Accepted Manuscript Post-Illumination Pupil Response after blue light: reliability of optimized melanopsinbased phototransduction assessment Wisse P. van der Meijden, MSc, Bart H.W. te Lindert, MSc, Denise Bijlenga, PhD, Joris E. Coppens, PhD, Germán Gómez-Herrero, PhD, Jessica Bruijel, MSc, J.J. Sandra Kooij, MD, PhD, Christian Cajochen, PhD, Patrice Bourgin, MD, PhD, Eus J.W. Van Someren, PhD PII:
S0014-4835(15)00236-5
DOI:
10.1016/j.exer.2015.07.010
Reference:
YEXER 6725
To appear in:
Experimental Eye Research
Received Date: 11 May 2015 Revised Date:
26 June 2015
Accepted Date: 13 July 2015
Please cite this article as: van der Meijden, W.P., te Lindert, B.H.W., Bijlenga, D., Coppens, J.E., Gómez-Herrero, G., Bruijel, J., Kooij, J.J.S., Cajochen, C., Bourgin, P., Van Someren, E.J.W., PostIllumination Pupil Response after blue light: reliability of optimized melanopsin-based phototransduction assessment, Experimental Eye Research (2015), doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2015.07.010. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
Post-Illumination Pupil Response after blue light: reliability of optimized melanopsin-
2
based phototransduction assessment
3
Wisse P. van der Meijden, MSc 1 ; Bart H.W. te Lindert, MSc 1 ; Denise Bijlenga, PhD 2 ;
5
Joris E. Coppens, PhD 1 ; Germán Gómez-Herrero, PhD 1 ; Jessica Bruijel, MSc 1 ; J.J. Sandra
6
Kooij, MD, PhD 2 ; Christian Cajochen, PhD 3; Patrice Bourgin, MD, PhD 4; Eus J.W. Van
7
Someren, PhD 1,5
RI PT
4
8 1
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Dept. Sleep and Cognition, Amsterdam, The
SC
9
Netherlands.
10 2
PsyQ Psycho-Medical Programs, Program Adult ADHD, The Hague, The Netherlands.
12
3
Center for Chronobiology, Psychiatric Hospital of the University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
13 4
Integrative Neurosciences, University of Strasbourg, Strasbourg, France
15 16
Sleep Disorders Center, CHU and FMTS, CNRS-UPR 3212, Institute of Cellular and
5
TE D
14
M AN U
11
Depts. of Integrative Neurophysiology and Medical Psychology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, VU University and
18
Medical Center, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
19
EP
17
Correspondence: Wisse van der Meijden, MSc, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Dept.
21
Sleep and Cognition, Meibergdreef 47, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Phone: +31
22
20 566 5492. Mail:
[email protected] .
23 24 25
AC C
20
Conflict of interest: No conflicting relationship exists for any author.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 26
Abstract
27
Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells have recently been shown highly relevant to the
29
non-image forming effects of light, through their direct projections on brain circuits that
30
regulate alertness, mood and circadian rhythms. A quantitative assessment of functionality of
31
the melanopsin-signaling pathway could be highly relevant in order to mechanistically
32
understand individual differences in the effects of light on these regulatory systems. We here
33
propose and validate a reliable quantification of the melanopsin-dependent Post-Illumination
34
Pupil Response (PIPR) after blue light, and evaluated its sensitivity to dark adaptation, time of
35
day, body posture, and light exposure history. Pupil diameter of the left eye was continuously
36
measured during a series of light exposures to the right eye, of which the pupil was dilated
37
using tropicamide 0.5%. The light exposure paradigm consisted of the following five
38
consecutive blocks of five minutes: baseline dark; monochromatic red light (bandwidth: 630
39
nm, luminance: 375 cd/m2) to maximize the effect of subsequent blue light; dark;
40
monochromatic blue light (bandwidth: 470 nm, luminance: 375 cd/m2); and post-blue dark.
41
PIPR was quantified as the difference between baseline dark pupil diameter and post-blue
42
dark pupil diameter (PIPR-mm). In addition, a relative PIPR was calculated by dividing PIPR
43
by baseline pupil diameter (PIPR-%). In total 54 PIPR assessments were obtained in 25
44
healthy young adults (10 males, mean age ± SD: 26.9 ± 4.0 yr). From repeated measurements
45
on two consecutive days in 15 of the 25 participants (6 males, mean age ± SD: 27.8 ± 4.3 yrs)
46
test-retest reliability of both PIPR outcome parameters was calculated. In the presence of
47
considerable between-subject differences, both outcome parameters had very high test-retest
48
reliability: Cronbach’s α > .90 and Intraclass Correlation Coefficient > .85. In 12 of the 25
49
participants (6 males, mean age ± SD: 26.5 ± 3.6 yr) we examined the potential confounding
50
effects of dark adaptation, time of the day (morning vs. afternoon), body posture (upright vs.
51
supine position), and 24-h environmental light history on the PIPR assessment. Mixed effect
52
regression models were used to analyze these possible confounders. A supine position caused
53
larger PIPR-mm (β = 0.29 mm, SE = 0.10, p = 0.01) and PIPR-% (β = 4.34 %, SE = 1.69, p =
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 0.02), which was due to an increase in baseline dark pupil diameter; this finding is of
55
relevance for studies requiring a supine posture, as in functional Magnetic Resonance
56
Imaging, constant routine protocols and bed-ridden patients. There were no effects of dark
57
adaptation, time of day, and light history. In conclusion, the presented method provides a
58
reliable and robust assessment of the PIPR to allow for studies on individual differences in
59
melanopsin-based phototransduction and effects of interventions.
60
RI PT
54
Keywords
62
Pupil, intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells, melanopsin, light, test-retest
63
reliability.
M AN U
SC
61
64
Abbreviations
66
PIPR
Post-Illumination Pupil Response
67
ipRGC
intrinsically photosensitive Retinal Ganglion Cell
68
ICC
Intraclass Correlation Coefficient
AC C
EP
TE D
65
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 69
1. Introduction
70
Light reaching the retina of the eyes does not only provide the brain with images of the
72
environment, but also generates several non-image forming effects. These include constriction
73
of the pupil diameter, changes in the arousal level of the brain, and entrainment of the
74
biological clock of the brain to the environmental 24-hour light-dark cycle. The observation
75
that this circadian photoentrainment was preserved in some blind individuals (Czeisler et al.,
76
1995) as well as in mice lacking rods and cones (Freedman et al., 1999; Lucas et al., 1999)
77
has led to the discovery of an entirely new photoreceptor system. Indeed, we now know that a
78
small subset of retinal ganglion cells express an opsin/vitamin A-based photopigment, called
79
melanopsin. This photopigment is maximally sensitive to short wavelengths (peak sensitivity
80
~ 480 nm) and render these cells intrinsically photosensitive (Berson et al., 2002; Brainard et
81
al., 2001). Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) were demonstrated to
82
be strongly involved in the mentioned non-image forming effects of light on pupil diameter
83
(Lucas et al., 2001), enhancement of mood and alertness (Lockley et al., 2006), and
84
modulation of circadian rhythms (Thapan et al., 2001). Given these strong and important
85
effects, a quantitative assessment of functionality of the melanopsin-signaling pathway could
86
be highly relevant in order to mechanistically understand individual differences in the effects
87
of light on the regulation of mood, alertness and circadian rhythms.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
88
RI PT
71
89
The pupillary light reflex may provide the most feasible non-invasive method to assess
90
functionality of the melanopsin-signaling pathway. The reflex is mediated through direct
91
connections between ipRGCs and the Olivary Pretectal Nucleus (Hattar et al., 2006), the
92
nucleus that controls pupil size (Trejo and Cicerone, 1984). However, this reflex is not
93
exclusively driven the melanopsin-signaling pathway, but also highly dependent on the input
94
from rods and cones (Lall et al., 2010). Still, there is one characteristic of the pupillary light
95
reflex that is specific to the melanopsin-signaling pathway. In contrast to rods and cones,
96
ipRGCs show a delayed repolarization after light offset, resulting in a sustained pupil
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 97
constriction. This phenomenon has been dubbed ‘post-illumination pupil response’ (PIPR)
98
(Dacey et al., 2005). The PIPR that can be recorded following exposure to bright blue light is
99
almost entirely attributable to ipRGC activity (Gamlin et al., 2007; Markwell et al., 2010) and can therefore be used to estimate functioning of the melanopsin signaling pathway (Park et
101
al., 2011). Several studies suggest that the processing of light by the ipRGC may be altered in
102
disorders including diabetes type II (Feigl et al., 2012), neuroretinal visual loss (Kardon et al.,
103
2009), glaucoma (Kankipati et al., 2011), and seasonal mood disorder (Roecklein et al.,
104
2013). In order to be of value in case-control, intervention, and mechanistic studies, it is of
105
great importance to assess the PIPR according to a maximally reliable standardized protocol.
106
Test-retest reliability of PIPR assessment has previously been evaluated for two other specific
107
protocols and was rated as moderate to high (Herbst et al., 2011; Lei et al., 2015). The light
108
stimuli in these two paradigms were of short duration (i.e., 400 ms and 20 s). In view of the
109
characteristic low sensitivity and slow kinetics of ipRGCs, however, longer stimulus duration
110
allows for more specific assessment of the melanopsin-signaling pathway (Berson et al.,
111
2002; Do et al., 2009). Accordingly, previous animal work showed that phase shifts in
112
circadian rhythms were larger with a 300-s light stimulus compared to light stimuli with a
113
shorter duration (Nelson and Takahashi, 1991). In addition, these circadian phase shift effects
114
did not grow any further with extending the light stimulus beyond 300 s. Others explained this
115
saturation effect by showing that light adaptation of ipRGCs was completed after 300 s of
116
light exposure (Wong et al., 2005). We therefore here propose a both feasible and reliable
117
PIPR assessment protocol using prolonged light exposure with a duration of 300 s.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
118
RI PT
100
119
We first assessed the within-subject between-day test-retest reliability. Because the pupil
120
response to light is dependent on many inputs, in part originating from the autonomic nervous
121
system (Heller et al., 1990), we moreover addressed sensitivity of our PIPR assessment
122
protocol to four possible confounders: 1) Dark adaptation, to check whether the eyes were
123
dark adapted and pupil diameter was stabilized prior to the light exposure (PIPR is quantified
124
relative to pre-exposure pupil diameter); 2) Time of day (i.e., morning vs. afternoon), to test
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT whether the protocol provides similar estimates across office hours; 3) Body posture, to check
126
whether the test could be applied in both upright and supine position (i.e., application in bed-
127
ridden participants and in magnetic resonance imaging environments (Chellappa et al.,
128
2014)); 4) Environmental light history, to confirm that the outcome measures were unaffected
129
by previous light exposure, which is of relevance with respect to planning of experimental
130
and clinical evaluations. Previous studies (Gooley et al., 2012; Mure et al., 2009; Mure et al.,
131
2007; Wong et al., 2005) showed an effect of short-term light history on pupil response. We
132
here add to these findings by assessing also long-term effects of prior light exposure (i.e.,
133
from 24-h prior to the test).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
125
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 134
2. Methods
135
To evaluate and validate our PIPR protocol, two experiments were performed, using similar
137
light exposure and pupillometry procedures. The aim of the first experiment was to estimate
138
the within-subject, between-day test-retest reliability. The second experiment aimed to
139
evaluate possible effects of dark adaptation, time of day, body posture, and environmental
140
light history on the outcomes of the PIPR protocol.
141
RI PT
136
2.1 Participants
143
In total 25 healthy young adults were recruited by advertisement and word of mouth. These
144
25 participants were distributed over the two experiments as follows: 2 of the 25 of
145
participated in both experiments, 13 of the 25 in experiment 1 only, and 10 of the 25 solely in
146
experiment 2. All participants were in good health, free of medication, non-smoking, and had
147
neither sleep complaints nor a history of ocular pathology, as indicated by the Duke clinical
148
Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders (Edinger et al., 2004). All participants worked
149
regular office hours and did not travel across time-zones for at least a month prior to
150
participation. Results from the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ) showed that none
151
of the participants was an extreme chronotype (mean mid-sleep on free days ± SD: 5:05 AM
152
± 1:05) and all in the center part of the normative distribution for the age range of our
153
participants (5:00 AM ± 1:23) (Zavada et al., 2005). According to Nagel anomaloscope tests
154
none of the participants suffered from color vision deficiency. Participants received oral and
155
written information on the study, signed informed consent before study participation, and did
156
not receive any incentive. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the
157
VU University Medical Center Amsterdam (protocol NL43319.029.13) and adhered to the
158
tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
142
159 160
2.2 Light exposure protocol
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Our light exposure protocol was designed to obtain a prolonged steady-state PIPR after blue
162
light, with a high signal-to-noise ratio (Mure et al., 2009). In order to obtain maximal
163
stimulation of the melanopsin-based phototransduction system, the pupil of the right eye was
164
dilated using 0.5% tropicamide (Nissen et al., 2011). In accordance with previous work (Mure
165
et al., 2009; Mure et al., 2007), the right eye was first pre-exposed to five minutes of bright
166
monochromatic red light (LED Cree C503B-RAS, Durham, NC, USA; peak wavelength (full
167
width half maximum): 630 (20) nm; luminance: 375 cd/m2) in order to maximize the PIPR
168
after blue light (supplementary material, Figure S1). The right eye was subsequently exposed
169
to 5 minutes of bright monochromatic blue light (LED Cree C503B-BAN, Durham, NC,
170
USA; peak wavelength (full width half maximum): 470 (20) nm; luminance: 375 cd/m2).
171
Wavelength and luminance of the light stimuli were calibrated using a spectrometer
172
(AvaSpec-3648-USB2, Avantes, Apeldoorn, The Netherlands). Both monochromatic lights
173
were transmitted through a diffuser and presented in free view. There were 5-minute blocks of
174
darkness before (here labeled ‘baseline’), between, and after (‘post blue’) the light blocks
175
(Figure 1).
176
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
161
2.3 Pupillometry
178
Participants had little to no experience with ophthalmological tests and were naïve to the
179
experimental paradigm (i.e., there was no rehearsal trial). They were placed in front of a
180
custom-made infrared pupillometry set-up built around a printed circuit board charge coupled
181
device camera (Sony d2463r, Sony Electronics Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Participants were
182
asked to focus on a fixation target, integrated in the set-up in front of their left eye. This
183
fixation target was projected at infinity to prevent accommodation. The eyes were separated
184
by a septum. The pupil diameter of the left eye was measured throughout the entire protocol
185
using infrared radiation. The pupil was illuminated from the lateral side with an 880 nm
186
infrared radiation emitting diode, in such a way that the pupil appeared as a black disk in a
187
bright iris on the camera. Visible radiation was blocked by a Wratten 87 gelatin filter (Kodak,
188
Rochester, NY, USA). The images were digitized at 25 Hz with a USB frame grabber
AC C
EP
177
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT (Grabby, Terratec, Alsdorf, Germany) and analyzed real time with custom software written in
190
C++ using the OpenCV image analysis library (Itseez, Nizhny Novgorod, Russia). Missing
191
data points (e.g. due to eye blinks) were interpolated using nearest neighbors interpolation.
192
Pupil diameter was assessed continuously from the baseline block until the post-blue block.
193
During baseline darkness the pupil diameter remained stable over the entire block. During
194
post-blue darkness we observed in several assessments that during the first minute after light
195
offset the pupil first dilated to an intermediate level before it constricted again to a level at
196
which the constriction was sustained (Figure 1). Similar pupil behavior has been shown
197
previously after cessation of bright light stimuli with durations of 2 minutes (Newsome, 1971)
198
and 3 minutes (Alpern and Campbell, 1963). These complicated dynamics are a result of the
199
interaction between the image forming and non-image forming photoreceptors after light
200
offset (Dacey et al., 2005; Gamlin et al., 2007). Another between-trial variation was observed
201
during the final minute of the post-blue block: in most trials the pupil constriction was
202
maintained over the entire minute, but in some trials the pupil already started redilating
203
towards baseline size within this minute. In view of these observed inter-assessment
204
differences in pupil dynamics, the first and last minute of the post-blue block were excluded
205
from the analysis: we used the averaged pupil diameter over minutes 2 to 4 of the post-blue
206
block. In order to optimize the comparison between the baseline and post-blue dark block we
207
decided to use equal metrics for both blocks. Accordingly, we calculated the averaged pupil
208
diameter over minutes 2 to 4 of the baseline block. From the baseline and post-blue pupil
209
diameter we calculated two PIPR outcome parameters (Kankipati et al., 2011; Roecklein et
210
al., 2013).
212
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
211
RI PT
189
1. PIPR-mm
= baseline pupil diameter – post-blue pupil diameter
2. PIPR-%
= 100 * PIPR-mm / baseline pupil diameter
213 214
2.4 Experiment 1
215 216
2.4.1 Procedures
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Fifteen participants (6 males, 9 females, mean age ± SD: 27.8 ± 4.3 yr) underwent the PIPR
218
assessment in upright position twice on consecutive days. Both tests within one participant
219
were at the same time of day. Between participants this time point ranged from 09:00 AM to
220
16:30 PM. Measurements were performed using the same set-up at two different locations:
221
ten participants (5 males, 5 females, mean age ± SD: 26.1 ± 3.3 yr) were assessed at the
222
Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience in Amsterdam and five (1 male, 4 females, mean age ±
223
SD: 31.2 ± 4.1 yr) at PsyQ Expertise Center Adult ADHD in The Hague.
RI PT
217
224
2.4.2 Statistical analysis
226
Mixed effect regression models were used to assess whether between-subject differences in
227
PIPR-mm and PIPR-% were confounded by the time point of measurement and the
228
assessment location. Mixed effect models are optimally suited to account for nested data
229
structures. The data were structured in a 2-level hierarchy: PIPR outcome parameters were
230
measured in two assessments that were nested in fifteen participants. Time point and location
231
were included in the model as regressors. The significance of their estimated effects was
232
evaluated using the Wald test and Likelihood-ratio tests were performed to compare models
233
(Twisk, 2013).
234
Cronbach’s α was calculated to examine the within-subject reliability of the PIPR assessment
235
(Cronbach, 1951). Values of Cronbach’s α > .90 are considered as satisfactory for clinical
236
application (Bland and Altman, 1997). To examine test-retest reliability, we computed the
237
two-way random effects single measures intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for absolute
238
agreement (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979). Bland-Altman plots were made to visually inspect test-
239
retest reliability (Bland and Altman, 1986). Data processing was conducted using MATLAB
240
(Version R2013A, The MathWork Inc, Natick, MA). Statistical analyses were conducted
241
using the software packages ‘lme4’ (Bates et al., 2013), ‘cocron’ (Diedenhofen, 2013), and
242
‘ICC’ (Wolak et al., 2012) for R (Version 3.1.1, R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
243
Vienna, Austria).
244
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
225
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 245
2.5 Experiment 2
246
2.5.1 Procedures
248
Twelve participants (6 males, 6 females, mean age ± SD: 26.5 ± 3.6 yr) underwent the PIPR
249
assessment twice with 3 days between assessments. An RGB multiband light sensor
250
(Dimesimeter, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, Troy, NY, USA) (Bierman et al., 2005),
251
integrated in a brooch, was worn to assess environmental light spectrum and intensity
252
exposure history over 24 h prior to the start of each assessment. The ability of the light sensor
253
to measure light in multiple bands enabled us to take the composition of the light into
254
account. The separate output values from the red, green, and blue band were weighted, based
255
on the sensitivity of the ipRGC network for each bandwidth, and integrated into a single light
256
exposure parameter, which was quantified as: irradiance, spectrally weighted for effects on
257
circadian rhythm (weighted W/m2) (Rea et al., 2005).
258
To investigate effects of dark adaptation duration and stabilization of baseline pupil diameter,
259
the start of the light exposure protocol was delayed by either 0, 5, or 10 minutes in darkness
260
(0 cd/m2), randomly assigned to the different participants, resulting in dark adaptation
261
durations of 5, 10 or 15 minutes. If the eyes are sufficiently adapted to the dark environment
262
within 5 minutes, baseline pupil diameter would not change with further extension of the dark
263
exposure duration. Otherwise, ongoing dark adaptation would result in a larger baseline pupil
264
diameter with increasing delay.
265
To investigate potential time-of-day effects of the PIPR outcomes during office hours, one
266
trial started in the morning (09:00 AM) and the other in the afternoon (01:00 PM). To assess
267
the effect of body posture on the PIPR outcomes, one assessment was performed in upright
268
(i.e. sitting) and the other in supine position. In supine position the pupillometry set-up was
269
placed above the participant’s head in such a way that the angle and distance relative to the
270
eyes were similar to the upright position. The orders of posture and time of day were
271
counterbalanced across participants. At the start of each trial, participants were placed in the
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
247
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 272
upright or supine position. Room lights were dimmed for 30 minutes (0.5 cd/m2) and
273
subsequently the light exposure protocol was commenced.
274
2.5.2 Statistical analysis
276
Mixed effect regression models were used to analyze the repeated assessments of PIPR-mm
277
and PIPR-%, and the effects of dark adaptation, time of day, posture and light history. To
278
dissect pre- and post-exposure effects, the same models were performed on baseline and post-
279
blue pupil diameter. The assessed data represented a 2-level hierarchy: variables were
280
measured in two assessments that were nested in twelve participants. Dark adaptation
281
duration, time of day, posture, and 24-h environmental light history were added to the model
282
as regressors. All analyses were performed using the R-package ‘lme4’.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
275
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 283
3. Results
284 285
3.1 Experiment 1
286
Interindividual differences in PIPR outcome measures were confounded neither by time point
288
of measurement (PIPR-mm: β = 0.09, SE = 0.10, P = 0.37; PIPR-%: β = 0.9, SE = 1.0, P =
289
0.38) nor by assessment location (PIPR-mm: β = 0.49, SE = 0.52, P = 0.36; PIPR-%: β = 7.8,
290
SE = 5.0, P = 0.15).
291
Cronbach’s α was larger than .90 for both PIPR-mm and PIPR-% (Table 1). The ICC point
292
estimations for PIPR-mm and PIPR-% were both >.85, which indicates almost perfect test-
293
retest reliability. Bland-Altman plots for both parameters indicate almost zero bias between
294
the two assessments (Figure 2).
295 296
3.2 Experiment 2
TE D
297
M AN U
SC
RI PT
287
298
3.2.1 Dark adaptation duration
300
PIPR-mm was smaller (P = 0.04), and PIPR-% had a tendency to be smaller (P = 0.09), with
301
increasing dark adaptation duration prior to the first light exposure (Table 2). This was caused
302
by a decrease in baseline pupil diameter with increasing dark adaptation duration prior to the
303
start of the light exposure protocol (P = 0.02). Post-blue pupil diameter was not affected by
304
dark adaptation duration (P = 0.99).
AC C
305
EP
299
306
3.2.2 Time of day
307
No differences in PIPR-mm (P = 0.11) and PIPR-% (P = 0.22) were found between the
308
morning and afternoon assessments (Figure 3).
309 310
3.2.3 Body posture
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 311
PIPR-mm (P = 0.01) and PIPR-% (P = 0.02) were larger during assessments in a supine
312
posture relative to an upright posture. The difference was explained by a larger baseline pupil
313
diameter during the supine posture (P = 0.007), whereas the post-blue pupil diameter did not
314
change with posture (P = 0.81).
RI PT
315
3.2.4 Environmental light history
317
The 24-h mean environmental spectrally weighted irradiance per subject was on average 0.16
318
(SD = 0.03) weighted W/m2. Previous environmental light history did not affect PIPR-mm (P
319
= 0.72) and PIPR-% (P = 0.75).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
316
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 320
4. Discussion
321
The aim of the present study was to present and validate a PIPR protocol with high robustness
323
and reliability to assess interindividual differences in the melanopsin-signaling pathway. The
324
two proposed outcome parameters both showed a Cronbach’s α > .90 and an ICC > .85,
325
indicating a very high reliability. PIPR outcome parameter estimates were larger in the supine
326
position, due to its dilating effect on the pupil diameter during baseline darkness. Outcome
327
measures were not affected by dark adaptation, time of day, and environmental light history.
RI PT
322
SC
328
Single measure reliability of PIPR assessment using monochromatic short-wavelength light (~
330
470 nm) was evaluated in two previous studies. One study reported an ICC of 0.80 when
331
using 20-s light stimulation (300 cd/m2) twice with 3 minutes between sessions (Herbst et al.,
332
2011). In the other study PIPR was induced by exposing specific retinal areas to a 400-ms
333
light stimulus (400 cd/m2) (Lei et al., 2015). All areas were stimulated three times each with
334
several minutes between sessions. The ICC was 0.84 for full-field and 0.87 for central-field
335
stimulation. These PIPR protocols with relative short durations may be more user-friendly.
336
Others moreover showed that comparable stimulus durations were sufficient to detect group
337
differences (Feigl et al., 2012; Kankipati et al., 2011; Roecklein et al., 2013). Light stimuli of
338
longer duration, as we evaluated here, however allow for a more specific targeting of the
339
melanopsin-signaling pathway; ipRGCs are less photosensitive and have a slower
340
photoresponse than rods and cones (Berson et al., 2002; Do et al., 2009). In addition, the brief
341
stimuli applied in previous work evoked a transient PIPR (i.e., the post-exposure pupil
342
diameter returned to baseline already during the assessment), while the prolonged stimulus in
343
our protocol induced a PIPR that remained stable during the entire 5-minute post-blue
344
assessment interval. This high PIPR robustness in our protocol may explain the high ICC
345
values of 0.90 for PIPR-mm and 0.87 for PIPR-% with a relatively large interval of 24 hours
346
between sessions. Although our protocol takes longer to complete than previously proposed
347
protocols, and may thus be slightly more difficult to accomplish in clinical settings, this
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
329
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT duration seems necessary to obtain a robust assessment that is not sensitive to the onset and
349
offset variability discussed in the introduction section (Alpern and Campbell, 1963; Dacey et
350
al., 2005; Gamlin et al., 2007; Newsome, 1971). We would even suggest that it could be
351
interesting for future studies, to extend the post-blue assessment interval and quantify the time
352
course of normalization of the pupil diameter, as a further probe to individual differences in
353
ipRGCs kinetics (Gooley et al., 2012). An example of such normalization curve is provided in
354
the supplementary material (Figure S2).
355
Both PIPR-mm and PIPR-% met the test-retest criteria for clinical use (Bland and Altman,
356
1997), and the potential of our PIPR protocol as a diagnostic tool was furthermore expressed
357
by the consistency of these outcome parameters assessed at different regular office hours.
358
Caution is however needed when performing the test outside these hours, since previous
359
studies showed that PIPR was altered during the evening and night as a result of a modulation
360
of melanopsin-based phototransduction (Figueiro et al., 2005; Munch et al., 2012; Zele et al.,
361
2011). We found that both PIPR outcome measures were dependent on body posture,
362
secondary to the effect of posture on the baseline pupil diameter during darkness. This should
363
be taken into account when performing future research on the effects of light when subjects
364
have to maintain a supine position such as in magnetic resonance imaging studies (Chellappa
365
et al., 2014). Previous work found a smaller pupil diameter in a supine position than in a
366
sitting position, in agreement with cardiovascular studies showing less sympathetic activation
367
in a supine position (Lee et al., 2007). We found the opposite, and consider that this indication
368
of increased sympathetic activity could be caused by ‘fighting against sleep’ (van der Meijden
369
et al., 2015). Such seemingly paradoxical changes have also been reported in EEG beta-
370
power, which indexes central nervous system activation (Ramautar et al., 2013). Because of
371
the dependency of PIPR measures on baseline pupil diameter it is recommended to reporting
372
them as well in any PIPR study.
373
We found that PIPR was not affected by 24-h environmental light history. This indicates that
374
correction for previous light exposure is not necessary, which simplifies the implementation
375
of PIPR assessment. To our knowledge we are the first to assess the effects of 24-hr light
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
348
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT history on PIPR. Previous work on short-term light history did show ipRGC modulation by
377
previous light exposure: 5 minutes of prior long-wavelength light increased the pupil response
378
to blue light, while prior short-wavelength light blunted this response (Mure et al., 2009;
379
Mure et al., 2007). The lack of effect of prior light exposure on our outcome measures should
380
not be interpreted as absence of effects of light history; rather, it indicates that our approach is
381
robust to differences in light history.
382
We found a decline in baseline pupil diameter with increasing dark adaptation duration prior
383
to the start of the light exposure protocol indicating that the eyes were adapted to the dark
384
using 5 minutes of darkness. If dark adaptation was incomplete the pupil would be growing
385
instead of declining with an extension of the baseline dark period. The enhancement of pupil
386
constriction over time spent in the dark may be caused by increasing sleepiness (Lowenstein
387
and Loewenfeld, 1964), but this effect is not expected to be large enough to be able to mask a
388
possible effect of uncompleted dark adaptation. We therefore assume that five minutes of dark
389
pre-exposure is both required and adequate for PIPR assessment.
390
The high reliability of our PIPR assessment protocol renders it a sensitive tool for research on
391
group differences in human ipRGC functioning and ipRGC modulation in response to
392
intervention, allowing for acceptable sample sizes. In view of ipRGC projections to the
393
biological clock and brain areas involved in sleep/wake regulation (Hattar et al., 2006), it
394
would be interesting to assess the association between PIPR and interindividual differences in
395
circadian phase. Interesting patient populations for future PIPR research include not only
396
patients with ophthalmological diseases but also patients with disorders associated with sleep
397
and alertness complaints (e.g., insomnia, narcolepsy, mood disorders and attention
398
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Kooij and Bijlenga, 2014)). A possible limitation of our study
399
could be that we used a period of only 5 minutes between red and blue light exposure, which
400
may not have been sufficient to completely exclude rod and cone interference on the priming
401
effect of red light (Mure et al., 2009). We however felt that a dark period of 5 minutes ruled
402
out most rod and cone interference while preserving feasibility of the test. Accordingly, in
403
future studies it may be interesting to investigate whether the PIPR after blue light assessment
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
376
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT can be further enhanced by increasing the intermediate dark period or, alternatively, by using
405
other priming light exposures. Accordingly, others showed that using low light levels the
406
sustained pupil constriction after light offset was enhanced by using intermittent green light
407
instead of continuous stimulation, which was explained by increased cone activation (Gooley
408
et al., 2012). We however do not expect that implementing intermittent light stimuli in our
409
paradigm would further increase the PIPR outcome parameters: the intensity and duration of
410
our light stimulus are expected to already saturate ipRGCs (Wong et al., 2005). Whereas in
411
our protocol acute effects of cones are likely minimal, because we excluded the first minute
412
after light offset and moreover the red light pre-exposure, it could be most interesting to add
413
complementary paradigms using intermittent light to dissect ipRGC activity from rods and
414
cones to the pupillary light reflex. In future studies it is therefore interesting to include
415
multiple light exposure protocols in order to make a multivariate finger print of rod, cone, and
416
ipRGC involvement in the PIPR. Experiment 1 was performed at two different environments
417
by different experimenters. We observed no systematic differences when comparing data
418
from both settings, which supports the applicability of our PIPR paradigm at multiple sites.
419
Experiment 2 was limited by the lack of objective measurements of circadian rhythms (e.g.,
420
melatonin levels (Cajochen et al., 2003)). Individual differences in sleep/wake rhythm may
421
lead to different sleepiness levels, which may be a potential bias in the assessment of the
422
time-of-day effects on pupil diameter (Lowenstein and Loewenfeld, 1964). In view of the
423
absence of extreme chronotypes in our population, however, we expect circadian sleep/wake
424
timing to be similar in all participants. Another limitation of experiment 2 was that during the
425
30-minute habituation period prior to the start of the light exposure protocol, room lights were
426
set at a very dimmed level instead of switching them completely off. This small amount of
427
light was maintained in order to prevent participants from falling asleep, especially in supine
428
condition (Romeijn et al., 2012). Complete darkness may have been preferred in order to
429
maximize repolarization of rods and cones in order to increase their response to the light
430
exposure protocol. Although the dim light exposure was standardized, it may have biased the
431
pupillary light reflex as a result of variance in membrane potentials of rods and cones (Lall et
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
404
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT al., 2010). The contribution of rods and cones to the pupillary light reflex is particularly
433
substantial during stimulation and immediately after stimulus offset (Dacey et al., 2005;
434
Gamlin et al., 2007). We however included pupil diameter measures from 1 minute after light
435
offset and therefore do not expect that our PIPR measures were affected by using dim light
436
during the habituation period instead of darkness. A limitation of both experiments in this
437
study was that we only included young healthy adults. Future studies can use the proposed
438
assessment protocol to evaluate the reliability of the proposed PIPR assessment protocol in
439
clinical, and in pediatric or older populations. In conclusion, the present protocol can reliably
440
quantify the PIPR outcomes to evaluate ipRGC functioning in case-control and intervention
441
studies.
SC
RI PT
432
M AN U
442 443 444
Acknowledgements
445
We thank Mark Rea, PhD & Mariana Figueiro, PhD (Lighting Research Center, Rensselaer
447
Polytechnic Institute, Troy, NY, USA) for discussion and helpful comments on a previous
448
version. This project has been funded with support from the NeuroTime Erasmus+: Erasmus
449
Mundus programme of the European Commission. This publication/communication reflects
450
the views only of the author, and the Commission cannot be held responsible for any use
451
which may be made of the information contained therein. This work was supported by Project
452
NeuroSIPE 10738, of the Dutch Technology Foundation STW, which is part of the
453
Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and partly funded by the Ministry
454
of Economic Affairs, Agriculture and Innovation.
455
AC C
EP
TE D
446
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 456
References
457
Alpern, M., Campbell, F., 1963. Behaviour pupil during dark-adaptation. J. Physiol. London
459
165, P5-P7.
460
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., 2013. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models
461
using Eigen and S4, 1.0-4 ed, p. R package.
462
Berson, D.M., Dunn, F.A., Takao, M., 2002. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that
463
set the circadian clock. Science 295, 1070-1073.
464
Bierman, A., Klein, T.R., Rea, M.S., 2005. The Daysimeter: a device for measuring optical
465
radiation as a stimulus for the human circadian system. Meas. Sci. Technol. 16, 2292.
466
Bland, J.M., Altman, D.G., 1986. Statistical Methods for Assessing Agreement between Two
467
Methods of Clinical Measurement. Lancet 1, 307-310.
468
Bland, J.M., Altman, D.G., 1997. Cronbach's alpha. B. M. J. 314, 572.
469
Brainard, G.C., Hanifin, J.P., Greeson, J.M., Byrne, B., Glickman, G., Gerner, E., Rollag,
470
M.D., 2001. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: evidence for a novel
471
circadian photoreceptor. J. Neurosci. 21, 6405-6412.
472
Cajochen, C., Krauchi, K., Wirz-Justice, A., 2003. Role of melatonin in the regulation of
473
human circadian rhythms and sleep. J. Neuroendocrinol. 15, 432-437.
474
Chellappa, S.L., Ly, J.Q., Meyer, C., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Phillips, C.,
475
Cooper, H.M., Vandewalle, G., 2014. Photic memory for executive brain responses. Proc.
476
Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 6087-6091.
477
Cronbach, L.J., 1951. Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests. Psychometrika
478
16, 297-334.
479
Czeisler, C.A., Shanahan, T.L., Klerman, E.B., Martens, H., Brotman, D.J., Emens, J.S.,
480
Klein, T., Rizzo, J.F., 3rd, 1995. Suppression of melatonin secretion in some blind patients by
481
exposure to bright light. N. Eng. J. Med. 332, 6-11.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
458
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Dacey, D.M., Liao, H.W., Peterson, B.B., Robinson, F.R., Smith, V.C., Pokorny, J., Yau,
483
K.W., Gamlin, P.D., 2005. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal
484
colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature 433, 749-754.
485
Diedenhofen, B., 2013. cocron: Statistical comparisons of two or more alpha coefficients, 1.0-
486
0 ed, p. R Package.
487
Do, M.T., Kang, S.H., Xue, T., Zhong, H., Liao, H.W., Bergles, D.E., Yau, K.W., 2009.
488
Photon capture and signalling by melanopsin retinal ganglion cells. Nature 457, 281-287.
489
Edinger, J., Kirby, A., Lineberger, M., Loiselle, M., Wohlgemuth, W., Means, M., 2004. The
490
Duke structured interview for sleep disorders. Duke University Medical Center.
491
Feigl, B., Zele, A.J., Fader, S.M., Howes, A.N., Hughes, C.E., Jones, K.A., Jones, R., 2012.
492
The post-illumination pupil response of melanopsin-expressing intrinsically photosensitive
493
retinal ganglion cells in diabetes. Acta Ophthalmol. 90, e230-234.
494
Figueiro, M.G., Bullough, J.D., Parsons, R.H., Rea, M.S., 2005. Preliminary evidence for a
495
change in spectral sensitivity of the circadian system at night. J. Circadian Rhythms 3, 14.
496
Freedman, M.S., Lucas, R.J., Soni, B., von Schantz, M., Munoz, M., David-Gray, Z., Foster,
497
R., 1999. Regulation of mammalian circadian behavior by non-rod, non-cone, ocular
498
photoreceptors. Science 284, 502-504.
499
Gamlin, P.D., McDougal, D.H., Pokorny, J., Smith, V.C., Yau, K.W., Dacey, D.M., 2007.
500
Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells.
501
Vision Res. 47, 946-954.
502
Gooley, J.J., Ho Mien, I., St Hilaire, M.A., Yeo, S.C., Chua, E.C., van Reen, E., Hanley, C.J.,
503
Hull, J.T., Czeisler, C.A., Lockley, S.W., 2012. Melanopsin and rod-cone photoreceptors play
504
different roles in mediating pupillary light responses during exposure to continuous light in
505
humans. J. Neurosci. 32, 14242-14253.
506
Hattar, S., Kumar, M., Park, A., Tong, P., Tung, J., Yau, K.W., Berson, D.M., 2006. Central
507
projections of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. J. Comp. Neurol.
508
497, 326-349.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
482
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Heller, P.H., Perry, F., Jewett, D.L., Levine, J.D., 1990. Autonomic components of the human
510
pupillary light reflex. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 31, 156-162.
511
Herbst, K., Sander, B., Milea, D., Lund-Andersen, H., Kawasaki, A., 2011. Test-retest
512
repeatability of the pupil light response to blue and red light stimuli in normal human eyes
513
using a novel pupillometer. Front. Neurol. 2, 10.
514
Kankipati, L., Girkin, C.A., Gamlin, P.D., 2011. The post-illumination pupil response is
515
reduced in glaucoma patients. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 52, 2287-2292.
516
Kardon, R., Anderson, S.C., Damarjian, T.G., Grace, E.M., Stone, E., Kawasaki, A., 2009.
517
Chromatic pupil responses: preferential activation of the melanopsin-mediated versus outer
518
photoreceptor-mediated pupil light reflex. Ophthalmology 116, 1564-1573.
519
Kooij, J.J., Bijlenga, D., 2014. High prevalence of self-reported photophobia in adult ADHD.
520
Front. Neurol. 5, 256.
521
Lall, G.S., Revell, V.L., Momiji, H., Al Enezi, J., Altimus, C.M., Guler, A.D., Aguilar, C.,
522
Cameron, M.A., Allender, S., Hankins, M.W., Lucas, R.J., 2010. Distinct contributions of
523
rod, cone, and melanopsin photoreceptors to encoding irradiance. Neuron 66, 417-428.
524
Lee, J.-C., Kim, J.-E., Park, K.-M., 2007. Posture change affects indices of pupil size-Korean
525
males in their twenties. J. Biomed. Eng. Res. 28, 1-7.
526
Lei, S., Goltz, H.C., Chandrakumar, M., Wong, A.M., 2015. Test-retest reliability of
527
hemifield, central-field, and full-field chromatic pupillometry for assessing the function of
528
melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 56, 1267-1273.
529
Lockley, S.W., Evans, E.E., Scheer, F.A., Brainard, G.C., Czeisler, C.A., Aeschbach, D.,
530
2006. Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and
531
the waking electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 29, 161-168.
532
Lowenstein, O., Loewenfeld, I.E., 1964. The Sleep-Waking Cycle and Pupillary Activity.
533
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 117, 142-156.
534
Lucas, R.J., Douglas, R.H., Foster, R.G., 2001. Characterization of an ocular photopigment
535
capable of driving pupillary constriction in mice. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 621-626.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
509
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Lucas, R.J., Freedman, M.S., Munoz, M., Garcia-Fernandez, J.M., Foster, R.G., 1999.
537
Regulation of the mammalian pineal by non-rod, non-cone, ocular photoreceptors. Science
538
284, 505-507.
539
Markwell, E.L., Feigl, B., Zele, A.J., 2010. Intrinsically photosensitive melanopsin retinal
540
ganglion cell contributions to the pupillary light reflex and circadian rhythm. Clin. Exp.
541
Optom. 93, 137-149.
542
Munch, M., Leon, L., Crippa, S.V., Kawasaki, A., 2012. Circadian and wake-dependent
543
effects on the pupil light reflex in response to narrow-bandwidth light pulses. Invest.
544
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci 53, 4546-4555.
545
Mure, L.S., Cornut, P.L., Rieux, C., Drouyer, E., Denis, P., Gronfier, C., Cooper, H.M., 2009.
546
Melanopsin bistability: a fly's eye technology in the human retina. PLoS One 4, e5991.
547
Mure, L.S., Rieux, C., Hattar, S., Cooper, H.M., 2007. Melanopsin-dependent nonvisual
548
responses: evidence for photopigment bistability in vivo. J. Biol. Rhythms 22, 411-424.
549
Nelson, D.E., Takahashi, J.S., 1991. Sensitivity and integration in a visual pathway for
550
circadian entrainment in the hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). J Physiol 439, 115-145.
551
Newsome, D.A., 1971. Afterimage and pupillary activity following strong light exposure.
552
Vision Res. 11, 275-288.
553
Nissen, C., Sander, B., Lund-Andersen, H., 2011. The effect of pupil size on stimulation of
554
the melanopsin containing retinal ganglion cells, as evaluated by monochromatic
555
pupillometry. Front. Neurol. 2, 92.
556
Park, J.C., Moura, A.L., Raza, A.S., Rhee, D.W., Kardon, R.H., Hood, D.C., 2011. Toward a
557
clinical protocol for assessing rod, cone, and melanopsin contributions to the human pupil
558
response. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 52, 6624-6635.
559
Ramautar, J.R., Romeijn, N., Gomez-Herrero, G., Piantoni, G., Van Someren, E.J., 2013.
560
Coupling of infraslow fluctuations in autonomic and central vigilance markers: skin
561
temperature, EEG beta power and ERP P300 latency. Int J Psychophysiol 89, 158-164.
562
Rea, M.S., Figueiro, M.G., Bullough, J.D., Bierman, A., 2005. A model of phototransduction
563
by the human circadian system. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 213-228.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
536
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Roecklein, K., Wong, P., Ernecoff, N., Miller, M., Donofry, S., Kamarck, M., Wood-Vasey,
565
W.M., Franzen, P., 2013. The post illumination pupil response is reduced in seasonal affective
566
disorder. Psychiatry Res.
567
Romeijn, N., Raymann, R.J., Most, E., Te Lindert, B., Van Der Meijden, W.P., Fronczek, R.,
568
Gomez-Herrero, G., Van Someren, E.J., 2012. Sleep, vigilance, and thermosensitivity.
569
Pflugers Arch. 463, 169-176.
570
Shrout, P.E., Fleiss, J.L., 1979. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability.
571
Psychol. Bull. 86, 420-428.
572
Thapan, K., Arendt, J., Skene, D.J., 2001. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression:
573
evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J. Physiol. 535, 261-
574
267.
575
Trejo, L.J., Cicerone, C.M., 1984. Cells in the pretectal olivary nucleus are in the pathway for
576
the direct light reflex of the pupil in the rat. Brain Res. 300, 49-62.
577
Twisk, J.W., 2013. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology. Cambridge
578
University Press.
579
van der Meijden, W.P., Fronczek, R., Reijntjes, R.H., Corssmit, E.P., Biermasz, N.R.,
580
Lammers, G.J., van Dijk, J.G., Thijs, R.D., 2015. Time- and state-dependent analysis of
581
autonomic control in narcolepsy: higher heart rate with normal heart rate variability
582
independent of sleep fragmentation. J. Sleep Res. 24, 206-214.
583
Wolak, M.E., Fairbairn, D.J., Paulsen, Y.R., 2012. Guidelines for estimating repeatability.
584
Methods Ecol. and Evol. 3, 129-137.
585
Wong, K.Y., Dunn, F.A., Berson, D.M., 2005. Photoreceptor adaptation in intrinsically
586
photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Neuron 48, 1001-1010.
587
Zavada, A., Gordijn, M.C., Beersma, D.G., Daan, S., Roenneberg, T., 2005. Comparison of
588
the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire with the Horne-Ostberg's Morningness-Eveningness
589
Score. Chronobiol. Int. 22, 267-278.
590
Zele, A.J., Feigl, B., Smith, S.S., Markwell, E.L., 2011. The circadian response of
591
intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. PLoS One 6, e17860.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
564
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 592
Figures
593
Figure 1. Example of the change of pupil diameter in the left eye throughout the light
595
exposure protocol.
596
The bar in the bottom indicates when the right eye was exposed to light; black = darkness, red
597
= monochromatic red light, blue = monochromatic blue light. The dashed line represents
598
mean pupil diameter during the first dark block (i.e., baseline pupil diameter). Post-
599
Illumination Pupil Response (PIPR) indicates the difference between baseline pupil diameter
600
and the mean pupil diameter during the last dark block (i.e., post-blue pupil diameter) and can
601
be expressed either as the absolute difference in mm (PIPR-mm, here 2.99 mm) or the
602
percentage change from baseline (PIPR-%, here 44.7 %).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
594
AC C
EP
TE D
603
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 2. Bland-Altman plots for PIPR-mm and PIPR-%.
605
Differences between the two measurements on two consecutive days (i.e., day 1 minus day 2)
606
are plotted against the mean of the two measurements. The bias (dotted line) is the mean
607
difference between all measurements on the first day and all measurements on the second day.
608
The 95% limits of agreement (dashed lines) define the range between which 95% of the
609
differences between measurements by the two devices will lie. The smaller the limits of
610
agreement, the better the agreement between two measurements.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
611
RI PT
604
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 3. Effects of the factors time of day and body posture on the outcome parameters
613
PIPR-mm and PIPR-%.
614
In the plots for both time of day (top row) as well as body posture (bottom row) each dashed
615
line connects the two repeated measures within a participant to visualize the difference
616
between the two conditions. Horizontal lines with an asterisk (*) above a plot indicate a
617
significant effect of the factor on the outcome parameter (P < 0.05).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
612
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 1. PIPR outcome parameters from session 1 and 2 and test-retest reliability outcomes.
Session 1
Session 2 Cronbach’s α
ICC
Bland-Altman bias
Mean ± SD
Min
Max
Mean ± SD
Min
Max
(95% CI)
(95% CI)
(95% limits of agreement)
PIPR-mm
2.88 ± 1.01
1.14
4.49
2.81 ± 0.88
1.48
4.10
0.95 (0.84 – 0.98)
0.90 (0.74 - 0.96)
0.08 (-0.78 – 0.94)
PIPR-%
46.9 ± 10.4
22.9
59.2
47.0 ± 9.0
32.6
59.3
0.93 (0.78 – 0.98)
RI PT
Parameter
0.87 (0.67 - 0.95)
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
PIPR, Post-Illumination Pupil Response; ICC, Intraclass Correlation Coefficient.
-0.11 (-10.3 – 10.1)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 2. Estimates of the effects of dark adaptation duration, time of day, body posture, and light history on PIPR outcome parameters and baseline and post-blue pupil diameter.
Intercept
PIPR-mm
PIPR-%
Baseline pupil diameter (mm)
Post-blue pupil diameter (mm)
1.79 ± 0.32***
38.97 ± 5.11***
4.62 ± 0.32***
2.93 ± 0.33***
-1.96 ± 0.87*
-27.91 ± 14.98*
-1.98 ± 0.72*
0.02 ± 1.00
0.17 ± 0.10
2.22 ± 1.74
0.17 ± 0.08
0.29 ± 0.10*
4.34 ± 1.69*
0.26 ± 0.08**
0.51 ± 1.40
-7.89 ± 23.92
1.54 ± 1.17
(/hour) Time of day (morning vs. afternoon) Body posture (supine vs. upright)
SC
Light history (weighted W/m2)
RI PT
Dark adaptation duration
Mean values ± SE are displayed. PIPR, Post-Illumination Pupil Response.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
* P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001.
-0.01 ± 0.12
-0.03 ± 0.11
0.43 ± 1.59
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Pupil diameter (mm)
8
6 PIPR 4
2
0 0
5
10
15
Time (min)
20
25
PIPR-mm
EP
2
1
0
−1
−2 1
2
AC C
Difference (mm)
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3
4
5
50
60
Mean (mm)
PIPR-%
Difference (%)
20
10
0
−10
−20 20
30
40 Mean (%)
Time of day 60
AC C
3
PIPR−%
PIPR−mm
4
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2 1 0
40
20
0
morning
afternoon
morning
afternoon
Body posture
*
* 60
3 PIPR−%
PIPR−mm
4
2 1 0 upright
40
20
0 supine
upright
supine
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
A method for quantifying melanopsin-dependent phototransduction is proposed. The quantification is based on the post-illumination pupil response after blue light. The presented method had very high test-retest reliability. Post-illumination pupil response differed between supine and upright position.
AC C
-
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure S1. PIPR-mm and PIPR-% with and without pre-exposure to bright red light. The PIPR after blue light was measured twice in twelve young healthy individuals (6 males, 6 females, mean age ± SD: 24.6 ± 2.1 yr). Ten participants did not participate in either of the two experiments described in the manuscript, while the other 2 participated in experiment 1. The participants were
RI PT
exposed to two light exposure conditions: one with pre-exposure to red light and one without preexposure. In the red light pre-exposure condition the light exposure protocol was as described in the manuscript (i.e., dark, red, dark, blue, dark), while in the no pre-exposure condition the first dark
SC
block and the red block were omitted from that protocol (i.e., dark, blue, dark). Prior to the start of both light exposure paradigms, the participants were exposed to 30 minutes of dim light (0.5 cd/m2).
M AN U
PIPR-mm and PIPR-% for both conditions were calculated using the methods described in the manuscript. In the plots, each dot represents one measurement and the dashed lines connect the two repeated measures within a participant to visualize the difference between the two conditions. One trial in the red light pre-exposure condition was left out, because of insufficient reliable data
TE D
points (i.e., our algorithm provided a negative PIPR values). Both PIPR outcome measures seemed somewhat higher with red light pre-exposure than without, but mixed effect regression models showed that the differences did not reach significance (PIPR-mm: β = 0.28, SE = 0.24, P = 0.27; PIPR-
EP
%: β = 5.3, SE = 3.7, P = 0.18). This is probably due a ceiling effect: other measures were already taken to increase the PIPR towards maximum values (i.e., using light sources with high luminance and
AC C
dilating the pupil of the exposed eye). Despite the PIPR increase after red light pre-exposure being non-significant, considerations of robustness (e.g. in case of a different protocol or submaximal exposure) and previous work give enough reason to recommend pre-exposure with red light in order to guarantee a maximal PIPR.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
60
4 PIPR−%
PIPR−mm
5
3 2
40 20
1 0
0 none pre-exposure
red
none pre-exposure
red
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure S2. Example of the change of pupil diameter beyond the light exposure protocol. Two healthy young adult males were exposed to the light exposure protocol as described in the manuscript with the baseline dark period reduced to 1 minute. The dashed line represents mean pupil diameter during this baseline dark period. Post-blue dark was extended to 30 minutes to monitor the
RI PT
normalization of pupil diameter after light offset. Interestingly, in participant 1 we observe pupil diameter reaching baseline size after approximately 15 minutes of post-blue darkness, while in
participant 2 redilation to baseline diameter is not completed within the 30-minute dark interval after
SC
blue light offset. This large difference in pupil redilation time between the two participants indicates that for future studies it may be interesting to extend the post-blue dark period to allow for
M AN U
quantification of the time course of pupil redilation in order to further examine individual differences
AC C
EP
TE D
in ipRGCs kinetics.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Participant 1
EP
6 4 2 0 0
AC C
Pupil diameter (mm)
8
10
20
30
40
30
40
Time (min)
Participant 2
Pupil diameter (mm)
8 6 4 2 0 0
10
20 Time (min)