Pharmacological manipulation of memory reconsolidation: Towards a novel treatment of pathogenic memories

Pharmacological manipulation of memory reconsolidation: Towards a novel treatment of pathogenic memories

European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect European Journal of Pharmacology j o u r n a l h o m e...

180KB Sizes 1 Downloads 32 Views

European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

European Journal of Pharmacology j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / e j p h a r

Review

Pharmacological manipulation of memory reconsolidation: Towards a novel treatment of pathogenic memories Leontien Diergaarde ⁎, Anton N.M. Schoffelmeer, Taco J. De Vries Department of Anatomy and Neurosciences, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, VU Medical Center, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands

A R T I C L E

I N F O

A B S T R A C T

Article history: Accepted 4 March 2008 Available online 18 March 2008

Well-consolidated memories, when retrieved, may return to a transiently fragile state, and need to be consolidated again in order to be maintained. This process has been referred to as memory reconsolidation and presumably serves to modify or strengthen memory traces. In recent years, our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms underlying this phenomenon has increased rapidly. Here, we will briefly review some of the pharmacological evidence, stressing a crucial role for the brain's major neurotransmitter systems, such as glutamate and noradrenaline, in memory reconsolidation. Pharmacological intervention of reconsolidation processes may have clinical relevance, especially for the treatment of psychiatric disorders that are characterized by pathological memories, including post-traumatic stress disorder and addictive behaviour. © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Memory Addiction Post-traumatic stress disorder Therapy (Rat) (Mouse)

Contents 1.

Introduction . . . . . 1.1. Glutamate . . . 1.2. Noradrenaline . 1.3. Dopamine. . . 1.4. Acetylcholine . 1.5. γ-Aminobutyric 1.6. Glucocorticoids 1.7. Cannabinoids . 2. Concluding remarks . References . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . acid (GABA) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

1. Introduction The traditional hypothesis of memory consolidation postulates, that during consolidation memory is initially labile after acquisition, but gradually becomes resistant to amnesic treatment. However, accumulating evidence indicates that, following retrieval, a wellconsolidated memory trace may return to a labile state during which it can be modified again. For example, several studies in rodents have shown that protein synthesis inhibition immediately upon retrieval of memory impairs subsequent expression of this type of memory (Tronson and Taylor, 2007), thereby indicating that this memory

⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +31 20 4448097; fax: +31 20 4448100. E-mail address: [email protected] (L. Diergaarde). 0014-2999/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.03.010

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

. . . . . . . . . .

453 454 455 455 455 455 456 456 456 456

updating mechanism, denoted as a reconsolidation process (similar to consolidation of memory) requires protein synthesis. Behavioural pharmacological studies in rats and mice have provided evidence for the existence of this phenomenon, and significantly contributed to our comprehension of the neurobiological mechanisms associated with memory reconsolidation processes, by showing that certain pharmacological agents can induce retrograde amnesia when administered upon a short memory reactivation trial (Table 1). Moreover, these studies suggest that drugs interfering with memory reconsolidation processes might be of great clinical importance, particularly for the treatment of psychiatric disorders that are characterized by strong pathogenic memories, such as post-traumatic stress disorder and drug addiction. In this article we will review how behavioural pharmacological studies in rodents have contributed to our understanding of the complex processes of memory reconsolidation. Further, we speculate

454

L. Diergaarde et al. / European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457

Neurotransmitter Receptor (sub) Drug system type

Effect on memory References reconsolidation

Glutamate

↓ ↓

1 2–5

↓ ↑

6–7 2



8



9



10–17



18

In addition to conditioned fear paradigms, appetitive learning paradigms have also recently been employed in reconsolidation research, for example, in a conditioned place preference paradigm (in which a context is associated with a pleasant stimulus such as the effect of a drug), and in food- or drug-rewarded (instrumental) learning tasks. Accordingly, either a decreased preference for the drug-paired context or a reduction in instrumental responding upon post-retrieval drug treatment is regarded to reflect a disruption of memory reconsolidation processes. Typically, a no-reactivation control condition (i.e. a non-contingent drug injection in the home cage) is included in reconsolidation experiments, in order to ensure that the drug effect depends on memory reactivation and thus affects molecular processes initiated by retrieval, rather than interfering with initial memory consolidation or other side-effects of the drug.



19

1.1. Glutamate

↑/↔

20–21

Table 1 Provisional overview of neurotransmitter involvement in memory reconsolidation

Noradrenaline

Dopamine

Acetylcholine

GABA

Glucocorticoids

Cannabinoids

NMDA

CPP, (antagonist) MK-801 (antagonist) APV (antagonist) D-cycloserine (partial agonist) AMPA-like DNQX (antagonist) MPEP mGlu5 (antagonist) βPropranolol adrenoceptor (antagonist) Timolol (antagonist) – Cocaine (blocks reuptake) – Amphetamine (increases release) SCH23390 D1 (antagonist) – Hemicholinium (blocks reuptake) Muscarinergic Scopolamine (antagonist) Midazolam GABAA (agonist) Muscimol (agonist) Glucocorticoid RU38486 (antagonist) Corticosterone (agonist) WIN55212-2 Cannabinoid (agonist) CB1 HU210 (agonist) AM251 (antagonist) SR141716A (antagonist)

The contribution of glutamate receptors, especially the N-methyl(NMDA) receptor, in learning and memory processes including long-term potentiation and consolidation has been well established. Recently, several studies have revealed that NMDA receptor activation is also critically involved in reconsolidation of memory. Using a contextual fear paradigm, Suzuki and colleagues demonstrated that a systemic injection of the NMDA receptor antagonist [D(−)-3(2-carboxypiperazine-4-yl)-propyl-1-phosphonic acid (CPP)] prior to brief, non-reinforced reexposure to the conditioning context, reduced conditioned freezing behaviour in mice when tested in the same context 24 h later (Suzuki et al., 2004). Furthermore, they showed that when the retrieval session was of longer duration, fear memory was extinguished in vehicle-treated animals when tested the following day. In contrast, fear memory was preserved in CPP-treated rats, indicating that NMDA receptor antagonism prevented extinction. These results indicate that a short reminder triggers a second cascade of consolidation, whereas a long reactivation session leads to the formation of a new memory that competes with the original memory, i.e. extinction. It also indicates that NMDA receptor dependent mechanisms may underlie both memory reconsolidation and extinction processes. Using the non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist ((−)-5-methyl-10,11-dihydro-SH-dibenzo[a,d] cyclohepten-5,10-imine maleate (MK-801), Lee et al. (2006) demonstrated that NMDA receptor blockade prior to reactivation may impair reconsolidation or extinction of fear memory up to 7 days postreactivation, depending on the duration of the reactivation session. Vice versa, D-cycloserine, a partial NMDA receptor agonist, administered either systemically or into the basolateral amygdala prior to recall, facilitated memory reconsolidation after short (i.e. 2 min) reexposure, or alternatively, blocked extinction of fear memory when reexposure was prolonged to 20 min. Additional evidence for the role of amygdalar NMDA receptors in fear-related memory reconsolidation, was provided by Ben Mamou et al. showing that intra-amygdala infusions with D(−)-2-amino-5phosphonovaleric acid (APV) prevented anisomycin produced amnesia of fear-related memory (Ben Mamou et al., 2006). Besides fear-related memories, post-retrieval pharmacological manipulation of the NMDA receptor has also been found to interfere with reconsolidation of reward-related memories. For example, systemic administration of MK-801 prior to memory reactivation impaired subsequent performance on a food-rewarded spatial discrimination learning task (Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997). Moreover, an intracerebroventricular injection of APV upon retrieval disrupted the subsequent expression of an odor-reward association (Torras-Garcia et al., 2005). Importantly, the detrimental effect of APV on memory was retrieval-dependent, since rats receiving a non-contingent APV injection in the absence of memory reactivation showed perfect retention. Similar to food-related memories, reconsolidation of long-term drug-related D-aspartate



22



23

↓/↔

2,24–25



26



27



28–29



10,30

↓, reversed by AM251 ↓, reversed by AM251 ↓/↔

31–32



1,31

32 25

1 (Suzuki et al., 2004), 2–5 (Kelley et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2006; Przybyslawski and Sara, 1997; Sadler et al., 2007), 6–7 (Ben Mamou et al., 2006; Torras-Garcia et al., 2005), 8 (Rose and Rankin, 2006), 9 (Salinska, 2006), 10–17 (Abrari et al., 2007; Bernardi et al., 2006; Brunet et al., 2008; Debiec and Ledoux, 2004; Diergaarde et al., 2006; Milton et al., 2008; Przybyslawski et al.1999; Robinson and Franklin, 2007), 18 (Roullet and Sara,1998), 19 (Rodriguez et al., 1993), 20–21 (Blaiss and Janak, 2006; Blaiss and Janak, 2007), 22 (Sherry et al., 2005), 23 (Boccia et al., 2004), 24–25 (Bermudez-Rattoni et al., 2005; Bucherelli et al., 2006), 26 (Bustos et al., 2006), 27 (Amaral et al., 2007), 28–29 (Jin, et al., 2007; Tronel and Alberini, 2007), 30 (Cai et al., 2006), 31–32 (Kobilo et al., 2007;Lin et al., 2006). For abbreviations: see text. ↑ Facilitated memory reconsolidation, ↓ impaired memory reconsolidation, ↔ no effect, – not applicable.

on the potential therapeutic efficacy of drugs that can ‘erase’ pathogenic memories by disrupting memory reconsolidation. Most experiments on memory reconsolidation thus far involved conditioned fear paradigms, i.e. procedures in which either a distinctive context (contextual fear conditioning) or tone (auditory fear conditioning) is paired with a foot shock, so that these contextual and/or auditory cues themselves induce a fear response, such as freezing behaviour. In memory reconsolidation experiments, rats are (after training) briefly reexposed to this cue (without the shock) for a limited period in order to reactivate the cue-shock association, immediately after which they receive an injection of a drug to disturb memory reconsolidation. The next day(s), memory retention is tested in the shock context or in the presence of the tone and the amount of freezing behaviour is used as index of conditioned fear memory.

L. Diergaarde et al. / European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457

memories also involves an NMDA receptor mediated mechanism. Thus, in conditioned place preference studies treatment with MK-801 before reexposure to the cocaine-associated context (Kelley et al., 2007) or after reexposure to the amphetamine-associated context (Sadler et al., 2007) appeared to block place preference. In the latter study, the memorydisrupting effects were only seen after four repeated treatments, consisting of memory reactivation tests followed by systemic MK-801, indicating that although long-term drug-related memories are less sensitive to interference, they can indeed be disrupted pharmacologically. Non-NMDA glutamate receptors, such as AMPA (α-amino-3hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid)-like receptors and the metabotropic glutamate receptor mGlu5 receptor, may also be involved in memory reconsolidation processes. In this regard, it was shown that administration of the non-NMDA-type glutamate receptor antagonist 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX) during retrieval eliminated retention of non-associative memory in the worm Caenorhabditis elegans (Rose and Rankin, 2006). Additionally, in 1-day old chicks, treatment with the mGlu5 receptor antagonist MPEP (2-methyl-6phenylethynyl pyridine hydrochloride) immediately after retrieval induced a transient impairment in passive avoidance performance (Salinska, 2006). 1.2. Noradrenaline

455

task as compared to non-learning counterparts (Cheng and Feenstra, 2006). More specifically, the dopamine D1 receptor is required for appetitive as well as aversive memory formation, since memory impairments have been reported after post-training injections with the dopamine D1 receptor antagonist SCH 23390 (Dalley et al., 2005; Izquierdo et al., 2007). Despite the well-established role of dopamine transmission in memory consolidation, dopaminergic mechanisms involved in reconsolidation of memory have not received much attention. In this respect, it was demonstrated that cocaine treatment after memory reactivation enhanced active avoidance memory in rats (Rodriguez et al., 1993). More recently, amphetamine has been shown to enhance reconsolidation of a morphine conditioned place preference (Blaiss and Janak, 2006), whereas performance on an appetitive classical conditioning task was not affected by a postretrieval amphetamine injection (Blaiss and Janak, 2007). Finally, it was shown that treatment with SCH23390 before retrieval impaired the subsequent expression of passive avoidance memory in chicks, indicating a role of dopamine D1 receptor involvement in memory reconsolidation (Sherry et al., 2005). However, it should be noted that since SCH23390 was administered prior to memory reactivation, this drug may have also affected memory retrieval by itself. Taken together, since the number of studies is rather limited, no firm conclusions can as yet be drawn regarding the exact role of dopamine neurotransmission in reconsolidation of memory.

Noradrenaline neurotransmission plays a critical role in learning and memory processes, as evidenced by post-training increases in noradrenaline release (Tomie et al., 2004; Tronel et al., 2004) and more specifically, β-adrenoceptor involvement in long-term protestation (Gelinas and Nguyen, 2005; Straube and Frey, 2003), and consolidation of memory (McGaugh, 2002). β-adrenergic receptor activation is also important for post-retrieval stabilization of memories, as systemic injections with the β-adrenoceptor antagonist propranolol impair expression of aversive memories in rats that received reactivation (Przybyslawski et al., 1999). Moreover, such memory-disrupting effect is still present 15 days after a single memory reactivation session followed by propranolol administration (Abrari et al., 2007) and was not reversed by a reminder shock (Abrari et al., 2007; Debiec and Ledoux, 2004). Administration of β-adrenoceptor antagonists after memory retrieval also induced retrograde amnesia in a spatial memory task (Przybyslawski et al., 1999; Roullet and Sara, 1998) and an instrumental task measuring context-induced reinstatement of sucrose seeking behaviour (Diergaarde et al., 2006), suggesting that appetitive memories also undergo β-adrenoceptor dependent reconsolidation. Recently, it was shown by means of a self-administration paradigm, that propranolol, when administered immediately upon memory reactivation, persistently disrupts reconsolidation of Pavlovian associations between environmental conditioned stimuli and appetitive reinforcers including sucrose and cocaine (Milton et al., 2008). Finally, reactivation of drug-related memories and concomitant propranolol administration disrupted subsequent expression of cocaine (Bernardi et al., 2006) and morphine conditioned place preference (Robinson and Franklin, 2007). Collectively, these findings indicate that disrupting drug-related memories by means of β-adrenoceptor antagonism may constitute a new approach for aiding relapse prevention.

It is well documented that endogenous acetylcholine release is implicated in long-term memory consolidation (Power et al., 2003). In support of this, it was shown that intracerebroventricular injections with hemicholinium, a choline uptake inhibitor, may impair longterm expression of inhibitory avoidance memory in mice when administered immediately after training. Furthermore, inhibitory avoidance memory was impaired when hemicholinium was given after memory reactivation, but not in hemicholinium-treated mice not receiving reactivation (Boccia et al., 2004). Collectively, these data indicate hat both consolidation and reconsolidation of this type of aversive memory involve an acetylcholine-dependent mechanism. Consolidation of contextual fear memory was also shown to be under cholinergic control, as evidenced by impaired performance after posttraining amygdala infusions with the muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine. However, post-reactivation blockade of the muscarinic receptor in the amygdala did not affect subsequent fear memory expression (Bucherelli et al., 2006). Hence, re-storage of contextual fear memory does not seem to involve activation of the muscarinergic receptor, at least not at the level of the amygdala. In contrast, it was shown that post-retrieval systemic injections with scopolamine one week after training disrupted expression of a conditioned place preference for cocaine. These results indicate that, as opposed to contextual fear memory, post-retrieval stabilization of cocaine-associated contextual memory may indeed involve activation of muscarinergic receptors (Kelley et al., 2007). Further research, however, should clarify whether muscarinergic receptor antagonists may also disrupt drug memory reconsolidation in a drug selfadministration paradigm.

1.3. Dopamine

1.5. γ-Aminobutyric acid (GABA)

Whereas traditionally, dopamine neurotransmission has been implicated in reward and reinforcement, recent research suggests that this neurotransmitter is also critically involved in associative learning and memory. For example, medial prefrontal dopamine levels have been reported to rise during the early, but not later learning phases of an appetitive instrumental task (van der Meulen et al., 2007). Employing the same paradigm, dopamine release within the nucleus accumbens was found to be highest in rats that learned this

Drugs acting at the GABAA receptor are widely prescribed to treat anxiety, insomnia, or epilepsy. Despite the wealth of studies showing that these drugs can induce amnesia, little is known regarding the involvement of this neurotransmitter in memory reconsolidation processes. In a recent study however, systemic injection with midazolam (a GABAA receptor agonist) after reexposure to the training context in rats, significantly reduced contextual freezing as compared to vehicle-treated control rats. This effect appeared to be long-lasting,

1.4. Acetylcholine

456

L. Diergaarde et al. / European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457

as no spontaneous recovery of contextual fear memory was observed (Bustos et al., 2006). In another study, muscimol inactivation of the hippocampus upon memory reactivation disrupted performance on an inhibitory avoidance task (Amaral et al., 2007). The reported effect on memory was transient and additional studies are needed to verify whether the GABAA receptor constitutes another drug target for interfering with traumatic memories. 1.6. Glucocorticoids It is well established that glucocorticoid hormones may profoundly affect mnemonic functioning, including storage and retrieval of memory (Roozendaal, 2003). Thus, recent experiments suggest that reconsolidation of aversive memories can be disrupted by interfering with glucocorticoid receptor signalling, as indicated by long-term impairments of inhibitory avoidance upon post-retrieval amygdala infusions of the glucocorticoid receptor antagonist RU38486 (Tronel and Alberini, 2007). Furthermore, fear memory did not re-emerge after a reminder shock was administered, suggesting that the effect of glucocorticoid receptor antagonists on reconsolidation of fear-related memory may be persistent. Similar data were obtained in a study of Jin et al., that showed that RU486 infused in the amygdala impaired longterm auditory fear memory when memory was reactivated 10 days post-training (Jin et al., 2007). Retrograde amnesia has also been reported upon post-retrieval administration of the endogenous stress hormone corticosterone (Abrari et al., 2007; Cai et al., 2006). However, the authors suggested that rather than reflecting a disruption of memory reconsolidation, the observed reduction in freezing behaviour should be interpreted as enhanced memory extinction, which is consistent with previous findings (Yang et al., 2006). 1.7. Cannabinoids In humans, acute administration of marijuana and its psychoactive constituents (i.e. cannabinoids) results in impaired encoding, consolidation and retrieval of memory (Ranganathan and D'Souza, 2006). In line with this, activation of the cannabinoid-1 (CB1) receptor by infusion of WIN55,212-2 into the insular cortex impaired acquisition of conditioned taste aversion memory (Kobilo et al., 2007). This report further showed that post-retrieval WIN55212-2 infusion into the insular cortex blocked expression of conditioned taste aversion memory, indicating that stimulating the brain endocannabinoid system disrupts both consolidation and reconsolidation of aversive memories. This was confirmed by a study showing that bilateral infusion of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor agonist WIN55212-2 or HU210 into the amygdala after memory retrieval, dose-dependently disrupted reconsolidation of fear memory, an effect that was reversed by co-administration of the cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonist AM251. However, post-retrieval administration of cannabinoid CB1 receptor antagonists alone does not seem to affect reconsolidation of aversive memory, since unimpaired memory retention was observed after post-retrieval intra-amygdala AM251 infusions (Lin et al., 2006), or SR141716A either when given systemically after memory reactivation (Suzuki, et al., 2004) or when infused into the insular cortex (Kobilo et al., 2007). In contrast, Bucherelli et al. showed that, blockade of cannabinoid CB1 receptors in the amygdala following retrieval by a low dose of AM251, interferes with reconsolidation of contextual fear memory (Bucherelli et al., 2006). Given these inconsistent findings, together with the lack of studies involving appetitive learning paradigms, additional experiments are needed to address the precise role of the endocannabinoid system in post-retrieval processes. 2. Concluding remarks Behavioural pharmacological experiments, such as those reviewed in this article, have contributed significantly to our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms involved in memory reconsolidation.

Especially the β-adrenoceptor and the NMDA receptor seem to play a key role in post-retrieval modification and stabilization of aversive as well as appetitive memories. Although the number of reconsolidation studies focusing on drugs acting at the dopamine D1-like receptor and the cannabinoid CB1 receptor, is rather limited, extensive evidence exists that both dopaminergic and cannabinoid mechanisms are involved in various aspects of memory processing, warranting further research in this area. More importantly, drugs interfering with memory reconsolidation may be of therapeutical relevance, by inducing retrograde amnesia for traumatic memories (post-traumatic stress disorder) or for drugassociated memories that can precipitate relapse (e.g. conditioned stimuli and drug-associated contexts), and as such may potentially represent a treatment for these disorders. In this respect, drugs acting at the β-adrenoceptor deserve special attention, since these drugs are already widely prescribed. Promising results were recently reported showing that post-retrieval propranolol effectively reduced physiological responding during subsequent mental imagery of a trauma (Brunet et al., 2008). This indicates that β-adrenoceptor antagonists can also disrupt memory reconsolidation in humans, and as such β-adrenoceptors may represent a promising target for the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. In relation to psychiatric disorders, it is particularly relevant to know whether relatively old memories are still susceptible to postretrieval amnesia, since several experiments have already shown that memories become increasingly impervious to pharmacological manipulation as they grow older (Milekic and Alberini, 2002; Suzuki et al., 2004). Furthermore, some studies indicate that pharmacological interference with memory reconsolidation induces long-term mnemonic disruption (Duvarci and Nader, 2004; Milekic et al., 2007; Sacchetti et al., 2007), whereas others indicate that post-reactivation amnesia phenomenon recovers over time, and thus, may reflect a temporary memory deficit (Lattal and Abel, 2004; Prado-Alcala et al., 2006). Collectively, future studies should take more into account the temporary nature of memory reconsolidation and involve multiple retention tests, since post-traumatic stress disorder is characterized by repeated recollections of unwanted traumatic memories and also because (pharmaco)therapy is mostly implemented long after the original memory has been formed. Studies focusing on pharmacological interference with fear memory reconsolidation mainly involve one-trial training protocols, which may bear relevance for a single traumatic event. However, with respect to drug addiction, this psychiatric condition involves multiple drug-associated memories, often formed, reactivated and restabilized over a period of many years. Hence, behavioural paradigms in which the rats are exposed to multiple drug-associated stimuli over a long period (i.e. drug self-administration paradigms) are of major importance in determining whether certain pharmacological agents, by interfering with memory reconsolidation, may ultimately reduce the impact of relapse-provoking drug-associated stimuli in drug-dependent individuals and ultimately diminish the risk of relapse. References Abrari, K., Rashidy-Pour, A., Semnanian, S., Fathollahi, Y., 2008. Administration of corticosterone after memory reactivation disrupts subsequent retrieval of a contextual conditioned fear memory: dependence upon training intensity. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 89, 178–184. Amaral, O.B., Luft, T., Cammarota, M., Izquierdo, I., Roesler, R., 2007. Temporary inactivation of the dorsal hippocampus induces a transient impairment in retrieval of aversive memory. Behav. Brain Res. 180, 113–118. Ben Mamou, C., Gamache, K., Nader, K., 2006. NMDA receptors are critical for unleashing consolidated auditory fear memories. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1237–1239. Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Okuda, S., Roozendaal, B., McGaugh, J.L., 2005. Insular cortex is involved in consolidation of object recognition memory. Learn. Mem. 12, 447–449. Bernardi, R.E., Lattal, K.M., Berger, S.P., 2006. Postretrieval propranolol disrupts a cocaine conditioned place preference. Neuroreport 17, 1443–1447. Blaiss, C.A., Janak, P.H., 2007. Post-training, but not post-reactivation, administration of amphetamine and anisomycin modulates Pavlovian conditioned approach. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 644–658.

L. Diergaarde et al. / European Journal of Pharmacology 585 (2008) 453-457 Blaiss, C.A., Janak, P.H., 2006. Post-training and post-reactivation administration of amphetamine enhances morphine conditioned place preference. Behav. Brain Res. 171, 329–337. Boccia, M.M., Acosta, G.B., Blake, M.G., Baratti, C.M., 2004. Memory consolidation and reconsolidation of an inhibitory avoidance response in mice: effects of i.c.v. injections of hemicholinium-3. Neuroscience 124, 735–741. Brunet, A., Orr, S.P., Tremblay, J., Robertson, K., Nader, K., Pitman, R.K., 2008. Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 503–506. Bucherelli, C., Baldi, E., Mariottini, C., Passani, M.B., Blandina, P., 2006. Aversive memory reactivation engages in the amygdala only some neurotransmitters involved in consolidation. Learn. Mem. 13, 426–430. Bustos, S.G., Maldonado, H., Molina, V.A., 2006. Midazolam disrupts fear memory reconsolidation. Neuroscience 139, 831–842. Cai, W.H., Blundell, J., Han, J., Greene, R.W., Powell, C.M., 2006. Postreactivation glucocorticoids impair recall of established fear memory. J. Neurosci. 26, 9560–9566. Cheng, J., Feenstra, M.G., 2006. Individual differences in dopamine efflux in nucleus accumbens shell and core during instrumental learning. Learn. Mem. 13, 168–177. Dalley, J.W., Laane, K., Theobald, D.E., Armstrong, H.C., Corlett, P.R., Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., 2005. Time-limited modulation of appetitive Pavlovian memory by D1 and NMDA receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 102, 6189–6194. Debiec, J., Ledoux, J.E., 2004. Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience 129, 267–272. Diergaarde, L., Schoffelmeer, A.N., De Vries, T.J., 2006. Beta-adrenoceptor mediated inhibition of long-term reward-related memory reconsolidation. Behav. Brain Res. 170, 333–336. Duvarci, S., Nader, K., 2004. Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. J. Neurosci. 24, 9269–9275. Gelinas, J.N., Nguyen, P.V., 2005. Beta-adrenergic receptor activation facilitates induction of a protein synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci 25, 3294–3303. Izquierdo, L.A., Barros, D.M., da Costa, J.C., Furini, C., Zinn, C., Cammarota, M., Bevilaqua, L.R., Izquierdo, I., 2007. A link between role of two prefrontal areas in immediate memory and in long-term memory consolidation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 88, 160–166. Jin, X.C., Lu, Y.F., Yang, X.F., Ma, L., Li, B.M., 2007. Glucocorticoid receptors in the basolateral nucleus of amygdala are required for postreactivation reconsolidation of auditory fear memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3702–3712. Kelley, J.B., Anderson, K.L., Itzhak, Y., 2007. Long-term memory of cocaine-associated context: disruption and reinstatement. Neuroreport. 18, 777–780. Kobilo, T., Hazvi, S., Dudai, Y., 2007. Role of cortical cannabinoid CB1 receptor in conditioned taste aversion memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3417–3421. Lattal, K.M., Abel, T., 2004. Behavioral impairments caused by injections of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin after contextual retrieval reverse with time. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 101, 4667–4672. Lee, J.L., Milton, A.L., Everitt, B.J., 2006. Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: inhibition and potentiation. J. Neurosci 26, 10051–10056. Lin, H.C., Mao, S.C., Gean, P.W., 2006. Effects of intra-amygdala infusion of CB1 receptor agonists on the reconsolidation of fear-potentiated startle. Learn. Mem. 13, 316–321. McGaugh, J.L., 2002. Memory consolidation and the amygdala: a systems perspective. Trends Neurosci. 25, 456. Milekic, M.H., Alberini, C.M., 2002. Temporally graded requirement for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron 36, 521–525. Milekic, M.H., Pollonini, G., Alberini, C.M., 2007. Temporal requirement of C/EBPbeta in the amygdala following reactivation but not acquisition of inhibitory avoidance. Learn. Mem. 14, 504–511. Milton, A.L., Lee, J.L., Everitt, B.J., 2008. Reconsolidation of appetitive memories for both natural and drug reinforcement is dependent on {beta}-adrenergic receptors. Learn. Mem. 15, 88–92.

457

Power, A.E., Vazdarjanova, A., McGaugh, J.L., 2003. Muscarinic cholinergic influences in memory consolidation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 80, 178–193. Prado-Alcala, R.A., Diaz Del Guante, M.A., Garin-Aguilar, M.E., Diaz-Trujillo, A., Quirarte, G.L., McGaugh, J.L., 2006. Amygdala or hippocampus inactivation after retrieval induces temporary memory deficit. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86, 144–149. Przybyslawski, J., Roullet, P., Sara, S.J., 1999. Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: role of beta adrenergic receptors. J. Neurosci. 19, 6623–6628. Przybyslawski, J., Sara, S.J., 1997. Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behav. Brain Res. 84, 241–246. Ranganathan, M., D'Souza, D.C., 2006. The acute effects of cannabinoids on memory in humans: a review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 188, 425–444. Robinson, M.J., Franklin, K.B., 2007. Central but not peripheral beta-adrenergic antagonism blocks reconsolidation for a morphine place preference. Behav. Brain Res. 182, 129–134. Rodriguez, W.A., Rodriguez, S.B., Phillips, M.Y., Martinez Jr., J.L., 1993. Post-reactivation cocaine administration facilitates later acquisition of an avoidance response in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 59, 125–129. Roozendaal, B., 2003. Systems mediating acute glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation and retrieval. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psych. 27, 1213–1223. Rose, J.K., Rankin, C.H., 2006. Blocking memory reconsolidation reverses memoryassociated changes in glutamate receptor expression. J. Neurosci 26, 11582–11587. Roullet, P., Sara, S., 1998. Consolidation of memory after its reactivation: involvement of beta noradrenergic receptors in the late phase. Neural Plast. 6, 63–68. Sacchetti, B., Sacco, T., Strata, P., 2007. Reversible inactivation of amygdala and cerebellum but not perirhinal cortex impairs reactivated fear memories. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 2875–2884. Sadler, R., Herzig, V., Schmidt, W.J., 2007. Repeated treatment with the NMDA antagonist MK-801 disrupts reconsolidation of memory for amphetamine-conditioned place preference. Behav. Pharmacol. 18, 699–703. Salinska, E., 2006. The role of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in memory consolidation and reconsolidation in the passive avoidance task in 1-day-old chicks. Neurochem. Int. 48, 447–452. Sherry, J.M., Hale, M.W., Crowe, S.F., 2005. The effects of the dopamine D1 receptor antagonist SCH23390 on memory reconsolidation following reminder-activated retrieval in day-old chicks. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 83, 104–112. Straube, T., Frey, J.U., 2003. Involvement of beta-adrenergic receptors in protein synthesis-dependent late long-term potentiation (LTP) in the dentate gyrus of freely moving rats: the critical role of the LTP induction strength. Neuroscience 119, 473–479. Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S.A., Frankland, P.W., Masushige, S., Silva, A.J., Kida, S., 2004. Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. J. Neurosci. 24, 4787–4795. Tomie, A., Tirado, A.D., Yu, L., Pohorecky, L.A., 2004. Pavlovian autoshaping procedures increase plasma corticosterone and levels of norepinephrine and serotonin in prefrontal cortex in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 153, 97–105. Torras-Garcia, M., Lelong, J., Tronel, S., Sara, S.J., 2005. Reconsolidation after remembering an odor-reward association requires NMDA receptors. Learn. Mem. 12, 18–22. Tronel, S., Alberini, C.M., 2007. Persistent disruption of a traumatic memory by postretrieval inactivation of glucocorticoid receptors in the amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry. 62, 33–39. Tronel, S., Feenstra, M.G., Sara, S.J., 2004. Noradrenergic action in prefrontal cortex in the late stage of memory consolidation. Learn. Mem. 11, 453–458. Tronson, N.C., Taylor, J.R., 2007. Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 8, 262–275. van der Meulen, J.A., Joosten, R.N., de Bruin, J.P., Feenstra, M.G., 2007. Dopamine and noradrenaline efflux in the medial prefrontal cortex during serial reversals and extinction of instrumental goal-directed behavior. Cereb. Cortex. 17, 1444–1453. Yang, Y.L., Chao, P.K., Lu, K.T., 2006. Systemic and intra-amygdala administration of glucocorticoid agonist and antagonist modulate extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 912–924.