Oral processing and texture perception influences satiation

Oral processing and texture perception influences satiation

Physiology & Behavior xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/p...

400KB Sizes 0 Downloads 55 Views

Physiology & Behavior xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/physbeh

Oral processing and texture perception influences satiation Bryony James



University of Auckland, New Zealand

A R T I C LE I N FO

A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Oral processing food texture satiation

This article offers a brief review of the state of understanding of the role of oral processing and food texture on satiation. Food texture is a sensory property that contributes to the manner in which food transits the oral cavity. Several studies have shown the impact of oral transit time on satiation, with longer times relating to enhanced satiation response. Recent studies have also begun to show an impact of texture on satiation independent of oral processing time. There are still many questions to answer before the underlying mechanisms of these impacts are understood.

1. Introduction

2. What is food texture?

There are many factors contributing to the current “obesity epidemic”, ranging from an energy imbalance in people's food consumption to environmental and genetic factors, and the role played by the gut microbiome, with much debate about potential causes and solutions [1]. One small part of the puzzle is that people are simply eating too much [2]. Stopping eating when you are full (satiation), and feeling full between meals (satiety) would appear to be simple parts of the equation managing food intake. However, the roles of satiation and satiety are fraught with contradictory opinions [3–6], fascinating but outside the scope of this brief review. Given that satiation could be a part of the puzzle it is worth considering what aspects of food, and of eating, contribute to this feature of an eating episode. These aspects will include factors related to the biology and physiology of the consumer including genetics [7] and metabolism [8,9]. They will also, naturally, include factors related to the composition and nutritional make-up of the food [10,11], and flavour and liking [12,13]. Further to these obvious aspects consideration must also be given to the situation in which the food is eaten (including social and environmental [14,15] factors) and prior experience and expectation [16,17]. These factors combine with the physical form of the food (solid, semi-solid, liquid etc) to affect how the food is chewed and swallowed during oral processing. Oral processing encompasses all the physical and physiological aspects of taking food into the mouth and preparing it for swallowing [18–20]. It combines the breakdown and restructuring of a food material in a feedback loop of sensory signals and motor control, and each step can reveal (or conceal) specific flavour or textural attributes [21,22].

Food texture, a vital part of food quality and acceptability, may be defined in a number of ways. A standard definition is “all of the rheological and structural attributes of a product perceptible by mechanical, tactile, visual and auditory receptors” [23]. Bourne [24] summarised numerous other definitions of texture and simplified an overall definition to: “the textural properties of a food are that group of physical characteristics that arise from the structural elements of the food, are sensed primarily by the feeling of touch, are related to the deformation, disintegration, and flow of the food under a force, and are measured objectively by functions of mass, time, and distance”. This approach emphasises that “texture” is a multi-parameter attribute. Textural properties are, first and foremost, sensory attributes [25] and are frequently assessed using sensory panels and tools such as Quantitative Descriptive Analysis [26] or, for dynamic changes in texture, Temporal Dominance of Sensation [27]. These measurements are necessarily subjective and work has been going on for many years to make and link objective instrumental measurements of food properties to texture [28]. Assessing and, ultimately, quantifying the physical properties of food that contribute to textural properties combines techniques more common in materials engineering [29,30] (fracture mechanics, rheology) with those of cognitive neuroscience (such as psychophysics, and neuroimaging) [31]. Relating instrumental measurements of a material property (such as Young's modulus or fracture toughness) to a textural perception (such as crunchiness or crispness) requires correlations of subjective and objective measurements in the presence of the response biases of a human judge. This is where the tools of psychophysics are used [32], treating the human observer as an instrument transducing physical



Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected].

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.015 Received 18 December 2017; Received in revised form 12 March 2018; Accepted 13 March 2018 0031-9384/ © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: James, B., Physiology & Behavior (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.015

Physiology & Behavior xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

B. James

Fig. 1. Model foods used in [66]. Each is a bite-sized disc, isocaloric, with matched oral processing time. Foods are not macro-nutrient matched.

as real foods, eaten in real situations, would have many factors varying together. However this approach has the unfortunate effect of confounding the impact on satiation of both texture and oral transit time. As such a fundamental question remains as to whether texture itself, independent of oral processing time or chewing effort, contributes to satiation. Teasing out whether there is a direct impact of texture on satiety is a further question that has received less attention to date but could be tackled using similar approaches to those described in the final section of this review.

information into sensory perception. In this context sensory scales of texture need to be derived that may be used as a transfer function to determine quantitative relationships. More recently functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been used to provide insight into the reward-related pathways in the brain and their response to chewing and food texture [33–35]. Early results related to texture are sparse, and concentrate on the motor-control needed for hard or less hard food [36,37]. 2.1. Texture, satiation and satiety

2.2. Textural complexity and satiation A recent, comprehensive review [38] of studies relating the role of food structure and oral processing to satiation and satiety indicates that, whilst oral processing time is clearly relevant, the mechanisms underpinning this relationship of chewing and satiation remain to be elucidated. Linking oral processing and texture perception to satiation and satiety is challenging because chewing, salivation and somatosensory stimulation interact in complex ways [39]. Strong evidence has come from studies relating food-form and bitesize/volume to satiation and satiety that suggests one important part of the mechanism is the extent of oro-sensory exposure [40,41]. This has been shown for beverages [42,43] and semi-solids [44,45] where increasing viscosity, and longer oral transit time, correlated positively with subjective satiation signals [46] and bio-markers [47]. Researchers have also compared solid and fluid forms of the same foods eg watermelon/watermelon juice, cheese/milk and coconut meat/coconut milk [48], meat [49], and complete meals [50] and found that solid food forms elicit enhanced satiation responses. Similarly researchers have shown an acceleration of the satiation response of the perceived texture of solid foods ranging from soft solids such as luncheon meat [51], to harder solids such as rye based products [52]. Specific textural sensations have been linked via liking and expectation to expected satiation of solid foods [53]. One mechanism suggested for the acceleration in satiation response with increased oral processing time is that some foods demand greater oral processing effort. Few studies have controlled for the work required for mastication but de Wijk et al [43], in studying the effect of viscosity on “bite size” for liquids and semisolids found that differences in amount consumed were eliminated when controlling for bite effort. However, Pentikäinen et al. [52] found that solid products demanding the most masticatory effort were not the most satiating samples in their study of rye products suggesting that there are still many other factors to be determined. Several authors have suggested that increased oral transit time increases the intensity of the Cephalic Phase Responses (CPRs) [39,54] CPRs include salivation and production of hormones such as cholecystokinin (CCK) and glucagon-like peptide 1 (GLP-1), often cited as biomarkers of satiation [55,56]. However, in all the studies mentioned a longer time in the mouth was frequently created by modifying food texture, for example changing the viscosity of a liquid [57], or the form of a solid product [58,59]. The ecological validity of an approach that varies texture in order to vary oral transit time is self-evident inasmuch

Isolating textural parameters from other variables, in order to answer this question, is difficult and raises a further issue inasmuch as textures are rarely present in isolation from other textures. Even a simple yoghurt might be perceived as smooth, creamy, gritty etc. and, arguably, solid foods are likely to be represented by an even larger number of textures evolving throughout the period of oral processing. Complexity has previously been considered in the context of food products, and is universally acknowledged as being difficult to quantify [60–62]. Some researchers, studying complexity in flavours, have acknowledged the interaction of intensity and number of sensations [63,64]. Complexity has also been quantified using appearance [65] and in that study Mielby et al. also suggested that it is a concept that falls into the “I know it when I see it” category. We have proposed the idea of “textural complexity” as a food property, defined in its most simple form as “a succession of perceived textures from first bite to the point of swallow” [66]. We have recently shown that there is an independent role played by texture itself in the satiation response that is independent of oral processing time [67,68]. These studies used gel-based model foods with differing inclusions to create different levels of textural complexity. The lowest complexity food had 15 unique textural descriptors and the highest had 27. Critically these small bite-sized food models (Fig. 1) had no significant difference in chewing time, chewing rate or number of chews. The food models were used as a preload in single-blind, randomised cross-over satiation trials with participants eating the preload and then eating ad libitum a two course meal of pasta followed by chocolate cake. In two separate trials panellists (n1 = 26, n2 = 36) ate significantly less of the ad libitum meal following the high complexity preload. 2.3. Exploring texture's impact on satiation The trials reported to date linking texture to satiation have built a broad and solid foundation [69] but currently miss some parts of the puzzle. The separate contributions of texture, oral processing time, and masticatory effort need more work to be teased apart. So too do the contributions of liking or hedonics. Satiation, considered as the opposite of “appetite” or “desire to eat” might be a different sensation than “fullness” with the latter's implication of stomach distension and an aversive component related to digestive well-being. In addition to this it 2

Physiology & Behavior xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

B. James

Fig. 2. Where food texture potentially fits into the satiety cascade [75].

sensitivity of the satiation response to the modulation of sensory input [76]. If these mechanisms underpinning the role of texture in satiation can be more clearly defined the opportunity, ultimately, is for manufacturers to develop products specific to demographic segments, different cultures and export markets, all aimed at promoting lower food intake in a health conscious age.

is well established that affective responses to foods factor greatly into their consumption [70]. The emerging concept of an individual's manner of oral processing, such as: “fast chewers, thorough chewers and suckers” [71] or “crunchers, chewers, suckers and smooshers” [72] suggests that textures will have an individualised aspect to their perception. This means that textural complexity, when assessed by dynamic tools might reflect these differences in the consumer as much as the food product [73]. For example Temporal Dominance of Sensation, TDS, almost always reveals strong panel consensus of a dominant texture at the start and end of oral processing but weaker consensus in the middle chewing sections, where panel-level agreement is confounded by inter-subject differences in oral processing strategy [74].

References [1] E.J. McAllister, N.V. Dhurandhar, S.W. Keith, L.J. Aronne, J. Barger, M. Baskin, R.M. Benca, J. Biggio, M.M. Boggiano, J.C. Eisenmann, M. Elobeid, K.R. Fontaine, P. Gluckman, E.C. Hanlon, P. Katzmarzyk, A. Pietrobelli, D.T. Redden, D.M. Ruden, C. Wang, R.A. Waterland, S.M. Wright, D.B. Allison, Ten putative contributors to the obesity epidemic, Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 49 (10) (2009) 868–913, http://dx. doi.org/10.1080/10408390903372599. [2] M. Camilleri, Peripheral mechanisms in appetite regulation, Gastroenterology 148 (6) (2015) 1219–1233. [3] D.A. Booth, A. Nouwen, Satiety. No way to slim, Appetite 55 (3) (2010) 718–721. [4] F. Bellisle, A. Tremblay, Satiety and Body Weight Control – Promise and Compromise. A Comment on Booth and Nouwen's paper “Satiety is no way to slim”, Appetite 57 (3) (2011) 769–771. [5] How important are satiation and satiety for weight regulation? in: M. Fantino, J.E. Blundell, F. Bellisle (Eds.), In Satiation, satiety & the control of food intake, Woodhead Publishing, Oxford, 2013, pp. 357–372. [6] The control of eating: is there any function for satiation and satiety? in: D.A. Levitsky, J.E. Blundell, F. Bellisle (Eds.), In Satiation, satiety & the control of food intake, Woodhead Publishing, Oxford, 2013, pp. 373–393. [7] S. Farooqi, S. O'Rahilly, Genetics of obesity in humans, Endocr. Rev. 27 (7) (2006) 710–718, http://dx.doi.org/10.1210/er.2006-0040. [8] K.G. Murphy, W.S. Dhillo, S.R. Bloom, Gut peptides in the regulation of food intake and energy homeostasis, Endocr. Rev. 27 (2006) 719–727, http://dx.doi.org/10. 1038/nature05484. [9] A. Rodríguez, V. Catalán, J. Gómez-Ambrosi, G. Frühbeck, Visceral and subcutaneous adiposity: are both potential therapeutic targets for tackling the metabolic syndrome? Curr. Pharm. Des. 13 (2007) 2169–2175. [10] B.J. Rolls, M. Hetherington, V.J. Burley, The specificity of satiety: the influence of foods of different macronutrient content on the development of satiety, Physiol. Behav. 43 (1988) 145–153. [11] C. De Graaf, T. Hulshof, J.A. Weststrate, P. Jas, Short-term effects of different amounts of proteins, fats, and carbohydrates on satiety, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 55 (1992) 33–38. [12] A. Drewnowski, Energy density, palatability, and satiety: Implications for weight control, Nutr. Rev. 56 (1998) 347–353. [13] D.J. Mela, Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity, Appetite 47 (2006) 10–17. [14] K.G. Elliston, S.G. Ferguson, N. Schüz, B. Schüz, Situational cues and momentary food environment predict everyday eating behavior in adults with overweight and

2.4. Where to from here? Ultimately understanding the role of texture in satiation will require an approach informed by the oral processing and materials engineering of food, the psychophysical assessment of food during mastication, and the neurological and physiological impacts of consuming food. If these three strands are woven together then texture might be said to occupy a location in Blundell's “satiety cascade” [75] as illustrated in Fig. 2. A complete understanding will incorporate the individuality of both the consumer and the food. To fully define the role that food texture plays in the satiation response, it will be necessary to link the breakdown and restructuring of solid food to the perception of texture during oral processing, and to the physiological, psychological and neurological responses that occur. Much progress is being made in determining the factors contributing to the sensory perception of texture, as outlined in this review. This perception needs to be linked to activity in cortical regions of the brain thought to link neural “reward circuits” to hedonic experiences to determine the response of these regions to experiences of texture. It is also critical to link the extent of satiation modulation to the degree of textural variation. It has been shown that current food models, assessed by sensory panels as “higher complexity”, accelerate satiation [67,68], but how much more complex does one food need to be than another to elicit a change in satiation response? This quantification is one of the missing links in satiation research: knowledge about the 3

Physiology & Behavior xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

B. James

[47] L. Marciani, P.A. Gowland, R.C. Spiller, P. Manoj, R.J. Moore, P. Young, A.J. FilleryTravis, Effect of meal viscosity and nutrients on satiety, intragastric dilution, and emptying assessed by MRI, Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 280 (2001) G1227–33. [48] D.M. Mourao, J. Mourao Bressan, W.W. Campbell, R.D. Mattes, Effects of food form on appetite and energy intake in lean and obese young adults, Int. J. Obes. 31 (2005) 1688–1695. [49] M.J.I. Martens, S.G.T. Lemmens, J.M. Born, M.S. Westerterp-Plantenga, A solid high-protein meal evokes stronger hunger suppression than a liquefied high-protein meal, Obesity 19 (2011) 522–527. [50] A. Santangelo, M. Peracchi, D. Conte, M. Fraquelli, M. Porrini, Physical state of meal affects gastric emptying, cholecystokinin release and satiety, Br. J. Nutr. 80 (1998) 521–527. [51] N. Zijlstra, M. Mars, A. Stafleu, C. de Graaf, The effect of texture differences on satiation in 3 pairs of solid foods, Appetite 55 (2010) 490–497. [52] S. Pentikäinen, N. Sozer, J. Närväinen, K. Sipilä, S.A. Alam, R.-L. Heiniö, J. Paananen, K. Poutanen, M. Kolehmainen, Do rye product structure, product perceptions and oral processing modulate satiety? Food Qual. Prefer. 60 (2017) 178–187. [53] J. Marcano, P. Varela, L.M. Cunha, S. Fiszman, Relating dynamic perception of reformulated cheese pies to consumers' expectations of satiating ability, Food Res. Int. 78 (2015) 369–377. [54] J. Dhillon, C.A. Running, R.M. Tucker, R.D. Mattes, Effects of food form on appetite and energy balance, Food Qual. Prefer. 48 (2016) 368–375. [55] C. de Graaf, W.A.K. Blom, P.A.M. Smeets, A. Stafleu, F.J. Hendriks, Biomarkers of satiation and satiety, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 79 (2004) 946–961. [56] J.A. Harrold, T.M. Dovey, J.E. Blundell, J.C.G. Halford, CNS regulation of appetite, Neuropharmacology 63 (2012) 3–17. [57] M. Mars, P.S. Hogenkamp, A.M. Gosses, A. Stafleu, C. de Graaf, Effect of viscosity on learned satiation, Physiol. Behav. 98 (2009) 60–66. [58] D.P. Bolhuis, C.G. Forde, Y. Cheng, H. Xu, N. Martin, C. de Graaf, Slow food: sustained impact of harder foods on the reduction in energy intake over the course of the day, Plos One 9 (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093370. [59] P.S. Hogenkamp, The effect of sensory-nutrient congruency on food intake after repeated exposure: do texture and/or energy density matter? Physiol. Behav. 136 (2014) 86–90. [60] L.H. Mielby, S. Jensen, M. Edelenbos, A.K. Thybo, An approach to Measuring Adolescents' Perception of Complexity for Pictures of Fruit and Vegetable Mixes, 28 (2013), p. 6675. [61] J. Bitnes, Ø. Ueland, P. Møller, M. Martens, Reliability of sensory assessors: issues of complexity, J. Sens. Stud. 24 (2009) 25–40. [62] J. Marcano, D. Morales, J.F. Vélez-Ruiz, S. Fiszman, Does food complexity have a role in eliciting expectations of satiating capacity? Food Res. Int. 75 (2015) 225–232. [63] M.T. Paulsen, Ø. Ueland, A.N. Nilsen, Å. Öström, M. Hersleth, Sensory perception of salmon and culinary sauces – an interdisciplinary approach, Food Qual. Prefer. 23 (2012) 99–109. [64] P.L.G. Weijzen, E.H. Zandstra, C. Alfieri, C. de Graaf, Effects of complexity and intensity on sensory specific satiety and food acceptance after repeated consumption, Food Qual. Prefer. 19 (2008) 349–359. [65] L.H. Mielby, H. Kildegaard, G. Gabrielsen, M. Edelenbos, A.K. Thybo, Adolescent and adult visual preferences for pictures of fruit and vegetable mixes – effect of complexity, Food Qual. Prefer. 26 (2012) 188–195. [66] D.S. Larsen, J. Tang, L. Ferguson, M.P. Morgenstern, B. James, Textural complexity is a food property – shown using model foods, Int. J. Food Prop. 19 (2015) 1544–1555. [67] D.S. Larsen, J. Tang, L.R. Ferguson, B.J. James, Increased textural complexity in food enhances satiation, Appetite 105 (2016) 189–194. [68] J. Tang, D.S. Larsen, L.R. Ferguson, B.J. James, The effect of textural complexity of solid foods on satiation, Physiol. Behav. 163 (2016) 17–24. [69] L. Chambers, Food texture and the satiety cascade, Nutr. Bull. 41 (2016) 277–282. [70] M.L. Kringelbach, A. Stein, T.J. van Hartevelt, The functional human neuroanatomy of food pleasure cycles, Physiol. Behav. 106 (2012) 307–316. [71] A.M. Carvalho-da-Silva, I. Van Damme, B. Wolf, J. Hort, Characterisation of chocolate eating behaviour, Physiol. Behav. 104 (2011) 929–933. [72] M. Jeltema, J. Beckley, J. Vahalik, Food texture assessment and preference based on mouth behavior, Food Qual. Prefer. 52 (2016) 160–171. [73] M. Devezeaux de Lavergne, J.A.M. Derks, E.C. Ketel, R.A. de Wijk, M. Stieger, Eating behaviour explains differences between individuals in dynamic texture perception of sausages, Food Qual. Prefer. 41 (2015) 189–200. [74] J.M.C. Po, J.A. Kieser, L.M. Gallo, A.J. Tésenyi, P. Herbison, M. Farella, Time frequency analysis of chewing activity in the natural environment, J. Dent. Res. 90 (2011) 1206–1210. [75] J. Blundell, P. Rogers, A. Hill, Evaluating the satiating power of foods: implications for acceptance and consumption, in: J. Solms, D.A. Booth, R.M. Pangborn, O. Raunhardt (Eds.), Food Acceptance and Nutrition, Academic Press, London, 1987, pp. 205–219. [76] D. Chapelot, Quantifying satiation & satiety, in: J.E. Blundell, F. Bellisle (Eds.), Satiation, Satiety & the Control of Food Intake, Woodhead Publishing, Oxford, 2013, pp. 12–39.

obesity, Health Psychol. 36 (2017) 337–345. [15] B. Wansink, Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers, Annu. Rev. Nutr. 24 (2004) 455–479. [16] E.T. Rolls, Taste, olfactory and food texture reward processing in the brain and obesity, Int. J. Obes. 35 (2011) 550–561. [17] L. Brondel, M. Romer, V. van Wymelbeke, N. Pineau, T. Jiang, C. Hanus, D. Rigaud, Variety enhances food intake in humans: role of sensory-specific satiety, Physiol. Behav. 97 (2009) 44–51. [18] J. Chen, Food oral processing—a review, Food Hydrocoll. 23 (2009) 1–25. [19] J.B. Hutchings, P.J. Lillford, The perception of food texture – the philosophy of the breakdown path, J. Texture Stud. 19 (1988) 103–115. [20] P.W. Lucas, J.F. Prinz, K.R. Agrawal, I.C. Bruce, Food texture and its effect on ingestion, mastication and swallowing, J. Texture Stud. 35 (2004) 159–170. [21] Engelen, L., de Wijk, R.A., Oral processing and texture perception. In: Food Oral Processing Chen, J. and Engelen, L., Eds.; Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, 2012; pp159–176. [22] E.A. Foegeding, C.J. Vinyard, G. Essick, S. Guest, C. Campbell, Transforming structural breakdown into sensory perception of texture, J. Texture Stud. 46 (2015) 152–170. [23] Sensory analysis – Vocabulary ISO 5492, ISO, Geneva, Switzerland, 2008. [24] M.C. Bourne, Food Texture and Viscosity, 2nd Ed., Concept and Measurement. Academic Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2002. [25] A.S. Szczesniak, Objective measurements of food texture, J. Food Sci. 28 (1963) 410–420. [26] H. Stone, Quantitative descriptive analysis: developments, applications, and the future, Food Technol. 52 (1998) 48–52. [27] N. Pineau, P. Schlich, S. Cordelle, C. Mathonnière, S. Issanchou, A. Imbert, M. Rogeaux, P. Etiévant, E. Köster, Temporal dominance of sensations: construction of the TDS curves and comparison with time–intensity, Food Qual. Prefer. 20 (2009) 450–455. [28] A. Kramer, Food Texture –. definition, Measurement and relation to other food quality attributes, In: Texture Measurements of Foods; D.Reidel Publishing Company, Holland, Dordrecht, 1973, pp. 1–9. [29] B. Bhandari, Y.H. Roos, Food materials science and engineering: An overview, in: B. Bhandari, Y.H. Roos (Eds.), Food Materials Science and Engineering, WileyBlackwell, Oxford, UK, 2012, pp. 1–25, , http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ 9781118373903.ch1. [30] O. Kravchuk, P. Torley, J.R. Stokes, Food texture is only partly Rheology, in: B. Bhandari, Y.H. Roos (Eds.), Food Materials Science and Engineering, WileyBlackwell, Oxford, 2012, pp. 349–372. [31] C. Spence, Multi-sensory integration and the psychophysics of flavour perception, in: J. Chen, L. Engelen (Eds.), Food Oral Processing, Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford, 2012, pp. 203–224. [32] H.R. Moskowitz, B. Drake, C. Åkesson, Psychophysical measures of texture, J. Texture Stud. 3 (1972) 135–145. [33] Y. Liu, J.-H. Gao, H.-L. Liu, P.T. Fox, The temporal response of the brain after eating revealed by functional MRI, Nature 405 (2000) 1058–1062. [34] A. Quintero, E. Icheso, C. Myers, R. Schitt, G.E. Gerstner, Brain activity and human unilateral chewing: an fMRI study, J. Dent. Res. 92 (2013) 136–142. [35] S.T. Francis, S. Eldeghaidy, Imaging methodolgies and applications for nutrition research: what can functional MRI offer? Proc. Nutr. Soc. 74 (2015) 89–98. [36] M. Onozuka, M. Fujita, K. Watanabe, Y. Hirano, M. Niwa, K. Nishiyama, S. Saito, Mapping brain region activity during chewing: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study, J. Dent. Res. 81 (2002) 743–746. [37] T. Takahashi, T. Miyamoto, A. Terao, A. Yokoyama, Cerebral activation related to the control of mastication during changes in food hardness, Neuroscience 145 (2007) 791794. [38] C.L. Campbell, T.B. Wagoner, E.A. Foegeding, Designing foods for satiety: the roles of food structure and oral processing in satiation and satiety, Food Struct. 13 (2016) 112. [39] P.A.M. Smeets, A. Erkner, C. de Graaf, Cephalic phase responses and appetite, Nutr. Rev. 68 (2010) 643–655. [40] C. de Graaf, Texture and satiation: the role of oro-sensory exposure time, Physiol. Behav. 107 (2012) 496–501. [41] D.P. Bolhuis, C.M.M. Lakemond, R.A. de Wijk, P.A. Luning, C. de Graaf, Both a higher number of sips and a longer oral transit time reduce ad libitum intake, Food Qual. Prefer. 32 (2013) 234–240. [42] R.D. Mattes, D. Rothacker, Beverage viscosity is inversely related to postprandial hunger in humans, Physiol. Behav. 74 (2001) 551–557. [43] R.A. de Wijk, N. Zijlstra, M. Mars, C. de Graaf, J.F. Prinz, The effects of food viscosity on bite size, bite effort and food intake, Physiol. Behav. 95 (2008) 527–532. [44] N. Zijlstra, M. Mars, R.A. de Wijk, M.S. Westerterp-Plantenga, J.J. Holst, C. de Graaf, Effect of viscosity on appetite and gastro-intestinal hormones, Physiol. Behav. 97 (2009) 68–75. [45] N. Zijlstra, R.A. de Wijk, M. Mars, A. Stafleu, C. de Graaf, Effect of bite size and oral processing time of a semisolid food on satiation, Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 90 (2009) 269–275. [46] M. Mars, P.S. Hogenkamp, A.M. Gosses, A. Stafleu, C. de Graaf, Effect of viscosity on learned satiation, Physiol. Behav. 98 (2009) 60–66.

4