Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Environment International journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/envint
Prenatal particulate air pollution exposure and sleep disruption in preschoolers: Windows of susceptibility
T
Sonali Bosea,b, Kristie R. Rossc, Maria J. Rosad, Yueh-Hsiu Mathilda Chiud, Allan Justd, Itai Klooge, Ander Wilsonf, Jennifer Thompsong, Katherine Svenssond, Martha María Téllez Rojoh, Lourdes Schnaasi, Erika Osorio-Valenciai, Emily Okeng, Robert O. Wrightb,d,j, ⁎ Rosalind J. Wrightb,d,j, a
Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care Medicine, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, United States of America Department of Pediatrics, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, United States of America Department of Pediatrics, Rainbow Babies and Children's Hospital, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH, United States of America d Department of Environmental Medicine and Public Health, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, United States of America e Department of Geography and Environmental Development, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, BeerSheba, Israel f Department of Statistics, Colorado State University, United States of America g Department of Population Medicine, Harvard Medical School and Harvard Pilgrim Health Care Institute, Boston, MA, United States of America h Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública Cuernavaca, Morelos, Mexico i Department of Developmental Neurobiology, National Institute of Perinatology “Isidro Espinosa de los Reyes”, Mexico City, Mexico j Institute for Exposomics Research, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, United States of America b c
A R T I C LE I N FO
A B S T R A C T
Handling Editor: Olga-Ioanna Kalantzi
Background: The programming of sleep architecture begins in pregnancy and depends upon optimal in utero formation and maturation of the neural connectivity of the brain. Particulate air pollution exposure can disrupt fetal brain development but associations between fine particulate matter (PM2.5) exposure during pregnancy and child sleep outcomes have not been previously explored. Methods: Analyses included 397 mother-child pairs enrolled in a pregnancy cohort in Mexico City. Daily ambient prenatal PM2.5 exposure was estimated using a validated satellite-based spatio-temporally resolved prediction model. Child sleep periods were estimated objectively using wrist-worn, continuous actigraphy over a 1-week period at age 4–5 years. Data-driven advanced statistical methods (distributed lag models (DLMs)) were employed to identify sensitive windows whereby PM2.5 exposure during gestation was significantly associated with changes in sleep duration or efficiency. Models were adjusted for maternal education, season, child's age, sex, and BMI z-score. Results: Mother's average age was 27.7 years, with 59% having at least a high school education. Children slept an average of 7.7 h at night, with mean 80.1% efficiency. The adjusted DLM identified windows of PM2.5 exposure between 31 and 35 weeks gestation that were significantly associated with decreased sleep duration in children. In addition, increased PM2.5 during weeks 1–8 was associated with decreased sleep efficiency. In other exposure windows (weeks 39–40), PM2.5 was associated with increased sleep duration. Conclusion: Prenatal PM2.5 exposure is associated with altered sleep in preschool-aged children in Mexico City. Pollutant exposure during sensitive windows of pregnancy may have critical influence upon sleep programming.
Keywords: Particulate matter Air pollution Prenatal Sleep Child Preschool-aged
1. Introduction Insufficient or poor-quality sleep in early childhood is one of the
most common problems reported by caregivers (Owens, 2005), with 20–40% of children experiencing sleep disturbance before reaching school age (Archbold et al., 2002; Stein et al., 2001). Abnormal sleep
Abbreviations: DLM, distributive lag models; PM, particulate matter; CNS, central nervous system; BMI, body mass index; REM, rapid eye movement; AS, active sleep; QS, quiet sleep; IS, indeterminate sleep; AOD, aerosol optimal depth; LUR, land use regression ⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Pediatrics, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box 1198, New York, NY 10029, United States of America. E-mail address:
[email protected] (R.J. Wright). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2019.01.012 Received 20 September 2018; Received in revised form 3 January 2019; Accepted 4 January 2019 0160-4120/ © 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY-NC-ND/4.0/).
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
influences placental methylation in the promoter regions of circadian pathway genes (e.g. Clock), specifically during the 3rd trimester (Nawrot et al., 2018), highlighting another potential mechanism for the alteration of sleep regulation by particulate air pollution. While recent epidemiologic data demonstrate associations between PM and sleep difficulties among children exposed postnatally (Tenero et al., 2017; Abou-Khadra, 2013; Accinelli et al., 2015), to date, to our knowledge, there are no published studies examining the impact of prenatal ambient PM2.5 on sleep in early childhood. In addition, evidence shows that neurotoxic effects of prenatal PM2.5 exposure in children occur during specific time periods during pregnancy (Chiu et al., 2016), suggesting that associations between prenatal toxic exposures and postnatal sleep disorders may depend on timing of exposure as well as dose. In order to examine associations between prenatal PM2.5 exposure on early childhood sleep outcomes accounting for timing of exposure, we implemented data-driven distributed lag models (DLMs) to estimate windows of vulnerability between prenatal PM2.5 exposure and sleep duration and efficiency estimated objectively at preschool age within a prospective pregnancy cohort established in Mexico City.
patterns persist into later life (Kuula et al., 2018; Fricke-Oerkermann et al., 2007), and have life-long adverse effects on health, including increased neurodevelopmental, metabolic, cardiovascular, and respiratory disorders (Chaput et al., 2016; Medic et al., 2017). Disordered sleep in young children, manifesting as altered duration, timing, maintenance, or quality of sleep (Blunden and Galland, 2014), is associated with hyperactivity (Touchette et al., 2009; Melendres et al., 2004), impaired attention (O'Callaghan et al., 2010; Gruber et al., 2011), cognitive function (Short et al., 2018; Hunter et al., 2016), and behavior problems (Sivertsen et al., 2015; Reynaud et al., 2018). While treating sleep disorders in young children improves some outcomes (Marcus et al., 2013; Mindell et al., 2006), complete response is the exception rather than the rule (Chervin et al., 2006; Biggs et al., 2014), suggesting there may be a critical early window in which sleep disruption may have lasting health and neurocognitive impacts (Bonuck et al., 2012; Biggs et al., 2015). Therefore, identifying factors that disrupt the ontogeny of sleep is critical in developing prevention strategies to promote healthy sleep development. Sleep is an active, complex neurophysiological process (Parrino and Vaudano, 2018; Bathory and Tomopoulos, 2017). Optimal formation of the neural connectivity required to process signals involved in sleep regulation begins in utero (Bennet et al., 2018a; Scher and Loparo, 2009), as early as seven weeks of gestation, when neuronal production and migration start laying down necessary myelinated neural networks across the brain. While there is continued maturation of homeostatic, circadian, and ultradian regulation of sleep following birth, the basic components—phasic rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM (NREM) sleep—are also established prenatally, as early as 28 weeks (Okai et al., 1992), and timely progression of the formation of these neural networks in utero until birth is necessary to establish the mature sleep wake cycle observed in the full-term newborn (Bennet et al., 2018a). Hence, developmental interruption—such as by preterm birth—may result in reduced sleep duration and efficiency (Yiallourou et al., 2017), altered sleep architecture (Yiallourou et al., 2017), and circadian rhythm abnormalities (Hibbs et al., 2014), manifesting postnatally. Notably, the plasticity of neurodevelopmental phenomena involved in establishing sleep phenotypes during gestation (Scher et al., 1992) also renders it vulnerable to the influence of environmental exposures, which may in turn result in childhood sleep disorders. For example, disruption of critical neurodevelopmental processes during pregnancy, such as by prenatal tobacco smoke and other toxins, has been linked with abnormal sleep-wake transitions, excessive arousals, and disorganized sleep patterns in infancy (Stephan-Blanchard et al., 2008; Horne et al., 2004) that persist into later childhood (Stone et al., 2009; Stone et al., 2010). Overlapping animal and epidemiologic data link ambient air pollution, in particular fine particulate matter (PM2.5), to adverse effects on central nervous system (CNS) development via oxidative stress and neuroinflammation (Costa et al., 2017; Block et al., 2012). Ambient PM2.5 impacts fetal brain structure, function, and neurohormonal development (Calderon-Garciduenas et al., 2016). For example, Peterson et al. demonstrated that prenatal air pollution is associated with diffusely reduced white matter on MRI by school age, with concomitant cognitive and behavioral disorders in children (Peterson et al., 2015). Notably, these anatomic alterations were more pronounced for prenatal rather than postnatal exposure. Traffic pollution is also linked to lower functional brain connectivity in children, presumably through its effects on white matter cell maturation and myelination (Pujol et al., 2016), and neuroinflammatory pathways known to impair brain development (Costa et al., 2017; Bennet et al., 2018b). In addition, prenatal PM alters neurotransmitters across multiple brain regions in animal models (Yokota et al., 2013), and consequent central changes in availability or sensitivity to neurotransmitters such as impaired glycine and GABAA receptor function in animal studies results in abnormal neuro-inhibitory transmission and sleep fragmentation, mimicking human REM sleep disorders (Brooks and Peever, 2011). Lastly, in humans, prenatal PM2.5 exposure
2. Methods 2.1. Participants Pregnant mothers were recruited from prenatal clinics in Mexico City belonging to the Mexican Social Security System from July 2007 to February 2011. Enrollment into the Programming Research in Obesity, Growth, Environment, and Social Stressors (PROGRESS) cohort included mothers at least 18 years of age, < 20 weeks pregnant, planning to stay in Mexico City for the next 3 years, with access to a telephone, and having no medical history of heart or kidney disease, daily alcohol consumption, or steroid or anti-epilepsy medication use. Pregnant women were excluded from participation if they had multiple gestation, maternal preeclampsia, psychiatric disease or use of psychotropic drugs, and for logistical reasons, those mothers living in a household outside the metropolitan area. Additional details of the cohort have been described previously (Burris et al., 2013). Eight-hundred and fifteen mother-child dyads were enrolled and continued longitudinal follow-up postnatally. Mothers provided written consent in their primary language and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the Harvard School of Public Health, the Mexican National Institute of Public Health, and the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai. Premature births with gestation < 37 weeks were specifically excluded from these analyses. Our analyses included n = 397 mothers and full term-infants (born > 37 weeks gestation) who completed the 4–5 year visit and had complete environmental air pollution and sleep outcome data. Those included in the analysis and those excluded did not differ significantly based on average PM2.5 exposure over pregnancy (22.7 vs. 22.6 μg/m3.), mean maternal age (27.7 vs. 27.5 years), maternal education (59 vs. 60% reporting at least a high school education), child BMI z-score (0.07 vs. 0.26), age at sleep evaluation (4.8 vs. 4.8 years), or sex (51 vs. 54% males), respectively (p-value for all > 0.05). 2.2. Ambient air pollution assessment Individual maternal daily pollutant exposure over pregnancy was estimated from home addresses obtained at enrollment and updated if the participant moved. PM2.5 levels across Mexico City were estimated at a 1 × 1 km spatial resolution, using hybrid models incorporating day-specific calibrations of aerosol optimal depth (AOD) data with land use regression (LUR) and meteorological variables (roadway density, temperature, relative humidity, planetary boundary layer, and daily precipitation), and calibrated against ground measurements performed at 12 monitoring stations covering Mexico City, as described previously 330
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
averages of daily PM2.5 exposure estimates throughout pregnancy were used in order to determine the time-varying associations between pollutant exposures and sleep outcomes and identify sensitive windows during gestation where these associations were significant. Specifically, we estimated overall associations between prenatal PM2.5 exposure and nocturnal sleep duration and sleep efficiency, in separate models, and identified sensitive windows by constructing distributed lag models (DLM) using the previously described approach (Wilson et al., 2017a) with no interaction. When the association varies with time, time periods with greater association (sensitive windows) graphically appear as a bump where PM2.5 is associated with sleep outcomes. In addition to determining sensitive windows, we employed similar DLMs to estimate cumulative effects over the entire pregnancy of PM2.5. All primary models were adjusted for maternal age and education, season of visit, child's BMI z-score, age, and sex. Secondary sensitivity analyses were conducted for sleep duration that included daytime naps, as well as for further adjustment for the following covariates: birthweight for gestational age (BWGA) Fenton z-score, maternal smoking in pregnancy, and post-natal PM2.5 exposure averaged over the first year of life, in separate models.
(Just et al., 2015). Mixed effect models with spatial and temporal predictors and day-specific random effects were used to account for temporal variations in the PM2.5–AOD relationship. For days without AOD data, models were fit with a seasonal smooth function of latitude and longitude and time-varying average based on local monitoring. The out of sample ten-fold cross validation R2 was 0.724. We similarly derived postnatal PM2.5 over the first year of the child's life. 2.3. Sleep outcomes Child sleep duration and sleep efficiency were the outcomes evaluated. Child sleep duration was estimated objectively using ActiGraph GT3x + accelerometers, measuring motion in oscillations, which was placed on the child's non-dominant wrist and worn over a 7-day period. Sleep duration was identified by periods with low to no physical activity (defined as < 1000 oscillations per minute). Primary outcomes included nocturnal sleep duration and sleep efficiency determined as follows. Using accelerometry software ActiLife v6.11.9, we used the Sadeh sleep algorithm to determine total nocturnal sleep time, defined as the number of minutes the child was asleep each night (Sadeh et al., 1994), and sleep periods were crosschecked against parent-completed sleep diaries. Nocturnal sleep efficiency, defined as the percentage of time in bed spent asleep, was calculated by dividing accelerometerderived sleep periods by the total time spent in bed. In addition to analyzing periods coincident with nighttime, sleep periods during the day (i.e. naps) were also identified and analyzed using the same algorithm. Compliance with accelerometry was high; 96.8% of children wore the Actigraph for at least 6 nights.
3. Results 3.1. Participant characteristics Mothers had an average (mean, SD) age of 27.7 (5.7) years at enrollment; 59% completed at least a high school education. Median [interquartile range (IQR)]) ambient prenatal PM2.5 concentration was 23.0 [21.1–24.3] μg/m3. Child sleep was estimated objectively at a mean [SD] age of 4.8 [0.5] years. Mean [SD] nocturnal sleep duration and efficiency were 7.7 [0.74] hours, and 80.1 [5.8]%, respectively (Table 1).
2.4. Other covariates We considered covariates related to air pollution exposure and child sleep. Demographic and health-related data were collected during an initial prenatal study visit. Maternal age, education level as an indicator of socioeconomic status (SES), and maternal prenatal smoking status were self-reported through questionnaire administered at enrollment. Hospital delivery records were used to collect newborn characteristics such as sex and birthweight. Gestational age was based on maternal report of last menstrual period (LMP) and by a standardized physical examination at birth, with discrepancies of > 3 weeks resolved by using the latter. We derived Fenton birth weight for gestational age z-scores, which accounts for the non-linear birth weight patterns during pregnancy (Fenton and Kim, 2013). Child height and weight were recorded at the 4–5 year visit by research staff using a wall-mounted stadiometer; body mass index (BMI) was calculated by dividing weight by height squared (kg/m2), and z-scores were derived using World Health Organization norms. Season of visit was defined according to weather patterns in Mexico City as dry cold (January–February; November–December), dry warm (March–April) and rainy (May–October).
3.2. Association between prenatal PM2.5 and child nocturnal sleep duration We evaluated the relationship between mean PM2.5 exposure averaged over the course of pregnancy, as well as individually for each of the trimesters, and nocturnal sleep duration. After adjusting for confounders, linear regression models did not identify significant associations between any of the average exposures (over gestation or within each trimester) and nocturnal sleep duration. Adjusted DLMs were used to estimate the overall associations Table 1 PROGRESS participant characteristics (N = 397).
2.5. Statistical analyses We restricted our sample to infants born ≥37 weeks gestation, as we considered prematurity to be a pathway variable, with well-established associations describing prenatal air pollution and prematurity (Stieb et al., 2012), as well as between prematurity and disruption of neural networks relevant to sleep (Curzi-Dascalova et al., 1988; Scheinost et al., 2016). Among the 397 mother-child dyads included in the analyses, for those infants (n = 289) born between 37 and 40 weeks of gestation, we used ambient post-natal exposure of the remaining days of a 40-week gestational period as PM2.5 exposure estimates in our models as in prior studies.(Bose et al., 2018; Bose et al., 2017) We first evaluated the relationship between mean PM2.5 exposure averaged over the full gestational period, as well as individually for each of the trimesters (adjusting for the remaining trimester exposures), and the sleep outcomes using linear regression models. Next, weekly 331
Variables
All children
Maternal age (year; mean, SD) Maternal education (n, %) < High school ≥High school Maternal smoking (n, %) Child sex (n, %) Male Female Child age (year; mean, SD) Child's BMI z-score (z-score; median, IQR) Season of visit (n, %) Dry/cold Dry/warm Rain Prenatal averaged PM2.5 level (μg/m3; median, IQR) Postnatal 1st year averaged PM2.5 level (μg/m3; median, IQR) Nocturnal sleep duration (hours; mean, SD) Total sleep duration per 24 h period (hours; mean, SD) Nocturnal sleep efficiency (%; mean, SD) Birthweight for gestational age (Fenton z-score; mean, SD)
27.7
5.7
163 234 3
41.1 59.0 0.8
203 194 4.8 0.07
51.1 48.9 0.5 −0.47–0.65
127 70 200 23.0 22.8
32.0 17.6 50.4 21.1–24.3 20.8–24.0
7.7 7.8 80.1 −0.49
0.74 0.72 5.8 0.85
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
Fig. 1. Association between weekly PM2.5 over gestation and child sleep: nocturnal sleep duration. Distributed lag models (DLMs) were used to estimate week-specific effects for preschool children. Models were adjusted for maternal age and education, season of visit, child BMI z-score, age, and sex. The x-axis indicates the gestational age in weeks. The y-axis represents the change in nocturnal sleep time corresponding to each interquartile range (IQR) of PM2.5 across the entire pregnancy. Solid lines represent the predicted time-varying change in sleep duration and gray areas indicate the 95% confidence intervals (CI). A sensitive window is identified when the estimated point-wise 95% CI does not include zero.
Fig. 2. Association between weekly PM2.5 over gestation and child sleep: nocturnal sleep efficiency. Distributed lag models (DLMs) were used to estimate week-specific effects for preschool children. Models were adjusted for maternal age and education, season of visit, child BMI, age, and sex. The x-axis indicates the gestational age in weeks. The y-axis represents the change in sleep efficiency corresponding to each interquartile range (IQR) of PM2.5 across the entire pregnancy. Solid lines represent the predicted time-varying change in sleep efficiency and gray areas indicate the 95% confidence intervals (CI). A sensitive window is identified when the estimated point-wise 95% CI does not include zero.
between weekly PM2.5 during gestation and nocturnal sleep duration. We identified a late sensitive window at 31–35 weeks whereby higher PM exposure during pregnancy was significantly associated with decreased total sleep time at preschool age (Fig. 1). In addition, there was also a second significant critical window at 39–40 weeks that was positively associated with total sleep time. There was a suggestive effect of an IQR change in PM exposure being associated with increased nocturnal sleep duration in weeks 21–27, but this did not reach statistical significance. There was no significant cumulative effect of PM2.5 exposure over the course of gestation upon child nocturnal sleep time. In addition to nocturnal sleep, we performed sensitivity analyses between PM2.5 exposure and total 24 h sleep duration to account for daytime nap periods, which had a similar shape and where the association between PM and sleep duration was significant between 21 and 27 weeks (Fig. S1). The cumulative effect of PM2.5 exposure over the course of gestation was not significant in either linear regression or DLM models (data not shown).
exposure over the entire gestational period demonstrated significantly reduced sleep efficiency (β = −0.78, 95%CI = −1.52 to −0.13 for each IQR change in PM2.5). Sensitivity analyses were also performed to account, individually, for BWGA Fenton z-score (Figs. S2 and S3), maternal smoking (Figs. S4 and S5), and post-natal PM2.5 exposure over the first year of life (Figs. S6 and S7), each of which revealed similar findings for both sleep duration and efficiency. 4. Discussion To our knowledge, this is the first prospective study to examine associations between prenatal PM2.5 and sleep outcomes in childhood. In addition to highlighting significant associations between average PM exposure over pregnancy and sleep, we leveraged advanced statistical modeling approaches with highly temporally- and spatially-resolved ambient PM2.5 exposure estimates to identify sensitive windows during which exposure to ambient fine particulate matter was significantly associated with alterations in nocturnal sleep duration and sleep efficiency in preschool children. Notably, while current American Association of Sleep Medicine (AASM) guidelines recommend that children 3–5 years of age receive 10–13 h of sleep per 24 h (Paruthi et al., 2016), children in our cohort slept on average 7.8 h over a 24 h period, consistent with other data demonstrating similarly short sleep durations among young children, especially within minority populations in the United States (Caldwell and Redeker, 2015; Martinez et al., 2018; Guglielmo et al., 2018). These results highlight the need to identify early risk factors for sleep deficiencies in childhood, particularly in higher risk, minority populations. There is a growing literature demonstrating the neurotoxic effects of prenatal exposure to fine particulate air pollution and early life neurodevelopmental processes. These effects may be mediated by oxidative stress and neuroinflammatory pathways, as PM has been shown to induce local and systemic inflammation. We believe this also provides
3.3. Association between prenatal PM2.5 and child nocturnal sleep efficiency Adjusted linear regression models demonstrated a statistically significant association between mean PM2.5 exposure averaged over pregnancy and nocturnal sleep efficiency (β = −0.247, p = 0.037, for each 1 μg/m3 change in PM2.5). Furthermore, when average exposures were examined by trimester, statistically significant associations were noted for the first trimester only (β = −0.204, p = 0.004 per 1 μg/m3 change in PM2.5). Consistent with these findings, best-fit DLMs examining the time-varying association between prenatal PM2.5 exposure and child sleep efficiency also identified a distinct sensitive window at 1–8 weeks of gestation, whereby increased pollutant exposure was associated with decreased nocturnal sleep efficiency in preschool children (Fig. 2). In addition, the estimated cumulative effect of pollutant 332
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
biologic plausibility for our findings related to sleep outcomes. Basic sleep programming occurs before birth (Okai et al., 1992), and is highly dependent upon staged progression of neuronal growth and maturation to optimize both structural and functional connectivity of the central nervous system (Bennet et al., 2018a; Scher et al., 1992). Disruptions to the development of this connectivity, such as by prematurity, hypoxic insult, and neuro-inflammation may impair neural network development that in turn alter sleep states in children (Bennet et al., 2018a). Indeed, data from both animal and human studies demonstrate that growth restriction and impaired neural architecture are associated with reduced active sleep and increased indeterminate sleep (IS) in the fetus, as well as altered sleep staging in the postnatal period (Bennet et al., 2018a). In addition to perinatal changes, studies of young children raise concern for long-term effects on sleep quality, including poor sleep, lower sleep efficiency, and increased awakenings tied to prenatal insults. Of these, prenatal environmental pollutants such as maternal tobacco smoke exposure—known to have adverse effects on fetal neural development (Holz et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2010; Chatterton et al., 2017)—has been shown to affect downstream sleep outcomes after birth, resulting in disruptions in sleep structure and continuity that are reflective of disturbed sleep patterns and altered arousability (Horne et al., 2004). In this report, we present findings that for the first time extend this literature to demonstrate that prenatal ambient PM2.5 exposure during critical periods of pregnancy is also associated with altered sleep outcomes in young children. Notably, overall estimates of prenatal PM2.5 in this Mexico City cohort of 23 μg/m3 were found to be relatively higher than third trimester exposures in other United Statesbased pregnancy cohorts, for example, in Boston (10.75 μg/m3) or New York (8.2 μg/m3) (Rosa et al., 2017). Another novel aspect of our work lies in the identification of sensitive windows during gestation. As neurodevelopment in utero is a highly orchestrated, timed process, using data-driven approaches to identify sensitive windows of gestation allows us to overlay these time periods with corresponding knowledge of embryologic events (instead of arbitrary intervals of pregnancy), providing insight into the underlying mechanisms being perturbed. For example, the early window identified to be associated with decreased sleep efficiency (1–8 weeks) corresponds to the time period during which neuron production and migration allow for myelin-producing cells to facilitate white matter maturation required for functional connectivity of the brain (Bennet et al., 2018a). One may conceptualize that early disruptions in laying down this foundation of neural architecture may reduce the ultimate potential of the “connectome (Tymofiyeva et al., 2012)” and alter sleep development by limiting prerequisite neural arborization. In contrast, the later window identified (30–32 weeks) for PM effects on sleep duration occurs just after the emergence of identifiable sleep-wake states (active and quiet sleep) but coincides with subsequent key adjustments that occur in the proportions of time the fetus spends in IS and QS (achieved by 35–36 weeks gestation to approximate the sleep states of the full term infant) (Okai et al., 1992). As a result, disruptions during this period and later may interfere with optimal sleep patterns and lead to the disorganized sleep states noted above. With regards to particular sleep outcomes, our findings were particularly strong for relationships between prenatal PM2.5 exposure and nocturnal sleep efficiency during early gestation. The specific timing of the sensitive window of 1–8 weeks gestation and the association during that window was consistently estimated with a number of different degrees of freedom for the DLM. The result is further supported by both the significant cumulative effect from the DLM and the association with mean PM2.5 exposure averaged over pregnancy, and in particular, the first trimester. For sleep duration, we found a negative association during in weeks 30–32 weeks. However, we also identified a late gestational window at 39–40 weeks whereby increased PM2.5 exposure was associated with increased sleep duration, though models estimating associations with sleep duration were less robust to the parameterization. Nonetheless, we speculate that while decreased sleep time may be
the pathologic result of a reduced inhibitory state and increased degree of arousals, increased sleep time may reflect the need to increase sleep time to overcome toxin-induced poor sleep efficiency. Further studies are warranted to replicate these findings and investigate pathologic mechanisms underlying these observed relationships. Our study has several strengths to highlight. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the effects of prenatal ambient particulate pollutant exposures on sleep outcomes in children. While a few recent studies have considered post-natal air pollutant exposures and sleep quality, no study, to our knowledge, has investigated the effect of exposures that occur during the prenatal period, when the foundation of sleep architecture is established. The relationships we report between PM2.5 exposures during pregnancy and sleep outcomes in childhood emphasize the importance of critical periods during gestation for which early alteration of sleep programming can have lasting results. In addition, we have leveraged highly spatially (1 km × 1 km) and temporally resolved (daily) ambient pollution data reflecting individual maternal environmental exposure estimates during the complete period of pregnancy to conduct data-driven analytic approaches that identify sensitive windows most susceptible to pollutant effects. These approaches overcome the potential bias that is introduced by considering exposure timing by arbitrary cut-offs (e.g. trimesters) (Wilson et al., 2017b), and place critical windows within the context of embryologic processes relevant to sleep development. Certain limitations worth noting include the generalizability of our study to other populations, given that this is a relatively homogenous Mexican cohort in an urban area with higher levels of air pollution. In addition, as the literature on prenatal environmental factors known to influence sleep development is relatively sparse, we may not have accounted for unidentified confounders in our models. Notably, we considered a number of key covariates known to be significant from prior work regarding particulate air pollution and other neurodevelopmental outcomes. Furthermore, while the associations we report have biological plausibility, further work is needed to strengthen potential causal relationships between prenatal pollutant exposure and child sleep outcomes. Lastly, we used accelerometry to capture sleep-wake patterns over time, which is limited in that it estimates sleep duration by movement and does not identify EEG-defined staging of sleep. Nevertheless, actigraphy allowed us to measure sleep duration and efficiency in a large cohort of children within their home environment over several nights to generate a robust dataset reflective of sleep patterns. Future studies using polysomnography to assess for sleep disordered breathing, movement disorders, and analysis of sleep stage cycling, continuity, and spectral analysis will help further characterize the adverse effects of prenatal PM2.5 exposure and the development of healthy sleep in children. In conclusion, increased prenatal exposure to PM2.5 during multiple critical windows over gestation was associated with altered sleep duration and decreased sleep efficiency in preschool-aged children. Given the high prevalence of sleep disorders in early childhood, as well as the tendency of these sleep abnormalities to persist later in life, these findings are not only significant for young children, but may have implications for sleep health across the lifespan. Furthermore, in light of the known adverse effects of inadequate sleep upon multiple other trajectories of health outcomes (e.g. metabolic, cognitive, behavioral), identification of early modifiable risk factors influencing suboptimal sleep programming is critical for designing targeted interventions to minimize potentially irreversible, compounding developmental consequences. Funding sources Supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants: P30 ES023515, R01ES014930, R01ES013744, R01ES021357, P30ES009089, P30ES023515, and R24ES028522. Statistical analysis consultation was received through Conduits, the Mount Sinai Health 333
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
System Clinical Translational Science Award (UL1TR001433) Funding sources had no role in the writing of the manuscript or decision to submit for publication. The corresponding author had full access to the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.
examining sensitive windows and sex-specific associations. Environ. Int. 87, 56–65 Feb. Costa, L.G., Cole, T.B., Coburn, J., Chang, Y.C., Dao, K., Roque, P.J., 2017. Neurotoxicity of traffic-related air pollution. Neurotoxicology 59, 133–139 Mar. Curzi-Dascalova, L., Peirano, P., Morel-Kahn, F., 1988. Development of sleep states in normal premature and full-term newborns. Dev. Psychobiol. 21 (5), 431–444 Jul. Fenton, T.R., Kim, J.H., 2013. A systematic review and meta-analysis to revise the Fenton growth chart for preterm infants. BMC Pediatr. 13 Apr 20. 59-2431-13-59. Fricke-Oerkermann, L., Pluck, J., Schredl, M., Heinz, K., Mitschke, A., Wiater, A., et al., 2007. Prevalence and course of sleep problems in childhood. Sleep 30 (10), 1371–1377 Oct. Gruber, R., Wiebe, S., Montecalvo, L., Brunetti, B., Amsel, R., Carrier, J., 2011. Impact of sleep restriction on neurobehavioral functioning of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sleep 34 (3), 315–323 Mar 1. Guglielmo, D., Gazmararian, J.A., Chung, J., Rogers, A.E., Hale, L., 2018. Racial/ethnic sleep disparities in US school-aged children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Sleep Health 4 (1), 68–80 Feb. Hibbs, A.M., Storfer-Isser, A., Rosen, C., Ievers-Landis, C.E., Taveras, E.M., Redline, S., 2014. Advanced sleep phase in adolescents born preterm. Behav. Sleep Med. 12 (5), 412–424 Sep 3. Holz, N.E., Boecker, R., Baumeister, S., Hohm, E., Zohsel, K., Buchmann, A.F., et al., 2014. Effect of prenatal exposure to tobacco smoke on inhibitory control: neuroimaging results from a 25-year prospective study. JAMA Psychiat. 71 (7), 786–796 Jul 1. Horne, R., Franco, P., Adamson, T.M., Groswasser, J., Kahn, A., 2004. Influences of maternal cigarette smoking on infant arousability. Early Hum. Dev. 79 (1), 49–58 Aug. Hunter, S.J., Gozal, D., Smith, D.L., Philby, M.F., Kaylegian, J., Kheirandish-Gozal, L., 2016. Effect of sleep-disordered breathing severity on cognitive performance measures in a large community cohort of young school-aged children. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 194 (6), 739–747 Sep 15. Just, A.C., Wright, R.O., Schwartz, J., Coull, B.A., Baccarelli, A.A., Tellez-Rojo, M.M., et al., 2015. Using High-resolution satellite aerosol optical depth to estimate daily PM2.5 geographical distribution in Mexico City. Environ. Sci. Technol. 49 (14), 8576–8584 Jul. Kuula, L., Pesonen, A.K., Merikanto, I., Gradisar, M., Lahti, J., Heinonen, K., et al., 2018. Development of Late Circadian preference: sleep timing from childhood to late adolescence. J. Pediatr. 194 (Mar 182-189.e1). Marcus, C.L., Moore, R.H., Rosen, C.L., Giordani, B., Garetz, S.L., Taylor, H.G., et al., 2013. A randomized trial of adenotonsillectomy for childhood sleep apnea. N. Engl. J. Med. 368 (25), 2366–2376 Jun 20. Martinez, S.M., Tschann, J.M., Butte, N.F., Gregorich, S.E., Penilla, C., Flores, E., et al., 2018. Sleep duration in Mexican American children: Do mothers' and fathers' parenting and family practices play a role? J. Sleep Res., e12784 (Nov 5). Medic, G., Wille, M., Hemels, M.E., 2017. Short- and long-term health consequences of sleep disruption. Nat. Sci. Sleep 9, 151–161 May 19. Melendres, M.C., Lutz, J.M., Rubin, E.D., Marcus, C.L., 2004. Daytime sleepiness and hyperactivity in children with suspected sleep-disordered breathing. Pediatrics 114 (3), 768–775 Sep. Mindell, J.A., Kuhn, B., Lewin, D.S., Meltzer, L.J., Sadeh, A., American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2006. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings in infants and young children. Sleep 29 (10), 1263–1276 Oct. Nawrot, T.S., Saenen, N.D., Schenk, J., Janssen, B.G., Motta, V., Tarantini, L., et al., 2018. Placental circadian pathway methylation and in utero exposure to fine particle air pollution. Environ. Int. 114, 231–241 May. O'Callaghan, F.V., Al Mamun, A., O'Callaghan, M., Clavarino, A., Williams, G.M., Bor, W., et al., 2010. The link between sleep problems in infancy and early childhood and attention problems at 5 and 14 years: evidence from a birth cohort study. Early Hum. Dev. 86 (7), 419–424 Jul. Okai, T., Kozuma, S., Shinozuka, N., Kuwabara, Y., Mizuno, M., 1992. A study on the development of sleep-wakefulness cycle in the human fetus. Early Hum. Dev. 29 (1–3), 391–396 Jun-Jul. Owens, J.A., 2005. Epidemiology of sleep disorders during childhood. In: Sheldon, S.H., Ferber, R., Kryger, M.H. (Eds.), Principles and Practices of Pediatric Sleep Medicine Philadelphia. Elsevier Saunders, PA, pp. 27–33. Parrino, L., Vaudano, A.E., 2018. The resilient brain and the guardians of sleep: new perspectives on old assumptions. Sleep Med. Rev. 39, 98–107 Jun. Paruthi, S., Brooks, L.J., D'Ambrosio, C., Hall, W.A., Kotagal, S., Lloyd, R.M., et al., 2016. Consensus statement of the American Academy of sleep medicine on the recommended amount of sleep for healthy children: methodology and discussion. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 12 (11), 1549–1561 Nov 15. Peterson, B.S., Rauh, V.A., Bansal, R., Hao, X., Toth, Z., Nati, G., et al., 2015. Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood. JAMA Psychiat. 72 (6), 531–540 Jun. Pujol, J., Martinez-Vilavella, G., Macia, D., Fenoll, R., Alvarez-Pedrerol, M., Rivas, I., et al., 2016. Traffic pollution exposure is associated with altered brain connectivity in school children. NeuroImage 129, 175–184 Apr 1. Reynaud, E., Forhan, A., Heude, B., Charles, M.A., Plancoulaine, S., EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study Group, 2018. Night-waking and behavior in preschoolers: a developmental trajectory approach. Sleep Med. 43, 90–95 Mar. Rosa, M.J., Pajak, A., Just, A.C., Sheffield, P.E., Kloog, I., Schwartz, J., et al., 2017. Prenatal exposure to PM2.5 and birth weight: a pooled analysis from three North American longitudinal pregnancy cohort studies. Environ. Int. 107, 173–180 Oct. Sadeh, A., Sharkey, K.M., Carskadon, M.A., 1994. Activity-based sleep-wake identification: an empirical test of methodological issues. Sleep 17 (3), 201–207 Apr. Scheinost, D., Kwon, S.H., Shen, X., Lacadie, C., Schneider, K.C., Dai, F., et al., 2016.
Conflicts of interest The authors share no financial conflicts of interest. Appendix A. Supplementary data Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2019.01.012. References Abou-Khadra, M.K., 2013. Association between PM(1)(0) exposure and sleep of Egyptian school children. Sleep Breath. 17 (2), 653–657 May. Accinelli, R.A., Llanos, O., Lopez, L.M., Matayoshi, S., Oros, Y.P., Kheirandish-Gozal, L., et al., 2015. Caregiver perception of sleep-disordered breathing-associated symptoms in children of rural Andean communities above 4000 masl with chronic exposure to biomass fuel. Sleep Med. 16 (6), 723–728 Jun. Archbold, K.H., Pituch, K.J., Panahi, P., Chervin, R.D., 2002. Symptoms of sleep disturbances among children at two general pediatric clinics. J. Pediatr. 140 (1), 97–102 Jan. Bathory, E., Tomopoulos, S., 2017. Sleep regulation, physiology and development, sleep duration and patterns, and sleep hygiene in infants, toddlers, and preschool-age children. Curr. Probl. Pediatr. Adolesc. Health Care 47 (2), 29–42 Feb. Bennet, L., Walker, D.W., Horne, R.S.C., 2018a. Waking up too early - the consequences of preterm birth on sleep development. J. Physiol. 596 (23), 5687–5708 Dec. Bennet, L., Dhillon, S., Lear, C.A., van den Heuij, L., King, V., Dean, J.M., et al., 2018b. Chronic inflammation and impaired development of the preterm brain. J. Reprod. Immunol. 125, 45–55 Feb. Biggs, S.N., Vlahandonis, A., Anderson, V., Bourke, R., Nixon, G.M., Davey, M.J., et al., 2014. Long-term changes in neurocognition and behavior following treatment of sleep disordered breathing in school-aged children. Sleep 37 (1), 77–84 Jan 1. Biggs, S.N., Walter, L.M., Jackman, A.R., Nisbet, L.C., Weichard, A.J., Hollis, S.L., et al., 2015. Long-term cognitive and behavioral outcomes following resolution of sleep disordered breathing in preschool children. PLoS One 10 (9), e0139142 (Sep 29). Block, M.L., Elder, A., Auten, R.L., Bilbo, S.D., Chen, H., Chen, J.C., et al., 2012. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop. Neurotoxicology 33 (5), 972–984 Oct. Blunden, S., Galland, B., 2014. The complexities of defining optimal sleep: empirical and theoretical considerations with a special emphasis on children. Sleep Med. Rev. 18 (5), 371–378 Oct. Bonuck, K., Freeman, K., Chervin, R.D., Xu, L., 2012. Sleep-disordered breathing in a population-based cohort: behavioral outcomes at 4 and 7 years. Pediatrics 129 (4), e857–e865 Apr. Bose, S., Chiu, Y.M., Hsu, H.L., Di, Q., Rosa, M.J., Lee, A., et al., 2017. Prenatal nitrate exposure and childhood asthma. Influence of maternal prenatal stress and fetal sex. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 196 (11), 1396–1403 Dec 1. Bose, S., Rosa, M.J., Mathilda Chiu, Y.H., Leon Hsu, H.H., Di, Q., Lee, A., et al., 2018. Prenatal nitrate air pollution exposure and reduced child lung function: timing and fetal sex effects. Environ. Res. 167, 591–597 Nov. Brooks, P.L., Peever, J.H., 2011. Impaired GABA and glycine transmission triggers cardinal features of rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in mice. J. Neurosci. 31 (19), 7111–7121 May 11. Burris, H.H., Braun, J.M., Byun, H.M., Tarantini, L., Mercado, A., Wright, R.J., et al., 2013. Association between birth weight and DNA methylation of IGF2, glucocorticoid receptor and repetitive elements LINE-1 and Alu. Epigenomics 5 (3), 271–281 Jun. Calderon-Garciduenas, L., Leray, E., Heydarpour, P., Torres-Jardon, R., Reis, J., 2016. Air pollution, a rising environmental risk factor for cognition, neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration: the clinical impact on children and beyond. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 172 (1), 69–80 Jan. Caldwell, B.A., Redeker, N.S., 2015. Maternal stress and psychological status and sleep in minority preschool children. Public Health Nurs. 32 (2), 101–111. Chaput, J.P., Gray, C.E., Poitras, V.J., Carson, V., Gruber, R., Olds, T., et al., 2016. Systematic review of the relationships between sleep duration and health indicators in school-aged children and youth. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 41 (6 Suppl. 3), S266–S282 Jun. Chatterton, Z., Hartley, B.J., Seok, M.H., Mendelev, N., Chen, S., Milekic, M., et al., 2017. In utero exposure to maternal smoking is associated with DNA methylation alterations and reduced neuronal content in the developing fetal brain. Epigenetics Chromatin 10 Jan 26. (4-017-0111-y. eCollection 2017). Chervin, R.D., Ruzicka, D.L., Giordani, B.J., Weatherly, R.A., Dillon, J.E., Hodges, E.K., et al., 2006. Sleep-disordered breathing, behavior, and cognition in children before and after adenotonsillectomy. Pediatrics 117 (4), e769–e778 Apr. Chiu, Y.H., Hsu, H.H., Coull, B.A., Bellinger, D.C., Kloog, I., Schwartz, J., et al., 2016. Prenatal particulate air pollution and neurodevelopment in urban children:
334
Environment International 124 (2019) 329–335
S. Bose et al.
cocaine and other drugs. Behav. Sleep Med. 7 (4), 196–207. Stone, K.C., LaGasse, L.L., Lester, B.M., Shankaran, S., Bada, H.S., Bauer, C.R., et al., 2010. Sleep problems in children with prenatal substance exposure: the Maternal Lifestyle study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 164 (5), 452–456 May. Tenero, L., Piacentini, G., Nosetti, L., Gasperi, E., Piazza, M., Zaffanello, M., 2017. Indoor/ outdoor not-voluptuary-habit pollution and sleep-disordered breathing in children: a systematic review. Transl. Pediatr. 6 (2), 104–110 Apr. Touchette, E., Cote, S.M., Petit, D., Liu, X., Boivin, M., Falissard, B., et al., 2009. Short nighttime sleep-duration and hyperactivity trajectories in early childhood. Pediatrics 124 (5), e985–e993 Nov. Tymofiyeva, O., Hess, C.P., Ziv, E., Tian, N., Bonifacio, S.L., McQuillen, P.S., et al., 2012. Towards the “baby connectome”: mapping the structural connectivity of the newborn brain. PLoS One 7 (2), e31029 (Feb). Wilson, A., Chiu, Y.M., Hsu, H.L., Wright, R.O., Wright, R.J., Coull, B.A., 2017a. Bayesian distributed lag interaction models to identify perinatal windows of vulnerability in children's health. Biostatistics 18 (3), 537–552 Jul 1. Wilson, A., Chui, Y.M., Hsu, H.L., Wright, R.O., Wright, R.J., Coull, B.A., 2017b. Potential for bias when estimating critical windows for air pollution in children's health. Am. J. Epidemiol. 186 (11), 1281–1289 Dec. Yiallourou, S.R., Arena, B.C., Wallace, E.M., Odoi, A., Hollis, S., Weichard, A., et al., 2017. Being born too small and too early may alter sleep in childhood. Sleep Nov;Epub ahead of print. Yokota, S., Moriya, N., Iwata, M., Umezawa, M., Oshio, S., Takeda, K., 2013. Exposure to diesel exhaust during fetal period affects behavior and neurotransmitters in male offspring mice. J. Toxicol. Sci. 38 (1), 13–23 Feb.
Preterm birth alters neonatal, functional rich club organization. Brain Struct. Funct. 221 (6), 3211–3222 Jul. Scher, M.S., Loparo, K.A., 2009. Neonatal EEG/sleep state analyses: a complex phenotype of developmental neural plasticity. Dev. Neurosci. 31 (4), 259–275 Jan. Scher, M.S., Steppe, D.A., Dahl, R.E., Asthana, S., Guthrie, R.D., 1992. Comparison of EEG sleep measures in healthy full-term and preterm infants at matched conceptional ages. Sleep 15 (5), 442–448 Oct. Short, M.A., Blunden, S., Rigney, G., Matricciani, L., Coussens, S., M Reynolds, C., et al., 2018. Cognition and objectively measured sleep duration in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Health 4 (3), 292–300 Jun. Sivertsen, B., Harvey, A.G., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Torgersen, L., Ystrom, E., Hysing, M., 2015. Later emotional and behavioral problems associated with sleep problems in toddlers: a longitudinal study. JAMA Pediatr. 169 (6), 575–582 Jun. Smith, A.M., Dwoskin, L.P., Pauly, J.R., 2010. Early exposure to nicotine during critical periods of brain development: mechanisms and consequences. J. Pediatr. Biochem. 1 (2), 125–141. Stein, M.A., Mendelsohn, J., Obermeyer, W.H., Amromin, J., Benca, R., 2001. Sleep and behavior problems in school-aged children. Pediatrics 107 (4), E60 (Apr). Stephan-Blanchard, E., Telliez, F., Leke, A., Djeddi, D., Bach, V., Libert, J.P., et al., 2008. The influence of in utero exposure to smoking on sleep patterns in preterm neonates. Sleep 31 (12), 1683–1689 Dec. Stieb, D.M., Chen, L., Eshoul, M., Judek, S., 2012. Ambient air pollution, birth weight and preterm birth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ. Res. 117, 100–111 Aug. Stone, K.C., High, P.C., Miller-Loncar, C.L., Lagasse, L.L., Lester, B.M., 2009. Longitudinal study of maternal report of sleep problems in children with prenatal exposure to
335