Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Child Abuse & Neglect
Research article
Over three decades of longitudinal research on the development of foster children: A meta-analysis Anouk Goemans ∗ , Mitch van Geel, Paul Vedder Institute of Education and Child Studies, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK Leiden, The Netherlands
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history: Received 11 December 2014 Received in revised form 28 January 2015 Accepted 2 February 2015 Available online 25 February 2015 Keywords: Meta-analysis Foster care Child development Longitudinal
a b s t r a c t Large numbers of children over the world experience foster care each year. How best to satisfy their developmental needs and how to avoid placement breakdowns and negative consequences of foster care are important challenges. In this study, a series of four metaanalyses is performed to examine the longitudinal developmental outcomes of children in foster care. The focus is on adaptive functioning and behavioral outcomes. A literature search identified 11 studies suitable for inclusion in the meta-analysis on adaptive functioning (N = 1,550), 24 studies for the meta-analysis on internalizing problems (N = 1,984), 21 studies for the meta-analysis on externalizing problems (N = 1,729) and 25 studies for the meta-analysis on total behavior problems (N = 2,523). No overall improvement or deterioration was found for adaptive functioning. However, studies with a timespan longer than one year and studies with larger sample sizes showed development toward more negative adaptive functioning than studies with shorter timespans or smaller samples. No overall increases or decreases in internalizing, externalizing or total behavior problems were found. Based on these results, it is concluded that foster care does not negatively or positively affect foster children’s developmental trajectories. Given that many children enter foster care with problems, this is a worrying situation. Further longitudinal research to find the factors necessary for improving foster children’s developmental chances is recommended. Furthermore, routine screening and targeted foster-care interventions are adviseable to ensure that all children, who cannot be raised by their own parents, receive the support conducive to their positive development. © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Introduction If, for any reason, children cannot be raised by their own parents, a child can be placed in a foster family. Foster care has been suggested as a better alternative than residential and group settings, because it most resembles the natural home environment of the child (Roy, Rutter, & Pickles, 2000; Webster, Barth, & Needell, 1999; Wilson & Conroy, 1999). In contrast with institutional or group care, which is characterized by a great discontinuity of caregivers, foster care contains the stability and continuity of care through which foster children can build a close relationship with their foster parents (Tizard & Hodges, 1978). Though foster care may be considered the preferred out-of-home placement option, much remains unclear about the development of children in foster care (Lawrence, Carlson, & Egeland, 2006; McWey, Cui, & Pazdera, 2010; Simmel, Barth, & Brooks, 2007). Several studies on foster children point at severe psychological problems at the start of placement in foster
∗ Corresponding author. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.02.003 0145-2134/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
122
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
families (Clausen, Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick, & Litrownik, 1998; Hochstadt, Jaudes, Zimo, & Schachter, 1987; James, 2004; Simms, Dubowitz, & Szilagyi, 2000; Zorc et al., 2013) and at high rates of problems emerging in foster care (Berkoff, Leslie, & Stahmer, 2006; Lloyd & Barth, 2011; Minnis, Everett, Pelosi, Dunn, & Knapp, 2006), but these studies provide a snapshot and do not give insight in the developmental trajectories of foster children. To understand such trajectories, longitudinal studies are needed (Heath, Colton, & Aldgate, 1994; McWey et al., 2010; Simmel et al., 2007; Taussig, 2002). Since the 70s and 80s of the last century, researchers conducted longitudinal studies to analyze the development of children in foster care (Fanshel & Shinn, 1978; Frank, 1980). Numerous longitudinal studies have been performed across various countries, but results have not been conclusive. Some studies suggest that foster care placements may improve children’s functioning (Ahmad et al., 2005; Barber & Delfabbro, 2005; Fernandez, 2009; White, 1997), but other studies reported no improvement (Leathers, Spielfogel, McMeel, & Atkins, 2011; Perkins, 2008) or even deterioration (Fanshel & Shinn, 1978; Frank, 1980; Lawrence et al., 2006). Because large numbers of children over the world experience foster care each year (Stahmer et al., 2009), it is important for these and future foster children to obtain a clearer picture of the impact of foster care on child development. The challenge of inconclusive findings on the development of foster children may be resolved in a meta-analysis. Using meta-analysis an overall effect size of studies as well as the variance of effect sizes across studies can be analyzed (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). In the current study, we performed a series of four meta-analyses to examine the longitudinal developmental outcomes of children in foster care. The focus is on adaptive functioning and behavioral outcomes (internalizing, externalizing and total behavior problems), which have often been used as outcome measures in longitudinal studies on foster care (Barber & Delfabbro, 2005; Fanshel & Shinn, 1978; Minty, 1999; Stahmer et al., 2009). We expect to find a positive development of foster children because foster care, which is characterized by a home-like setting and continuity of care that cannot be offered by other kinds of out-of-home-placements, is considered as second best in absolute sense when children cannot stay with their parents (Burns et al., 2004). Methodological differences in design, as reflected in the moderators, are expected to play a role in the varying outcomes of individual studies. Studies on the development of foster children varied in study length (Frank, 1980; White, 1997), involved small samples and reported considerable attrition across waves (Farmer, Mustillo, Burns, & Holden, 2008) resulting in limited generalizability (Farmer et al., 2008; Maluccio & Fein, 1985). These methodological differences might be reflected in publication type (e.g., peer-reviewed journals are less likely to publish studies with small sample sizes and insignificant results compared to book chapters, reports, and dissertations). Furthermore, the age of foster children is known to be related to behavior problems (McWey et al., 2010). Moderator analyses will therefore be performed on five variables: study length, sample size, attrition, publication type, and mean age. Method Search Strategy Four online databases were used to systematically search for longitudinal studies on the development of foster children published until 2013. ERIC, Medline, PsycINFO and ProQuest Dissertations & Theses were searched with the search terms foster children or foster care combined with longitudinal, repeated measures, or pretest posttest. Second, the reference lists of the retrieved articles, dissertations, books and reports were scanned for relevant studies. Third, the Doctoral Dissertation by Van Oijen (2010) and reviews by Minty (1999) and Maluccio and Fein (1985) were scanned for further potential articles for inclusion in the meta-analysis. This search resulted in 130 studies which included articles, reports, dissertations and book chapters. A flow diagram of our search is presented in Fig. 1. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria We searched for studies on the behavioral and adaptive development of children in regular foster care. Because multiple studies include several outcomes (Stahmer et al., 2009; Van Oijen, 2010) and respondents may only be included in a metaanalysis once, separate meta-analyses will be conducted for adaptive functioning, internalizing problems, externalizing problems, and total problem behavior. Adaptive functioning is generally defined as meeting age and culturally appropriate standards of personal independence and social functioning (Horwitz, Balestracci, & Simms, 2001). Internalizing behavioral problems are considered problems that primarily affect a person him or herself and are characterized by emotional symptoms such as anxiety, depression, withdrawal and somatic complaints. Externalizing behavior problems primarily affect a person’s social environment and refer to delinquent or aggressive behaviors (Achenbach, 1991; Horwitz et al., 2001). Studies were excluded if they did not provide longitudinal data on these outcome measures. Only studies that reported data allowing to compute an effect size between two moments of measurement were included in the meta-analysis. This means that a study had to provide codeable data for the same group of children on at least two points in time, though between the two time points there were sometimes small variations in sample size because of attrition or non-response. The length of time that elapsed between these two points in time was not used as an exclusion criterium. For longitudinal quasi-experimental studies with an intervention and control group, the control group was included in the meta-analysis. We chose to include control groups of quasi-experimental studies because they provide longitudinal data comparable to those of other children in foster care; intervention groups were excluded because the intervention may have changed the children’s behavior in a manner that is atypical for foster children receiving ‘care-as-usual’. For the same reason, studies on therapeutic foster care or foster care
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Records identified through database searching (n = 1020)
123
Additional records identified through other sources (n = 28)
Records after duplicates removed (n = 731)
Records screened (n = 703)
Records excluded (n = 573 )
Full-text articles assessed for eligibility (n = 130)
Full-text articles excluded, with reasons (n = 101)
Studies included in quantitative synthesis (meta-analysis) (n = 29) Fig. 1. Flow diagram of all stages of the literature search.
interventions were excluded. Foster children in all age groups, from infancy to adolescence, were included. Excluded were studies on foster children older than 18 years at the start of the study. Furthermore, some studies included foster children as well as other kinds of out-of-home placed children (e.g., adoptees, institutionalized children). These studies were included only when they reported on the development of foster children separately, and for this information only. Some studies, clearly longitudinal, could not be included because statistics relevant for our meta-analysis were not reported (Aarons et al., 2010; Vanderfaeillie, Van Holen, Vanschoonlandt, Robberechts, & Stroobants, 2013) or foster children and children in other kinds of out-of-home placements or reunified children were combined (Horwitz et al., 2001; Raslaviciene & Zaborskis, 2002; Slinning, 2004; Stahmer et al., 2009). Authors of the articles reporting on these studies were contacted to request additional information. This resulted in the inclusion of four (Bulat, 2010; Minnis et al., 2006; Stahmer et al., 2009; Vanderfaeillie et al., 2013) additional studies. The language in which studies were written was not used as an exclusion criterion, thus the current meta-analysis is not limited to English language publications. Four Dutch studies were included in the analyses (Bastiaensen, 2001; Damen & Pijnenburg, 2005; Damen & Veerman, 2005; Van Oijen, 2010). One article in German was excluded because the age range of the participants exceeded 18 years at the start of the study (Linderkamp, Schramm, & Michau, 2009). Furthermore, one Lithuanian (Raslaviciene & Zaborskis, 2002) and one French (Dumaret & Duyme, 1982) article were excluded because they both focused on other kinds of out-of-home placements. The 11 studies summarized in Table 1 met the inclusion criteria for the meta-analysis on adaptive functioning of foster children and were used in the meta-analysis. Table 2 gives an overview of the 29 studies that met the inclusion criteria for the meta-analyses on behavioral functioning (internalizing, externalizing or total behavioral problems) of foster children.
124
Study (year of publication) Ahmad et al. (2005) a Barber and Delfabbro (2005) a Bogart (1988) b , c Fanshel and Shinn (1978) a Fernandez (2008) a Jacobsen et al. (2013) a Matthews (1997) b McAuley and Trew (2000) a Perkins (2008) b Stahmer et al. (2009) a White (1997) b , d
Country Iraq Australia USA USA Australia Norway USA Northern Ireland Canada USA USA
N (Attrition) 89 (5.30%) 109 (53.62%) 20 (n/a) 205 (63.39%) 39 (9.30%) 56 (6.67%) 48 (52.45%) 12–15 (16.67–25.00%) 201 (45.23%) 758 (n/a) 10 (0.00%)
Study interval 24 months 24 months 3–6 months 5 years 24 months 12 months 6 months 8 months 12 months 21.3 months 5 weeks
FP = foster parent, prof. = professional, T = teacher, FC = foster child. a Studies published in peer-reviewed journals. b Studies published in non peer-reviewed journals. c Study length differs for type of informant (foster parents: 3 months, social workers: 6 months). d Experimental design.
Age range at T1 (mean) 6–18 years (11.1) 4–17 years (10.8) 3–16 years (9.8) 0–12 years (n/a) 4–15 years (8.8) 22–25 months (23.3months) 0.7–17.9 years (n/a) 4–11 years (8.4) 10–17 years (12.9) 0–15 years (4.8) 7–9 years (8.5)
% Female 50.00 48.51 55.00 49.36 50.85 37.50 47.50 36.84 50.00 52.83 60.00
Measure (informant) CBCL Total Competence (FP) Social adjustment (prof.) VABS (FP), CBCL Total Competence (FP & prof.) CBC (prof.) Adaptive Functioning (T) ITSEA Competence Domain (FP) VABS (FP) CBCL Total Competence (FP) Pro Social Behavior (FC) VABS (FP) CBCL Adaptive Behavior (FP)
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Table 1 Studies included in the meta-analysis on adaptive functioning.
Table 2 Studies included in the meta-analyses on internalizing (Int), externalizing (Ext) and/or total behavior problems (Total). Study (year of publication)
% Female
Measure (informant)
Meta-analyses
Ahmad et al. (2005) a Barber and Delfabbro (2005) a Bastiaensen (2001) b Bogart (1988) b , c Bulat (2010) a
Iraq Australia Netherlands USA Croatia
89 (5.30%) 109 (53.62%) 53 (49.52%) 20 (n/a) 60 (46.43%)
24 months 24 months 24 months 3–6 months 5 years
6–18 years (11.1) 4–17 years (10.8) 8–13 years (10.4) 3–16 years (9.8) 10–17 years (13.3)
50.00 48.51 47.10 55.00 50.00
Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Total Int
Damen and Pijnenburg (2005) b Damen and Veerman (2005) b Fanshel and Shinn (1978) b Fernandez (2008) a Frank (1980) a , d
Netherlands Netherlands USA Australia USA
51 (n/a) 41 (36.92%) 205 (63.39%) 51 (13.56%) 50 (0.00%)
6 months 6 months 5 years 2 years 5 years
0–18 years (10) 5–13 years (9.5) 0–12 years (n/a) 4–15 years (8.8) 6–12 years (n/a)
50.53 54.55 49.36 50.85 n/a
Gonzalez (1999) b Haight et al. (2010) a Jacobsen et al. (2013) a Lawrence et al. (2006) a , e Leathers et al. (2011) a Leon, Ragsdale, Miller, and Spacarelli (2008) a
USA USA Norway USA USA USA
15 (n/a) 7 (30.00%) 56 (6.67%) 15 (n/a) 6 (53.85%) 142 (n/a)
12–24 months 7 months 12 months Pre- and postplacement 12 months 6 months–3.2 years
2–11 years (n/a) 7–14.5 years (9.6) 22–25 months (23.3months) 0–9 years (n/a) 4–12 years (8.6) 10.4–17.9 years (13.2)
53.33 n/a 37.50 40.00 28.00 27.00
Linares, Li, Shrout, Brody, and Pettit (2007) a
USA
156 (n/a)
12 months
3–14 years (8.4)
42.31
Love et al. (2008) a, f
USA
20–22 (4.55%/13.04%)
6 months
6–17 years (n/a)
60.87
Matthews (1997) b McAuley and Trew (2000) a , g McWey et al. (2010) a Minnis et al. (2006) a Newton et al. (2000) a Perkins (2008) b
USA Northern Ireland USA Scotland USA Canada
27 (73.27%) 12–15 (16.67–25.00%) 106 (85.42%) 88 (16.98%) 415 (10.75%) 201 (45.23%)
6 months 8 months 3 years 9 months 12 months 12 months
1.8–6.7 years (n/a) 4–11 years (8.4) 13–16 years (14) 5–16 years (11.6) 2–17 years (6.6) 10–17 years (12.9)
47.50 36.84 52.00 44.34 53.50 50.00
Rushton et al. (1995) a , h
UK
12/15 (33.33%/16.67%)
5–8 years
5–9 years (6.8)
0.00
USA Netherlands Belgium USA
752 (n/a) 78/59 (15.22%/15.38%) 49 (36.36%) 10 (0.00%)
21.3 months 21–24 months 24 months 5 weeks
0–15 years (4.8) n/a (14.4) 6–12 years (9.3) 7–9 years (8.5)
52.83 56.50 63.27 60.00
CBCL (FP) CBC (prof.) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP) CBCL Anxiety & Depression (FP), YSR, CDI (FC) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP) CBC (prof.) TRF (T) Psychosocial Problems (prof.) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP) ITSEA (FP) TRF (T) CBCL (FP) Negative Affect (FC) Loneliness, CDI (FC), ECBI (P) BAI (FC), CBCL (FP),CDI (FC) CBCL (FP) TRF (T) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP) SDQ (FP) CBCL (FP) Emotional disorder, conduct disorder, indirect aggression (FC) Parental interview (FP) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP), YSR (FC) CBCL (FP) CBCL (FP)
Stahmer et al. (2009) Van Oijen (2010) b , i Vanderfaeillie et al. (2013) a White (1997) b , j
N (Attrition)
Study interval
Age range at T1 (Mean)
Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total
Int, Ext, Total Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Total
125
FP = foster parent, prof = professional, T = teacher, FC = foster child. a Studies published in peer-reviewed journals. b Studies published in non peer-reviewed journals. c Study length differs for type of informant (foster parents: 3 months, social workers: 6 months). d Percentage female is not reported. However, according to the authors, the sample is a balanced representation according to sex. e Length of placement differs from 1 to 45 months due to the design of this study: pre-post placement measurement. f Sample size and attrition differs for type of instrument: BAI & CDI, N = 22 (4.55%), CBCL, N = 20 (13.04%). g Attrition rate differs for type of informant (mother: 21.05%, father: 25.00%, teacher: 16.67%). h Sample size, attrition, and study length differs for type of instrument: Rutter A scales, N = 12 (33.33%), 8 years. Parental interview, N = 15 (16.67%), 5 years. i Sample size and attrition rate differs for type of informant/instrument (foster parents/CBCL: N = 78, attrition rate = 15.22%, foster child/YSR: N = 59, attrition rate = 15.38%). j Experimental design.
Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int, Ext, Total Int Total A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
a
Country
126
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Coding Decisions and Extraction of Effect Sizes We always used two time-points based on the same sample of children to compute Hedges g. Hedges g is an effect size measure like Cohen’s d but is computed slightly different, incorporating an adjustment which removes the bias of Cohen’s d. Hedges g is defined as the difference between the two means, divided by the pooled standard deviation (Borenstein et al., 2009). If an article provided more than two time points, we choose to code the two time points that were farthest apart (the first and the last moment of measurement). If articles included multiple independent samples (e.g., foster children from different foster care institutions; boys and girls) these were entered in the meta-analyses separately. If articles included multiple dependent samples (e.g., multiple informants for the same study population) the findings were averaged in the meta-analyses (Borenstein et al., 2009), also when study length differed for type of informant (Bogart, 1988; Rushton, Treseder, & Quinton, 1995). Furthermore, several studies distinguished several aspects of the general domain of adaptive or behavioral functioning, but did not give an overall score (Fanshel & Shinn, 1978; Perkins, 2008). In these studies, the statistics for different aspects of adaptive or behavioral functioning were combined into one general estimator of that domain (Goodman, 1997). Two studies (McWey et al., 2010; Stahmer et al., 2009) used the same data for foster children. Because samples included in meta-analysis must be independent, both articles could not be included in the same meta-analysis. One of these studies is included in the meta-analysis on adapative functioning and total behavioral problems, but did not report internalizing and externalizing behavior problems separately (Stahmer et al., 2009). Therefore, the other study is included in the meta-analyses with respect to each of these (McWey et al., 2010). If the same author(s) published more than one article on the same study sample, we chose to include the study with the larger sample size. Information on the test–retest correlations is often missing from articles presenting longitudinal data (Morris & DeShon, 2002). In many meta-analyses that analyze longitudinal data the problem of missing correlations is resolved by treating the means as independent (Jones, Riley, Williamson, & Whitehead, 2009). This approach is likely to result in a biased metaanalysis (Mavridis & Salanti, 2012; Morris & DeShon, 2002; Riley, 2009), and thus we chose to include test–retest correlations in our calculation of effect sizes to account for the dependent nature of the data. To solve the problem of unreported test–retest correlations, the use of an aggregate test–retest correlation coefficient, wherein available correlation coefficients are aggregated, is advised as an option to account for the dependent nature of the data in a meta-analysis based on longitudinal data (Morris & DeShon, 2002). We coded test–retest correlation coefficients, or obtained test–retest correlation coefficients from paired sample t-tests (Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine, 2009). Two of the authors independently coded the means and standard deviations for both time points as well as the sample size and the moderators. The coded moderators were study length, sample size, publication type, attrition, and mean age. Differences between authors were resolved by discussion. Prior to discussion, the authors coded identically 90 percent of the time.
Analyses Data were analyzed using the program Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005). Raw mean difference, standard deviation of both time points, aggregated correlation and sample size were used to compute effect sizes. For two studies (Fernandez, 2008; Love, Koob, & Hill, 2008) raw mean difference and paired t were used to compute effect sizes. Four meta-analyses were conducted to examine the developmental outcomes of foster children with respect to four domains: adaptive functioning, externalizing behavior problems, internalizing behavior problems and total problems. Data were analyzed using a random effects model, which does not assume a common underlying effect size for all included studies, and is commonly more appropriate for meta-analyses based on a literature search than a fixed effects model (Borenstein et al., 2009). The Q test is used to test whether studies are homogeneous. A significant Q test suggests true heterogeneity between included effect sizes. The I2 was used to quantify the heterogeneity between the effect sizes of included studies; the I2 can be interpreted as the percentage of total variability in a set of effect sizes due to true heterogeneity (Huedo-Medina, Sánchez-Meca, Marin-Martinez, & Botella, 2006). If I2 is large, then it would make sense to speculate about reasons for the variance, and possibly to apply techniques such as subgroup analysis to try to explain it. Moderator analyses will be performed on five variables: study length, sample size, attrition, publication type, and mean age. The Q statistic was used to test for the significance of moderators. The jackknife procedure is used to identify studies with large influence on the overall effect size estimate. This procedure gives insight whether the overall effect size is biased by the influence of any one study (Borenstein et al., 2009). Furthermore, because non-significant results may be missing in the studies sampled, due to publication bias, the effect sizes computed in the meta-analysis may be overestimated. To assess the risk of such publication bias, we used Duvall and Tweedie’s trim-andfill procedure the Kendall method. Duvall and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure is an iterative procedure that imputes effect sizes until the error distribution closely approximates normality. This procedure provides a more unbiased estimate of the effect size than the observed estimate. The association between the standardized effect sizes and the variance of these effect sizes was calculated using the Kendall method. A significant correlation indicates that small studies with non-significant results tend not to be published, which suggests that publication bias exists. A non-significant Kendall coefficient suggests the absence of such publication bias.
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Study name
Statistics for each study Hedges's g
Ahmad et al. (2005) Barber & Delfabbro (2005) Bogart (1988) Fanshel & Shinn (1978) Fernandez (2008) Jacobsen et al. (2013) Matthews (1997) McAuley & Trew (2000) Perkins (2008) Stahmer et al. (2009; kin) Stahmer et al. (2009; non-kin) White (1997)
-1,18 -0,43 0,05 -0,07 1,08 0,18 0,06 0,15 -0,06 -0,46 -0,55 1,02 -0,10
Lower limit -1,50 -0,66 -0,25 -0,17 0,61 -0,13 -0,27 -0,21 -0,23 -0,60 -0,67 0,11 -0,32
127
Hedges's g and 95% CI
Upper limit -0,86 -0,20 0,35 0,03 1,55 0,49 0,40 0,51 0,10 -0,33 -0,44 1,92 0,12 -2,00
-1,00
0,00
1,00
2,00
Fig. 2. Forest plot for the meta-analysis on adaptive functioning.
Table 3 Results of the meta-analysis on adaptive functioning, internalizing behavior problems, externalizing behavior problems and total behavior problems. k Adaptive functioning Internalizing behavior problems Externalizing behavior problems Total behavior problems
11 24 21 25
Samples 12 34 29 35
N 1,550 1,984 1,729 2,523
g −.10 −.10 −.04 −.10
CI −.32, .12 −.27, .07 −.24, .15 −.28, .07
I2 92.35 91.47 91.28 94.36
Q 143.86 386.78 321.16 602.50
Results Meta-analysis on Adaptive Functioning Eleven studies eligible for inclusion yielded twelve effect sizes (Table 1). These 11 studies reported on the development of a total of 1,550 foster children, ranging in age from 0 to 18 years. Analyses revealed no overall significant difference between the effect sizes of both measurement points (g = −.10, p = .38, N = 1,550). This means that this meta-analysis showed no improvement or deterioration in the adaptive functioning of foster children during their stay in foster care. The studies included in the meta-analysis were highly heterogeneous (Q(10) = 143.86, p < 0.001), and a fairly high proportion of the observed variance reflected real differences in effect size (I2 = 92.35), meaning that studies included in this meta-analysis tended to provide different effect sizes (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). The forest plot provided in Fig. 2 gives a graphic depiction of the effect sizes of the included studies. In this forest plot, a positive effect size corresponds with an improvement in adaptive functioning. The Duvall and Tweedie trim-and-fill procedure showed that four studies to the left of the mean needed to be imputed to shift the observed point estimate from −.10 (CI: −.32, .12) to −.32 (CI: −.54, −.10), which suggests that four studies with significant deterioration of adaptive functioning would result in a significant overall effect. The jackknife procedure showed that the overall effect remained the same when one study at a time was removed from the meta-analysis. The Kendall’s was .26 (z = 1.17, p = .12), which suggests the absence of publication bias. Results are displayed in Table 3. Five moderator analyses were performed to compare studies on the methodological characteristics study length, sample size, attrition, publication type, and mean age. Studies with a timespan less than one year (g = .08, p = .33, k = 6, N = 350) and studies with a timespan of one year or longer (g = −.31, p = .06, k = 5, N = 1,200) yielded a significant difference, Q(1) = 4.62, p < .05. This means that studies following children’s development during a longer timespan showed more negative adaptive functioning development than studies with a shorter timespan. Furthermore, studies with large sample sizes (N ≥ 80) (g = −.44, p < .01, k = 5, N = 1,362) and studies with small sample sizes (N < 80) (g = .33, p < .05, k = 6, N = 188) also differed significantly from each other, Q(1) = 14.58, p < .001. Studies with larger sample sizes showed more negative adapative functioning outcomes compared to studies with smaller sample sizes. The moderator analyses for attrition, type of publication and mean age did not yield a significant difference.
128
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Fig. 3. Forest plot for the meta-analysis on internalizing behavior problems.
Meta-analysis on Internalizing Behavior Problems Twenty-four studies eligible for inclusion yielded thirty-four effect sizes (Table 3). In these 24 studies a total of 1,984 foster children participated, ranging in age from 0 to 18 years. Analyses revealed no overall significant difference between the effect sizes of both measurement points (g = −.10, p = .25, N = 1,984). Thus, this meta-analysis showed no increase or decrease in the internalizing behavior problems of foster children during their stay in foster care. The outcomes of the studies were heterogeneous (Q(33) = 386.78, p < .001, I2 = 91.47), which indicates that studies included in this meta-analysis tended to provide different effect sizes (Fig. 3). To accommodate the understanding of the reader, it is good to mention that in this forest plot, a positive effect size represents an increase in behavior problems. Duvall and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure suggested that ten studies to the left of the mean needed to be imputed to shift the observed point estimate from −.10 (CI: −.28, .07) to −.30 (CI: −.48, −.11), which suggests that ten studies with significant decreases of internalizing behavior problems would result in a significant overall effect. When one study at a time was removed, the overall effect remained non-significant. The Kendall’s was .03 (z = .24, p = .41) which suggests an absence of publication bias. Results are displayed in Table 3. The five moderator analyses were performed to compare studies on methodological characteristics (study length, sample size, attrition, type of publication and mean age). Comparing studies with low attrition rates (g = −.39, p = .05, k = 9, N = 828) with studies with high attrition rates (g = .07, p = .74, k = 12, N = 842) did yield a significant difference, Q(1) = 4.18, p < .05. Studies with low attrition rates showed a significant decrease in internalizing behavior in contrast with studies with higher attrition rates. Furthermore, none of the moderator analyses yielded significant differences, which means that the moderators cannot explain variation between studies. Meta-analysis on Externalizing Behavior Problems Twenty-one studies eligible for inclusion yielded twenty-nine effect sizes (Table 3). These 21 studies were about a total of 1,729 foster children, ranging in age from 0 to 18 years. Analyses revealed no overall significant difference between the effect sizes of both measurement points (g = −.04, p = .66, N = 1,729). This means that this meta-analysis showed no increase or decrease in the externalizing behavior problems of foster children during their stay in foster care. This meta-analysis
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
129
Fig. 4. Forest plot for the meta-analysis on externalizing behavior problems.
yielded a heterogeneous set of studies (Q(28) = 321.16, p < .001), and a large proportion of the observed variance reflected real differences in effect size (I2 = 91.28). Fig. 4 provides a graphic depiction of the effect sizes of the studies included in this meta-analysis. Increases in behavior problems are represented by positive effect sizes. Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure indicated no publication bias. The jackknife procedure showed no difference in overall effect when one study at a time was removed. The Kendall’s was .02 (z = .13, p = .45) which again suggests the absence of publication bias. Results are displayed in Table 3. Five moderator analyses were performed to compare studies on methodological characteristics (study length, sample size, publication type, attrition, and mean age). None of the moderator analyses on externalizing behavior problems turned out to be significant.
Meta-analysis Total Behavioral Problems Twenty-five studies eligible for inclusion yielded thirty-five effect sizes (Table 3). A total of 2,523 foster children (0–18 years) participated in these studies. Analyses revealed no overall significant difference between the effect sizes of both measurement points (g = −.10, p = .24, N = 2,523). This demonstrates that this meta-analysis showed no increase or decrease in the total behavior problems of foster children during their stay in foster care. The studies included in the meta-analysis were highly heterogeneous (Q(34) = 602.50, p < 0.001, I2 = 94.36), indicating that effect sizes differ among studies included in this meta-analysis. Results are displayed in Table 3. The forest plot is provided in Fig. 5. As for internalizing and externalizing behavior problems, positive effect sizes correspond with increases in total behavioral problems. Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure indicated that nine studies should be imputed to the left of the mean to shift the point estimate from −.10 (CI: −.28, .07) to −.27 (CI: −.44, −.09), which suggests that nine studies with significant decreases of total behavior problems are needed to gain a significant overall effect. The overall effect size remained the same when one study at a time was left out. The Kendall’s was −.05 (z = .43, p = .34) which suggests the absence of publication bias. Like the moderator analyses for externalizing behavior problems, none of the moderator analyses for total behavior problems turned out to be significant. This means that comparing studies on study length, sample size, publication type, attrition, and mean age yielded no significant differences
130
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Fig. 5. Forest plot for the meta-analysis on total behavior problems.
Discussion In this paper a series of meta-analyses was reported examining the longitudinal developmental outcomes of children in foster care. The focus was on adaptive functioning and behavioral outcomes. Contrary to our expectations, we found that generally foster children did not improve their functioning during their stay in foster care. For adaptive functioning, studies with larger sample sizes (N ≥ 80), and studies with a timespan longer than one year showed development toward more negative adaptive functioning. The meta-analyses on behavioral functioning showed no overall increases or decreases in internalizing, externalizing or total behavior problems. Comparing studies on attrition rate, showed that studies with low attrition rates (<25%) showed a significant decrease in internalizing behavior problems in contrast with studies with high attrition rates (≥25%). Contrary to expectations, none of the other moderator analyses on methodological characteristics for the meta-analyses on internalizing, externalizing or total behavior problems was significant. This showed that most of the methodological differences in design, as reflected in the five moderators, do not play a decisive role in the varying outcomes of studies. One explanation could be that although studies were categorized for each moderator variable, the categories did not correspond to meaningful distinctions; perhaps they simply were too broad. For example the moderator ‘age’ was categorized into younger and older than 10 years. This means that the study of Jacobsen, Moe, Ivarsson, Wentzel-Larsen, and Smith (2013) with foster children with an average age of less than two years, belonged to the same category as the almost 10-year old foster children in the study of Bogart (1988). The number of studies included in the meta-analyses did not allow for more narrow categories. Another explanation might be that although these single variables were, based on previous research, expected to be related to the development of foster children, it might be that more complex interaction models explain foster care outcomes. For example, it can be imagined that not solely entry of foster care at younger age is responsible for more positive developmental outcomes (McWey et al., 2010), but that, in addition, for benefiting from foster care, these young children should experience an endured stay in the foster family without placement disruptions (Gonzalez, 1999). Former research indicates major developmental problems at the start of the placement in foster families (Berkoff et al., 2006; Clausen et al., 1998; Dubowitz, Zuravin, Starr Jr, Feigelman, & Harrington, 1993; James, 2004; Simms et al., 2000). Foster children experience internalizing or externalizing problems at three to four times the rate of the general population (Clausen et al., 1998) and 13–36% of foster children are characterized by adaptive functioning delays (Berkoff et al., 2006). The results
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
131
of this meta-analysis showed that foster children do not improve during their stay in the foster family. Though stability may sound positive where it regards the development of children, foster children often enter foster care with high levels of problem behavior (Clausen et al., 1998; James, 2004; Simms et al., 2000; Zorc et al., 2013). This is also true for the studies included in our meta-analyses. Although several studies reported mean scores within the normal range, large percentages of foster children actually do score in the subclinical or clinical range at the same time (e.g., Bastiaensen, 2001; Haight, Black, & Sheridan, 2010; Newton, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 2000). It is worrisome that during a stay in a foster family problems do not decrease, even more so because foster children’s problem behaviors may be one of the main reasons for a foster care breakdown (Oosterman, Schuengel, Slot, Bullens, & Doreleijers, 2007). Apparently, foster care is incapable of reducing these problems or of improving the development of foster children. With regard to adaptive functioning, the studies with samples that were larger than 80 children even suggest that the adaptive functioning of children may deteriorate during their stay in foster care. The moderator analyses further suggest that a longer stay in foster care is not related to better development, as studies longer than a year do not report better outcomes than studies shorter than a year, and with respect to adaptive functioning the studies that were longer than a year even suggested a deterioration in foster children’s levels of adaptive functioning. Because of the experienced trauma and the broken attachment it perhaps should not be expected that children show rapid improvement in foster care, but that even a longer stay in foster care is not associated with improvement in developmental outcomes is worrisome, since it represents a continuation of high degrees of behavior problems during their placement. Moreover, especially externalizing problem behaviors are a major reason for placement breakdown (Oosterman et al., 2007). It is recommended that child welfare tries to prevent or at least reduce the experienced traumas of foster children by being closely involved at an earlier stage before out-of-home placement is realized. Parent-focused intervention (e.g., Schaeffer, Swenson, Tuerk, & Henggeler, 2013) might help to identify problems at an early stage. And although it may not eventually prevent out-of-home placements, it may help to make the start of children in foster care less problematic. Although the meta-analyses reported no overall developmental improvements for foster children, individual studies in the meta-analyses showed a wide range of outcomes. There clearly are some studies that did show positive developmental effects for foster children (Ahmad et al., 2005; Barber & Delfabbro, 2005; Fernandez, 2008) which raises the question which conditions for foster care should be met for improvement and whether there is a common factor in studies in which foster children were reported to benefit from foster care. Knowledge about these conditions can help to formulate foster care policies which ensure that foster children flourish when placed in a foster family. Differences between studies can be due to the fact that studies are conducted in different countries (Bogart, 1988; Clausen et al., 1998; Farmer et al., 2008; Leathers et al., 2011). In the United States, for instance, foster care agencies strongly focus on permanency planning. The U.S. ‘Adoption and Safe Families Act’ (1997) is aimed at finding permanent homes for foster children who cannot return to their biological parents, because their safety is not warranted with these parents. As a consequence termination of parental right is more rule than exception in the USA. In Australia, by contrast, termination of parental rights is rare, meaning that children can, and do, remain in foster care indefinitely (Barber & Delfabbro, 2005). On a national level, studies rely on a limited number of foster care agencies for the inclusion of foster children. Foster care policies can vary from one agency to another, especially in the United States where discrepancies between various state policies exist (see for example www.childwelfarepolicy.org where users can compare child welfare state policies) (Blakey et al., 2012; Boots, 1999). Although it is known that placement stability is one of the factors beneficial for foster children (Newton, Litrownik, & Landsverk, 2000; Rubin, O’Reilly, Luan, & Localio, 2007), states follow different approaches to reduce placement disruptions and do not monitor the effectiveness of their approaches (Blakey et al., 2012). Another important factor can be found in the use or accessibility and the fidelity of interventions provided by the foster care institutions for foster parents and foster children. These interventions are focused on strengthening the parents’ and foster children’s skills, decreasing the foster children’s behavioral problems, supporting foster parents and enhancing the relationship formation and have shown to be effective in reducing problem behaviors (Chamberlain et al., 2008; Dozier et al., 2006; Fisher & Chamberlain, 2000; McNeil, Herschell, Gurwitch, & Clemens-Mowrer, 2005; Price et al., 2008). Moreover, therapeutic foster care is an even more intensive support service for children in foster care, being characterized by specially trained and intensively supervised foster parents (Curtis, Alexander, & Lunghofer, 2001). Given the problematic developmental functioning of foster children at the start of the foster placement and the lack of improvement during their stay in the foster family, interventions should be considered as part and parcel of regular foster care (Fisher, Chamberlain, & Leve, 2009). Limitations When interpreting the results of the current study, two limitations must be kept in mind. First, the longitudinal development of the group of children in foster care as a whole is examined; no distinctions between possibly relevant groups of foster children are made (e.g., kinship/non-kinship, type or severity of maltreatment) because these distinctions were not reported clearly and frequently enough to allow for be meaningful inclusion in a meta-analysis. Nevertheless, subgroups might reveal different developmental trajectories. Recently, a systematic review compared kinship foster care with non-kinship foster care. This review suggested that children in kinship foster care did better than children in non-kinship foster care in terms of their behavioral development, and that kinship is the viable out-of-home placement option (Winokur, Holtan, & Batchelder, 2014). Furthermore, research suggested that foster care outcomes may differ depending the type or severity of maltreatment a child experienced (Jackson, Gabrielli, Fleming, Tunno, & Makanui, 2014). Foster care might be particularly helpful for children who entered the foster family after having experienced sexual abuse or neglect (McWey et al., 2010;
132
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Tarren-Sweeney, 2008). Second, the number of changes of foster homes was not taken into account, while placement instability has been shown to negatively contribute to behavior problems in foster children (Newton et al., 2000; Rubin, O’Reilly, Luan, & Localio, 2007). However, this information could not be included in the meta-analyses because it was not reported consistently. Conclusion Considering the general lack of improvement after a placement in foster care it seems that the new situation that foster care is to many children, is a challenging situation. Many of these children come from a family situation that was ended with the traumatic event of a temporary or permanent loss of their parents. Not only are the children required to cope with these traumatic events, once in foster care they also need to adjust to a new family and living situation. In these taxing circumstances problems in behavioral and adaptive functioning may persist in spite of major environmental improvements (Marinkovic & Backovic, 2007). A clear conclusion of this study is that overall foster children showed no improvement or deterioration in developmental characteristics with which they entered foster care. Developmental outcomes for foster children are not as positive as some suggest (Ahmad et al., 2005; Barber & Delfabbro, 2005; Fernandez, 2008, 2009; Horwitz et al., 2001), and even though foster care is not as detrimental as claimed by others (Frank, 1980; Lawrence et al., 2006; Lloyd & Barth, 2011), the lack of improvement is a cause for concern. Given that many foster children enter foster care with developmental problems, further longitudinal research is needed to find out what conditions and educational processes are conducive to positive foster care experiences, and to examine whether interaction models can explain differences in the varying outcomes of individual studies. Future studies therefore should not only focus on social–emotional problems, but also on psychosocial strengths; both of children and foster parents (Jee et al., 2011). Furthermore, more research is needed to compare the effectiveness of different child welfare policies. In addition to this, it would help if foster care agencies would uniformly assess and monitor the developmental functioning of their foster children systematically (Burns et al., 2004; Fisher et al., 2009; Garwood & Close, 2001; Harden, 2004). It seems that large variation in the functioning of foster children exists. Screening would stimulate and facilitate foster care professionals to signal developmental challenges and specific developmental needs at an early stage, could enhance creating trauma-informed service systems (Fraser et al., 2014; Kramer, Sigel, Conners-Burrow, Savary, & Tempel, 2013), and start and support early intervention and treatment of foster children (Leslie et al., 2005). Targeted foster-care interventions to address specific needs (Chamberlain et al., 2008; Dozier et al., 2006) or even therapeutic foster care (Meadowcroft, Thomlison, & Chamberlain, 1993) is needed to ensure that all children who cannot be raised by their own parents, receive the support that they need. References Aarons, G. A., James, S., Monn, A. R., Raghavan, R., Wells, R. S., & Leslie, L. K. (2010). Behavior problems and placement change in a national child welfare sample: A prospective study. Journal of The American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(1), 70–80. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont. Ahmad, A., Qahar, J., Siddiq, A., Majeed, A., Rasheed, J., Jabar, F., & von Knorring, A. L. (2005). A 2-year follow-up of orphans’ competence, socioemotional problems and post-traumatic stress symptoms in traditional foster care and orphanages in Iraqi Kurdistan. Child: Care, Health and Development, 31(2), 203–215. Barber, J., & Delfabbro, P. (2005). Children’s adjustment to long-term foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 27(3), 329–340. Bastiaensen, P. A. C. M. (2001). Belaste pleegouders en verscheurde pleegkinderen. Onderzoek naar de wijze waarop pleegouders de opvoedingssituatie beleven en pleegkinderen de relatie met het pleeggezin en het gezin van oorsprong beleven. Zundert, The Netherlands: Vorsselmans. Berkoff, M. C., Leslie, L. K., & Stahmer, A. C. (2006). Accuracy of caregiver identification of developmental delays among young children involved with child welfare. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(4), 310–318. Blakey, J. M., Leathers, S. J., Lawler, M., Washington, T., Natschke, C., Strand, T., & Waltonb, Q. (2012). A review of how states are addressing placement stability. Children and Youth Services Review, 34(2), 369–378. Bogart, N. (1988). A comparative study of behavioral adjustment between therapeutic and regular foster care in the treatment of child abuse and neglect. Retrieved from Memphis State University. Boots, S. W. (1999). Family care or foster care? How state policies affect kinship caregivers. The Urban Institute, Series A No. A-34 (No. A-34). Retrieved from The Urban Institute Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 2). Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. Bulat, L. R. (2010). A longitudinal study of depressiveness in children in public care. International Journal of Social Welfare, 19(4), 412–423. Burns, B. J., Phillips, S. D., Wagner, H. R., Barth, R. P., Kolko, D. J., Campbell, Y., & Landsverk, J. (2004). Mental health need and access to mental health services by youths involved with child welfare: A national survey. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(8), 960–970. Chamberlain, P., Price, J., Leve, L. D., Laurent, H., Landsverk, J. A., & Reid, J. B. (2008). Prevention of behavior problems for children in foster care: Outcomes and mediation effects. Prevention Science, 9(1), 17–27. Clausen, J. M., Landsverk, J., Ganger, W., Chadwick, D., & Litrownik, A. (1998). Mental health problems of children in foster care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 7(3), 283–296. Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Curtis, P. A., Alexander, G., & Lunghofer, L. A. (2001). A literature review comparing the outcomes of residential group care and therapeutic foster care. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 18(5), 377–392. Damen, H., & Veerman, J. W. (2005). Meer indien nodig, minder als het kan: Een evaluatieonderzoek naar de Integrale Pleegzorg Nederland. Damen, H. R., & Pijnenburg, H. M. P. H. (2005). Onbaatzuchtig en professioneel: Innovatie in de Brabantse pleegzorg: Eindrapport monitorproject Versterking Pleegzorg Noord-Brabant 2003–2004 Praktikon. Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lindhiem, O., Gordon, M. K., Manni, M., Sepulveda, S., Ackerman, J., Bernier, A., & Levine, S. (2006). Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: An example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal of Social Issues, 62(4), 767–785. Dubowitz, H., Zuravin, S., Starr, R. H., Jr., Feigelman, S., & Harrington, D. (1993). Behavior problems of children in kinship care. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 14(6), 386–393.
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
133
Dumaret, A., & Duyme, M. (1982). Devenir scolaire et professionnel de sujets placés en villages d’enfants. Applied Psychology, 31(4), 455–473. Fanshel, D., & Shinn, E. B. (1978). Children in foster care: A longitudinal investigation. New York: Columbia University Press. Farmer, E. M. Z., Mustillo, S., Burns, B. J., & Holden, E. W. (2008). Use and predictors of out-of-home placements within systems of care. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 16(1), 5–14. Fernandez, E. (2008). Unravelling emotional, behavioural and educational outcomes in a longitudinal study of children in foster-care. British Journal of Social Work, 38(7), 1283–1301. Fernandez, E. (2009). Children’s wellbeing in care: Evidence from a longitudinal study of outcomes. Children and Youth Services Review, 31(10), 1092–1100. Fisher, P. A., & Chamberlain, P. (2000). Multidimensional treatment foster care a program for intensive parenting, family support, and skill building. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 8(3), 155–164. Fisher, P. A., Chamberlain, P., & Leve, L. D. (2009). Improving the lives of foster children through evidenced-based interventions. Vulnerable children and youth studies, 4(2), 122–127. Frank, G. (1980). Treatment needs of children in foster care. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50(2), 256. Fraser, J. G., Griffin, J. L., Barto, B. L., Lo, C., Wenz-Gross, M., Spinazzola, J., & Bartlett, J. D. (2014). Implementation of a workforce initiative to build trauma-informed child welfare practice and services: Findings from the Massachusetts Child Trauma Project. Children and Youth Services Review, 44, 233–242. Garwood, M. M., & Close, W. (2001). Identifying the psychological needs of foster children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 32(2), 125–135. Gonzalez, E. T. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral/emotional development of latino children in foster care. Alameda: California School of Professional Psychology. Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586. Haight, W., Black, J., & Sheridan, K. (2010). A mental health intervention for rural, foster children from methamphetamine-involved families: Experimental assessment with qualitative elaboration. Children and Youth Services Review, 32(10), 1446–1457. Harden, B. J. (2004). Safety and stability for foster children: A developmental perspective. Future of Children, 31–47. Retrieved from JSTOR. Heath, A. F., Colton, M. J., & Aldgate, J. (1994). Failure to escape: A longitudinal study of foster children’s educational attainment. British Journal of Social Work, 24(3), 241–260. Higgins, J., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557– 560. Hochstadt, N. J., Jaudes, P. K., Zimo, D. A., & Schachter, J. (1987). The medical and psychosocial needs of children entering foster care. Child Abuse & Neglect, 11(1), 53–62. Horwitz, S. M., Balestracci, K. M., & Simms, M. D. (2001). Foster care placement improves children’s functioning. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 155(11), 1255–1260. Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Marin-Martinez, F., & Botella, J. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I index? Psychological Methods, 11(2), 193–206. Jackson, Y., Gabrielli, J., Fleming, K., Tunno, A. M., & Makanui, P. K. (2014). Untangling the relative contribution of maltreatment severity and frequency to type of behavioral outcome in foster youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 38, 1147–1159. Jacobsen, H., Moe, V., Ivarsson, T., Wentzel-Larsen, T., & Smith, L. (2013). Cognitive development and social–emotional functioning in young foster children: A follow-up study from 2 to 3 years of age. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44(5), 666–677. James, S. (2004). Why do foster care placements disrupt? An investigation of reasons for placement change in foster care. Social Service Review, 78(4), 601–627. Jee, S. H., Szilagyi, M., Conn, A. M., Nilsen, W., Toth, S., Baldwin, C. D., & Szilagyi, P. G. (2011). Validating office-based screening for psychosocial strengths and difficulties among youths in foster care. Pediatrics, 127(5), 904–910. Jones, A. P., Riley, R. D., Williamson, P. R., & Whitehead, A. (2009). Meta-analysis of individual patient data versus aggregate data from longitudinal clinical trials. Clinical Trials, 6(1), 16–27. Kramer, T. L., Sigel, B. A., Conners-Burrow, N. A., Savary, P. E., & Tempel, A. (2013). A statewide introduction of trauma-informed care in a child welfare system. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(1), 19–24. Lawrence, C. R., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. (2006). The impact of foster care on development. Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 57–76. Leathers, S. J., Spielfogel, J. E., McMeel, L. S., & Atkins, M. S. (2011). Use of a parent management training intervention with urban foster parents: A pilot study. Children and Youth Services Review, 33(7), 1270–1279. Leon, S. C., Ragsdale, B., Miller, S. A., & Spacarelli, S. (2008). Trauma resilience among youth in substitute care demonstrating sexual behavior problems. Child Abuse and Neglect, 32(1), 67–81. Leslie, L. K., Gordon, J. N., Lambros, K., Premji, K., Peoples, J., & Gist, K. (2005). Addressing the developmental and mental health needs of young children in foster care. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(2), 140–151. Linares, L. O., Li, M., Shrout, P. E., Brody, G. H., & Pettit, G. S. (2007). Placement shift, sibling relationship quality, and child outcomes in foster care: A controlled study. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(4), 736–743. Linderkamp, F., Schramm, S. A., & Michau, S. (2009). Die psychische Entwicklung von Pflegekindern und Pflegeeltern: Ergebnisse einer prospektiven Längsschnittstudie. Verhaltenstherapie & Psychosoziale Praxis, 41(4), 863–880. Lloyd, E. C., & Barth, R. P. (2011). Developmental outcomes after five years for foster children returned home, remaining in care, or adopted. Children and Youth Services Review, 33(8), 1383–1391. Love, S. M., Koob, J. J., & Hill, L. E. (2008). The effects of using community mental health practitioners to treat foster children: Implications for child welfare planners. The Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice: Objective Investigations of Controversial and Unorthodox Claims in Clinical Psychology, Psychiatry, and Social Work, 6(1), 31–39. Maluccio, A. N., & Fein, E. (1985). Growing up in foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 7(2), 123–134. Marinkovic, J. A., & Backovic, D. (2007). Relationship between type of placement and competencies and problem behavior of adolescents in long-term foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 29(2), 216–225. Matthews, D. M. (1997). Time in foster care and children’s developmental status. ProQuest Information & Learning, US. Mavridis, D., & Salanti, G. (2012). A practical introduction to multivariate meta-analysis. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 22, 1–26. McAuley, C., & Trew, K. (2000). Children’s adjustment over time in foster care: Cross-informant agreement, stability and placement disruption. British Journal of Social Work, 30(1), 91–107. McNeil, C. B., Herschell, A. D., Gurwitch, R. H., & Clemens-Mowrer, L. (2005). Training foster parents in parent–child interaction therapy. Education and Treatment of Children, 28(2), 182–196. McWey, L. M., Cui, M., & Pazdera, A. L. (2010). Changes in externalizing and internalizing problems of adolescents in foster care. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72(5), 1128–1140. Meadowcroft, P., Thomlison, B., & Chamberlain, P. (1993). Treatment foster care services: A research agenda for child welfare. Child Welfare, 73(5), 565–581. Minnis, H., Everett, K., Pelosi, A. J., Dunn, J., & Knapp, M. (2006). Children in foster care: Mental health, service use and costs. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(2), 63–70. Minty, B. (1999). Annotation: Outcomes in long-term foster family care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(7), 991–999. Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 105–125. Newton, R. R., Litrownik, A. J., & Landsverk, J. A. (2000). Children and youth in foster care: Disentangling the relationship between problem behaviors and number of placements. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(10), 1363–1374.
134
A. Goemans et al. / Child Abuse & Neglect 42 (2015) 121–134
Oosterman, M., Schuengel, C., Slot, N. W., Bullens, R. A., & Doreleijers, T. A. (2007). Disruptions in foster care: A review and meta-analysis. Children and Youth Services Review, 29(1), 53–76. Perkins, J. N. (2008). Foster parenting practices as predictors of foster child outcomes. Ottawa, ON: University of Ottawa. Price, J. M., Chamberlain, P., Landsverk, J., Reid, J. B., Leve, L. D., & Laurent, H. (2008). Effects of a foster parent training intervention on placement changes of children in foster care. Child Maltreatment, 13(1), 64–75. Raslaviciene, G., & Zaborskis, A. (2002). The development of mixed emotional and behavioral disorders in children raised in foster care institutions. Medicina (Kaunas, Lithuania), 38(7), 759–768. Riley, R. D. (2009). Multivariate meta-analysis: The effect of ignoring within-study correlation. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society), 174(4), 789–811. Roy, P., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (2000). Institutional care: Risk from family background or pattern of rearing? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(2), 139–149. Rubin, D. M., O’Reilly, A. L., Luan, X., & Localio, A. R. (2007). The impact of placement stability on behavioral well-being for children in foster care. Pediatrics, 119(2), 336–344. Rushton, A., Treseder, J., & Quinton, D. (1995). An eight-year prospective study of older boys placed in permanent substitute families: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(4), 687–695. Schaeffer, C. M., Swenson, C. C., Tuerk, E. H., & Henggeler, S. W. (2013). Comprehensive treatment for co-occurring child maltreatment and parental substance abuse: Outcomes from a 24-month pilot study of the MST-Building Stronger Families program. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(8), 596–607. Simmel, C., Barth, R. P., & Brooks, D. (2007). Adopted foster youths’ psychosocial functioning: A longitudinal perspective. Child & Family Social Work, 12(4), 336–348. Simms, M. D., Dubowitz, H., & Szilagyi, M. A. (2000). Health care needs of children in the foster care system. Pediatrics, 106(Suppl. 3), 909–918. Slinning, K. (2004). Foster placed children prenatally exposed to poly-substances. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(1), 19–27. Stahmer, A. C., Hurlburt, M., Horwitz, S. M., Landsverk, J., Zhang, J., & Leslie, L. K. (2009). Associations between intensity of child welfare involvement and child development among young children in child welfare. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33(9), 598–611. Tarren-Sweeney, M. (2008). Retrospective and concurrent predictors of the mental health of children in care. Children and Youth Services Review, 30(1), 1–25. Taussig, H. N. (2002). Risk behaviors in maltreated youth placed in foster care: A longitudinal study of protective and vulnerability factors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26(11), 1179–1199. Tizard, B., & Hodges, J. (1978). The effect of early institutional rearing on the development of eight year old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19(2), 99–118. Van Oijen, S. (2010). Resultaat van pleegzorgplaatsingen: Een onderzoek naar breakdown en de ontwikkeling van adolescente pleegkinderen bij langdurige pleegzorgplaatsingen. The Netherlands: University Library Groningen. Vanderfaeillie, J., Van Holen, F., Vanschoonlandt, F., Robberechts, M., & Stroobants, T. (2013). Children placed in long-term family foster care: A longitudinal study into the development of problem behavior and associated factors. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(4), 587–593. Webster, D., Barth, R. P., & Needell, B. (1999). Placement stability for children in out-of-home care: A longitudinal analysis. Child Welfare, 79(5), 614–632. White, D. S. (1997). Using social problem-solving interventions with foster children: A means of preventing later psychopathology. ProQuest Information & Learning, US. Wilson, L., & Conroy, J. (1999). Satisfaction of children 3 in out-of-home care. In Family foster care in the next century. Winokur, M., Holtan, A., & Batchelder, K. E. (2014). Kinship care for the safety, permanency, and well-being of maltreated children. Retrieved from John Wiley and Sons, Ltd. for The Cochrane Collaboration. Zorc, C. S., O’Reilly, A. L. R., Matone, M., Long, J., Watts, C. L., & Rubin, D. (2013). The relationship of placement experience to school absenteeism and changing schools in young, school-aged children in foster care. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(5), 826–833.