assessed as the interval between the first day of the 3-day timed mating period of the mothers and parturition. All infants were separated from their mothers at birth and reared for the first 30 days postpartum in the laboratory nursing according to a standard procedure described by Ruppenthal (1979). Infants were nursery reared both to prevent confounding due to differential maternal treatment and to enable continuous access to the infants for testing. All subjects were individually housed in 41cm x 51-cm x 42-cm cages in visual, auditory, and olfactory contact with other infants during the first month of life. Subjects had free access to Similac infant formula (Ross Laboratories). There were four groups of infants born between April 1984, and June 1990. The first group consisted of 11 infants (9 males, 2 females) born in 1984, who were provided from birth with a cloth as a surrogate artificial mother and are referred to as cloth-reared. A second group of 8 infants (4 males, 4 females), 4 born in 1984 and 4 born in 1985, were reared with an upright cloth-covered stationary surrogate and are referred to as stationary-surrogate infants. A third group consisted of 7 infants (3 males, 4 females), 3 born in 1984 and 4 born in 1985, were reared from birth with an upright cloth-covered movable surrogate and a cloth-covered water-filled plastic pillow, and are referred to as movable-surrogate infants. The remaining 10 infants (6 males, 4 females), 6
158
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
born in 1989 and 4 born in 1990, were reared from birth with a cloth-covered movable surrogate, a cloth-covered water-filled plastic pillow, two loops of plastic garden hose as a climbing apparatus, and a variety of brightly colored manipulatable infant toys rotated among infants daily. In addition, 4 days per week, they were socialized in dyads for 15 min. The latter infants are referred to as peer-toy infants. No differences across groups were detected for birth weight or gestation length. Infant Neurobehavioral Assessment Scale
A 20-min battery of developmental tests was administered on the following days postpartum: 4,6,8, 11, 15, l&22,25, and 29. Testing occurred between 10:00 a.m. and 12:00 p.m., approximately midway between feedings. The test battery was designed to assess a number of variables that mature across the first month of life. These variables fall into three categories: temperament, interactive, and neuromotor. Ratings were based on scales ranging from 0 to 2. The complete list of test items is given in Table 1. Observers were trained to reliability by the first author. Observers were not blind to the rearing condition of the infants; however, they were unaware of the experimental hypothesis being tested. The test began with the examiner wrapping the infant in a cloth diaper from the waist down, leaving the arms free to move. The orientation items were administered first. They consisted of ratings of the infants’ orienting and visual following responses elicited both to a plastic toy (Mickey Mouse face) and to a lipsmacking sound (simulating a monkey) made by the examiner. The percentage of time during which the infant was attentive was rated as well as the duration or length of the looks while orienting to the toy or sound. Neuromotor functions included ratings of muscle tonus, coordination, balance, tremulousness, response speed, and spontaneous motor activity. In addition, reflex items included righting reactions, grasp reflex, placing responses, rotation or tonic deviation of the head and eyes, rooting, the Moro response, and a measure of visuo-vestibular responses (Ayres, 1976). Muscle tonus was assessed while the infant was held in both the prone and supine positions and was based on the infant’s ability to maintain its head in a position against gravity. In addition, muscle tonus was assessed during both passive flexing and extending of the relaxed limbs, as well as while the infant was actively contracting its muscles. Temperament ratings were based on behaviors noted during the administration of the orienting and neuromotor items. The goal was to maintain the infant in a quiet, alert state throughout the administration of the sequence of items. Therefore, the infant’s temperament was constantly monitored and the examiner intervened to console the infant when appropriate. Ratings were subsequently made of numerous temperamental dimensions including response intensity, soothability, irritability, fearfulness, and distress to limitations (see Table 1 for further detail).
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
Definition
of Meosures
ASSESSMENT
IN RHESUS
TABLE I on Neurobehoviorol
159
Assessment
Items
Scale
Definition
Orientotion Visual
orientation
Eyes oriented toword positions in infont’s contact; 2 = direct
Visual
following
Eyes following moving object (plastic horizontol ond vertical directions (0 I = starts to follow; 2 = complete Exominer rating of length of looks on
Durotton
of looking
Attention
looking; Examiner ottention
span
attentive Stote
toy (plastic periphery prolonged
Mickey Mouse (0 = no orient; contact).
face) held I = direct
Mickey Mouse = contact but following). orlenting items
in foul brief
face) in both no following; (0 = no
1 = brief looks; 2 = I- to 2-s looks). roting of ottention on previous items (0 = lock of on oil items; 1 = ottenttve 25% of the time; 2 = 75%
of the time).
Control
Irritability
Amount
Consolobility
distress noted continuously; I = distress opparent 50% of examination; 2 = distress not opporent during the examlnotion) Eose of consoling infont during distress (0 = Impossible to soothe
Struggle
or console Infont; 1 = infant IS consoled with difficulty using holding, swoddling, rocking, and/or strokmg; 2 = infont wos eosy to console simply by picking up). Amount of squirming (0 = 25% of time squirming; I = infant
during
Predomlnont
Motor
testing state
of distress
noted
during
the entire
exominotion
(0 =
squirmed 50% of time; 2 = contmuous squirming noted.]. State of infont during exominotion (0 = alert, awoke, ond owore; 1 = alert but somewhot ogitoted; 2 = extremely agitated).
Maturity
Muscle
tonus-prone
Muscle tonus-supine Coordination Response
speed
Lobryinthion
righting
Ability to hold heod hanging
head up when held down; I = heod
prone lifted
(0 = flaccid tone but not mointolned
2 = head llfted and maintained for ot leost 3 s). Assessed exactly OS above but infont held I” supine Quality of movement roted (0 = clumsy movements;
with for 3 s;
position. 1 =
odequote movements, 2 = agile movements]. Exominer rating of speed of responding (0 = 25% of responses quick; I = 75% of responses qutck; 2 = 011 responses quick). Reolignment of head when body is tilted 45” sidewoys (0 = heod and lines
body in some plane; up with the vertical
Observotion
of motor
I = plone).
heod
portlolly
rights;
2 = head
Activity Motor
octtvity
Coordinotion
activity
during
o 5min
observotionol
(0 = motion 25% of the time; 1 = motion 50% continuous motion). Quality of movement roted (0 = clumsy movements; odequote movements; 2 = agile movements).
period
of the time;
2 =
1 =
(continued)
SCHNEIDER
160
TABLE
AND
SUOMI
1 (Continued) Definrtion
Items Locomotion
Quality of locomotion 2 = coordinated
Possrvity
Duration time;
Individual
of time spent inactive (0 = none; 2 = Inactive 75% or more).
I = weak
and
grasp
Auditory
startle
Auditory
orient
Tactrle
Attempts
1 = inoctrve
50%
of the
and
grosp
Response to tactile (0 = no response; definite curvation
plantor
Response grasping; voluntary
response righting
Visuo-vestibular
orient/follow
toy
(0 = no attempt;
stimulus lateral 1 = barely noted).
to examiner’s 1 = portiol release).
and parallel to vertebral column apparent curvatron of spine; 2 =
index finger placed grasp; 2 = strong
rn palm reflexive
response
Tremulousness
vocalizations extremely Duration and excursion rotation (lo/20 s) on
loud length rotary
Self-mouth
Infant’s behavior when placed in on enclosed calm 90% of time; 1 = calm 50% of time; distress). manipulatrons
mild
and/or shrill in expression). of postrotary nystagmus following board (0 = for no observable 2 = for of nystogmus
Examiner ratrng of tremulousness (0 = no incidents; incrdents, 2 = 3-4 incidents). Number of vocalizations in o 1-min trme period.
Vocalization Self-quieting
Fearfulness
(0 = no reflex grasp without
resistance to supine position with attempts to turn over). Examiner rating of quality of vocal reactions (0 = vocalizations in intensity; 1 = vocalizations moderote or average, 2 =
intensity
nystagmus; 1 = for barely apparent nystagmus; nystagmus 1 mm or greater in length). Duration seconds was recorded in both directions.
motor
1 =
Time noted for infant to turn from supme to prone (0 = portral righting; 1 = nghts in 5 to 15 s; 2 = rights in less than 5 s). Infant IS pulled from supine to sitting (0 = limbs extend and heod lags; 1 = arms moderotely flexed with no heod lag; 2 =
Pull-to-sit
Response
vrsual
mode rn infant’s periphery (0 = no orientmg; I = partial headturning; 2 = full head-turn with visual inspection of source). Response to tactile stimulus to four extremities (0 = no response; 1 = barely discernible response; 2 = easily apparent response).
response
Palmar
to grasp
gross swat; 2 = intentional grasp with finger flexion). Response to sudden noise (0 = no response; 1 = eye blink and/or head ierk; 2 = body terk). Eyes oriented toward lipsmacking noise srmulating monkey sound
response
Galont’s
Fine
attempt;
Items
Reach
Body
was roted (0 = none; locomotion noted).
1 =
in
l-2
area for 5 min 2 = continuous
(0 =
Durotion of time engoged in fine motor activities in observational period (0 = none, I = less thon 5 s, 2 = more than 5 s). Fear grimaces or trembling (0 = none; 1 = feor grimaces early testing session; 2 = fear noted frequently). Inserting hands 2 = insertion
or feet lasted
in mouth (0 = none; 15 s or more).
1 = brief
insertion;
(continued)
in
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
TABLE
1 (Conttnued)
Items
Definition
Mointenonce
of balance
Possive range of motion (muscle tonus) Active
Placing
Infant
is held
in the sitting
position
and
support
is withdrawn
(0 =
infant falls over; 1 = places hands out but falls; 2 = uses hands for support and maintoins balance]. Degree of resistance to passive flexion and extension of limbs (0 = barely discernible resistance; 1 = moderate resistance; 2 = strong resistance). Strength of muscles when actively contracting (0 = some strength but connot withstand slight resistance; 1 = withstands moderate
power
resistance; 2 = extremely Infant places foot or hand darsum of hand or foot definite response).
response
Parachute
response
Rotation (tonic of heod and Moro
161
IN RHESUS
deviotion eyes]
reflex
Distress
to limitations
Rooting
reflex
strong) on surface following tactile stimulus (0 = absent; I = weak response;
to 2 =
Upper extremity limb extension following head-first descent toward surface (0 = no orm extension; 1 = partial extension and openmg of the honds; 2 = definite orm extenston with open hands). Degree to which rotation, both weak Degree flexion
head and/or eyes with head free and
turn into restroined
the
dlrection of (0 = obsent; 1 =
turn; 2 = definite response). of abduction and extension in upper extremities following of head ond sudden withdrawal of support while supine
(0 = none observable; 1 = 45” of abduction, 2 = 90” or more of abduction). Infant’s movement was restricted for 10 s and degree of squirming was noted (0 = resistance 25% of the time; 2 = resistance 50% of the time; 3 = constant resistonce). Infont’s response (head-turning toward stimulus at the corner of the mouth turn toward stimulus; 2 = full turn].
stimulus) (0 = no
to light response;
tactile 1 = ihreak
Data Analysis
Separate analyses of variance were computed with years as the betweensubjects variable and days of testing as the repeated measure for the stationary-surrogate (1984 vs. 1985), movable-surrogate (1984 vs. 1985), and peer-toy (1989 vs. 1990) infants. Because there were no significant differences across years, subjects from similar rearing conditions were combined across years for subsequent analyses. To determine if there were significant effects for rearing condition and days of testing, all measures were evaluated using mixed-design analyses of variance (ANOVA) with rearing condition as the between-subjects variable and days of testing as the repeated measure. When an overall F was significant for rearing condition, the Fisher test was used to compare all possible pairs of
162
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
treatment means (Keppel, 1982). Correlational analysis was used to test the stability of performance over time. Linear trend analyses were performed to assess whether the best-fitting linear function for the four groups differed. The slopes of the best-fitting functions were computed for each subject by taking the sum of the cross products of the scores multiplied by the linear coefficients of orthogonal polynomials. ANOVAs were performed with groups as a between-subjects factor. Similar procedures were repeated using the quadratic coefficients of orthogonal polynomials to assess whether the best-fitting quadratic functions differed across groups. Analyses were performed on individual measures as well as on composite scores. Composite scores were based on prior factor analyses described by Schneider, Moore, et al. (in press). These consisted of four composite scores: orientation, state control, motor maturity, and activity. The orientation score was composed of visual orienting, visual following, duration of looking, and attention span. State control consisted of irritability, consolability, struggle during testing, and predominant state. Motor maturity was composed of muscle tonus-prone, muscle tonus-supine, coordination, response speed, and labyrinthian righting. Finally, activity was composed of motor activity, coordination, locomotion, and passivity (see items defined in Table 1). All measures were examined for sex differences using ANOVA (sex by days of testing), and none was found. RESULTS Developmental Change
Developmental changes across the first month of life were examined using ANOVA with days of testing as the repeated measure (see Table 2, pp. 164165). The infants’ scores increased with age on orientation, F(8,256) = 8.22, p < .OOl, on motor maturity, F(8, 256) = 12.22, p < .OOOl, and on activity, F(8,256) = 22.11, p < .OOOl, composite scores. Along similar lines, analyses of individual items demonstrated significant (all ps < .05) increases with age on individual orienting items (visual orienting. visual following, reach and grasp, duration of looking, and attention span), significant increases in neuromotor capabilities (muscle tonus, body righting, pull-to-sit, response speed, motor activity, coordination, locomotion, balance, muscle tonus while passive, active power, and parachute response), and significant decreases in the primitive reflexes (moro, rooting, and grasping) across testing session (see Table 2 for details). Within the domain of temperament, ratings of response intensity, fearfulness, and distress to limitation increased significantly with age. Finally, both auditory orienting and visuo-vestibular responsivity decreased significantly with age. Stability Across Testing Sessions. Stability was summarized in two ways: (a) at 3- to 5-day intervals between tests, and (b) at l- to 3-week intervals
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
IN RHESUS
163
between tests. The 3- to 5-day interval data provide a detailed picture of patterns of stability and instability during the neonatal period. The I- to 3-week interval data provide a picture of long-term neonatal continuity. Table 3 (pp. 166-167) shows the correlations across the first month of life at 3to 5-day intervals between tests. As can be seen in Table 3, overall, the correlations were moderate to highly significant. In fact, orientation, motor maturity, and activity composite scores were all significantly correlated across the 9 testing days from the first day of testing. State control, on the other hand, showed stability only from Day 12 postpartum. Examination of individual items also revealed moderate stability across the first month of life for the majority of items (see Table 3 for details). Table 4 (p. 168) shows correlations at l- to 3-week intervals. For Days 6 to 15, measures of motor maturity showed reliable continuity, all rs < .05. These included the motor maturity composite score, activity composite score, muscle tonus-supine, locomotion, coordination, balance, parachute reaction, tremulousness, and visuo-vestibular responses. Correlations from Day 15 to Day 29 improved slightly over the 6- to 15-day interval. Motor maturity and activity measures continued to show stability. These included the motor maturity composite score, activity composite score, muscle tonus-prone, motor activity, coordination, placing, tremulousness, and visuo-vestibular responses. Moreover, orientation items began to emerge as reliable indices, with the orientation composite score and duration of looking showing reliable continuity. Finally, analysis of correlations between Days 6 and 29 revealed little agreement between test scores. Rearing Eflecrs. Comparison of the four rearing conditions indicates that there was an overall significant rearing effect for the orientation composite score across groups of infants, F(3,32) = 5.73, p < .003 (see Figure 1, p. 169). Post-hoc testing revealed that the peer-toy group showed significantly greater responsiveness to the orientation items when compared to the cloth-reared, stationary-surrogate, and moving-surrogate groups, all ps < .05. Moreover, linear and quadratic trend analysis revealed differences in the linear component, F(3, 32) = 3.03, p < .05, across groups with the peer-toy group demonstrating a steeper slope. There were no group differences on orientation for the quadratic component. In addition, an inspection of the means for the individual items in the orientation grouping showed significant differences across rearing conditions for visual orient, F(3,32) = 10.15, p < .OOOl, visual follow, F(3,32) = 10.07, p < .OOOl, and for reach and grasp, F(3,32) = 3.40, p < .05 (see Figure 2, p. 170). Post-hoc testing revealed that the peer-toy infants were significantly more responsive than the cloth-reared, stationarysurrogate, and moving-surrogate infants, all ps < .05, on these three measures. Linear trends were also significantly different across groups for visual orient, F(3, 32) = 4.36, p < .Ol, visual follow, F(3,32) = 6.47, p < .OOl, and reach and grasp, F(3, 32) = 3.00. p < .05.
and
of look span response response grosp grasp tonus-prone
Reach Auditory Auditory Duration Attention Tactile Galont Polmar Plantor Muscle
Response
speed
Muscle tonus-supine Body righting Pull-to-sit Labyrinthian righting
grasp startle orient
orientation follow
Measures
Visuol Visual
lndividuol
1.29 1.56 1.00 0.82 0.93
0.59 0.96
1.34 0.76 0.60 1.31
1.16 0.86 1.19
0.49 0.86 I .65
0.41 0.40
0.57 0.75
0.71 0.98
6
Scares
1.31 1.69 0.89
0.94 1.13 1 .oo 1.46
0.99 0.67 0.91
0.25 0.75 1.65
0.24 0.23
0.55 0.66
State control Activity
4 0.52 0.91
Scores
Orientation Motor maturity
Composite
Mean
far
0.75 0.78 0.74 0.82 1.39 1.32 1.08 1.10 1.31 0.65 0.70 1.59 1.49 i .a7 1.18 0.71 1.01
0.78 0.67 0.95 0.76 1.61 1.35 1.04 1.13 1.25 0.71 0.54 1.42 1.32 1.65 1.04 0.63 0.99
0.68 1.13
1.17
1.Ol 0.62 0.92
12 0.98
a 0.95
Days
Neurobehaviorol Postpartum
1.81 1.51 0.92 1.01
1.60 1.64
1.24 1.06 1.40 0.41 0.27
0.78 1.10 i .4a
0.81 1.oo 1.oo
0.69 1.34
1.31
1.15
15
2
18
1.31
1.17
I .a9 1.44 0.48 . 1.11
1.78 1.76
1.31 1.05 1.42 0.27 0.30
0.78 1.34 1.50
0.95 0.92 1 .oo
0.81 1.34
Measures
TABLE
1.90 1.51 0.64 1.18
1.69 1.42
1.22 1.29 1.19 0.10 0.18
0.86 1.04 1.51
0.95 1.60 0.83
0.68 1.40
1.29
1.15
22
Across
1.97 1.65 0.85 1.17
1.64 1.43
1.20 1.26 1.12 0.05 0.19
0.66 0.68 1.57
1.10 1.60 0.97
0.75 1.41
1.34
1.21
25
First Month
2.00 1.60 0.75 1.32
1.77 1.40
1.29 1.10 1.08 0.11 0.11
0.62 0.68 1.56
1.10 1.50 1.10
0.79 1.46
1.37
1.19
29
of Life Significant
n.s. ns. n.s. n.s.
I
l
n.s *
tt*
l **
t* tt
*** l *
***
ns.
et*
*t*
n.s.
ns. n.s. n.s. n.s.
l **
n.s.
l **
***
ns. ns. ns.
l ** ***
n.s ***
***
*t*
Da&’
l **
n.s. ns.
***
**
Effects Rearing Condition”
Response
limitation
0.49
0.50 1.28 1.78 0.57 0.61
1.42 1.17 1.17
0.94 0.75 0.61
0.99 0.42 0.64
1.32 1.78 1.35
0.69 12.44 0.64 0.90
1.07 1.47 13.61
df = 8, 256.
df = 3, 32
0.81
1.07 1.74 0.58 0.49
0.94 1.65 0.67 0.54 0.88
0.94 1.03 0.83
1.06 0.79 0.57 1.15
0.99 0.36 0.71
0.89 1.53 1.30
7.33 0.44 0.79
1.68 16.07 0.51
1.10
0.85 1.10 1.04 0.76
1 .oo 0.68 0.57
0.87 0.36 0.50
0.69 1.26 1.09
8.39 0.50 0.61
1.67 17.54 0.68
0.78
rearing condition: days of testing: lower possiwty p < ,001.
0.51 1.19
0.89 0.82 1.75 0.92
0.89 0.60 1.75 1.08 0.39 1.33
0.78 0.89 1.01
0.44 0.93 0.93
0.73 0.66 0.50
0.84 0.28 0.34
0.85 0.19 0.28 0.53 0.69 0.49
0.43 0.89 1.29
0.82 7.25 0.33 0.44
0.78 9.92 0.49 0.29 0.18 0.78 1.40
1.02 1.76 19.39
0.92 1.64 19.06
0 Effects are significant for b Effects are significant for ’ Higher score represents l p < .05. ** p < .Ol. ***
Rooting
Ratotion Moro Distress
Balance Possive ROM Active power Placing Parachute
Self-mouth Struggle Predominant
Consolability Fearfulness
Coordination Locomotion Passivity Irritability
Vocalization Self-quieting Motor activity
state
intensity
VOR excursion VOR duration Tremulousness
0.64 0.34
0.78 1.35 1.67 0.58
1.56 1.31 1.39
1.22 1.06 0.60
1.39 1.14 0.46 0.67
0.81 1.43 1.74
0.53 6.28 0.36
1.11 1.53 13.92
0.60 0.81 0.50
0.87 1.31 1.68
1.62 1.28 1.21
1.08 0.64 0.60
1 .Ol 0.46 0.56
0.90 1.49 1.81 1.42
0.72 7.81 0.31
1.07 1.56 14.37
0.40 0.83 0.37
0.77 1.49 1.67
1.59 1.37 1.33
0.69 0.86 0.83 0.67
1.51 1.07 0.44
0.81 1.56 1.75
0.51 13.22 0.47
1.12 1.46 14.37
0.69 0.81 0.18
0.68 1.51 1.57
1.69 1.33 1.31
1.14 0.92 0.65
1.83 1.41 1.06 0.54 1.12
0.64 0.93 1.64
0.60 16.83
1.67 14.43
1.28
n.5.
**
l
ns. n.5. II
ns. n.s.
n.s. n.s. n.s. **
n.s. n.s. *
n.s. n.s.
tt*
t**
l **
n.s.
l **
n.s.
l *t
***
l **
n.5. n.s.
n.s. n.s. ns. *** *t*
n.s. *** *** *** n.5. *
l * **
*
n.s. It
*.*
l *
n.s.
n.s. n.s. n.s.
Scores”
ond
span response response grosp grasp tonus-prone
Reach Auditory Auditory Duration Attention Tactile Golont Polmor Plontor Muscle Muscle tonus-supine Body righting Pull-to-sit Lobyrinthion righting
grosp stortle orient of looking
orient follow
Measures
Visual Visual
Individual
Motor maturity Stote control Activity
Orientotion
Composite
Correlations
14 .03 .15 .22
.38* .60* .35* .60***
.41* .24 .17
.45** .42* .44** .29
.47” .52** .42’ .42’ - .07 - .09 .40*
.06 09 .05
.24 .48”
.47** .40’ .42* .68*”
6-8
.23 .44” .26
.38* .42*
.58”’ .63’*’ - .09 .64***
4-6
for Neurobehoviorol
Measures
.52* .68*** .20 .I5 .33* .30 .35’ .04
.17 42’ .lO .36’ .34’ .28 .43** .27 .21 .43** .33* .67”’ .13 .36’ .14
.45** .l 1 .09 .22 .57”’
.27 .45** .33’ .54***
.26 .I3
.31
37’
.53*** ,431”
.50** .62”’ .43** .64”’
15-18
Postpartum
Month
.41* -30 .l 1
.37’
.39*
12-15
Days
the First
.33* .42’ .55”’
3
.42* .34’ .14
%12
TABLE Across of Life
-
.34* .ll .04 .24 .32
.31 .07 .29
.48” .33* .52** .55***
.28 .35* .54”’ .43** .02 .55***
.05 .44** .51”
.I6 .67”’ .19
.oo .41’ .20
.15 .11
.42’ .36’ .53*** .34*
.ll .13 .63***
.24 .61**’ .I1
.63**’
.56**’ .54”’
.60**’ .70***
.62”*
.51** .51”
25-29
.53*** .64”*
22-25
mtervols)
.23 .35* .16
.29 .42* .15
.39*
.43** .37* .52**
.31
18-22
(3- to 5doy
“df = 34. * p < .05. ** p < .Ol.
Response speed Response intensity VOR excursion VOR duration Tremulousness Vocalization Self-quieting Motor activity Coordination Locomotion Passive Irritability Consolobility Fearfulness Self-mouth Struggle Predominant stote Balance Passive ROM Active power Placing Porochute Rotation Moro Distress limitations Rooting
l
** p < ,001
.48** .48** .33' .75*** .16 .25 .I5 .23 .67"* .44** .69*** .12 .24 .51** .36 .29 .13 .50* .64*'* .59*** .17 .49** .19 .80*** -42' .49**
.08 .51** .lO .45** .40 .39' .19 .56*" .44** .39* .49" .27 .51** .37' .14 .30 .59*** .27 .13 ,668" .03 .74"' .09 .67"' .I2 .32
.17 .34* .29 .63*" .33* .28 .38' .27 .19 .59*** .34* .34* .12 .Ol .42* .02 .18 .36* .38* .51* .09 -44" .04 .43** .07 .43** .35* .61**' .37' .68*'* .53*** .33* .48** .37* .36* .08 .36* .54"' .53*** .24 .56*'* .41 .34' .31 .43** .41* .49** .59*** .26 .44** .32 .22
.53*** .51** .28 .51*** .25 .42* .I9 .45** .52** .51** .47** .40* .23 .32 .55*** .42 .32 .54*** .21 .lO .49** .72'** .32 .55*** .28 .31
.31 .39* .30 .58'*' .25 .44** .28 .71*** .48*' .l 1 .28 .lO .32 p.02 .55*** .41 .37' .21 .27 .27 .40* .17 -.05 .50** .34* .37*
.52** .34* .17 .56"* .47.I7 .41* .73"' .61*** .52** .67'** .29 .41* .51** .50* .43 .55*** .49* .76"' .58*** .66*'* .52** .30 .82"' .41* .46**
.25 .46** .35* .46** .50** .42* .26 .60*** .61*** .15 .26 .48** .49** .39* .74*** .61 .38' .57-* .41* .47** .32 .31 .53** .62*" .54*** .53***
Correlotions
TABLE 4 Measures
for Neurobehaviorol
(1. to 3-week Davs
Composite
Scores”
6-15
.oo
Orientation Motor maturity State control
/57*** - .22
Activity Individual
15-29 .38* .43” .20
.47”
.35*
&29 -.ll .09 .05 .34’
Measures
Visual Visual
orientation follow
.lO -.Ol
.21 .14
Reach Auditory Auditory Duration
and
.29 .23 - .23
.32 .32 .oo
Attention Tactile Gafont
span response response
.02 -.14 .21
.43** .31 .34*
Polmor Plontar Muscle
grosp grasp tonus-prone
.35* .18 .05
.lO .lO -.13 .33*
grasp startle orient of look
.15 .37* .14 .25
Muscle tonus-supine Body righting Pull-to-sit Labyrinthion righting Response speed Response intensity VOR excursion VOR duration Tremulousness Vocolizotion Self-quieting
Fearfulness Self-mouth Struggle Predominant Balance
.13 .29 .20
.19 .59*** .34*
.34* .52** .35*
.34* .48** .27 - .20 -.ll
Passivity Irritability Consolability
.Ol .43” .03 -.14 .33* .13
stote
Possive ROM Active power Placing Parachute
- .09 - .04 .53*** .23 .32 .30
limitations
“df = 34. l p < .05. ** p < .Ol.
***
p < ,001.
.20 .oo - ,004
.32 .20 .OB
- .OB - .04 .30
Motor activity Coordinotion Locomotion
Rototion Distress Rooting
intervals)
Postoortum
.14 .20 .36* .41* -.14 .39’ .13 .20 - .08 .42**
.07 .08 .02 .22 .lO - .06 - .33* .06 .02 .12 .17 .oo .02
.J3 - .07 - .32 - .20 .15 .23 .17 -.ll .28 .26 .36’ .32 .03 .ll .07 .18 .25
.34** .oo .06
.15 .02 .20 .23
.13 - .05 .33* .30
- .20 .25 .03 .30
.02 .09 .09
- .25 .24 .15
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
169
IN RHESUS
2.0 uz v)
1.8 1.6 -
z E .-
1.4 -
r2
1.2 -
tz
1.0 -
5
0.8 -
iis
0.6 -
64
0.4 -
c
;j 0
V * M
Cloth Sfationaw Movable. Peers/toys
,
,
(
,
,
,
,
4
8
12
16
20
24
28
Days Postpartum Figure
1. Mean
scores
on orientotion
across
first month
of life as o function
of rearing
condition.
Likewise, comparison of means on the motor maturity composite score revealed a significant overall effect of rearing conditions, F(3,32) = 7.54, p < .0006 (see Figure 3, p. 171). The peer-toy infants demonstrated an advantage in motor maturity when compared to the cloth-reared and stationary-reared infants, but not in comparison to the moving-surrogate infants. Furthermore, there was a significant difference for motor maturity between the movingsurrogate and stationary-surrogate infants. Inspection of the scores that compose the motor maturity composite score revealed significant differences across groups for coordination, F(3, 32) = 3.13, p < .04, muscle tonussupine, F(3,32) = 5.84, p < .003, and muscle tonus-prone, F(3,32) = 5.72, p < .003 (see Figure 4, p. 172). For coordination, there was significant advantage for the peer-toy group over the cloth-reared group. For muscle tonus-supine, there was a significant advantage for the peer-toy infants over the cloth-reared and the stationary-reared infants. In addition, the infants reared in the movable-surrogate condition were superior to those reared in the stationary-surrogate condition. For muscle tonus-prone, both the peertoy infants and the movable-surrogate infants were superior to the clothreared infants, all ps < .05. Linear and quadratic trend analysis revealed no significant differences across groups.
170
SCHNEIDER
AND
20 _ T I
Visual Cloth Slstionwy Movable
‘S-
PeeKKoys
-
SUOMl
Orient
1.0 -
0.5 -
0.0
0
I 0
I 16
Visual
0.0
! 0
I 8
1 24
Follow
I 16
Reach
and
24
Grasp
2.0,
I 0
1 16
Days Figure across
2. Mean scores on visual orient first month of life as o function
24
Postpartum
(upper), v~suol follow of rearing condition.
(middle),
and
reach
and
grosp
(lower)
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
171
IN RHESUS
2.0 s! E ul
z 5 .-
1.8 1.6 1.4
c2
1.2
m”
1.0
5
0.8
z 5
0.6
:
L_ + * V
0.4 2
Cloth Stationary Movable Peers/toys
0.2
s 0.0 0
I 4
I 0
1 12
I 16
I 20
I 24
I 28
Days Postpartum Figure
3. Mean
scores
on motor
maturity
across
first month
of life OS o function
of reoring
condition.
In addition to the effects on the composite scores, infants were found to differ across rearing conditions in self-quieting, F(3, 32) = 5.92, p < .003, fearfulness during testing. F(3, 32) = 2.94, p < .05, balance, F(3, 32) = 13.64, p < .OOOl, distress to limitations, F(3, 32) = 4.08, p < .02, vocalizations, F(3, 32) = 4.19, p < .02, rotation reflex, F(3,32) = 2.92, p < .05, and moro reflex, F(3, 32) = 9.38, p < .05. The peer-toy infants were rated as superior in self-quieting, lower in fearfulness, superior in balance, higher in distress to limitations, emitted fewer vocalizations in a novel environment; they demonstrated a lower level of rotation or tonic deviation of head and eyes and an attenuated moro response when compared to the infants reared without early exposure to peers and toys. DISCUSSION This prospective study documents developmental changes, individual stability, and effects of environmental experience in nursery-reared rhesus monkey infants tested repeatedly from birth through the first month of age. The overall finding that the large majority of the infants’ responses changed significantly with age was not surprising. Clinically and intuitively, these results make sense, in that one would expect the infants’ orientation to visual stimuli, their neuromotor capabilities, and their activity level to increase
172
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
Coordination 20-
1.5-
l.O-
ClOlh Stationary MOWble
0.5 PWdTOYS
Muscle
0.0
! 0
Tonus-Supine
I 0
I 16
Muscle
Tonus-Prone
I 24
20
1.5
1.0 :
0.01 0
8
Days Figure prone
16
24
Postpartum
4. Mean scares on coordination (upper), muscle tonus-supine (middle), (lower) across first month of life as a function of reoring condition.
and
muscle
tonus-
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
IN RHESUS
173
rapidly across the first month of life. Prior studies have documented increases in gross motor skills, including locomotion and climbing, decreases in the presence of early primitive reflexes, such as rooting and grasping, and increases in visual orienting behaviors with age (see Rogers, 1988, for a review). However, the present study is unique in that it expands upon prior work to provide normative data on a number of variables emphasized in the human literature, yet ignored in other studies on nonhuman primates (e.g., muscle tonus, righting reactions, and a variety of temperamental characteristics). The data on individual behavioral stability over time parallel reports on human children (Honzik, 1976) that indicate that correlation coefficients vary widely depending on the particular measure at hand, the size of the interval between tests, and the age of the subjects at the time of testing. The correlations across 3- to 5-day intervals in the present study were moderately high for most of the measures. This finding was interesting given the lack of day-today stability over the first few weeks of life reported for human neonates (Sameroff, 1978). There are several potential explanations for these findings. First, the nursery-rearing condition permitted all subjects to be reared under identical conditions in terms of housing, lighting, nutrition, and caregiver contact. This is in direct contrast to studies with human neonates in which these factors are subject to wide variance across subjects. Second, because rhesus monkey neonates are born in a more mature state than that noted with human neonates, especially with regard to motor maturity, they may indeed have greater behavioral stability than human neonates. Third, the structure of our procedure differs from that typically used in human neonatal studies (Brazelton, 1973), in which the sequence of test items varies based on the responses of the infant. In our procedure, infants were presented with an invariant sequence; the use of a standard procedure might have enhanced our finding of increased behavioral stability across testing sessions. It should be noted that, whereas the nursery-rearing condition controls for potential differences in maternal care, nursery rearing produces dramatic effects on early behavioral ontogeny and on the activity of brain biogenic amine systems (Kraemer, Ebert, Schmidt, & McKinney, 1989). This limits direct generalization of these data to rhesus monkey infants reared in species-typical environments. The finding that stability coefficients for l- to 3-week intervals were lower than those found for 3- to 5-day intervals is consistent with reports on human children that correlations decline as the time span between tests lengthens (Honzik, 1976). Correlations between Days 6 and 15 were only significant for motor maturity and activity composite scores, muscle tonus, tremulousness, coordination, and locomotion. Similarly, correlations from Day 15 to Day 29 were significant for measures of motor maturity and activity; however, orientation and duration of looking were also stable during this interval. The finding that orientation items showed stability only from Day 15 on was not
174
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
surprising given that nursery-reared rhesus monkey neonates were found to visually orient and follow an object only during the second week of life; hence, one would not expect to obtain reliable measurements of this function prior to 15 days of age. Lastly, there was poor agreement between assessments performed at 6 days of life and 29 days of life. Several conclusions can be made from these data. First, as with the human data, the smaller the interval between testing, the better the correlations. Second, the grouping of test items a priori into motor maturity, activity, state control, and orientation composite scores has apparently served to improve correlations. Third, the specific nature of the measure and the time of testing is important. For example, observations of items related to motor maturity and activity appear to be more stable during the early neonatal period in hand-reared rhesus monkey infants. Orientation items, emerging later in the neonatal period, only show stability from Day 15 postpartum on. Finally, as with human neonates, when one takes two measurements that span a time period that involves rapid developmental changes within the individual, one is not apt to obtain satisfactory measures of continuity. Therefore, some researchers (Wohlwill, 1973) have advocated the need for repeated assessments across time in order to plot a developmental curve. Continuities as well as discontinuities then become windows into the developing systems (Brazelton, . 1990). The finding that infants reared in the toy-peer condition had significantly higher orienting scores, better motor maturity scores, and lower ratings on the dimension of fearfulness compared to nursery-reared infants without exposure to peers and novel toys requires some comment. Because we had tested the neonate repeatedly, we were able not only to determine that infants in the peer-toy condition were better on visual orienting, visual following, and reach and grasp but also to conclude that the slope of the bestfitting line was steeper for the peer-toy infants on these items. These data illustrate the usefulness of implementing repeated testing in order to express data in terms of developmental functions. Furthermore, these comparisons of infants reared under different nurseryrearing conditions demonstrate that the nursery environment can have striking consequences even at a very early age. While the effects of environmental experience have been extensively documented in nonhuman primates (see Mason & Berkson, 1975; Sackett, 1968, for reviews), the present study is one of the few studies to describe effects expressed during the first few weeks of life. These data underscore the benefits of early exposure to social and nonsocial stimuli in nonhuman primate studies in which the experimental design requires nursery rearing to decrease differential maternal treatment and to enable the investigator repeated access to the infants for testing. Further investigation is needed to explore whether these data will generalize the infants reared in species-typical environment, that is, with maternal and peer contact.
NEUROBEHAVIORAL
ASSESSMENT
IN RHESUS
175
Caution is warranted regarding generalization of these data for human infants. One might be tempted to liken the nursery-rearing state in the present study to the condition in which human infants are reared in neonatal intensive care units (NICU). However, it is important to note that the rhesus monkey neonates in the present study were healthy normal infants, whereas occupants of NICUs either have documented handicaps or have experienced potentially brain-injurious events associated with long-term developmental sequelae. Whereas supplementary stimulation was beneficial to the normal nursery-reared monkeys in the present study, one cannot assume that such stimuli would benefit human infants in NICUs. In fact, high-risk infants can tolerate only limited amounts of handling or stimulation due to problems with temperature regulation and stress tolerance (Anderson, 1986). Furthermore, it is not certain whether these data would generalize to healthy human newborns given that human neonates are significantly less mature during the first month of life when compared to macaques. Taken as a whole, our data illustrate that it is possible to measure neurobehavioral development in normal healthy nursery-reared rhesus monkey infants during the first month of life using instruments adapted directly from tests typically used with human neonates. Furthermore, individual behavioral stability was demonstrated, despite the finding that the infants were undergoing rapid developmental change across the first month of life. However, the degree of stability depended on the age of the infant when tested, the size of the time interval between tests, and the particular nature of the function measured. Two points spaced widely across the neonatal period were not particularly useful, whereas repeated measurements characterizing the shape of the developmental function as well as the periods of stability and instability are promising. Finally, our data demonstrate that the early nursery experience can alter an infant’s performance on neonatal measures, particularly on those pertaining to motor maturity and orientation. REFERENCES Anderson, Ayres,
J. (1986).
Sensory
intervention
with
unit. American Journal of Occuputional A.J. (1976). The Southern California Western Psychological Services.
Bard.
K.A.. Platzman, K.A., Lester. B.M.. nonsocial stimuli in neonatal chimpanzees 15. 43-56. Brazelton, T.B. (1973). Neonafal Behavioral cine No. 50). London: Heinemann. Brazelton. Castell. Champoux,
the
preterm
Therapy, posrrorary
infant
& Suomi. S.J. and humans.
Assessment
in the
neonatal
intensive
40, 19-26. nystagmus lest manual. (1992).
fnfunt
Scale (Clinics
Los
Orientation
care
Angeles:
to social
Behavior
and
and Development,
in Developmental
Medi-
T.B. (1990). Saving the bathwater. Child Development. 61, 1661-1671. R., & Sackett, G. (1973). Motor behaviors of neonatal rhesus monkeys: Measurement techniques and early development. Developmenfal Psychobiology, 6, 191-202. M.B., DiGregorio, enrichment for matology, 22. 61-67.
mental
G., Schneider, group-housed
M.L.. & Suomi, S.J. rhesus macaque infants.
(1990).
Inanimate
American
Journal
environ-
of Pri-
176 Clarren,
SCHNEIDER
AND
SUOMI
S.K., & Bowden. D.M. (1982). Fetal alcohol syndrome: A new primate model for binge drinking and its relevance to human ethanol teratogenesis. Pediatrics, 101. 819-824. Ehrlich, A. (1974). Infant development in two prosimian species: Greater galago and slow loris. Developmenral Psychology. 7, 439454. Golub. M.S., Sassenrath, E.N.. & Chapman, L.F. (1981). Mother-infant interaction in rhesus monkeys treated chronically with Delta-9-Tetrahydrocannabinol. Child Development. 52, 389-392. Gunderson, V.M., Grant-Webster, K.S., & Sackctt. G.P. (1989). Deficits in visual recognition in low birth weight infant pigtailed monkeys (Mncncu rtemesrrinuj. Child Developmenr, 60, 119-127. Hallock, M.B.. Worobey, J., & Self. P.A. (1989). Behavioral development in chimpanzee (Pun troglodytes) and human newborn across the first month of life. lnterna!ional Journal of Behavioral Development. 12, 527-540. Honzik, M.P. (1976). Value and limitations of infant tests: An overview. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Origins of inrelligence. New York: Plenum. Kagan. J. (1982). Heart rate and heart rate variability as signs of a temperamental dimension in infants. In C.E. lzard (Ed.), Measuring emotions in infanfs and children. New York: Cambridge University Press. Keppel, G. (1982). Design and analysis: A researcher’s handbook. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. King, J.E., Fobes, J.T., & Fobes, J.L. (1974). Development of early behaviors in neonatal squirrel monkeys and cotton-top tamarins. Developmental Psychobiology, 7, 97-109. Kraemer, G.W., Ebert. M.H., Schmidt, D.E., & McKinney, W.T. (1989). A longitudinal study of the effect of different social rearing conditions on cerebrospinal fluid norepinephrine and biogenic amine metabolites in rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychophurmacolog.v, 2, 17s 189. Levin, E.O., Schneider, M.L.. Ferguson, S.A., Schantz, S.L., & Bowman, R.E. (1988). Behavioral effects of developmental lead exposure in rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 21. 371-382. Mason, W.A., & Berkson, G. (1975). Effects of maternal mobility on the development of rocking and other behaviors in rhesus monkeys: A study with artificial mothers. Developmenral Psychobiology, 8, 197-2 11. Mitchell, G. (1970). Abnormal behavior in primates. In L.A. Rosenblum (Ed.). Prima/e behavior: Development in field and laboratory research. New York: Academic. Mowbray, J.B., & Cadell, T.E. (1962). Early behavior patterns in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 3X-357. Phillips, N.K., & Lockard, J.S. (1985). A gestational monkey model. Effects of phenytoin versus seizures on neonatal outcome. Epilepsia, 26, 697-703. Rogers, W.R. (1988). Behavioral testing. In Y.W. Brans & T.J. Kuehl (Eds.). Nonhuman primates in perinaral research. New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M.K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M.E. Lamb & A.L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmentalpsychology (Vol. 1). Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum. Ruppenthal, G.C. (1979). Survey of protocols for nursery-rearing infant macaques. In G.C. Ruppenthal (Ed.), Nursery care of nonhuman primate. New York: Plenum. Sackett, G.P. (1968). Abnormal behavior in laboratory-reared rhesus monkeys. In M.W. Fox (Ed.). Abnormal behavior in animals. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders. Sackett, G.P.. Gunderson. V.. & Baldwin, D. (1982). Studying the ontogeny of primate behavior. In J.L. Fobes & J.E. King (Eds.), Primate behavior. New York: Academic. Sameroff. A.J. (1978). Summary and conclusions: The future of newborn assessment. Monographs of ihe Society for Research in Child Development, 43(5-6, Serial No. 177). Schneider, M.L. (in press). The effect of mild stress during pregnancy on birth weight and
NEUROBEHAVIORAL neuromotor
maturation
in infant
ASSESSMENT rhesus
monkey
infants
IN RHESUS (Macaca
177
Infant Behav-
mulafra).
ior and Development. Schneider,
M.L., stimulation
Occupational Schneider.
M.L., temperament
Kraemer, G.W., & Suomi. S.J. (1991). on motor maturation and response
Therapy
of Research,
of vestibular-proprioceptive in rhesus monkey
infants.
S.J., & Champoux. M. (1991). Laboratory assessment of enrichment in rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulamr). American Journal of Primarology, 25, 137-155. Thomas. A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1968). Temperament and behavior disorders in children. New
Wohlwill,
Moore, and
Journal
The effects to challenge 11. 135-154.
C.F.. Suomi. environmental
York: University Press. J.F. (1973). The study of behaviorul
development. 21 November
New
York:
Academic.
1990; Revised
20 May
1991
W