Journal Pre-proof
NEURODEVELOPMENTAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN BORN TO OPIOID-DEPENDENT MOTHERS: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS Samantha J. Lee PhD , Samudragupta Bora PhD , Nicola Austin MBChB, DM , Anneliese Westerman BA , Jacqueline M.T. Henderson PhD PII: DOI: Reference:
S1876-2859(19)30457-7 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2019.11.005 ACAP 1436
To appear in:
Academic Pediatrics
Received date: Accepted date:
30 April 2019 12 November 2019
Please cite this article as: Samantha J. Lee PhD , Samudragupta Bora PhD , Nicola Austin MBChB, DM , Anneliese Westerman BA , Jacqueline M.T. Henderson PhD , NEURODEVELOPMENTAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN BORN TO OPIOID-DEPENDENT MOTHERS: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS, Academic Pediatrics (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2019.11.005
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of Academic Pediatric Association
NEURODEVELOPMENTAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN BORN TO OPIOIDDEPENDENT MOTHERS: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS Samantha J. Lee, PhDa, Samudragupta Bora, PhDb, Nicola Austin, MBChB, DMc, Anneliese Westerman, BAa, & Jacqueline M.T. Henderson, PhDa Affiliations: aSchool of Psychology, Speech and Hearing, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch 8140, New Zealand (
[email protected];
[email protected];
[email protected]); bMothers, Babies and Women’s Health Program, Mater Research Institute, Faculty of Medicine, The University of Queensland, South Brisbane, Queensland 4101, Australia (
[email protected]); cDepartment of Paediatrics, University of Otago, PO Box 4345 Christchurch 8140, New Zealand (
[email protected]). Address correspondence to: Jacki Henderson, School of Psychology, Speech and Hearing, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch 8140, New Zealand. [
[email protected]], +64 (3) 369 4358. ORCID iD: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4781-0361 Keywords: Prenatal Opioid Exposure; Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects; Analgesics; Opioid; Child Development; Meta-Analysis Running header: A meta-analysis of prenatally opioid-exposed children’s outcomes Abstract word count: 249 Main text word count: 3758 Funding sources: This study was supported by the Health Research Council of New Zealand (grant number 14/584 to [to JMTH]), the Canterbury Medical Research Foundation Post Graduate Publishing Bursary (grant number Lee_BUR2018-001 [to SJL]), and Lottery Health
Research (grant number 352179 [to SJL]). The funding sources had no involvement in the preparation or completion of this manuscript. Conflict of interest: The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose. Declarations of interest: None.
MeSH KEYWORDS
Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects; Heroin; Opiate Substitution Treatment; Methadone; Buprenorphine; Narcotics; Analgesics; Opioid; Opioid-Related Disorders; Neurodevelopmental Disorders; Cognition; Intelligence; Psychomotor Performance; Language Development; Child Behavior; Child; Infant; Meta-analysis
ABSTRACT Background: Children born to opioid-dependent mothers are at risk of adverse neurodevelopment. The magnitude of this risk remains inconclusive. Objectives: To conduct a meta-analysis of studies that assessed neurodevelopmental outcomes of children aged 0 to 12 years born to opioid-dependent mothers, compared with children born to non-opioid-dependent mothers, across general cognitive, language, motor, and social-emotional domains. Data Sources: PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Google Scholar databases. Study Eligibility Criteria: English-language publications between January 1993 and November 2018, including prenatally opioid-exposed and non-opioid-exposed comparison children, reporting outcomes data on standardized assessments. Study Appraisal and Synthesis Methods: Two reviewers independently extracted data. Pooled standardized mean differences (SMDs) were analyzed using random effects models. Risk of bias was assessed with the Newcastle-Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale. Results: Across 16 studies, individual domain outcomes data were examined for between 93 to 430 opioid-exposed and 75 to 505 non-exposed infants/children. Opioid-exposed infants and children performed more poorly than non-exposed infants and children across all outcomes examined, demonstrated by lower infant cognitive (SMD=0.77) and psychomotor scores (SMD=0.52), lower general cognition/IQ (SMD=0.76) and language scores (SMD=0.65–0.74), and higher parent-rated internalizing (SMD=0.42), externalizing (SMD=0.66), and attention problems (SMD=0.72). Limitations: Most studies examined early neurodevelopment; only three reported school-age outcomes thereby limiting the ability to assess longer-term impacts of prenatal opioid exposures.
Conclusions and Implications of Findings: Children born to opioid-dependent mothers are at modest- to high-risk of adverse neurodevelopment at least to middle childhood. Future studies should identify specific clinical and social factors underlying these challenges to improve outcomes.
NEURODEVELOPMENTAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN BORN TO OPIOIDDEPENDENT MOTHERS: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS
The prevalence of opioid use among pregnant women has dramatically increased over the past two decades,1,2 evidenced by an increase of 1.19 to 5.63 per 1000 hospital births per year over the period of 2000 to 2009 in the United States.2 In light of the current opioid epidemic, these rates are likely to continue to increase. This is a global health concern with the maternal morbidities associated with substance dependency, alongside the adverse effects of prenatal opioid exposure on infant’s clinical and neurodevelopmental outcomes.3-7 Beyond early childhood, however, there remains a gap in the literature examining the longer-term neurodevelopmental outcomes of children prenatally exposed to opioids. In contrast, the adverse neonatal outcomes associated with prenatal opioid exposure are well documented, and include premature delivery, small for gestational age,3,4 reduced brain growth and abnormal neural development,5-7 and Neonatal Abstinence Syndrome (NAS), or Neonatal Opioid Withdrawal Syndrome.8 Infants with NAS experience central nervous system irritability, autonomic over-reactivity, gastrointestinal dysfunction, and respiratory distress, and often require extended pharmacological treatment and hospitalization.8,9 NAS affects more than 5 per 1,000 hospital births, with a five-fold reported increase in the United States since 2000: these rates are also likely to rise.2,10 Children born to opioid-dependent mothers are also at increased risk of exposure to postnatal psychosocial adversities (poverty, caregiver psychopathology, compromised parenting),4,11,12 thereby increasing the risk of long-term suboptimal neurodevelopmental outcomes. Two published meta-analyses reported poorer neurodevelopmental outcomes for opioid-exposed children relative to their non-opioid-exposed peers, including lower cognitive and psychomotor scores during infancy,13,14 and at preschool age.13 Poorer social-emotional
outcomes were also reported in a narrative synthesis,14 and a meta-analysis,13 however, data from the latter study were pooled from measures that were not directly comparable (standardized measures of social development vs. non-standardized, observational ratings of infant attention). Recent data describing opioid-exposed children’s emotional and behavior problems have not yet been quantitatively synthesized. Furthermore, significant shortcomings of the meta-analyses are reflected by the few empirical studies in this field, and the paucity in long-term follow-up studies, with between only five and eight infant and preschool outcome studies reviewed, respectively.13,14 The historical nature of the included empirical studies is a further limitation in these meta-analyses, thus resulting in a wide range in the quality of the published evidence. Taking together the recent increase in empirical studies15-20 alongside recent data describing the younger cohorts at school age, there is a need for a contemporary meta-analysis to confirm findings reported in the earlier meta-analyses. This is necessary to elucidate the longer-term influence of prenatal opioid exposure across different developmental domains. Moreover, it is of fundamental importance to include an investigation of social-emotional outcomes given their critical influence on other domains21 and trajectories of developmental pathways in highrisk populations. Thus, a systematic review and meta-analysis of contemporary empirical studies were performed to investigate the neurodevelopmental outcomes of children prenatally exposed to opioids relative to non-exposed children across the first 12 years in the domains of general cognition, language, motor, and social-emotional outcomes.
METHODS A systematic search of PubMed, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), PsycINFO, and Google Scholar electronic databases, limited to a 25-
year period (January 1, 1993 to November 30, 2018) was conducted. Search terms were developed in consultation with an academic librarian, including broad keywords and MeSH terms. Various combinations of the following terms were used: opiate, opioid, heroin, methadone, buprenorphine, prenatal exposure, follow up, neurodevelopment, development, cognition, intelligence, psychomotor, motor, language, education, school performance, achievement, social, behavior, delay, impairment, deficit, outcome. An example of the electronic search strategy is provided in online Supplementary Table 1. Studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1) sample included children aged 0 to 12 years, (2) with prenatal exposure to an opioid substance including either illicit and/or licit/prescribed opioids (measured through maternal self-report, medical records/prescriptions, and/or analysis of biological samples), (3) included a non-opioid exposed comparison group, (4) was a cohort, case-control, or cross-sectional study, and (5) reported neurodevelopmental outcome data (general cognition/IQ, executive function, language, educational achievement, motor skills, social-emotional/mental health). If no studies were identified that were in addition to those already included in previous metaanalyses, then a new meta-analysis was not conducted for that particular outcome domain. Studies were excluded if they met any of the following criteria: (1) non-human population, (2) no comparison group, (3) prenatal opioid exposure not explicitly specified (e.g., drug-exposed, NAS diagnosis), (4) non-English language publication, (5) non-peerreviewed publication, (6) non-original publication (e.g., case report, review, systematic review, meta-analysis etc.), and (7) original publication reporting outcomes exclusively from an intervention trial. Randomized controlled trials were excluded in order to examine outcomes for children prior to any possible intervention effects. Electronic database search results were imported to EndNote X9, where duplicates were discarded. Two reviewers (SJL, AW) independently conducted title and abstract
screenings for relevant studies. Reference lists from published reviews were also handsearched. Full-text screening was conducted, with discrepancies resolved by a third reviewer (JMTH). Care was taken to include data from each cohort only once for each domain outcome, thus ensuring sample independence. For duplicate cohorts, the article that reported data from the latest follow-up (oldest age) or for the larger sample was selected. For this meta-analysis, infant general cognitive development (i.e., Bayley Mental Development Index scores) and child IQ were treated as separate outcomes, and therefore the same cohort could be represented in both of these domains. Two reviewers (SJL, AW) independently extracted and verified the following information from each article: authors and year of publication, study location, sample size at recruitment, sample size at final data collection phase, age(s) at which follow-up occurred, type of prenatal opioid exposure, measure(s) used to determine prenatal opioid and other drug exposures, prescribed dose of maternal opioid substitute during pregnancy (if applicable), percent of exposed children treated for NAS, composition of the comparison group, percent of male children, measures used to assess the neurodevelopmental outcomes included in the current meta-analysis, whether or not a blinded assessor conducted the assessment, and the covariates examined in relation to the neurodevelopmental outcomes. Means and standard deviations for each outcome measure were extracted. In several cases, study authors were contacted by email to request further information. If no response from the corresponding author was received within seven days, a reminder was sent. Various tests to measure general cognition, psychomotor skills, language, and socialemotional outcomes were used across studies. A few excluded studies (n=4) reported other outcomes that we intended to include (e.g., executive functions, educational achievement), however the data from those reports were insufficient or unstandardized, and thus could not be used for a meta-analysis. It was also intended to analyze outcomes separately for infants,
preschool, and school-age children, however this was not possible for all outcomes due to a paucity of studies, particularly of school-age children. We were able to analyze cognitive development data separately for infants and older children, with preschool and school-age children’s IQ data analyzed together. For psychomotor development, no new studies in the preschool developmental period were identified that were in addition to those reported in an existing meta-analysis.13 Therefore, only infant psychomotor data were analyzed in the current meta-analysis. For language outcomes there were only two studies, both reporting preschool children’s data. Finally, due to a small number of studies, infant, preschool, and school-age children’s social-emotional outcome data were analyzed together. The potential risk of bias for the included studies was evaluated according to the Newcastle-Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale (NOS).22 Two reviewers (SJL, SB) independently scored each of the studies. Studies were scored from 0 to 9, with 0 indicating low quality and high risk of bias, and 9 indicating high quality and low risk of bias. Statistical analyses The statistical data analyses were performed in Review Manager, version 5.3. Random effects meta-analyses were used to calculate summary estimates, with the standardized mean difference (SMD) and 95% confidence interval (CI) as the measure of effect size. Where studies had conducted independent subgroup analyses, a combined effect across subgroups was calculated. Heterogeneity among studies was assessed visually by examining the forest plots, and quantitatively using the Q and I2 statistics. Where evidence of moderate to high heterogeneity was observed, the jackknife sensitivity analysis was conducted to evaluate whether a single study significantly affected the results, by excluding each study one at a time and then repeating the analysis.23 Results from the sensitivity analyses are presented in online Supplementary Tables 2 to 5, with the most important modifications reported in-text.
RESULTS Study Selection Figure 1 shows the study selection flow diagram. Our search strategy provided 4,702 unique articles. Following a title and abstract screen, 76 articles underwent full-text screen, and 16 were identified as eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis. The final number of studies for each domain are as follows: infant general cognition (n=9), infant psychomotor development (n=7), preschool and school-age general cognition (n=5), preschool language development (n=2), preschool and school-age internalizing behavior (n=4), externalizing behavior (n=4), and attention problems (n=4). Study Characteristics The characteristics of the included studies are presented in Table 1. In total, 16 studies representing 11 unique cohorts were included. Cohorts were born in Germany (n=1), United States of America (n=1), Australia (n=1), Norway (n=2), New Zealand (n=1), Israel (n=1), Finland (n=2), United Kingdom (Scotland, n=1), and The Netherlands (n=1). At recruitment, the opioid-exposed group sample sizes ranged from 15 to 133, and comparison group sample sizes from 13 to 256. Overall retention rates, representing the number of children with outcome data to the final follow-up, ranged from poor to excellent (47% to 93%). The majority of the studies followed their sample to preschool age; only three studies (two cohorts) examined children older than age 5 years. Of the 11 opioid-exposed cohorts, two were prenatally exposed to illicit heroin, four to prescribed methadone, two to heroin and/or methadone, one to illicit buprenorphine, one to prescribed buprenorphine, and one to either prescribed methadone or buprenorphine studied as one combined group. A multi-method approach (self-report or confirmed opioid substitution therapy enrollment in addition to toxicology results) to determine prenatal opioid
exposure was taken in the majority of studies, with only two relying on maternal selfreport.18,24 Three studies reported the opioid-dependent mothers’ mean prescribed methadone dose during pregnancy, which ranged from 20mg/day to >86mg/day. Only one study reported the mean dose of prescribed buprenorphine, which was 13mg/day. Eleven studies reported the percentage of opioid-exposed infants that required treatment for neonatal opioid withdrawal symptoms, which ranged from between 48% and 91% infants. The composition of the comparison groups differed across studies, with some recruiting comparison infants based on selected a priori infant perinatal risk factors25,26 and maternal social/obstetric factors.27-29 Two comparison cohorts were selected as environmental controls, that is, the children were living in a lower socio-economic or drug abuse lifestyle environment, or they were living in a higher socio-economic environment that matched the opioid-exposed children’s foster family environment.16-18,24,29 Other studies randomly recruited comparison group children from local health care centers20,30-34 and one study randomly recruited a regionally representative comparison group.15 Five studies reported that neurodevelopmental assessments were administered and/or scored by examiners blinded to the child’s study group/exposure history. Most studies examined additional risk factors in relation to the children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes. These were predominantly additional drug exposures (i.e., alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis), perinatal factors such as gestational age and birth weight, and maternal social background factors including socioeconomic status and/or maternal education. Few studies examined potential environmental intervening factors. Risk of Bias Overall, the quality of the included studies indicated a medium to low risk of bias (Table 1). Nine studies fulfilled criteria for good quality (Newcastle-Ottawa Quality Assessment Scale [NOS] score ≥7), six were fair (score 5–6), and one was poor (score ≤4).
Study shortcomings included a lack of a representative opioid-exposed cohort (n=5), a lack of examiner blinding (n=11), and low retention rates (n=8). Infant Cognition and Psychomotor Development Nine studies involving 430 exposed and 505 non-exposed infants examined cognitive performance (Figure 2). Opioid-exposed infants had lower cognition scores than non-exposed infants (SMD = -0.77 [95% CI, -1.06 to -0.48]). However, there was significant heterogeneity across studies. The summary effect size remained in the moderate to high range following sensitivity analyses. The exclusion of Ornoy et al. (1996) reduced the heterogeneity from high (I2 = 74%) to moderate (I2 = 53%; Supplementary Table 2). Seven studies involving 354 exposed and 400 non-exposed infants examined infant psychomotor performance (Figure 2). Opioid-exposed infants had lower psychomotor scores than non-exposed infants (SMD = 0.52 [95% CI, -0.78 to -0.25]). In sensitivity analyses, the summary effect size remained moderate with the exclusion of Ornoy et al. (1996), but the heterogeneity was reduced (I2 = 66% to 46%; Supplementary Table 3). Preschool and School-Age Cognition and Language Development Five studies involving 217 exposed and 200 non-exposed children examined general cognition/IQ (Figure 3). Opioid-exposed children had lower IQ scores compared with nonexposed children (SMD = -0.76 [95% CI, -1.25 to -0.28]). There was significant heterogeneity across study effect sizes, but the summary effect remained in the moderate/large range with the exclusion of each study (Supplementary Table 4). The exclusion of Ornoy et al. (2016) reduced the heterogeneity from high (I2 = 82%) to moderate (I2 = 65%). Two studies assessed opioid-exposed preschool children’s language development, including expressive (93 exposed, 75 non-exposed) and receptive (93 exposed, 76 nonexposed) language skills (Figure 3). Opioid-exposed children had lower expressive (SMD = -
0.65 [95% CI, -0.97 to -0.34]) and receptive language scores (SMD = -0.74 [95% CI, -1.12 to -0.36]) than non-exposed children. Preschool and School-Age Social-Emotional Development Data for internalizing and externalizing problems were examined for 215 exposed and 231 non-exposed preschool and school-age children (Figure 4). Opioid-exposed children had higher internalizing scores than non-exposed children (SMD = 0.42 [95% CI, 0.17 to 0.68]). Opioid-exposed children had higher externalizing scores than non-exposed children (SMD = 0.66 [95% CI, 0.32 to 1.00]). There was significant heterogeneity in the effect sizes for externalizing behavior. The exclusion of Levine and Woodward (2018) attenuated the summary effect estimate (0.51) and reduced the heterogeneity (I2 = 66% to 22%). With the exclusion of Ornoy et al. (2016), the effect size estimate increased (0.81) and the heterogeneity reduced (I2 = 31%; Supplementary Table 5). Data for parent-rated attention problems were examined from 208 exposed and 219 non-exposed preschool and school-age children (Figure 4). Opioid-exposed children had higher attention problem scores than nonexposed children (SMD = 0.72 [95% CI, 0.42 to 1.02]).
DISCUSSION In this systematic review and meta-analysis, the neurodevelopmental outcomes of children who were born to opioid-dependent mothers were compared with children born to non-opioid-dependent mothers during pregnancy. Results from 16 studies were consistent in showing that infants and children who were prenatally exposed to opioids performed more poorly than their non-exposed counterparts across all outcomes examined, as demonstrated by lower cognitive, psychomotor and language scores, and higher parent-rated internalizing, externalizing, and attention problem scores. These findings support and extend those of previous researchers who reported similar results. Baldacchino and colleagues conducted a
meta-analysis of five studies of opioid-exposed infant’s and children’s cognitive and psychomotor development, and observer-rated infant attention and social development. Findings of the current meta-analysis were in accordance with their results, which showed that opioid-exposed children performed worse than their non-exposed peers across these domains.13 The current meta-analysis provided additional information by including more recently published studies, and by analyzing preschool language data and parent-rated internalizing, externalizing and attention problem data across childhood. These findings strengthen the conclusion that children who are born to opioid-dependent mothers are at risk for longer-term neurodevelopmental problems across a wide range of domains. Further, our data are consistent with the results of a recent meta-analysis that reported lower cognitive and psychomotor performance amongst infants born to opioid-dependent mothers treated with methadone during pregnancy, compared with non-opioid-exposed controls.14 Taken together, findings suggest that being born to an opioid-dependent mother is a strong indicator for later neurodevelopmental problems. Several biological and socio-environmental risk factors may increase opioid-exposed children’s risk for longer-term adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes. Although examining the specific factors associated with opioid-exposed children’s elevated neurodevelopmental risk was outside the scope of the current study, there are several potential mechanisms at play. Briefly, prenatal exposure to opioid substances can have direct effects on neural development.5-7 Neurological alterations in opioid-exposed infants have been observed shortly after birth, before postnatal confounding environmental effects can influence neurodevelopment.5,6 However, potential neonatal confounders, including maternal concomitant use of cigarettes, alcohol, and other substances during pregnancy, as well as the increased risk of prematurity and other perinatal complications associated with opioidexposure, could also influence their neurodevelopmental outcomes.3-5
The postnatal environment that opioid-exposed children are raised in is proposed as either exacerbating their early biological vulnerability, or buffering them from any negative drug effects. With appropriate monitoring and an enriched early caregiving environment, these children have been reported to demonstrate optimized outcomes.35 Unfortunately, opioid-exposed children are more likely to be born into a multiple-risk social context, with their mothers typically coming from low socioeconomic family backgrounds themselves, and more often having educational underachievement, a lack of partner support, and comorbid psychiatric disturbance and poly-drug use problems than mothers without opioid dependence.11,30 Understanding whether prenatal opioid exposure has any independent effects on children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes or not is a difficult task for researchers. Some of the heterogeneity in effect sizes in the current meta-analysis might be explained by the differing levels of both pre and postnatal risk exposures that characterize opioid-exposed children. First, it is unclear from the current results whether the different types of opioids children were exposed to had any differential impact on their outcomes. Opioid-exposed children are not a homogenous group. First, children’s prenatal exposure to opioid substances, and their subsequent neurodevelopmental outcomes, may be impacted by genetic variations in opioid metabolism.36 Further, the effects of illicit compared to prescribed opioids, or the effects of different opioid substitutes, cannot be assumed to be equivalent. Children’s outcomes may be associated with differences in the pharmacological actions of these drugs, as well as the differences relating to maternal opioid-use history, polydrug use, treatment retention, and behavioral stability that are found across the different populations using them.37,38 The timing, frequency, and extent of maternal opioid use across pregnancy may also be of importance. Although a recent population-based study found that infants born to opioid-dependent women stabilized with methadone prior to conception had similar short-term outcomes compared to women who began treatment during their
pregnancies,38 children’s long-term outcomes have not been examined in relation to the timing of their exposure. Of importance, infants of the latter group of women in that study had up to a fourfold increase of removal by social services at the hospital.38 This demonstrates the increase in socio-familial issues associated with untreated opioiddependence during pregnancy which, importantly, are well known as having a critical influence on children’s neurodevelopmental outcomes.8 We performed subgroup analyses for infant cognitive outcomes, and results were no different when cohorts of illicit opioid-exposed children or prescribed opioid-exposed children were compared to controls. This same procedure was not possible with the other outcomes examined however, nor was an examination of methadone-exposed compared to buprenorphine-exposed children’s outcomes, given the small number of studies. A more indepth analysis to examine this pertinent issue will only be possible with a greater number of child outcome studies in future. Identifying opioid treatments with the greatest reduction in harm is central to the optimal management of opioid-dependence in pregnancy for the mother and fetus, recognizing that poly-drug use and confounding by indication will influence this to an extent.37 Also worthy of note is that the wide variation in care (e.g., pharmacologic, family-centered support) received by infants who are prenatally exposed to opioids during the neonatal period potentially influences later child risks.39,40 The composition of the comparison groups across studies was also variable. It would be expected to see greater between-group difference in studies that compared opioid-exposed children to low-risk and regionally representative groups than when opioid-exposed children were compared to groups that were similar on other, potentially confounding, factors. Nevertheless, with the exception of one cohort,18,24 group differences were found in studies where demographically similar opioid-exposed and non-exposed groups were compared. The expected direction of effect in all analyses was not followed in one cohort where the non-
exposed comparison group included high-risk children living in familial drug-use environments that may have increased their risk for poor performance.18,24 A further limitation of the current meta-analysis, and something that should be explored in future research, is that the contribution of environmental intervening processes to opioid-exposed children’s outcomes across neurodevelopmental domains could not be examined. The included studies varied in their consideration of and adjustment for perinatal and social background risks, and very few studies to date have considered intervening factors including caregiving factors associated with maternal substance use disorders, such as psychiatric comorbidities, and how these affect child outcomes.8 Another noteworthy limitation includes that very few studies have conducted followup assessments of opioid-exposed children beyond the preschool developmental period.16-18 There was a wide range of follow-up rates (47 – 93%), and it is of concern that attrition in these high-risk samples represents impairment or lifestyle factors that may predispose children to poorer outcomes. It is possible that the findings of this meta-analysis underestimate the significance of the opioid-exposed children’s neurodevelopmental impairments. It is also of concern that opioid-exposed children’s early observed impairment will worsen with age, and that new impairments may emerge. Importantly, theories of cascading effects of impairment across domains suggest that problems in one domain, (e.g., social-emotional), will undermine functioning in one or more other domains (e.g., cognitive), particularly following a developmental transition such as starting school.21 Another key issue is that as opioid-exposed children transition from pre-school to school age, difficulties within new domains of functioning (e.g., executive, educational) may begin to emerge. Potentially, the effects of prenatal opioids, or other associated factors, on children’s longer-term outcomes may not become apparent until the child reaches an age at which skills within a certain domain of interest are developing.16
In conclusion, children born to opioid-dependent mothers have an increased risk for neurodevelopmental difficulties. This methodologically robust meta-analysis of 16 studies provided evidence for poorer development across the domains of general cognition, psychomotor, language, and social-emotional development among this high-risk group. Impairments across these domains are likely to have a large negative influence on opioidexposed children’s schooling, peer relationships, and mental health. Follow-up studies examining these salient middle childhood and adolescent outcomes will be necessary to understand the longer-term consequences of prenatal opioid exposure and opioid-exposed children’s early difficulties. Finally, given their complexity of several risk factors, long-term surveillance of a large number of prenatally opioid-exposed children is needed.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Thank you to Kerry Gilmour and Margaret Paterson, librarians at the University of Canterbury, for their help and guidance with the original literature search. We would also like to thank those researchers who contributed requested data for this project. This study was supported by the Health Research Council of New Zealand (grant number 14/584 to [to JMTH]), the Canterbury Medical Research Foundation Post Graduate Publishing Bursary (grant number Lee_BUR2018-001 [to SJL]), and Lottery Health Research (grant number 352179 [to SJL]).
REFERENCES 1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
Brogly SB, Turner S, Lajkosz K, et al. Infants born to opioid-dependent women in Ontario, 2002-2014. J Obstet Gynaecol Can. 2017;39:157-65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jogc.2016.11.009 Patrick SW, Schumacher RE, Benneyworth BD, et al. Neonatal abstinence syndrome and associated health care expenditures: United States, 2000-2009. JAMA. 2012;307:1934-40. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.3951 Norgaard M, Nielsson MS, Heide-Jorgensen U. Birth and neonatal outcomes following opioid use in pregnancy: a Danish population-based study. Subst Abuse. 2015;9:5-11. https://doi.org/10.4137/SART.S23547 Wouldes TA, Woodward LJ. Maternal methadone dose during pregnancy and infant clinical outcome. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2010;32:406-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2010.01.007 Monnelly VJ, Anblagan D, Quigley A, et al. Prenatal methadone exposure is associated with altered neonatal brain development. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;18:9-14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.033 Walhovd KB, Watts R, Amlien I, Woodward LJ. Neural tract development of infants born to methadone-maintained mothers. Pediatr Neurol. 2012;47:1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.04.008 Yuan Q, Rubic M, Seah J, et al. Do maternal opioids reduce neonatal regional brain volumes? A pilot study. J Perinatol. 2014;34:909-13. https://doi.org/10.1038/jp.2014.111 Reddy UM, Davis JM, Ren Z, Greene MF. Opioid Use In Pregnancy, Neonatal Abstinence Syndrome, and Childhood Outcomes: Executive Summary of a Joint Workshop by The Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, American Congress of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, American Academy of Pediatrics, Society for Maternal-Fetal Medicine, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, and The March of Dimes Foundation. Obstet Gynecol. 2017;130:10. https://doi.org/10.1097/AOG0000000000002054 Bagley SM, Wachman EM, Holland E, Brogly SB. Review of the assessment and management of neonatal abstinence syndrome. Addict Sci Clin Pract. 2014;9:19. https://doi.org/10.1186/1940-0640-9-19 Patrick SW, Davis MM, Lehman CU, Cooper WO. Increasing incidence and geographic distribution of neonatal abstinence syndrome: United States 2009 to 2012. J Perinatol. 2015;35:667. https://doi.org/10.1038/jp.2015.63 Davie-Gray A, Moor S, Spencer C, Woodward LJ. Psychosocial characteristics and poly-drug use of pregnant women enrolled in methadone maintenance treatment. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;38:46-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ntt.2013.04.006 Hatzis D, Dawe S, Harnett P, Barlow J. Quality of caregiving in mothers with illicit substance use: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Subst Abuse. 2017;11:1178221817694038. https://doi.org/10.1177/1178221817694038 Baldacchino A, Arbuckle K, Petrie DJ, McCowan C. 'Neurobehavioral consequences of chronic intrauterine opioid exposure in infants and preschool children: a systematic review and meta-analysis': Erratum. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0438-5 Monnelly VJ, Hamilton R, Chappell FM, et al. Childhood neurodevelopment after prescription of maintenance methadone for opioid dependency in pregnancy: a
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23. 24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2019;61:750-60. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14117 Levine TA, Woodward LJ. Early inhibitory control and working memory abilities of children prenatally exposed to methadone. Early Hum Dev. 2018;116:68-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.11.010 Nygaard E, Moe V, Slinning K, Walhovd KB. Longitudinal cognitive development of children born to mothers with opioid and polysubstance use. Pediatr Res. 2015;78:330-5. https://doi.org/10.1038/pr.2015.95 Nygaard E, Slinning K, Moe V, Walhovd KB. Behavior and attention problems in eight-year-old children with prenatal opiate and poly-substance exposure: a longitudinal study. PLOS ONE. 2016;11:e0158054. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158054 Ornoy A, Finkel-Pekarsky V, Peles E, et al. ADHD risk alleles associated with opiate addiction: study of addicted parents and their children. Pediatr Res. 2016;80:228-36. https://doi.org/10.1038/pr.2016.78 Konijnenberg C, Melinder A. Executive function in preschool children prenatally exposed to methadone or buprenorphine. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;21:570-85. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.967201 Konijnenberg C, Sarfi M, Melinder A. Mother-child interaction and cognitive development in children prenatally exposed to methadone or buprenorphine. Early Hum Dev. 2016;101:91-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.08.013 Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, et al. Developmental cascades: linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Dev Psychol. 2005;41:733-46. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733 Wells GA, Shea B, O'Connell D, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp Accessed 10 November 2018. Miller RG. The jacknife: a review. Biometrika. 1974;1:1-15. https://doi.org/10.2307/2334280 Ornoy A, Michailevskaya V, Lukashov I, et al. The developmental outcome of children born to heroin-dependent mothers, raised at home or adopted. Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20:385-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/0145-2134(96)00014-2 Bunikowski R, Grimmer I, Heiser A, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome after prenatal exposure to opiates. Eur J Pediatr. 1998;157:724-30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004310050923 McGlone L, Mactier H. Infants of opioid-dependent mothers: neurodevelopment at six months. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:19-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.10.006. Hans SL, Jeremy RJ. Postneonatal mental and motor development of infants exposed in utero to opioid drugs. Infant Ment Health J. 2001;22:300-15. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.1003 Hunt RW, Tzioumi D, Collins E, Jeffery HE. Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84:29-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.01.013 Moe V, Slinning K. Children prenatally exposed to substances: gender related differences in outcome from infancy to 3 years of age. Infant Ment Health J. 2001;22:334-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/imhj.1005 Salo S, Kivisto K, Korja R, et al. Emotional availability, parental self-efficacy beliefs, and child development in caregiver-child relationships with buprenorphine-exposed 3-
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
year-olds. Parent Sci Pract. 2009;9:244-59. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295190902844563 Salo S, Politi J, Tupola S, et al. Early development of opioid-exposed infants born to mothers in buprenorphine-replacement therapy. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2010;28:161-79. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830903219109 van Baar A, de Graaff BM. Cognitive development at preschool-age of infants of drug-dependent mothers. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1994;36:1063-75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11809.x Sarfi M, Sundet JM, Waal H. Maternal stress and behavioral adaptation in methadone- or buprenorphine-exposed toddlers. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36:707-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.08.006 Melinder A, Konijnenberg C, Sarfi M. Deviant smooth pursuit in preschool children exposed prenatally to methadone or buprenorphine and tobacco affects integrative visuomotor capabilities. Addiction. 2013;108:2175-82. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12267 Kaltenbach K, O'Grady KE, Heil SH, et al. Prenatal exposure to methadone or buprenorphine: early childhood developmental outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;185:40-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.11.030 Wachman EM, Hayes MJ, Sherva R, et al. Association of maternal and infant variants in PNOC and COMT genes with neonatal abstinence syndrome severity. Am J Addict. 2017;26:42-49. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.12483 Brogly SB, Saia KA, Walley AY, et al. Prenatal buprenorphine versus methadone exposure and neonatal outcomes: systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2014;180:673-86. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwu190 Guan Q, Sproule BA, Vigod SN, et al. Impact of timing of methadone initiation on perinatal outcomes following delivery among pregnant women on methadone maintenance therapy in ontario. Addiction. 2019;114:268-77. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14453. Bogen DL, Whalen BL, Kair LR, et al. Wide variation found in care of opioidexposed newborns. Acad Pediatr. 2017;17:374-80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2016.10.003. Gadomski A, Riley M, Ramiza K, et al. Treating neonatal abstinence syndrome in a rural hospital: lessons learned. Acad Pediatr. 2018;18:425-29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2018.02.006
Figure 1. Study selection flowchart
Cognition
25 27 28 15 26 16 24 30 31
Psychomotor
25 27 28 15 26 29 24
Figure 2. Forest plot of infant cognition and psychomotor performance scores
General cognition
28 20 16 18 32
Expressive language 28
32
Receptive language 28 32
Figure 3. Forest plot of general cognition scores at preschool and school age, and language scores at preschool age
Internalizing behavior
15
17 18 33
Externalizing behavior
15
17 18 33
Attention problems
15 34 17
18
Figure 4. Forest plot of social-emotional outcome scores at preschool and school age
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y
#1
Germany
Bunikowski et al. (1998)25
#2
United States of America
Hans and Jeremy (2001)27
N at recruitment
N with data at final followup (% retention)
93;
69 (74%);
POE: 46
POE: 27 (59%)
NE: 47
NE: 42 (89%)
89;
78 (88%);
POE: 42
POE: 33 (79%)
NE: 47
NE: 45 (96%)
Age at followup (y)
POE type
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range)
% NAS/ NOWS tx
Hunt et al. (2008)28
Australia
236;
111 (47%);
POE: 133
POE: 67 (50%)
NE: 103
NE: 44 (43%)
% male
Outcome data analyzed
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
1
Prescribed methadone (n=22) or illicit heroin (n=12)
Confirmed OST, heroin NR
NR
91
Selected from same hospital if nicotineexposed
POE: 56 NE: 47
Griffiths Mental Development Scale, Performance & Locomotor
NR
SB
6
4 mo., 8 mo., 1, 1.5, 2
Prescribed methadone
Confirmed OST, maternal urine, selfreport
20 (3 – 40)
NR
Selected from same hospital. Comparable maternal age, SES, race, education, parity
NR
Bayley Scales, MDI & PDI
Yes
PDE, B/IF, SB, CF
8
56
Matched for maternal age, height, ethnicity, and obstetric history
POE: 58
Bayley Scales, MDI & PDI (age 1.5)
NR
None
3
(dose across pregnancy)
#3
Comparison group composition
1.5, 3
Prescribed methadone
Confirmed OST, maternal urine
NR
NE: 38
SBIS (age 3) Reynell Language Scale
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y
#4
Norway
Konijnenberg et al. (2015/2016)
N at recruitment
N with data at final followup (% retention)
72;
67 (93%)
POE: 36
POE: 35 (97%)
NE: 36
NE: 32 (89%)
Age at followup (y)
4
POE type
Prescribed methadone (n=24) or buprenorphine (n=11)
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Confirmed OST, maternal urine, meconium, self-report
19,20
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range) 85.96 (0 – 260)
% NAS/ NOWS tx 63c
and 12.76 (3 – 24)
Comparison group composition
% male
Recruited through local health care centers based on their due date
POE: 54
A regionally representative group that was selected at random from the same hospital
POE: 57
Selected from the same hospital and matched for gestational age, birth weight, and maternal post code at delivery
NR
NE: 39
Outcome data analyzed
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
WPPSI-R, full scale IQ (provided by authors)
NR
PDE, B/IF, SB, CF
7
Bayley Scales, MDI & PDI (age 2)
Yes
PDE, B/IF, SB
8
No
PDE
8
(dose at delivery)
#5
New Zealand
Levine and Woodward (2018)15
210;
190 (90%);
POE: 100
POE: 87 (87%)
NE: 110
NE: 103 (94%)
2, 4.5
Prescribed methadone
Confirmed OST, maternal urine, meconium, self-report
62.4 (6.7 – 195)
88
(dose across pregnancy)
#6 McGlone and Mactier (2015)26
United Kingdom
152;
107 (70%);
POE: 102
POE: 81 (79%)
NE: 50
NE: 26 (52%)
6 mo.
Prescribed methadone
Self-report, maternal urine, infant urine, meconium
NR
48
NE: 46
SDQ (age 4.5)
Griffiths Mental Development Scales, Performance & Locomotor
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y
#4
Norway
Melinder et al. (2013)34
N at recruitment
N with data at final followup (% retention)
72;
49 (68%);
POE: 36
POE: 26 (72%)
NE: 36
NE: 23 (64%)
Age at followup (y)
4
POE type
Prescribed methadone (n=18) or buprenorphine (n=8)
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Confirmed OST, maternal urine, meconium, self-report
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range) 85.00 (range, NR)
% NAS/ NOWS tx 61 and 87
and 13.13 (range, NR)
Comparison group composition
% male
Recruited through local health care centers based on their due date
POE: 44
Matched for family/foster family social background
POE: 60
Matched for family/foster family social background
POE: 56
NE: 50
Outcome data analyzed
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
Child Behavior Checklist, Attention subtest
NR
B/IF, SB
5
Bayley Scales II, PDI
No
B/IF, SB
7
Bayley Scales II, MDI (age 3)
No
B/IF, SB, CF
8
n/a
B/IF, SB
8
(dose at delivery)
#7
Norway Moe and Slinning (2001)29
#7
Norway
Nygaard et al. (2015)16
#7 Nygaard et al. (2016)17
Norway
136;
81 (60%);
POE: 78
POE: 36 (46%)
NE: 58
NE: 45 (78%)
130;
103 (79%);
POE: 72
POE: 55 (76%)
NE: 58
NE: 48 (83%)
130;
104 (80%);
POE: 72
POE: 57 (79%)
NE: 58
NE: 47 (81%)
1, 2, 3
1, 2, 3, 4.5, 8.5
Illicit heroin
Illicit heroin
Maternal urine, infant urine/blood, self-report
n/a
Maternal urine, infant urine/blood, self-report
n/a
77
79
NE: 55
NE: 60
WISC-R (age 8.5) 4.5, 8.5
Illicit heroin
Maternal urine, infant urine/blood, self-report
n/a
79
Matched for family/foster family social background
POE: 58 NE: 60
Child Behavior Checklist
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y
#8
Israel
Ornoy et al. (1996)24
N at recruitment
339;
N with data at final followup (% retention) n/a
Age at followup (y)
6 mo. to 6
POE type
Illicit heroin
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Self-report
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range) n/a
% NAS/ NOWS tx 74
Comparison group composition
Four NE groups:
POE: 83
POE: 52
Outcome data analyzed
Bayley Scales, MDI & PDI
NE: 56 – 66
(up to age 2)
Children without prenatal opioid exposure that were raised by opioiddependent fathers
POE: 40
WISC-R
NE: 57
Child Behavior Checklist (short form)
Randomly recruited from well-baby clinics
NR
Bayley Scales III, Cognition
1. Addicted fathers (n=76)
NE: 256
% male
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
Yes
None
6
NR
B/IF
6
Yes
B/IF, SB, CF
6
2. Low SES/neglect (n=50) 3. Moderate/ high SES, but suspected delay (n=50) 4. Average SES, normal control (n=80)
#8
Israel
Ornoy et al. (2016)18
84:
n/a
5 to 16.5
Illicit heroin
Self-report
n/a
53
POE: 38 NE: 46
#9 Salo et al. (2009)30
Finland
41;
POE: 21 (75%)
POE: 28
NE: n/a – recruited at follow-up
NE: 13
3
Illicit buprenorphine
Infant urine
NR
NR
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y
#10
Finland
Salo et al. (2010)31
N at recruitment
87;
N with data at final followup (% retention) n/a
Age at followup (y)
5 to 12 mo.
POE type
Prescribed buprenorphine
POE: 15
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Confirmed OST, maternal urine, selfreport
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range) NR
% NAS/ NOWS tx NR
Comparison group composition
Two NE groups:
% male
Outcome data analyzed
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
NR
Bayley Scales III, MDI
Yes
B/IF, SB, CF
5
1. Depressed mothers and their children (n=15)
NE: 72
2. Randomly recruited from well-baby clinics (n=57)
#4
Norway
Sarfi et al. (2013)33
#11 Van Baar and de Graaf (1994)32
Netherlands
74;
68 (92%);
POE: 38
POE: 33 (87%)
NE: 36
NE: 35 (97%)
70;
52 (74%);
POE: 35
POE: 22 (63%)
NE: 35
NE: 30 (86%)
2.5
3.5, 4, 4.5, 5.5
Prescribed methadone (n=24) or buprenorphine (n=12)
Confirmed OST,
Prescribed methadone and illicit heroind
Maternal urine, infant urine, self-report
NR
NR
Recruited through local health care centers based on their due date
NR
Child Behavior Checklist Internalizing & Externalizing
n/a
B/IF, CF
7
Mean, NR
83
Recruited based on lack of perinatal complications associated with prenatal drug exposure (low risk group)
POE: 48
Revision of the Amsterdam Children’s Intelligence Test (age 4.5, 5.5)
NR
PDE, B/IF, CF, ChF
7
maternal urine, meconium, self-report
(0 – 80)
(dose across pregnancy)
NE: 43
Reynell Language Scale
Table 1. Characteristics of the Studies Cohort #a Place Author, y a
N at recruitment
N with data at final followup (% retention)
Age at followup (y)
POE type
Ascertainment of Drug Exposure
Mean OST dose, mg/day (range)
% NAS/ NOWS tx
Comparison group composition
% male
Outcome data analyzed
Blinded assessor
Additional Predictors/ Covariatesb
NOS
Cohorts have been numbered for easy identification of duplicate cohorts examined across studies where different outcomes were assessed
b
PDE, additional prenatal drug exposures: tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, cocaine/other stimulants, benzodiazepines, anti-depressants, additional opioids. B/IF, birth/infant factors: sex, gestational age, birth weight, length, head circumference, neonatal opioid withdrawal treatment, opioid dose, APGAR score, breastfeeding status. SB, social background: maternal/parental age, ethnicity, education level, socio-economic status, marital status. CF, caregiving factors: maternal IQ/vocabulary, psychopathology/symptomology, parenting behavior, maternal vs. foster care status, home environment quality (Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment score). ChF: child factors, age at follow-up, IQ, behavior, attention, ADHD, inhibitory control. SF, study factors: site. c
No breakdown of the rates of NAS for BE and ME children provided.
d
Prenatal exposure to both methadone and heroin.
Other abbreviations: GCI, General Cognitive Index. MDI, Mental Development Index. NAS, neonatal abstinence syndrome. NE, non-exposed. NOS, Newcastle-Ottawa Scale. NOWS, neonatal opioid withdrawal syndrome. NR, not reported. PDI, psychomotor development index. POE, prenatally opioid-exposed. OST, opioid substitution treatment. SBIS, Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. SES, socioeconomic status. WISC-R, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children –Revised. WPPSI-R, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Revised.
What This Systematic Review Adds Children born to opioid-dependent mothers are at risk of adverse neurodevelopment. Exposed infants have lower general cognition and psychomotor scores than non-exposed infants.
Exposed children have lower general cognition and language scores, and increased socialemotional problems compared to non-exposed children.
Developmental surveillance of children born to opioid-dependent mothers is encouraged. Researchers should continue to investigate the longer-term neurodevelopmental outcomes of children born to opioid-dependent mothers, and aim to elucidate specific clinical and psychosocial factors associated with these outcomes.