Accepted Manuscript ‘Primary’ and ‘secondary’ variants of psychopathy in a volunteer sample are associated with different neurocognitive mechanisms Arjun Sethi, Eamon McCrory, Vanessa Puetz, Ferdinand Hoffmann, Annchen R. Knodt, Spenser R. Radtke, Bartholomew D. Brigidi, Ahmad R. Hariri, Essi Viding PII:
S2451-9022(18)30082-X
DOI:
10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.002
Reference:
BPSC 272
To appear in:
Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging
Received Date: 27 November 2017 Revised Date:
7 March 2018
Accepted Date: 5 April 2018
Please cite this article as: Sethi A., McCrory E., Puetz V., Hoffmann F., Knodt A.R., Radtke S.R., Brigidi B.D., Hariri A.R. & Viding E., ‘Primary’ and ‘secondary’ variants of psychopathy in a volunteer sample are associated with different neurocognitive mechanisms, Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging (2018), doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.002. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
‘Primary’ and ‘secondary’ variants of psychopathy in a volunteer sample are associated with different neurocognitive mechanisms Arjun Sethia, Eamon McCrorya, Vanessa Puetza, Ferdinand Hoffmanna, Annchen R. Knodtb,
RI PT
Spenser R. Radtkeb, Bartholomew D. Brigidib, Ahmad R. Haririb, Essi Vidinga
AFFILIATIONS
SC
a) Developmental Risk & Resilience Unit, Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, UK
M AN U
b) Laboratory of NeuroGenetics, Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, USA
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR
TE D
Arjun Sethi: Developmental Risk & Resilience Unit, Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London, WC1H 0AP. Phone: +44 (0)207 679 5367, e-mail:
AC C
EP
[email protected].
WORD COUNTS: Abstract: 245, Body: 3,946, References 62, Figures: 1, Tables: 3, Supplements: 1
SHORT TITLE: Primary and secondary psychopathy differ neurally KEY WORDS: psychopathy; maltreatment; anxiety; fear; amygdala; fMRI 1
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Introduction: Recent work has indicated that there at least two distinct subtypes of psychopathy. Whilst ‘primary’ psychopathy is characterized by low anxiety and thought to result from a genetic predisposition, ‘secondary’ psychopathy is characterized by high anxiety and thought to develop in response to environmental adversity. Primary psychopathy
RI PT
is robustly associated with reduced neural activation to others’ emotions and, in particular, distress. However, it has been proposed that the secondary presentation has different neurocognitive correlates.
SC
Methods: Primary (n=50), Secondary (n=100) and Comparison (n=82) groups were drawn from a large volunteer sample (n=1444) using a quartile-split approach across psychopathic
M AN U
trait (affective-interpersonal) and anxiety measures. Participants performed a widely-utilized emotional face processing task during fMRI.
Results: The Primary group showed reduced amygdala and insula activity to fear. The Secondary group did not differ from Comparisons in these regions. Instead, they showed reduced activity compared to Comparisons in several areas including the superior temporal
TE D
sulcus/inferior parietal lobe, thalamus, pallidum, and substantia nigra. Both groups with psychopathic features also showed reduced activity in response to fear in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). During anger processing, the Secondary group exhibited reduced
EP
activity in the ACC in comparison with the Primary group.
AC C
Conclusions: Distinct neural correlates of fear processing characterize individuals with primary and secondary psychopathic features. The reduced neural response to fear that characterizes individuals with the primary variant of psychopathic traits is not observed in those with the secondary presentation. The neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning secondary psychopathy warrant further, systematic investigation.
2
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
INTRODUCTION Psychopathy is a personality disorder characterized by criminal and violent behavior (1–3). What distinguishes individuals with psychopathy from their peers with disruptive behavior disorders is their lack of empathy, shallow affect and callous treatment of others. This set of
RI PT
interpersonal and affective traits are captured by Factor 1 of Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (PCL-R), which dissociates from a second factor (Factor 2) that captures a less aetiologically specific range of antisocial behaviors and lifestyle factors, which are also
SC
shared with other individuals with disruptive behavior disorders (4). Psychopathic traits as captured both by Factor 1 and Factor 2 symptoms are continuously distributed in the
M AN U
population and can also be reliably measured in volunteer samples with instruments that have specifically been validated for that purpose (5–9). Individuals diagnosed with criminal psychopathy as well as those with high levels of Factor 1 traits in volunteer samples show attenuated physiological and neural responses to affective stimuli, including stimuli indexing other people’s distress (10–14). This blunted reactivity to others’ distress is thought, at least
TE D
in part, to explain why individuals with high levels of Factor 1 traits find it unproblematic to aggress against other people and do not readily empathise with their victims (15). The original descriptions of psychopathy describe an individual with marked lack of anxiety
EP
and consideration for others (16), and indeed, anxiety does appear at odds with the callous and unempathetic presentation that Factor 1 traits describe. Yet accumulating evidence now
AC C
suggests that these traits (or at least behaviors that can look callous and unempathetic) may co-occur with high levels of anxiety in some individuals (17, 18). Such individuals have been described as presenting with a ‘secondary’ variant of psychopathy, in contrast with the ‘primary’ variant that occurs in the absence of clinically significant anxiety. This primary/secondary distinction has also been drawn in youths presenting with callousunemotional traits, who are thought to be at risk of developing psychopathy later in life (19, 20). (Note, some studies have employed a different nosological use of ‘secondary’ psychopathy, where the term refers to individuals who present with Factor 2 3
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(lifestyle/antisocial behaviors) but not Factor 1 traits of psychopathy e.g. (21). Here, we use the term ‘secondary’ psychopathy to refer to those individuals who present with psychopathic features and anxiety). In both childhood and adulthood, those with a secondary presentation do not appear to
RI PT
present with a simple combination of callous/unempathetic and anxious vulnerabilities, but rather show distinct behavioral and psychiatric features not explained by either set of vulnerabilities alone (20, 22–24).
Such
a secondary presentation has been linked to
SC
experiences of significant childhood adversity (23), and is proposed to have a distinct aetiology from the primary, putatively heritable, presentation (25). Secondary psychopathy
M AN U
has therefore been suggested to represent a ‘behavioral phenocopy’ of primary callous and unempathetic traits - arising as an adaptation to environmental adversity (25). In addition to a range of studies indicating heightened levels of childhood maltreatment experience in the secondary group, epigenetic evidence has also provided preliminary support for distinct aetiological pathways to callous-unemotional (Factor 1 type) traits in primary and secondary
TE D
subtype youths (26). Given the potentially distinct aetiologies and profiles, it appears highly likely that the neural mechanisms underpinning the primary and secondary presentations might differ, yet this has not been investigated to date.
EP
Blunted amygdala response to others’ distress, in particular fear, is recognised as a characteristic neurocognitive deficit in primary psychopathy (13). It is thought that this
AC C
reduced capacity to affectively resonate with others’ distress is central to the callous and unempathetic presentation of these individuals (15). However, Factor 1 behavioral presentation in secondary psychopathy may arise from distinct (or partially overlapping) neural mechanisms. Given that both anxiety and maltreatment are typically associated with hyper-responsivity to emotionally evocative stimuli (27, 28) one would assume that individuals with a secondary presentation would share such hyper-responsivity. That is, they would not be expected to share the pattern of hypo-responsivity to emotionally evocative stimuli associated with primary psychopathy presentation. In line with this, behavioral 4
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
findings have suggested that individuals with secondary psychopathy display greater attentional capture to others’ distress than individuals with primary psychopathy (29). For the first time, we directly compared amygdala response to fear in individuals with high Factor 1 traits and low anxiety (‘Primary’ psychopathic presentation), individuals with high
RI PT
Factor 1 traits and high anxiety (‘Secondary’ psychopathic presentation), and individuals who did not have elevated levels of either trait/symptom domain, drawn from a large volunteer sample with a wide range of behavioral presentation. We predicted that amygdala hypo-
SC
reactivity would be related to the primary, but not secondary psychopathic presentation. We also conducted exploratory analyses to investigate whether processing of anger differed in
M AN U
these groups. Anger is a threat emotion with heightened salience to individuals with anxiety and individuals with maltreatment histories (28, 30–32) and disruption to anger processing (heightened amygdala response and lower prefrontal regulatory response) has been proposed to explain increased threat reactive aggression sometimes seen in these populations (33). Given the rich behavioral and psychiatric data available in this sample, we
TE D
were also able to replicate previous work (conducted in forensic and at-risk samples) regarding the characteristic behavioral and psychiatric profile associated with secondary psychopathy.
AC C
Participants
EP
METHODS AND MATERIALS
Participants were drawn from a dataset of 1144 individuals enrolled into the Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS), which assessed a range of behavioral and biological traits among non-patient, young adult, student volunteers. Participants were excluded from the DNS in cases of: a) medical diagnosis of cancer, stroke, diabetes requiring insulin treatment, chronic kidney or liver disease, or lifetime history of psychotic symptoms, b) use of psychotropic glucocorticoid, or hypolipidemic medication, or c) conditions affecting cerebral blood flow and metabolism. Individuals were not excluded for past or present diagnosis for
5
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
DSM-IV Axis 1 or select Axis II (antisocial and personality) disorders so that a broad range of behavioral phenotypes related to psychopathology could be assessed. However, none of the participants were taking psychoactive medications for at least 14 days prior to their study enrolment. All participants provided informed consent in accordance with Duke University
RI PT
guidelines, were in good general health and met study inclusion criteria. Measures
Measures used to create the groups: Psychopathic traits were measured using the Self-
SC
Report Psychopathy – Short Form (SRP–SF) (8). The SRP-SF consists of 29 questions divisible into four factors: Interpersonal, affective, lifestyle and antisocial. For the purposes of
M AN U
deriving meaningful subgroups according to Factor 1 traits in this study, interpersonal and affective scales were summed to provide a measure similar to Factor 1 of the PCL-R (4), the current gold standard in assessing psychopathy. Similarly, lifestyle and antisocial facets were summed in order to make a comparative measure to Factor 2 measure of the PCL-R. Anxious disposition was assessed using the 20 question trait measure of the State and Trait
TE D
Anxiety Inventory (STAIT-T) (34).
Measures of cognitive ability, current functioning and lifetime diagnoses: IQ was measured
EP
using full scale scores from the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (35). Previous history of maltreatment was assessed using the Child Trauma Questionnaire
AC C
(CTQ) (36). Current functioning was assessed as follows: aggression was measured using the Buss Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ) (37); anger was measured using the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory – trait subscale (STAXI) (38); and impulsivity was measured with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (39); perception of own stress was measured with the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (40); current anxious and depressive symptomatology were measured with the Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (MASQ) (41, 42); alcohol and recreational drug use were assessed with the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (43) and the Recreational Drug Use (RDU) questionnaire; The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (44) was used to 6
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
assess lifetime diagnoses of alcohol abuse, substance abuse, major depressive disorder (MDD), generalised anxiety disorder (GAD), bipolar disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), antisocial personality disorder (APSD), borderline personality disorder (BPD), two year risk of suicidality.
RI PT
Group definition
Previous studies examining psychopathy and anxiety in at-risk and forensic samples have used median split or clustering approaches. To ensure that we captured participants with an
SC
elevated psychopathic and anxious trait profile in this high-functioning volunteer sample we adopted a quartile split approach using SRP interpersonal-affective (Factor 1) scores and
M AN U
STAI-T scores to derive Primary, Secondary, and Comparison groups in this sample: i) Comparison if scoring within the bottom quartile for both Factor 1 and anxiety measures (n=82), ii) Primary if scoring within the top quartile for Factor 1 and the lower quartile for anxiety (n=50), and iii) Secondary if scoring within the top quartile for both Factor 1 and
TE D
anxiety measures (n=100). Task
We employed a widely adopted blocked perceptual face processing paradigm during which
EP
subjects perform a match-to-sample of emotional or neutral faces during the task blocks and geometric shapes during the control blocks. This paradigm has been shown to robustly elicit
AC C
amygdala reactivity over a wide variety of protocols and populations (45–50). Details for the paradigm version used in the DNS are provided in the supplementary methods fMRI Analysis
At the first level, regressors for each condition of interest were entered into the general linear model (GLM) from which contrasts of interest (Fear>Shapes, Anger>Shapes) were derived. Contrasts of interest were then individually entered into second level random-effects model to assess group differences in a pairwise manner. Statistical maps were initially produced with an uncorrected threshold of p<0.005. For whole brain analyses, a family-wise error 7
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(FWE) correction of p < 0.05 at the cluster level was used to correct for multiple comparisons. To test specific hypotheses about reactivity in the amygdala and OFC these regions of interest (ROIs) were assessed using a voxel-wise small volume corrected FWE p<0.05. A bilateral amygdala ROI was derived from the AAL atlas. A bilateral anterior insula
RI PT
ROI was derived from Deen et al (51). The OFC ROI was a 12mm radius sphere (with areas of non-brain overlap removed) centred on peak activation coordinates from a prior study that found differences in OFC anger activity in individuals with impulsive aggression (52), and consistent with prior work examining OFC activity to anger (53). Due to group differences in
SC
sex composition within each group all fMRI analyses controlled for sex. Cohen’s d was
M AN U
calculated for cluster maxima from t statistic maps as: ݀ = 2ݐ/√݂݀, where t is the t statistic and df is the degrees of freedom associated with the test. Behavioral analysis
Binary logistic regressions were planned to analyse categorical data including prior
TE D
psychiatric diagnoses, however this was not possible in several instances due to complete/quasi-complete separation of data in several variables. For consistency, all categorical data were therefore analysed using chi square tests. Continuous data were analysed using ANOVAs. All behavioral analyses were performed using SPSS v22
AC C
RESULTS
EP
(http://www.ibm.com/software/products/en/spss-statistics).
Demographics, current functioning and lifetime psychiatric diagnoses All demographic information is listed in Supplementary Table 1. Age did not significantly differ between groups. There was a significant difference in the proportion of males and females across groups (X2(2, 229) = 47.16, p < 0.001; % Female: 42% Secondary, 18% Primary, 75% Comparison). There was a trend towards overall group difference in IQ (F(2, 227)=2.68, p=0.070), however group mean IQs were of highly similar levels (Group mean range: 120.57-123.86, see Supplementary Table 1). 8
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Psychopathy scores In line with previous research, both Primary (p<0.001) and Secondary (p<0.001) groups had higher levels of Factor 1 traits than the Comparison group but importantly did not differ from each other (p=0.710). Both Primary and Secondary groups had higher scores than the
RI PT
Comparison group on Factor 2 of the SRP (all p<0.001), and the Secondary group had higher Factor 2 scores than the Primary group (p=0.028). See Supplementary Table 1 for more detail.
SC
Internalising symptoms
The Primary group did not differ from the Comparison group on current symptomatology of
M AN U
depression (p=1.000) or anxiety (p=1.000) or on diagnoses of major depressive disorder (p = 0.199) or generalised anxiety disorder (p=1.000). The Secondary group differed from both Primary and Comparison groups on depressive and anxious symptoms (all p<0.001). The Secondary group differed from the Primary (p=0.005) and Comparison (p<0.001) groups in
TE D
depression diagnoses. Additionally, the Secondary group had significantly more diagnoses of generalised anxiety disorder than the Comparison group (p=0.014) with a trend to significance when compared to the primary group (p=0.054). The Secondary group also
EP
showed higher diagnoses of bipolar disorder compared to Comparison (p=0.001) and Primary (p=0.039) groups, whilst the Primary and Comparison groups did not differ from
AC C
each other (p=0.199). See Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 for more detail. Externalising symptoms
Both the Primary and Secondary group had greater alcohol (both: p<0.001) and substance use (Primary: p=0.002; Secondary: p<0.001) than the Comparison group but did not differ from each other (both: p=1.000). However, results examining lifetime DSM-IV diagnoses differed from this, with the Secondary group more likely to have received a diagnosis of alcohol (p = 0.002) and substance abuse compared to Comparisons (p=0.009) and the Primary group only being more likely to have a lifetime diagnosis of alcohol (p=0.015), but 9
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
not substance abuse (p = 0.299) compared to Comparisons. The primary and secondary groups did not differ from each other in likelihood of alcohol (p=0.654) or substance abuse diagnoses (p=0.184). Similar to prior research, both Primary and Secondary groups differed from the Comparison
RI PT
group on aggression, anger, and impulsivity measures (all p<0.001), but also differed from each other (all p<0.001), with the Secondary group being most impaired on all measures. See Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 for more detail.
SC
Suicidality
The Secondary group had heightened risk of suicidality when compared to the Primary
M AN U
(p=0.024) and Comparison (p<0.001) groups. The Primary group had a trend-wise risk of increased suicidality compared to the Comparison group (p=0.068). See Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 for more detail. Maltreatment history
TE D
The Secondary group had more severe history of maltreatment than the Primary or Comparison groups (both: p<0.001) who did not differ from each other (p=0.575). This pattern was observed for each subscale of the CTQ except emotional neglect and sexual
EP
abuse (Supplementary table 1). Both Primary (p=0.018) and Secondary (p<0.001) groups had heightened emotional neglect compared to the Comparison group, though the
AC C
Secondary group still reported significantly higher levels of emotional neglect compared to the primary group (p<0.001). The Secondary group reported higher levels of sexual abuse when compared to Comparisons (p=0.024). The Primary group did not differ from either group (Comparison: p=1.000; Secondary: p=0.389).
Neurocognitive differences Neural responses to fear 10
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Consistent with previous studies, in comparison with the Comparison group the Primary group exhibited blunted responses to fearful facial expressions in the left amygdala (SVC: pFWE=0.044) with a trend in the right (SVC: pFWE=0.122), as well as in the bilateral anterior insula (SVC: Left: pFWE=0.012; Right: pFWE=0.038; Table 1; Figure 1). By contrast, the
RI PT
Secondary group showed no reductions in these regions when compared to the Comparison group. The Primary group also showed trend-level reductions in fear responses compared to the Secondary group within the right amygdala (SVC: pFWE=0.055) and bilateral anterior
SC
insula (SVC: Left: pFWE=0.063; Right: pFWE=0.070; Table 1; Figure 1).
In whole brain analyses the Primary group showed reduced activity in responses to fearful
M AN U
facial expressions in clusters encompassing the insula bilaterally, as well as a frontal cluster extending into the bilateral ACC, and the bilateral precuneus and surrounding regions within the cuneus and cingulate cortex (Table 2; Figure 1). Whole brain analyses also confirmed trends towards reduced activity within the bilateral insula in the Primary compared to the Secondary group (Table 2; Figure 1). Whilst the Secondary group also showed reductions
TE D
within the ACC compared to the Comparison group, another cluster extending into the left thalamus, bilateral substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA) and pallidum was also observed that was not seen when comparing Primary and Comparison groups.
EP
Neural responses to anger
AC C
Whole brain analyses revealed reduced bilateral ACC activity in response to angry facial expressions in the Secondary group compared to the Primary group (pFWE<0.011; Table 3; Figure 1). Even after SVC, there were no significant differences in OFC activity between any groups during processing of anger.
DISCUSSION Here, we provide novel evidence that individuals with secondary psychopathic features are not identical to the primary variant in neural activity to emotional facial expressions. 11
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Specifically, when processing fear secondary psychopathic features were associated with reduced activity in the superior temporal sulcus/inferior parietal lobe, pallidum, SN/VTA, and thalamus, while primary psychopathic features were associated with blunted activity in the amygdala and anterior insula. The primary and secondary variants did, however, exhibit
RI PT
some overlap in their neurocognitive presentation, with reduced activity in the anterior cingulate during fear processing observed in both groups in comparison with comparison participants. Contrary to our predictions, we did not find any evidence of altered processing
SC
of anger in the secondary psychopathy group when compared to controls.
In line with previous research in incarcerated and volunteer samples, we found that primary
M AN U
psychopathic features are associated with blunted amygdala and insula activity to fear (13 for a review). Importantly, our results extend this body of work to show that these particular neurocognitive hallmarks are observed only in the primary variant. The hypo-active neurocognitive profile in primary psychopathy is consistent with the ‘cold’ clinical presentation of these individuals, who exhibit reduced empathy and correspondingly, a
TE D
constellation of interpersonal behaviors that show limited regard for others. However, the presence of Factor 1 traits in the absence of this neurocognitive profile in individuals with secondary psychopathy suggest that they may arise via partly distinct mechanisms, though
EP
our data do not elucidate what these may be.
Based on prior research we expected that individuals with secondary psychopathic features
AC C
would show increased neural activity to others’ distress. In prior studies, individuals with secondary callous/unemotional traits have shown increased attentional capture to distressing stimuli compared to their low anxiety counterparts (though it is noteworthy that in this sample too, the secondary group did not differ from comparisons) (29), and higher startle modulation to aversive images when compared to primary and comparison groups (54). The absence of any differences between the secondary group and comparisons in amygdala activity to fear in the present study are not what one might have expected given the startle modulation data by Kimonis et al. (54). However, several methodological 12
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
differences should be noted when considering these findings. Firstly, previous studies have used distressing and aversive (including threatening and victim related) scenes that are intended to elicit robust behavioral responses, and may therefore represent more affectively salient stimuli than fearful facial expressions used in the present study. The use of fearful
RI PT
facial expressions afforded ready comparability with prior data on psychopathy/psychopathic traits in adults and enabled us to confirm that those with the primary variant presented in line with data from prior studies using such stimuli. Further studies with a variety of paradigms will be important to more comprehensively examine the neurocognitive deficits associated
SC
with the secondary variant. Secondly, differences between these studies may also reflect sampling strategies. Both previous studies used samples of juvenile offenders, contrasting to
M AN U
the approach we have adopted with student volunteers, which is likely to have captured a less severe secondary group. Though these factors may explain the inconsistencies between behavioral and neural measures of emotional responsiveness, it is noteworthy that the behavioral and psychological profile of the secondary subtype is highly consistent with
TE D
prior studies, suggesting that we did accurately capture primary and secondary groups. Another possibility is that whilst there is increased behavioral emotional reactivity in secondary individuals, this is determined by some other neural factor than amygdala hyper-
EP
activity.
Our analyses revealed several commonalities and differences in fear processing between
AC C
the two groups that may in part underpin the secondary presentation. Firstly, we show that both primary and secondary variants showed reduced activity in the ACC during fear processing, compared to the comparisons. The ACC has been variously associated with the acquisition, expression and extinction of fear conditioning, as well as the regulation of emotional conflict and the top down regulation of emotion (55). It may be that the reduced ACC activity reflects an impairment in a common mechanism for both groups; however, it is equally possible that ACC activity reflects different cognitive processes in the primary and secondary variants (e.g. relative impairments in conditioning to vs. regulation of distress
13
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
stimuli). This question needs to be investigated systematically in the future and (for example) related to the psychophysiological profile of the subtypes. Secondly, we show reduced activity in the SN/VTA, pallidum, and thalamus in response to fear that was unique to the secondary group. The VTA sends dopaminergic projections to the amygdala, where
RI PT
dopamine signalling plays an important role in the acquisition of fear responses (56). Similarly the thalamus has been linked to the acquisition and expression of fear conditioning (57, 58). The role of pallidum in such a model is less clear, though it sends extensive projections to the thalamus (59) and is also linked to the amygdala (60). Although purely
SC
speculative, a possible explanation for the presentation of Factor 1 traits in maltreated individuals may be abnormal development of fear conditioning responses. Future studies
M AN U
may seek to address how different components of fear conditioning are affected in primary and secondary psychopathy by using experimental paradigms that afford inference regarding this. Thirdly, superior temporal sulcus/temporoparietal junction activity was also observed to be hypoactive in the secondary group. These regions have been tightly linked to
TE D
perspective taking and inferring others mental states and emotions (61), and may reflect another mechanism by which blunted affect to others’ emotions could be instantiated in individuals with Factor 1 traits. However, future studies are required to systematically
EP
examine how these processes are altered in secondary psychopathy. Finally, we hypothesised that atypical or impaired anger processing within the OFC may play a role in
AC C
the development of callous aggressive behaviors in the secondary group. We did not find evidence for this hypothesis in the current sample, but it would be interesting to explore whether such a difference is observed in forensic or at-risk groups. In line with prior research into primary and secondary variants of psychopathy (20, 22, 23, 29, 54, 62–64), our volunteer sample of individuals with high psychopathic personality traits showed largely similar increases in externalising symptoms such as substance and alcohol abuse when compared with comparison participants. However, the primary and secondary groups differed significantly on several dimensions. Notably, the secondary group showed
14
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
markedly higher maltreatment history and internalising symptoms, as well as heightened levels of suicidality and aggression, than either the primary or comparison groups. Although the present work suggests a highly similar behavioral phenotype can be captured in volunteer samples to those in offending settings, additional studies are required to replicate
RI PT
our neurocognitive findings in forensic and vulnerable samples. Indeed, a study in forensic groups with high levels of maltreatment may provide a more stringent test of any effects of maltreatment on brain function and reactive aggression.
SC
Although our data suggest several possibilities about the neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning Factor 1 traits in the presence of anxiety, it is important not to rely on reverse
M AN U
inference and future studies must use methods that can directly interrogate a number of different processes. For instance, hypoactive fear processing in the thalamus, SN/VTA and ACC suggest abnormalities in fear conditioning may play a role in the development of Factor 1 traits in the presence of anxiety, possibly in response to environmental trauma. Assessing how different aspects of fear conditioning are altered in primary and secondary individuals,
TE D
and particularly perhaps in response to maltreatment, is therefore warranted. In conclusion, we provide novel evidence that individuals presenting with Factor 1 traits differ in their neurocognitive presentation depending on the presence or absence of concurrent
EP
anxiety. Whilst individuals with primary psychopathic features show blunted affective responses to others distress that has come to be seen as a central neurocognitive
AC C
phenotype of psychopathy, this pattern is not observed in individuals with a secondary psychopathic presentation. Whilst individuals high on Factor 1 traits appear to present with reduced ACC activity to fear independent of anxiety levels, those with the high-anxiety secondary variant of these traits appear to show additional abnormalities in circuitry that plays a role in fear learning and regions associated with mentalization. We do not, however, find amygdala hyper-activity to fear in the secondary group. Though this is unexpected, it is also not clear how such a hyper-activity could be understood alongside the callous disregard for others that is characterised by Factor 1 traits. Whilst future work will be required to 15
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
directly assess any mechanisms underpinning Factor 1 traits in the presence of anxiety, and whether these mechanisms do, as proposed, stem for childhood maltreatment, the present study shows a critical divergence in the neurocognitive presentation of these two groups.
RI PT
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank all members of the Laboratory of NeuroGenetics for their assistance in conducting the Duke Neurogenetics Study, which was supported by Duke University and NIH grant
SC
R01DA033369. A.R.H. is further supported by NIH grant R01AG049789. E.V. is a Royal
M AN U
Society Wolfson Research Merit Award holder and British Academy Mid-Career Fellow. DECLARATION OF CONFLICTSOF INTEREST
AC C
EP
TE D
The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
16
Sethi et al.
REFERENCES
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1. Hare RD, McPherson LM (1984): Violent and aggressive behavior by criminal
SC
psychopaths. Int J Law Psychiatry. 7: 35–50.
431.
M AN U
2. Serin RC (1991): Psychopathy and Violence in Criminals. J Interpers Violence. 6: 423–
3. Hare RD, Neumann CS (2008): Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 4: 217–46.
Health Systems Inc.
TE D
4. Hare RD (2003): The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (Hare PCL-R). Toronto: Multi
5. Salekin RT, Trobst KK, Krioukova M (2001): Construct validity of psychopathy in a
EP
community sample: a nomological net approach. J Pers Disord. 15: 425–41. 6. Neumann CS, Hare RD (2008): Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: links to
AC C
violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 76: 893–9. 7. Mahmut MK, Menictas C, Stevenson RJ, Homewood J (2011): Validating the factor structure of the Self-Report Psychopathy scale in a community sample. Psychol Assess. 23: 670–8.
8. Paulhus DL, Neumann CS, Hare RD (2015): Manual for the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale. (M.-H. Systems, editor). . 9. Neumann CS, Pardini D (2014): Factor Structure and Construct Validity of the Self-Report
17
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Psychopathy (SRP) Scale and the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI) in Young Men. J Pers Disord. 28: 419–433. 10. Patrick CJ, Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1993): Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle reflex modulation. J Abnorm Psychol. 102: 82–92.
RI PT
11. Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2005): Psychopathy, startle blink modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology. 42: 753–62.
12. Carré JM, Hyde LW, Neumann CS, Viding E, Hariri AR (2013): The neural signatures of
SC
distinct psychopathic traits. Soc Neurosci. 8: 122–35.
in Adults. J Pers. 83: 723–37.
M AN U
13. Seara-Cardoso A, Viding E (2015): Functional Neuroscience of Psychopathic Personality
14. Seara-Cardoso A, Sebastian CL, Viding E, Roiser JP (2016): Affective resonance in response to others’ emotional faces varies with affective ratings and psychopathic traits
TE D
in amygdala and anterior insula. Soc Neurosci. 11: 140–52.
15. Blair JR (2013): The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci. 14: 786–99.
EP
16. Cleckley HM (1941): The Mask of Sanity. Oxford, England: Mosby. 17. Skeem J, Johansson P, Andershed H, Kerr M, Louden JE (2007): Two subtypes of
AC C
psychopathic violent offenders that parallel primary and secondary variants. J Abnorm Psychol. 116: 395–409. 18. Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Youngstrom EA, Kogos Youngstrom J, Feeny NC, Findling RL (2013): Distinguishing primary and secondary variants of callous-unemotional traits among adolescents in a clinic-referred sample. Psychol Assess. 25: 966–78. 19. Viding E, McCrory EJ (2015): Developmental risk for psychopathy. In: Thapar A, Pine DS, Leckman JF, Scott S, Snowling MJ, Taylor E, editors. Rutter’s Child Adolesc
18
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Psychiatry. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781118381953. 20. Fanti KA, Demetriou CA, Kimonis ER (2013): Variants of callous-unemotional conduct problems in a community sample of adolescents. J Youth Adolesc. 42: 964–79. 21. Dean AC, Altstein LL, Berman ME, Constans JI, Sugar CA, McCloskey MS (2013):
RI PT
Secondary psychopathy, but not primary psychopathy, is associated with risky decisionmaking in noninstitutionalized young adults. Pers Individ Dif. 54: 272–277.
22. Kimonis ER, Skeem JL, Cauffman E, Dmitrieva J (2011): Are secondary variants of
M AN U
primary variants? Law Hum Behav. 35: 381–91.
SC
juvenile psychopathy more reactively violent and less psychosocially mature than
23. Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Isoma Z, Donoghue K (2013): Maltreatment profiles among incarcerated boys with callous-unemotional traits. Child Maltreat. 18: 108–21. 24. Cecil CAM, McCrory EJ, Barker ED, Guiney J, Viding E (n.d.): Characterising youth with callous-unemotional traits and concurrent anxiety: Evidence for a high-risk clinical
TE D
groupe. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. .
25. Viding E, Kimonis ER (n.d.): Callous-Unemotional Traits. In: Patrick CJ, editor. Handb
EP
Psychopathy, 2nd ed. .
26. Cecil CAM, Lysenko LJ, Jaffee SR, Pingault J-B, Smith RG, Relton CL, et al. (2014):
AC C
Environmental risk, Oxytocin Receptor Gene (OXTR) methylation and youth callousunemotional traits: a 13-year longitudinal study. Mol Psychiatry. 19: 1071–7. 27. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Sebastian CL, Mechelli A, Bird G, Kelly PA, Viding E (2011): Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr Biol. 21: R947-8. 28. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Kelly PA, Bird G, Sebastian CL, Mechelli A, et al. (2013): Amygdala activation in maltreated children during pre-attentive emotional processing. Br J Psychiatry. 202: 269–76. 19
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
29. Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Cauffman E, Goldweber A, Skeem J (2012): Primary and secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy differ in emotional processing. Dev Psychopathol. 24: 1091–103. 30. Bishop SJ, Duncan J, Lawrence AD (2004): State Anxiety Modulation of the Amygdala
RI PT
Response to Unattended Threat-Related Stimuli. J Neurosci. 24: 10364–10368.
31. Bishop SJ (2007): Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends Cogn Sci. 11: 307–316.
SC
32. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Viding E (2011): The impact of childhood maltreatment: a
M AN U
review of neurobiological and genetic factors. Front psychiatry. 2: 48. 33. Blair RJR (2016): The Neurobiology of Impulsive Aggression. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 26: 4–9.
34. Spielberger C (1983): State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
TE D
Psychologists Press.
35. Wechsler D (1999): Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education.
EP
36. Pennebaker JW, Susman JR (1988): Disclosure of traumas and psychosomatic processes. Soc Sci Med. 26: 327–32.
AC C
37. Buss AH, Perry MP (1992): The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 63: 452– 459.
38. Spielberger CD (1999): The State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (STAXI-2): Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. 39. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES (1995): Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 51: 768–74. 40. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R (1983): A global measure of perceived stress. J
20
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Health Soc Behav. 24: 385–96. 41. Watson D, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, Clark LA, Strauss ME, McCormick RA (1995): Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. J Abnorm Psychol. 104: 3–14.
RI PT
42. Watson D, Clark LA, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, Strauss ME, McCormick RA (1995): Testing a tripartite model: II. Exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression in student, adult, and patient samples. J Abnorm Psychol. 104: 15–25.
SC
43. Bohn MJ, Babor TF, Kranzler HR (1995): The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
Alcohol. 56: 423–32.
M AN U
(AUDIT): validation of a screening instrument for use in medical settings. J Stud
44. Sheehan D V, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. (1998): The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin
TE D
Psychiatry. 22-33-57.
45. Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, et al. (2002): Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.
EP
Science. 297: 400–3.
AC C
46. Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, Weinberger DR (2005): A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 62: 146–52. 47. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Ziolko SK, Price JC, Moses-Kolko EL, Berga SL, Hariri AR (2006): Capacity for 5-HT1A-mediated autoregulation predicts amygdala reactivity. Nat Neurosci. 9: 1362–3. 48. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Coleman RL, Ziolko SK, Becker C, et al. (2009): Medial prefrontal cortex 5-HT(2A) density is correlated with amygdala reactivity, 21
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
response habituation, and functional coupling. Cereb Cortex. 19: 2499–507. 49. Manuck SB, Brown SM, Forbes EE, Hariri AR (2007): Temporal stability of individual differences in amygdala reactivity. Am J Psychiatry. 164: 1613–4. 50. Zhou Z, Zhu G, Hariri AR, Enoch M-A, Scott D, Sinha R, et al. (2008): Genetic variation
RI PT
in human NPY expression affects stress response and emotion. Nature. 452: 997– 1001.
51. Deen B, Pitskel NB, Pelphrey KA (2011): Three Systems of Insular Functional
SC
Connectivity Identified with Cluster Analysis. Cereb Cortex. 21: 1498–1506.
M AN U
52. Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, Phan KL (2007): Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 62: 168–78.
53. Blair RJ, Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Dolan RJ (1999): Dissociable neural responses
TE D
to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain. 883–93.
54. Kimonis ER, Fanti KA, Goulter N, Hall J (2017): Affective startle potentiation differentiates primary and secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy. Dev
EP
Psychopathol. 29: 1149–1160.
55. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R (2011): Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and
AC C
medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 15: 85–93. 56. Pezze MA, Feldon J (2004): Mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways in fear conditioning. Prog Neurobiol. 74: 301–20. 57. LeDoux JE, Cicchetti P, Xagoraris A, Romanski LM (1990): The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 10: 1062–9. 58. Penzo MA, Robert V, Tucciarone J, De Bundel D, Wang M, Van Aelst L, et al. (2015): The paraventricular thalamus controls a central amygdala fear circuit. Nature. 519:
22
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
455–9. 59. Haber SN (2003): The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. J Chem Neuroanat. 26: 317–30. 60. Yim CY, Mogenson GJ (1983): Response of ventral pallidal neurons to amygdala
RI PT
stimulation and its modulation by dopamine projections to nucleus accumbens. J Neurophysiol. 50: 148–61.
SC
61. Frith CD, Frith U (2006): The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Neuron. 50: 531–534. 62. Tatar JR, Cauffman E, Kimonis ER, Skeem JL (2012): Victimization History and
M AN U
Posttraumatic Stress: An Analysis of Psychopathy Variants in Male Juvenile Offenders. J Child Adolesc Trauma. 5: 102–113.
63. Vaughn MG, Edens JF, Howard MO, Smith ST (2009): An Investigation of Primary and Secondary Psychopathy in a Statewide Sample of Incarcerated Youth. Youth Violence
TE D
Juv Justice. 7: 172–188.
64. Lee Z, Salekin RT, Iselin A-MR (2010): Psychopathic traits in youth: is there evidence for
AC C
EP
primary and secondary subtypes? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 38: 381–93.
23
Sethi et al.
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
Figure 1. a) Reduced activity in the amygdala (far left), insula (middle left), ACC and PCC (middle right), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (far right) in the Primary group compared to the Comparison group during fear processing; b) Trendwise reduced activity in the amygdala (left) and insula (right) in the Primary compared to Secondary group during fear processing; c) Reduced activity in the superior temporal sulcus/inferior parietal lobe (far left), ACC and PCC (middle left), thalamus and globus pallidus (middle right), and susbtantia nigra (far right) in Secondary group compared to the Comparison group during fear processing.
24
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 2. Significant whole brain results for Fear > Shapes p(FWE-corr)
k
Cohen’s d
Z
xyz
Table 1. Significant anterior insula and amygdala (SVC) results for Fear > Shapes p(FWEcorr)
Cohen’ sd
Left amygdala
34
0.044
0.56
Left anterior insula
21 3
0.012
0.69
0.029
0.64
Comparison >Primary
0.056 0.066 Secondary>Primary Right amygdala
Left anterior insula Right anterior insula
3.42
0.61
3.41
0.60
3.36
-44 -4 0
062
3.53
46 0 4
3.18
3.88
3.62
55
0.038
26
0.055
0.52
3.10
30 4 -18
0.082
0.49
2.95
0.063
0.56
3.37
0.070
0.56
3.33
30 2 -24 -38 -16 2 46 -2 -6
0.075
0.55
3.31
46 0 4
0.099
0.53
3.21
42 2 -12
12 2 37
13
AC C
EP
TE D
Right anterior insula
0.61
-22 -2 14 -38 4 12 -38 -16 2 -34 6 12 -44 6 -6
M AN U
Right anterior insula
xyz
SC
0.054
Z
RI PT
k
25
Sethi et al.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Comparison >Primary
Right insula ext. rolandic operculum
Left middle frontal ext. L/R ACC, left superior frontal
Secondary>Primary Right superior temporal gyrus ext. insula
Comparison >Secondary
EP
L/R ACC ext. frontal pole
0.008
<0.001
0.073
0.086
TE D
Left temporal pole ext. insula
0.001
AC C
Right superior temporal sulcus, inferior parietal, thalamus, bilateral SN/VTA, and pallidum
Left superior temporal gyrus ext. left supramarginal gyrus
L/R MCC ext. posterior cingulate, precunueus
472
1150
746
0.73
4.07
-14 -68 24
0.67
3.76
-20 -58 18
0.65
3.65
-6 -42 44
0.73
4.07
16 -54 22
0.59
3.33
8 -64 20
0.53
2.99
10 -46 6
RI PT
Left insula ext. postcentral gyrus, rolandic operculum, superior temporal gyrus, amygdala
0.065
1327
0.72
4.03
-38 2 -12
0.70
3.9
-56 -18 18
0.64
3.62
-38 -16 2
0.72
4.02
38 -16 0
SC
Right Precuneus ext. cuneus
<0.001
2279
0.63
3.53
46 0 4
0.56
3.16
58 4 6
0.69
3.88
-30 20 36
0.68
3.82
8 32 10
0.68
3.81
-4 32 28
0.66
3.94
48 0 -12
0.55
3.31
46 0 4
0.55
3.29
46 -8 -8
0.64
3.8
-40 0 -16
0.56
3.39
-50 4 -16
0.56
3.37
-38 -16 2
0.62
4.08
-12 52 -8
0.57
3.74
-4 36 8
0.54
3.55
-6 32 -4
0.56
3.71
36 -20 2
0.54
3.58
20 -8 2
0.51
3.38
-6 -16 -10
0.53
3.48
-46 -28 10
0.52
3.44
-30 -34 14
0.49
3.23
-42 -36 24
0.52
3.44
-2 -42 42
0.50
3.3
-8 -48 20
0.47
3.13
8 -44 24
M AN U
Left Cuneus ext. L mid cingulate (MCC) and R Precuneus
<0.001
0.003
0.030
0.015
457
436
1863
895
571
659
26
Sethi et al.
Table 3. Whole brain results Anger > Shapes
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
p(FWE-corr)
0.011
740
Cohen’s d
Z
xyz
0.68
4.04
-26 26 24
0.61
3.64
8 32 20
0.59
3.52
-14 30 28
AC C
EP
L/R ACC
TE D
Primary>Secondary
k
27
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Sethi et al.
Supplement
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ‘Primary’ and ‘Secondary’ Variants of Psychopathy in a Volunteer Sample Are Associated With Different Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supplemental Information
RI PT
Supplementary Methods Task
The fMRI paradigm used here has been shown to robustly elicit amygdala responses over a
SC
wide variety of protocols and populations (1–6). The paradigm consisted of four task blocks interleaved with five control blocks. A total of four emotion categories were used for each task
M AN U
block: fearful (F), angry (A), surprised (S), and neutral (N), taken from a standardized facial expression set (7). Participants viewed the task blocks in one of four randomly assigned orders as determined by a Latin Square (i.e., FNAS, NFSA, ASFN, SANF). During task blocks, participants viewed a trio of faces and matched one of two faces identical to a target face. Each trial in the task blocks lasted for 4 seconds with a variable interstimulus interval of 2-6
TE D
seconds (mean = 4 seconds), for a total block length of 48 seconds. In the control blocks, each of six geometric shape trios was presented for 4 seconds with a fixed interstimulus interval of 2 seconds for a total block length of 36 seconds. Each block was preceded by a
EP
brief instruction (“Match faces” or “Match shapes”) lasting 2 sec resulting in a total task time
AC C
of 390 seconds. In the present study, we restricted analyses to fearful and angry expressions.
fMRI Acquisition
Participants were scanned using one of two identical General Electric MR570 3T scanners at the Duke-UNC Brain Imaging and Analysis Centre equipped with high power high duty cycle 50mT/m gradients at 200 T/m/s slew rate, and an 8 channel head coil for parallel imaging at high bandwidth up to 1 MHz. Thirty-four interleaved AC-PC aligned axial functional slices were acquired for full-brain coverage using an inverse-spiral pulse sequence to reduce susceptibility artefacts (TR = 2000ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 60°; FOV = 240 mm; 3.75 × 3.75 × 4 mm
1
Sethi et al.
Supplement
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT voxels; interslice skip = 0). Four initial volumes were acquired and discarded to achieve steady state equilibrium. A semi-automated high-order shimming program was used to ensure global field homogeneity. High resolution 3D structural images (TR = 7.7 s; TE; 3.0 ms; flip angle = 12°; voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 4 mm; FOV = 240 mm, interslice skip = 0) were also acquired in
RI PT
34 axial slices to assist registration of functional data.
fMRI Preprocessing
Data preprocessing and analysis were performed with SPM8 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).
SC
Functional volumes were aligned to the first volume in the time series to correct for head motion, before being spatially normalised to a standard stereotactic space (MNI template)
M AN U
using a 12 parameter affine model and resampled to 2mm3 voxel size. Co-registered data were then smoothed with a 6mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian filter. Voxel-wise signal
AC C
EP
TE D
intensities were normalised to the global mean.
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Sethi et al.
Supplement
Supplementary Table S1. Demographics, SRP & maltreatment scores Pair-wise contrasts
RI PT
Group Comparison (n = 82)
Primary (n = 50)
Secondary (n = 100)
Omnibus test
75%
18%
42%
X2(2, 229) = 47.16, p < 0.001
Age
19.61 (1.24)
19.80 (1.37)
19.53 (2.33)
F(2, 229) = 0.37, p = 0.691
IQ
122.23 (9.04)
123.86 (6.06)
120.57 (8.73)
Factor 1 traits
15.26 (1.18)
36.64 (4.39)
Factor 2 traits
17.44 (3.74)
Anxiety
Demographics
Comparison vs. Primary
Comparison vs. Secondary
Primary vs. Secondary
p < 0.001
p = 0.005
F(2, 227) = 2.68, p = 0.070
p = 0.834
p = 0.555
p = 0.073*
37.57 (6.07)
F(2, 229) = 627.17, p < 0.001
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p = 0.710
26.88 (5.49)
29.64 (7.70)
F(2, 229) = 94.81, p < 0.001
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p = 0.028*
26.56 (2.70)
27.30 (2.05)
51.17 (6.50)
F(2, 229) = 774.57, p < 0.001
p = 0.1000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
CTQ Total maltreatment
3.30 (5.34)
5.46 (5.35)
16.67 (12.53)
F(2, 229) = 54.13, p < 0.001
p = 0.575
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
Emotional abuse
0.94 (2.20)
1.06 (4.83)
4.83 (4.54)
F(2, 229) = 37.74, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
Physical abuse
0.46 (1.29)
0.52 (1.09)
2.16 (3.14)
F(2, 229) = 15.63, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p = 0.001 *
p < 0.001*
Sexual abuse
0.04 (0.33)
0.22 (1.42)
0.58 (1.80)
F(2, 229) = 3.70, p = 0.026
p = 1.000
p = 0.024*
p = 0.389
Emotional neglect
1.27 (2.22)
2.96 (2.97)
6.22 (4.28)
F(2, 229) = 49.66, p < 0.001
p = 0.018*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
Physical neglect
0.60 (1.33)
0.70 (1.45)
2.88 (3.13)
F(2, 229) = 26.98, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
EP
AC C
Maltreatment History
M AN U
TE D
Psychopathy & Anxiety
SC
p < 0.001
Gender (% Female)
* Significant at p 0.05. Bonferroni correction applied for pairwise contrasts
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Sethi et al.
Supplement
Supplementary Table S2. Individual functioning
RI PT
Pair-wise contrasts
Comparison vs. Secondary
Primary vs. Secondary
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
F(2, 229) = 55.84, p < 0.001
p = 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
F(2, 229) = 221.35, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
F(2, 229) = 129.31, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
F(2, 229) = 56.27, p < 0.001
p = 1.000
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
7.01 (5.34)
F(2, 229) = 13.77, p < 0.001
p < 0.001*
p < 0.001*
p = 1.000
3.24 (4.23)
F(2, 229) = 11.31, p < 0.001
p = 0.002*
p < 0.001*
p = 1.000
Comparison (n = 82)
Primary (n = 50)
Secondary (n = 100)
BPAQ
43.36 (8.48)
61.96 (13.23)
84.08 (17.80)
F(2, 229) = 188.26, p < 0.001
STAXI - Anger
12.70 (2.56)
16.08 (3.78)
20.91 (6.34)
F(2, 229) = 67.90, p < 0.001
54.57 (8.01)
60.84 (8.39)
69.69 (11.37)
8.73 (4.08)
9.14 (4.30)
21.69 (5.09)
MASQ Depression
15.09 (3.06)
15.06 (3.15)
30.91 (10.73)
MASQ Anxiety
13.78 (2.42)
14.86 (2.90)
23.01 (9.10)
AUDIT
3.73 (3.05)
7.12 (5.10)
Recreational Drug Use
0.89 (1.63)
3.08 (4.16)
Omnibus test
BIS Stress Perceived Stress Scale
EP
AC C
Substance Abuse
TE D
Mood
M AN U
Impulsivity
SC
Aggression
Comparison vs Primary
* Significant at p 0.05. Bonferroni correction applied for pairwise contrasts
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Sethi et al.
Supplement
Supplementary Table S3. Lifetime psychiatric diagnoses Pair-wise Contrasts
RI PT
Group
Comparison (n = 82)
Primary (n = 50)
Secondary (n = 100)
Omnibus test
Alcohol abuse
6.10%
20.00%
23.20%
X2(2, 228) = 10.17, p = 0.006*
Substance abuse
2.40%
6%
13.10%
X2(2, 228) = 7.42, p = 0.024*
MDD
0%
2.00%
18.20%
X2(2, 228) = 22.92, p < 0.001*
GAD (6 months)
0%
0%
7.07%
Bipolar Disorder
0%
2.00%
PTSD
0%
APSD
SC
Lifetime diagnoses
Comparison vs. Primary
p = 0.015*
p = 0.002*
Primary vs Secondary
p = 0.654 p = 0.184
p = 0.199
p < 0.001*
p = 0.005*
X2(2, 228) = 9.63, p = 0.008*
--
p = 0.014*
p = 0.054
12.10%
X2(2, 228) = 13.99, p = 0.001*
p = 0.199
p = 0.001*
p = 0.039*
0%
1.00%
X2(2, 228) = 1.34, p = 0.512
--
--
--
0%
0%
1.00%
X2(2, 228) = 1.34, p = 0.512
--
--
--
Borderline PD
0%
0%
2.30%
X2(2, 202) = 2.69, p = 0.261
--
--
--
Suicidality (2 year)
0%
4.00%
17.00%
X2(2, 228) = 18.81, p < 0.001
p = 0.068
TE D
M AN U
p = 0.009*
p < 0.001*
p = 0.024*
AC C
EP
p = 0.299
Comparison vs. Secondary
5
Sethi et al.
Supplement
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Supplementary Table S4. Ranges and distributions of key variables across and within groups Group
Comparison (n = 82)
Primary (n = 50)
Secondary (n = 100)
15.26 ± 1.18 (14 – 17)
36.64 ± 4.39 (31 – 50)
37.57 ± 6.07 (31- 67)
[0.35, -1.1.40]
[1.10, 0.96]
[1.86, 5.47]
Anxiety
17.44 ± 3.74 (14 – 30)
26.88 ± 5.49 (16 – 39)
29.64 ± 7.70 (15 – 62)
[1.44, 1.84]
[0.05, -0.31]
[1.16, 2.77]
26.56 ± 2.70 (20 – 30)
27.30 ± 2.05 (23 – 30)
51.17 ± 6.50 (44 -73)
[-0.53, -0.58]
[-0.32, -1.07]
[1.53, 2.30]
43.37 ± 8.48 (30 – 71)
61.96 ± 13.23 (38 – 98)
84.08 ± 17.80 (43 – 139)
[0.82, 0.54]
[0.47, -0.28]
[0.83, 1.05]
12.70 ± 2.56 (10 – 23)
16.08 ± 3.78 (10 – 27)
20.91 ± 6.34 (11 – 40)
[1.35, 2.55]
[1.19, 1.65]
[0.92, 0.78]
Aggression
BPAQ
TE D
STAXI - Anger
SC
Factor 2 traits
M AN U
Factor 1 traits
RI PT
Psychopathy & Anxiety
AC C
EP
Mean ± SD (Range) [Skewness, Kurtosis]
6
Sethi et al.
Supplement
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Supplementary Table S5. Correlation matrix between key variables across sample (n = 1144)
Factor 2
STAIT - Trait
BPAQ
STAIT - trait
BPAQ
STAXI Anger
BIS Total
CTQ Total
Pearson Correlation
0.72**
0.19**
0.58**
0.40**
0.29**
0.26**
Sig. (2-tailed)
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
Pearson Correlation
0.20**
0.53**
0.34**
0.47**
0.27**
Sig. (2-tailed)
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
Pearson Correlation
0.45**
Sig. (2-tailed)
<0.001
Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-tailed)
0.39**
0.40**
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
0.71**
0.40**
0.32**
<0.001
<0.001
<0.001
0.34**
0.24**
<0.001
<0.001
Pearson Correlation
M AN U
BIS Total
0.41**
SC
STAXI Anger
RI PT
Factor 1
Factor 2
Sig. (2-tailed)
0.32** <0.001
AC C
EP
TE D
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
7
Sethi et al.
Supplement
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Supplementary References 1. Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, et al. (2002): Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science. 297: 400–3.
RI PT
2. Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, Weinberger DR (2005): A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 62: 146–52. 3. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Ziolko SK, Price JC, Moses-Kolko EL, Berga SL, Hariri AR (2006): Capacity for 5-HT1A-mediated autoregulation predicts amygdala reactivity. Nat Neurosci. 9: 1362–3.
SC
4. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Coleman RL, Ziolko SK, Becker C, et al. (2009): Medial prefrontal cortex 5-HT(2A) density is correlated with amygdala reactivity, response habituation, and functional coupling. Cereb Cortex. 19: 2499–507.
M AN U
5. Manuck SB, Brown SM, Forbes EE, Hariri AR (2007): Temporal stability of individual differences in amygdala reactivity. Am J Psychiatry. 164: 1613–4. 6. Zhou Z, Zhu G, Hariri AR, Enoch M-A, Scott D, Sinha R, et al. (2008): Genetic variation in human NPY expression affects stress response and emotion. Nature. 452: 997–1001.
AC C
EP
TE D
7. Ekman P, Friesen W V (1976): Pictures of facial affect, Consulting. Palo Alto, CA.
8