120
ical. and electrophysiological either PAIN
02157
similarity
of the behavioral
els. Kruger minimized
On the controversy Kruger
of autotomy:
a response
to L.
repeated
among deafferented alluded
expressed
including
by him (Rodin
(Kruger
presented
of this journal,
pain.
by one of us (Levitt primary
animal
tors interpret perceived
objection
skepticism
was earlier
1984) and again in a recent
to rebut that skeptical
view was
19850 several years ago. Now,
of
it is
and in this journal.
Kruger
is the principal this anomolous
chronic
This
negative
abnormal
interpretation
aesthetics
was directed
intercede.
We
dependent
variable.
behavioral
against
those
was rejected
by Kruger;
in other
that
these
formats (Levitt
1992), but they received not yet appeared Major
lest animal-rights two conclusions
of major
the word
unfortunate havioral
of London (Wall
and they have
to denote
of reduced
of ‘autotomy
signs of positive
emphasis
as a
investigators That
was an
the total be-
is placed
behavior
upon
self-
which is directed
noxious sensibility; (b) the logic and
scores’ remain
sensory disorders
sensory loss (Bennett
Those
this effect.
than the compulsive
territories
by rodents
as it misrepresents
misleading
which
et al. 1979). That work
neurectomy.
inasmuch
(a)
rather
interpretation
poorly
to the work
acral self-mutilation
‘autotomy’
syndrome:
abnormalities
among these animal modthose changes that occur
non-sensory disturbances
widely distributed
moot; and (c) behavioral
in cutaneous
areas
and Xie 1988; Kingery and Vallin
of partial
1989; Seltzer
et al. 1990) tend to be ignored.
With tion
untestable.
his conclusion
that autotomy
is solely attributable
reasoned
of nerve or root deafferenta-
to central
neural
disturbances.
that almost any psychophysiological
able for the explanation sensations. derived
This
from
of autotomy,
specious
and
two claims: (a)
of pain.
except
obscure
disturbing was mainly
occurs when
there
does not occur when
be examined. Examples of self-mutilation, tions are presumed first claim. These afflicted
in which persistent disturbing
not to be present, included:
the animal
by the Lesch-Nyhan
severe social isolation, three examples (Levitt
caught
syndrome,
in a trap,
the animal
were considered the
to be likely.
such examples ts supported
in an earlier
presence
That
rebuttal
of disturbing
point
of this claim
sensations
can also be argued
does not allow a strong argument
by unambiguous
do not present with autotomy response
to chronic
pain.
autotomy.
experimental
of the pathophysi-
studies. The early statement
that autotomy
is a response to ongoing
sprouts of C fibers. Despite hypothesis,
generated
essential
in injured
and this is the
It is important
to notice
consists of at least two hypotheses: perceived
pain, and (b) autotomy
impulses in injured
assessment of many experiments
of abnormal
activity
then proceeded
afferents
give
such a statement
really
is a response
to abnormal
by Kruger
to
nerve
has been his
He might be correct.
then made was also to reject the
that since autotomy primary
is not a function
afferents,
sensations. Other
it is not a
considerations
nociception,
autotomy
inflammation.
whereas
the experimental
is limited to territories in hyperalgesic derives (Levitt
of chronic
models
increased
in which
nociception.
autotomy Autotomy
of reduced pain sensibility and does not occur
or allodynic areas. Moreover,
a misconception
cannot be a
to models of re-
This claim is misleading
occurs are based upon conditions of reduced
of the
behavioral
this claim derives from
pattern
1985, 1988, 1990, 1991a.
models of chronic peripheral
self-mutilation
from
which
1992). That
inflammation
autotomy
behavioral
(Albe-Fessard
to the likely conclusion
to a chronic disturbance
that autotomy
is
in the CNS, as in cases of dorsal
and such central alterations
of abnormal
peripheral
are essentially independent
input from injured primary afferents.
1985, 1987, 1988, 1990. 1991a,
sion long ago, based on the comparability
1992) reached
One of
that conclu-
of morphological,
biochem-
than in the inflammation
of the analgesic dependent
self-directed
of reduced
pain
theoretical
The
primary
models,
or hypoalgesic
variable
behavior.
sensibility
significance.
evidence.
pat-
et al. 1990).
(often
That
including
Autotomy question
tissues. Hence,
unappreciated)
it is directed analgesic
is merely
A constructive compulsive provided
rationale
for the
behavior,
the
toward
1985, 1988, 1991a). That evidence
animal
self-directed
learned
responses
readily
observed
toward tissues
scratching,
revealed
was
evidence
that in the intact
which
un-
are evoked
Veterinarians
by
accept
as a clinical sign of somatic pain or itch. A
recently
published
historical
(Kirkup
1988) cited records of compelling
survey
of
human cases with persistent social isolation,
human
pain
relevance.
individuals
management Those records
somatic pain, due to disease
those individuals
hand or foot. Moreover,
analgesics or anesthetics,
of pertinent
licking and biting were natural
to the aversive sensations
of behavior
of the chronic
results in autotomy,
many forms of acute noxious somatic stimuli.
obtaining
tissues
the facts wherein
interpretation
which
(Levitt
the painful
the
is the
tissues has special
should concern
previously by an objective evaluation
dcscrihed
is
insensitive.
self-directed
or injury; during
pattern
and it causes
this chronic compulsive pattern of behavior is directed which are peripherally
this pattern
to be unwarranted.
rhizotomies, us (Levitt
viz.,
cause of chronic
(a) autotomy
to be untenable.
in nociceptive
indicate this latter conclusion attributable
that
remain.
which pertain to the second of these
function of pain or other abnormal Kruger
nociceptive
is a response
but the logical error which Kruger first hypothesis. He concluded
evolving modifi-
elements
principal
C fibers. A main effort
hypotheses, which he inferred
of
against the view
alludes
because these models are based on conditions
compulsive
pain,
the
is that other models of chronic pain and, therefore,
He apparently
sponse to chronic peripheral
is generated
impulses
was
for
evidence.
The second claim of Kruger
most significant
rise to ongoing
under
example of tail removal. The essential point is that the uncertainty
pain which is based upon ongoing ectopic nerve impulse activity that
abnormal
the
the child
reared
of longer duration
in neuroma
sensa-
to support
and the lizard which sheds its tail. The first
19X5), wherein
considered
were offered
has generated
cations of the original
is
evidence of pain in other widely accepted models. These claims must
In the models with nervous system lesions, this behavioral
indicated
is no
there
tern includes scratching, licking, biting, etc., which also occurs in the
The London school provided an interpretation numerous
Kruger
is reason-
abnormal
interpretation
autotomy
and (b) autotomy
hypothesis
ological mechanism which gives rise to autotomy, and that hypothesis of the hypothesis
in
that persistent disturbing sensations motivate autotomy: a view which
peripheral
designation.
amputation,
extremists were
have been made
from Kruger,
has been given by Kruger
has focused upon the chronic adopted
the
to urge
1985, 1987, 1988, 1990, 1991a,b,
little attention
at the University
consequence
Kruger
in this journal.
attention
originated
of
moreover,
prompted
be discontinued,
will argue
Some investiga-
pains or dysesthesias in analgesic body
derived and are ill advised. Some of our arguments previously
after
sensory neurons in the dorsal horn and instead
to ubiquitous
evidence
effect to be a reflection
of this phenomenon
that scientific enquiry
toward
(1992)
models of nervous system injury in which acral self-mutila-
tion, autotomy,
parts.
That
and Kruger
1991). An attempt
timely to pursue the polemics further, The
Kruger
of certain animal models of chronic abnormal
sensation,
horn
and also based upon the
the CNS; this is an hypothesis which has not been specifically tested.
to the editor
his rejection
unpleasant article
letter
in the dorsal
the facts concerning
and which is virtually In a recent
alterations
nerve injury or dorsal rhizotomies,
‘self-amputated’
before the discovery of medical with such pains sought aid in
relief by surgical amputations.
Literature reviews (Levitt 1985, 1988, 1990, 199ia) have revealed that chronic compulsive self-directed behavior and acral self-mutilation by animals occur in the veterinary clinic or in the experimental laboratory as consequences of lesions in either peripheral sensory nerve, dorsal root ganglia, dorsal roots, or dorsal piexi. They also occur as a result of lesions in the spinal cord or brain (Levitt 199ib; Xu et al. 1992). Signs of ali~ynia and hyperalgesia have been sometimes noted. Signs of acral self-mutilation have been also noted i young human cases with peripheral sensory neuropathy or syringomyelia (Levitt 1985, 199ia,b); it also has been described in an human infant with brachial plexus injury (Rossitch et al. 1992). These cases were verbally incompetent patients who could not communicate positive sensory disturbances. Two adult human cases with brachiai plexus injury have been recently described (Albe-Fessard et al. 1990; Procacci and Maresca 19901, who bit and mutilated their finger tips during sleep and complained of painful dysesthesias during wakefulness. The DREZ lesion has been particularly successful for the relief of the human pains of brachial plexus avulsion (Nashold and Ostdahl iY79; Friedman et al. 1988). That surgical lesion was also found to abolish the pains of brachial plexus *‘aversion” in one of the patients who committed autotomy (Procacci and Maresca 1990). Unfortunately, no specific comment was made regarding the probable effect of DREZ surgery on autotomy. We have found that autotomy was markedly attenuated or prevented by DREZ surgery in rats which were totally deafferented by ganglionectomies (Gvelmen-Levitt et al. 1991; Rossitch et al. 1992). Thus, experiences with surgical relief of human deafferentation pains provided a testable hypothesis regarding the prevention of deafferentation autotomy in rats, and that hypothesis was confirmed. Eventually we expect that studies of autotomy will provide testable hypotheses regarding treatment of human deafferentation pains, and these hypotheses will be upheld. Such an event will provide the ultimate validation of autotomy. Hence, our assessment of unambiguous evidence leads us to conclusions which are contrary to some of those reached by Kruger. The total syndrome of behavioral response to certain sensorineural lesions reflects signs of spontaneous and peripherally induced dysesthesias. Acral self-mutilation is a special element of the larger syndrome and it likely signifies the spontaneous dysesthesias which are referred to territories of reduced peripheral sensibilities. The validity of this assertion receives impressive support from the fact that lesions in sensory components of the peripheral or central nervous system which originate autotomy are essentially the same as those which are known to originate dysesthesias in humans. Moreover, additional support for this validity was provided by the prophylactic effect of the DREZ lesion in deafferented rats. inasmuch as such abnormal disturbing sensations are among the most poorly understood, we must question the wisdom which urges cessation of inquiry into autotomy. In addition, we find it disconcerting to consider that such censorship might be fostered by fear of a radical terrorist movement. A rational expectation would envisage great encouragement for the scientific study of autotomy (and the totality of the syndrome).
References Albe-Fessard, D., Giamberardino, A. and Rampin, O., Comparison of different models of chronic pain, Adv. Pain Res. Ther., 13 (1990) 11-27. Bennett, G.J. and Xie, Y.-K., A peripheral mononeuropathy in rat that produces disorders of pain sensation like those seen in man, Pain, 33 (1988) 87-107. Friedman, A.H., Nashold, B.S., Jr. and Bronec, P.R., Dorsal root entry zone lesions for the treatment of brachial plexus avulsion injuries: a follow-up study, Neurosurgery, 22 (1988) 269-373. Kingery, W.S. and Vallin, J.A., The development of chronic mechanical hyperalgesia, autotomy and collateral sprouting following sciatic nerve section in rat, Pain, 38 (1989) 321-332.
Kirkup, J., Surgery before general anesthesia. In: R.D. Mann (Ed.), The History of the Management of Pain, Parthenon Publ., CarnForth, 1988, pp. 15-30. Kruger, L., Deafferentation - the name of the game ain’t mainly in the pain. In: J.M. Besson and G. Guilbaud tEds.1, Lesions of Primary Afferent Fibers as a Tool for the Study of Clinical Pain, Elsevier, ~sterdam, 1991, pp. 117-133. Kruger, L. The non-sensory basis of autotomy in rats: a reply to the editorial of Devor and the article by Blumenkopf and Lipman, Pain, 49 (1992) 153-155. Levitt, M., Dysesthesias and self-mutilation in humans and subhumans: a review of clinical and experimental studies, Brain Res. Rev., 10 (19851247-290. Levitt, M., The central cause of deafferentation dysesthesias, Cephalalgia, 7, Suppi. 6 (1987) 49-52. Levitt, M., Experimental deafferentation syndromes, Appl. Neurophysiol., 51 (1988) 128-135. Levitt, M., The theory of chronic deafferentation dysesthesias, J. Neurosurg. Sci., 34 (1990) 71-98. Levitt, M., The behavioral syndrome of deafferentation dysesthesias. In: B.S. Nashold, Jr. and J. Gvelmen-Levitt fEds.1, Deafferentation Pain Syndromes: Pathophysiology and Treatment, Adv. Pain Res. Ther., 19 (1991a) 209-215. Levitt, M., Chronic dysesthesias of central neural origin in subhuman primates. In: B.S. Nashold, Jr. and J. Ovelmen-Levitt (Eds.), Deafferentation Pain Syndromes: Pathophysiology and Treatment, Adv. Pain Res. Ther., 19 (1991b) 229-238. Levitt, M., Mechanisms of compulsive self-directed behavior by humans and subhumans from deafferentations. in: F. Sicuteri, L. Terenius, C.A. Maggi and L. Vecchiet (Eds.), Pain versus Man, Adv. Pain Res. Ther., 20 (1992) 161-166. Nashold, B.S., Jr. and Ostdahl, R.H., Dorsal root entry zone lesions for pain relief, J. Neurosurg., 51 (1979) 59-69. Ovelmen-Levitt, J., Rossitch, E., Jr., Huang, S.-N., Abdulhak, M., Levitt, M., Nashold, B.S., Jr., Central nervous system lesions and deafferentation by C5-T2 ganglionectomies, Sot. Neurosci. Abst., 17 (1991) 727. Procacci, P. and Maresca, M., Autotomy. Letter to the Editor, Pain, 43 (1990) 394. Rodin, B.E. and Kruger, L., Deafferentation in animals as a model for the study of pain: an alternative hypothesis, Brain Res. Rev., ‘7(1984) 213-228. Rossitch, E., Jr., Oakes, W.J., Gvelmen-Levitt, J. and Nashold, B.S., Jr., Self-mutilation following brachial plexus avulsion injury sustained at birth, Pain, 1992, in press. Rossitch, E., Jr., Abdulhak, M., Ovelmen-Levitt, J., Levitt, M. and Nashold, B.S., Dorsal root entry zone (DREZ) lesions reduce the expression of deafferentation dysesthesias in the rat, submitted to J. Neurosurg., 1992. Seltzer, Z., Dubner, R. and Shir, Y., A novel behavioral model of neuropathic pain disorders produced in rats by partial static nerve injury, Pain, 43 (1990) 205-218. Wall, P.D., Devor, M., Inbal, R., Scadding, J.W., Schonfeld, D., Seltzer, Z. and Tomkiewicz, M.M., Autotomy following peripheral nerve lesions: ex~rimental anesthesia dolorosa, Pain, 7 (1979) 103-114. Xu, X.J., Hao, J.-X., Aldskogius, H., Seiger, A. and Wiesenfeld-Hallin, Z., Chronic pain-related syndrome in rats after ischemic spinal cord lesion: a possible animal model for pain in patients with spinal cord injury, Pain, 48 (1992) 279-290. M.
Levitt
J. Ovelmen-Levitt E. Rossitch, Jr. B.S. Nashold, Jr.