Accepted Manuscript Psychological and lifestyle factors that influence the serial reporting of post concussive-like symptoms in a non-concussed population Dr Arun Prasad Balasundaram, PhD, Dr Josie Athens, PhD, Professor Anthony Gerard Schneiders, PhD, Assoc/Prof Paul McCrory, MD, PhD, Professor Stephen John Sullivan, PhD PII:
S1934-1482(17)30131-4
DOI:
10.1016/j.pmrj.2017.01.004
Reference:
PMRJ 1845
To appear in:
PM&R
Received Date: 22 March 2016 Revised Date:
22 January 2017
Accepted Date: 29 January 2017
Please cite this article as: Balasundaram AP, Athens J, Schneiders AG, McCrory P, Sullivan SJ, Psychological and lifestyle factors that influence the serial reporting of post concussive-like symptoms in a non-concussed population, PM&R (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2017.01.004. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
TITLE Psychological and lifestyle factors that influence the serial reporting of post concussive-like
RI PT
symptoms in a non-concussed population Author Information 1) Dr Arun Prasad Balasundaram, PhD
2) Dr Josie Athens, PhD
M AN U
University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
SC
Centre for Health, Activity and Rehabilitation Research, School of Physiotherapy,
Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, Dunedin School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
3) Professor Anthony Gerard Schneiders, PhD
Australia
TE D
School of Human, Health & Social Sciences, Central Queensland University, Branyan,
4) Assoc/Prof Paul McCrory, MD, PhD
EP
The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne Brain Centre, University of Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
AC C
5) Professor Stephen John Sullivan, PhD Centre for Health, Activity and Rehabilitation Research, School of Physiotherapy, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand 6) Corresponding Author: Dr Arun Prasad Balasundaram Mailing address: Forskningsveien 3A, Harald Schjelderups hus, 0373, Oslo, University of Oslo, Norway, Telephone: 47 228-45397, Email ID:
[email protected]
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
TITLE
2
Psychological and lifestyle factors that influence the serial reporting of post concussive-
3
like symptoms in a non-concussed population
RI PT
4
5
SC
6
M AN U
7
8
9
13
14
15
16
EP
12
AC C
11
TE D
10
17
18
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ABSTRACT
20
Background: Symptoms related to concussion are generally found to be non-specific in
21
nature as they are also reported by non-concussed individuals. What is currently not
22
known is whether the symptoms vary over time, and whether they are also influenced
23
by a multitude of factors.
24
Objective: To investigate the potential influence of psychological, lifestyle, and
25
situational factors on the change in postconcussion-like symptoms reported over seven
26
consecutive days in a cohort of normal individuals.
27
Design: Longitudinal observational study.
28
Setting: Real-world context.
29
Participants: Convenience sample of 180 non-concussed university students were
30
enrolled. Of these, 110 participants only provided data for the entire period of the study.
31
Methods: An experience sampling methodology was employed to document the
32
symptoms reported over time. Stepwise multivariate linear mixed-effects modeling
33
performed to identify the predictors that contribute to the serially reported symptoms.
34
Independent Variables: Gender, time of the day, location, primary activity and type of
35
interactant (person) of the participant, physical activity status, trouble sleeping, alcohol
36
consumption, caffeine consumption, stress, anxiety, depression, mental and physical
37
fatigue and life stressors.
38
Main Outcome Measures: The key outcome measures were the change in total
39
symptom score (TSSchange) and symptom severity score (SSSchange) reported over 7
40
consecutive days.
41
Results: The predictors of location at the time of reporting, physical fatigue (Estimate =
42
- 0.98, p<. 001) and mental fatigue (Estimate = - 0.53, p<. 001) contributed to the
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
19
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT TSSchange. Post-hoc analysis of the variable of location at the time of reporting revealed
44
that participants reported increased TSSchange when they were at a café/restaurant
45
compared to flat/university.
46
Conclusions: A number of factors within the context of daily life influenced the
47
postconcussion-like symptoms reported over time. These findings indicate that
48
clinicians need to be cautious when interpreting the serially assessed symptom scores to
49
track the recovery profile of a concussed athlete in order to make decisions on return-to-
50
play. Additional investigation is warranted to examine the change in symptom scores
51
reported over time by concussed individuals considering that this study was conducted
52
in a non-concussed cohort.
53
Key words: Experience sampling methodology; Return-to-play; Real-world context;
54
Serial assessment.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
43
INTRODUCTION
TE D
55
Clinicians frequently use the serial assessment of symptoms to track the recovery of a
57
concussed athlete [1]. The symptoms are expected to resolve spontaneously in 80% of
58
athletes following a concussion within 10 days [2]. Athletes are monitored over this
59
timeframe and sometimes beyond with the premise that the resolution of symptoms
60
reflects changes in the underlying neurological status of a concussive brain injury. This
61
information is used to assist in making decisions on the appropriate time to return-to-
62
play (RTP) or training [3]. However, it is well established that symptoms are non-
63
specific in nature [4], thus changes in the symptom scores of recovering athletes may
64
not necessarily reflect their true neurological status. Moreover, postconcussion-like
65
symptoms are often reported by non-concussed college and high school athletes [5, 6]
66
and normal healthy individuals [7, 8]. Additionally, the reporting of postconcussion-like
AC C
EP
56
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT symptoms is shown to be influenced by a myriad of factors such as stress and
68
depression [9], sleep problems [1, 10], physical fatigue [11] and anxiety [12].
69
Nevertheless, these previous findings reported, including in recent research [13] have
70
been in cross-sectional studies; therefore, it is still not known whether these same
71
factors would have an impact on the postconcussion-like symptoms when reported
72
consecutively over a period of time. There is also a need to explore the contribution of
73
additional factors such as the location, the primary activity and the person who interacts
74
with the participant at the time of reporting symptoms. This is because associations
75
have been reported between these factors and physical symptoms at the time of
76
reporting [14].
77
A recent study documented symptom scores across different (random) times within the
78
same day (intra-day variability) and concluded that time of the day did not contribute to
79
symptom reporting in concussed individuals [15]. However, it is not known whether the
80
time of day would have an impact on the postconcussion-like symptoms reported across
81
multiple days (inter-day variability) in a non-concussed cohort. Gender differences in
82
the reporting of symptoms in non-concussed individuals have been found in several
83
cross-sectional studies [5, 16, 17, 18], where females reported an increased severity of
84
symptoms (SSS) compared to males. Furthermore, there was an increase in the change
85
of symptom scores reported by females compared to males when tested on only two
86
different occasions (test-retest) seven days apart in a sample of non-concussed
87
undergraduate students [19]. Nonetheless, the influence of gender on the
88
postconcussion-like symptoms reported over an extended period of time in non-
89
concussed individuals is yet to be investigated.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
67
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The contribution of these factors to the reported symptoms, which are likely to vary on
91
a day-to-day basis, has to be understood within a real-world context [20] as it may
92
depend on the situation and time they are reported. By not accounting for these potential
93
factors when interpreting serially assessed symptoms, there is a possibility that
94
clinicians could assume that a concussed athlete has attained asymptomatic status,
95
thereby leading to a premature return-to-play (RTP) or conversely a decision that would
96
prevent them from returning-to-play. Thus, the purpose of this study was to investigate
97
whether the postconcussion-like symptoms serially reported by non-concussed
98
individuals are influenced by gender, time of the day, and various psychological,
99
lifestyle and situational factors experienced and occurring within the context of daily
SC
M AN U
100
RI PT
90
life.
METHODS
101
Study Design
103
A longitudinal observational design was used, where an experience sampling
104
methodology was adopted [21] and employed to capture postconcussion-like symptoms
105
reported over a 7-day period.
106
Setting
107
The postconcussion-like symptoms were measured over time in a real-world context.
108
The recruitment and study period was between June and October 2012.
109
Subjects
110
One hundred and eighty university students aged 18-30 years, who reported not having
111
an episode of concussion in the previous 3 months, were included in the study.
112
Participants who did not have a mobile phone and access to the internet for the entire
AC C
EP
TE D
102
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT study period were excluded. The recruitment of participants followed the main strategy
114
of soliciting expression of interest by using the sign-up sheets, where they provided
115
their email address to contact them with study details. Secondary strategies included the
116
advertisement of posters on the notice boards throughout the university, and in the
117
university Facebook page. The Human Ethics Committee of the University of Otago
118
approved this study, and informed consent was obtained from all participants by
119
sending a link containing an electronic consent form to their email address.
120
Measures
121
The panel survey [22] was specifically developed for this study that measured the same
122
constructs from the same cohort over 7 consecutive days. The dependent variable and
123
the factors within the context of daily life (predictor variables) used in this study are
124
detailed below.
125
Dependent variable
126
The SCAT2 post-concussion symptom scale [23] was used, which was the current
127
version at the time of the study. This scale consists of 22-symptom items, each rated on
128
a 7-point Likert scale from 0 to 6, which allows the calculation of two scores, the total
129
symptom score (TSS) and symptom severity score (SSS). The TSS is calculated by
130
counting the number of symptoms endorsed >0 (maximum score = 22) and the SSS is
131
summing up the rating of each of the 22 symptoms (maximum score=132; 22 x 6). This
132
scale appears to be developing as a reference scale due to its widespread use. Although
133
not formally established, researchers have reported this scale to have good reliability as
134
well as content and face validity [24].
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
113
135
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Predictor Variables
137
In order to explore the key factors of stress [25], anxiety and depression [26], physical
138
fatigue [27] and mental fatigue [28], specific and relevant items were extracted from
139
each of these recognised scales to include in the panel survey. These variables were
140
framed in relation to the participant’s behaviour in the previous 2 hours. Additionally,
141
brief custom-designed questions were developed for variables of physical activity status
142
(yes/no), alcohol (yes/no) and caffeine consumption (yes/no) in the previous 2 hours,
143
trouble sleeping the previous night (yes/no), and life stressors (e.g. relationship
144
problems, exam stress) experienced at the time of reporting. Furthermore, the variables
145
of location, primary activity and the type of interactant (e.g. family or friend) of the
146
participant were also included based on the work of previous researchers [21, 29]. These
147
authors have suggested their inclusion, when employing a study design similar to the
148
one used in this study.
149
There may be a certain degree of overlap regarding the relationships between constructs
150
such as stress, anxiety and depression with the items in the postconcussion-like
151
symptoms scale. However, these associations have only been shown in studies using
152
cross-sectional data, and thus, there is a lack of evidence with longitudinal data. Apart
153
from the variables of stress and anxiety, this study also included a range of lifestyle and
154
situational factors. There is no study to date, which has systematically investigated the
155
association between these predictor variables and the serially reported symptoms
156
(repeated measurements).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
136
157 158 159
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Procedures
161
A pilot testing was undertaken with a small group (n=12) of participants in order to
162
establish the face validity of the panel survey and its ease of use. The procedures of the
163
experience sampling methodology were implemented based on those outlined by
164
Conner and colleagues [30], and this is detailed from hereon. Following the enrolment
165
of the participants, they were arbitrarily but randomly divided into one of six groups
166
with each group comprising 30 participants. The procedure of this type was carried out
167
for practical reasons, as it is realistically only possible to obtain the symptomology data
168
for 30 participants on each day. This is because the primary investigator had to
169
manually send out the text messages and also monitor the symptom data obtained from
170
all 30 participants. For each group, the first day of the study started with the signalling
171
of each participant by sending a text message to their mobile phone at a designated time
172
asking them to check their email to find a link to complete the panel survey.
173
Each participant was selectively sent a text message between 9am and 8pm (NZST), and
174
in order to control for the expected variability associated with the time of day, they
175
received it at semi-random times of the day on all 7 days [31]. Each day was equally
176
divided into three time zones: morning (9.00am to 12.59pm), afternoon (1.00pm to
177
4.59pm) and evening (5.00pm to 8.59pm). For pragmatic reasons, ten participants were
178
assigned to each of the three (i.e. Morning, afternoon and evening) time zones on each
179
day. For example, on day 1, the first 10 participants in a particular group received the
180
text message in the morning zone; the next 10 received it in the afternoon and the last
181
10 in the evening and this order was randomly changed on the subsequent days.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
160
182
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The participants were asked to complete the panel survey ‘as close in time’ as possible
184
upon receiving the text message on all 7 consecutive days. However, it was decided a
185
priori to accept only those responses that were submitted within 4 hours on the same
186
day they received their text message [30]. Each panel survey consisted of a total of 21
187
questions and the time taken to complete the entire survey was approximately 5 minutes
188
on each occasion. All those participants who met the study criteria on all 7 days were
189
entered into a prize draw to receive one of five $40 NZD iTunes store vouchers.
190
Statistical Analyses
191
A total of 180 participants (6 groups’ x 30 each) were enrolled in this study; however,
192
only the data of those participants who complied with the a priori criterion were
193
included in the analyses. The symptom score (TSS & SSS) reported on day 1 was
194
subtracted from each of the symptom score (TSS & SSS) reported for days 2 to 7 (e.g.,
195
t1 minus t0, t2 minus t0...... t6 minus t0), where t0 represents day 1, t1 is day 2 and so forth
196
on each day for each participant. The change in postconcussion-like symptoms was
197
computed in this way for each participant for each day. Following this, the descriptive
198
statistics (mean & SD) were calculated from the computed values of change score for
199
each day. In the final step, this calculated average difference in the symptom score,
200
which is expressed as TSSchange and SSSchange respectively was used as the outcome
201
measure for all subsequent analyses.
202
The baseline linear mixed-effects model was established by conducting a series of linear
203
mixed-effects models. Initially, a null model or unconditional linear mixed-effect model
204
(no predictor) was conducted to estimate the intra-class correlation (ICC) which
205
explains the between subject variability and the average correlation between repeated
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
183
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT measurements [32]. Furthermore, the estimation of the ICC is also needed to
207
substantiate the necessity to use the linear mixed-effects modelling in any study [32].
208
Following the generation of the null model, a random intercept model with the inclusion
209
of the variable of time (days) was conducted, followed by a random slope model with
210
the inclusion of the same variable of time (days). Once the random slope model was
211
obtained, an auto-regressive covariance structure was added due to the correlation of
212
repeated measurements. Additionally, in the subsequent random slope model with the
213
already existing covariance structure, heteroscedasticity was accounted for decreasing
214
variances over time. Thus, a baseline linear mixed-effects model was derived through
215
these series of steps.
216
During the process of establishing a baseline linear mixed-effects model, the model
217
comparisons for a series of linear mixed-effects models were based on the information
218
criterion of -2 log likelihood which follows a Chi-Squared distribution [33]. Maximum
219
likelihood was used as an estimation method in all models leading to the establishment
220
of a baseline linear mixed-effects model [33]. Using the baseline model, a univariate
221
linear mixed-effects model was conducted with a threshold value of p < 0.20 set to
222
identify the significance of the fixed-effects component of each predictor variable.
223
This initial univariate linear mixed-effect analysis was conducted in order to filter the
224
predictors to be included in the subsequent multiple linear mixed-effects analyses. All
225
the predictor variables were included in the model as time-varying covariates with the
226
exception of gender, which was added as a time-invariant variable.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
206
227
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Following the univariate model, a stepwise multiple linear mixed-effects model with a
229
backward elimination method [34] was performed. A level of significance was set at p <
230
0.05 to identify the individual predictor contribution to the fixed-effects component of
231
the multivariate model. A post-hoc analysis was performed for any significant
232
categorical predictor variable by adjusting the confidence intervals using the Westfall
233
method [35]. All the data analyses were performed using the R statistical software,
234
version 3.0.2 [36]. RESULTS
235
SC
RI PT
228
Of the 180 participants recruited, only 110 participants (26 males and 84 females) met
237
the a priori criterion for inclusion in the analyses. A total of 70 participants did not
238
comply with the study criterion of responding within 4 hours from the time of text
239
message. Participants were excluded from analyses for not responding within 4 hours
240
(n=12), albeit on the same day, completed the following day (n=29) or did not complete
241
the questionnaire at all (n=29). The details of frequencies for both the compliant and
242
non-compliant participants are provided in a supplementary table (Table S1).
243
The mean age of all participants was 20.4 ± 2.3. The average change in the TSS and
244
SSS for all participants was –1.7 (SD=4.0) & –3.6 (SD=12.6), for males it was –1.62
245
(SD=3.62) & –4.08 (SD=11.3), and for females –1.75 (SD=4.15) & –3.48 (SD=13.0).
246
The descriptive results for all predictors that include both the categorical and continuous
247
variables obtained on the first day (baseline=t0) are presented as supplementary tables
248
(Tables S2-S4).
249
Univariate linear mixed-effects results for the TSSchange and SSSchange
250
The results for each predictor variable modelled in the univariate linear mixed-effects
251
analyses for the TSSchange and SSSchange are presented in Table 1. On examination of the
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
236
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT results, the predictor variables of gender, time of the day, type of interactant (person),
253
physical activity status and caffeine consumption were found to be non-significant, thus
254
omitted from further analyses.
255
Multiple linear mixed-effects and post-hoc results for the TSSchange
256
Three predictor variables that include, location of the participant at the time of reporting,
257
and physical and mental fatigue were shown to contribute to the TSSchange (Table 2).
258
Initially, an average of 2.19 unit increase in the TSSchange was found when participants
259
reported while they were at a café/restaurant. A subsequent post-hoc analysis revealed
260
that participants reported increased TSSchange when they were at a café/restaurant
261
compared to flat/college or university (Table 3).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
252
262
Multiple linear mixed-effects and post-hoc results for the SSSchange
264
The predictor variables of anxiety, physical and mental fatigue all contributed to the
265
SSSchange. There was a 6.82 unit average increase in the SSSchange when the participants
266
felt ‘anxious frequently’ (Table 4). Overall, the post-hoc results suggest that the
267
participants reported increased SSSchange when they experienced anxiety ‘a great deal of
268
time’ compared to feeling anxious either only ‘from time to time, but not too often’ or
269
‘only occasionally’ (Table 5).
EP
AC C
270
TE D
263
DISCUSSION
271
This study investigated the influence of factors within the context of daily life, gender
272
and time of the day on the changes in TSS and SSS reported over 7 days. Specifically,
273
the predictor variables of physical and mental fatigue contributed to the changes in
274
symptoms reported over time. This finding may be attributed to academic workloads
275
during university study which involves completing assignments, attending lectures and
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT laboratories. It has been shown in undergraduate nursing students that increased
277
academic workloads during the course of study led them to report excessive physical
278
and mental fatigue [37]. In certain situations, the cause of fatigue in university students
279
can also be due to sleep deprivation [38, 39]. It must be noted that approximately one
280
third of the participants in this study were recruited during the period leading up to their
281
university examinations.
282
Hence, this could be explained in part that a situation may have been created where the
283
participants had a change in their sleep patterns while approaching end of the semester,
284
which in turn may have led them to be fatigued. However, the results of our study
285
suggest that when participants reported more physical and mental fatigue they simply
286
have endorsed fewer TSS and SSS across time. In this instance, the role of practice
287
effects cannot be ruled out considering that the participants completed the panel survey
288
repeatedly for 7 consecutive days. This could be indicative of either habitual, repeated
289
responding or greater self-awareness and accuracy [40]. Practice/learning effects have
290
also been shown to occur when balance and/or neuropsychological tests were repeated
291
[41, 42].
292
Although this study found mental and physical fatigue to be predictive of symptom
293
change, these symptoms overlap with the 22 items on the SCAT2 symptom scale. This
294
implies that it is possible to rule out concussion as the cause of fatigue. However, it is
295
difficult to establish a relationship between fatigue and concussion symptomatology in
296
case of concussed individuals. More precisely, it is not possible to determine whether
297
the reported fatigue is due to the consequence of the concussion directly or
298
psychological factors, or combination of both.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
276
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The location of the participant at the time of reporting contributed only to the change in
300
TSS reported over time, but not to the SSS. Specifically, the participants reported an
301
increased TSSchange while they were at university as opposed to other locations. These
302
findings were anticipated considering the amount of stress in an academic setting. Our
303
study found that participants demonstrated decreased change in TSS when they were at
304
a flat/residential college, which may be considered a more relaxed environment as they
305
are in the company of their friends and fellow resident/flatmates.
306
It was also identified that the predictor variable of anxiety contributed only to the
307
change in SSS. In university students, anxiety is one of the most common mental health
308
problems [43, 44], and the reasons cited include family issues, financial and relationship
309
problems [45], and the prevalence is highest in females aged between 18-34 years [46].
310
Given the similar demographics of our participants, one or more of these issues may be
311
the reasons that apply to them.
312
The predictor variable that is, time of the day did not contribute to the change scores of
313
TSS and SSS. These findings are similar to the study which found that time of day did
314
not have an influence on the symptoms (SSS) reported [15]. However, direct
315
comparisons cannot be made because the present study recruited non-concussed
316
participants, whereas the previous study [15] involved concussed athletes. Furthermore,
317
they examined the influence of time of day by documenting symptoms (SSS) each day
318
for 30 days, whereas this study obtained symptom data for only 7 days. An explanation
319
for this non-significant finding may be because participants were asked to respond
320
within 4 hours from the time of receiving their text message. Hence, there could have
321
been an overlap in the responses provided by the participants despite sending the text
322
messages out at a pre-defined schedule for each of the three time zones of the day.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
299
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The gender of the participants did not influence the changes of either the TSS or SSS.
324
These results are contrary to those reported in cross-sectional studies [5, 16, 17, 18],
325
which have found that gender differences exist for the reporting of symptoms.
326
Additionally, Maria et al [19] explored the reporting of postconcussion-like symptoms
327
in non-concussed participants and noted that females had an increase in the change
328
symptom scores compared to males. These contrasting results may be due to different
329
time frames, where this study examined the change in symptoms reported for 7
330
consecutive days, while others [19] investigated the change in symptom score (SSS) at
331
two time points (days 1 & 7). The disparity in the sample (26 males & 84 females) in
332
this study may have also resulted in these findings.
333
While a number of predictor variables were significant in the univariate analyses;
334
however, they were non-significant when included in the multivariate model. This is
335
likely due to the adoption of a conservative approach using the Bayesian information
336
criterion [47], which usually removes a maximum number of predictors to derive a final
337
best model. In general, these findings may be attributed to the different methodology
338
adopted in this study, where data was obtained in a real-world context. Specifically, all
339
participants reported the symptoms for the entire study period at the same time while
340
they were engaged in their routine daily life activities.
341
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study which examined the influence of
342
factors within the context of daily life on the symptoms reported over a definitive period
343
of time in non-concussed individuals. This is a major contribution to the literature,
344
because this could act as a precursor for future studies to further investigate the same
345
aims followed in this study in persons with a concussive brain injury. In doing so,
346
researchers would be able to identify the factors which they need to account for while
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
323
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT interpreting the serially reported symptoms in concussed athletes. Another important
348
strength of this study is the use of a novel approach (experience sampling methodology)
349
to document the symptoms. This allowed all the participants to report their symptoms at
350
the same moment they experienced it, thus reducing any recall bias often associated
351
with reporting at a later time. The experience sampling methodology has been widely
352
used in the field of psychology, but this is the first of its kind in the area of exercise and
353
sports sciences.
354
The generalisability of the study findings may be a concern considering it was
355
conducted using the real-world paradigm. Questions may be raised that the variation
356
and determinants of symptomology are not representative of the typical settings in
357
which clinicians conduct assessment. This is true to a certain degree; however, it must
358
be noted that with the advent of technologies such as smart phones and other similar
359
devices (e.g. iPAD), these days, athletes can be asked to report their symptoms using
360
concussion apps [48], and the symptom reports are subsequently sent to the clinician’s
361
office. These concussion apps are specially designed to be used by athletes, parents and
362
coaches for the ease of obtaining symptoms at a location of their convenience, including
363
when serial (7-10 days and beyond) reporting of symptoms are undertaken to monitor
364
recovery. In such instances, athletes are not always assessed for symptomology in a
365
clinical setting rather in a real-world setting where contextual factors could have an
366
influence on the reporting of symptoms. Thus, these findings could act as a precursor
367
until a study is conducted in athletes with a concussion, for the results to be used in a
368
clinical scenario.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
347
369
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Although, this study had taken a rigorous methodological approach, it is not without
371
some potential limitations. The data with missing occasions were excluded from the
372
analyses since an a priori decision was to include only those participants who had met
373
the study criterion. In particular, we did not conduct analyses for making comparisons
374
between the included vs excluded participants considering it was not included as one of
375
the aims of the study. It is possible to have yielded different results with the inclusion of
376
missing data considering that mixed-effects models can handle them. However, such an
377
approach would have only resulted in the deviation from complying with the criterion
378
set for the purposes of the study. The present data were highly skewed towards female
379
participants, where the recruitment did not control for this to occur considering the
380
study was designed using a real-world approach. Nonetheless, it is likely that the
381
findings could have been different with the inclusion of equal numbers of male and
382
female participants.
383
Custom-designed questions were developed and utilized for measuring the predictor
384
variables of interest. The rationale for not using standardized questionnaires for these
385
measures was to minimize the burden for the participants. This is likely to occur with
386
the participants having to complete lengthy questionnaires in a real-world context for a
387
period of 7 days. The experience sampling methodological approach adopted in this
388
study followed the guidelines of obtaining data in the real-world [30]. The guidelines
389
[30] state that it is preferable to keep the questions to a minimum number in order to
390
prevent dropout rates, where there is a possibility of participants failing to comply with
391
the study requirements. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that our findings may be limited
392
due to not using standardized/recognized questionnaires. Finally, the lack of a power
393
analysis is also a limitation of the study.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
370
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT CONCLUSION
394
The serially reported symptoms were influenced by a number of factors within the
396
context of daily life in a non-concussed cohort. Therefore, the current practice of serial
397
assessment of symptoms needs to be approached with caution by the clinicians when
398
making decisions on RTP. This is required considering that it could be speculated that
399
the same number of factors identified in this study could also impact the symptom
400
reporting in concussed athletes. In order to investigate this, a study is warranted in
401
future to explore the influence of the same predictor variables used in this study on the
402
serially reported symptoms in a concussed cohort. This study had an adult population;
403
hence there is a need to determine whether the change in symptoms (TSS & SSS)
404
reported over time is different in other population groups (e.g. children, adolescents).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
395
405
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
TE D
406
Special thanks to Mr. Bruce Knox, research technical advisor from the School of
408
Physiotherapy, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand for the technical
409
development of the panel survey platform and its associated databases.
411
REFERENCES
AC C
410
EP
407
412
1. McClure DJ, Zuckerman SL, Kutscher SJ, Gregory AJ, Solomon GS. Baseline
413
neurocognitive testing in sports-related concussions: The importance of a prior
414 415
night's sleep. Am J Sports Med 2013; 42:472-478.
2. McCrory P, Meeuwisse WH, Aubry M, et al. Consensus statement on concussion in
416
sport: the 4th International Conference on Concussion in Sport held in Zurich,
417
November 2012. Br J Sports Med 2013; 47:250-25.
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 418
3. McCrory P, Johnston K, Meeuwisse WH, et al. Summary and agreement statement
419
of the 2nd international conference on concussion in sport, Prague 2004. Br J Sports
420
Med 2005; 39:196-204. 4. Iverson GL, Brooks BL, Ashton VL, Lange RT. Interview versus questionnaire
RI PT
421 422
symptom reporting in people with the post-concussion syndrome. J Head Trauma
423
Rehabil 2010; 25:23-30.
5. Jinguji TM, Bompadre V, Harmon KG, et al. Sport Concussion Assessment Tool 2:
SC
424
Baseline values for high school athletes. Br J Sports Med 2012; 46:365-370.
426
6. Valovich McLeod TC, Barr WB, McCrea M, Guskiewicz KM. Psychometric and
M AN U
425
427
measurement properties of concussion assessment tools in youth sports. J Athl Train
428
2006; 41:399-408.
430
7. Iverson GL, Lange RT. Examination of "post concussion-like" symptoms in a healthy sample. Appl Neuropsychol 2003; 10:137-144.
TE D
429
8. Sullivan KA, Edmed SL. An examination of the expected symptoms of post-
432
concussion syndrome in a nonclinical sample. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2011;
433
27:293-301.
435 436 437 438
9. Edmed SL, Sullivan KA. Depression, anxiety, and stress as predictors of post concussion-like symptoms in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Res 2012; 200:41-45.
AC C
434
EP
431
10. Mihalik JP, Lengas E, Register-Mihalik JK Oyama S, Begalle RL, Guskiewicz KM. The effects of sleep quality and sleep quantity on concussion baseline assessment. Clin J Sport Med 2013; 23:343-348.
439
11. Piland SG, Ferrara MS, Macciocchi SN, Broglio SP, Gould TE. Investigation of
440
baseline self-report concussion symptom scores. J Athl Train 2010; 45:273-278.
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 441
12. Iverson GL. A biopsychosocial conceptualization of poor outcome from mild
442
traumatic brain injury. In Vasterling JJ, Bryant RA, Keane TM, eds. PTSD and Mild
443
Traumatic Brain Injury. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012, 37-60. 13. Balasundaram, AP, Josie A, Schneiders AG, McCrory P, Sullivan SJ. The influence
RI PT
444 445
of psychological and lifestyle factors on the reporting of postconcussion-like
446
symptoms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016; 31:197-205
449
SC
448
14. Pennebaker JW, ed. The psychology of physical symptoms. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 1982.
15. Anthony CA, Peterson AR. Utilization of a text-messaging robot to assess intraday
M AN U
447
450
variation in concussion symptom severity scores. Clin J Sport Med 2015; 25:149-
451
152.
452
16. Covassin T, Swanik CB, Sachs M, et al. Sex differences in baseline neuropsychological function and concussion symptoms of collegiate athletes. Br J
454
Sports Med 2006; 40:923-927.
456 457
fitness level affect baseline concussion symptoms? J Athl Train 2013; 48:654-658. 18. Schneider KJ, Emery CA, Kang J, Schneider GM, Meeuwisse WH. Examining sport concussion assessment tool ratings for male and female youth hockey players with
461
AC C
458
17. Mrazik M, Naidu D, Lebrun C, Game A, Matthews-White J. Does an individual's
EP
455
TE D
453
462
mild head injury status. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2001; 16:133-140.
459 460
463 464
and without a history of concussion. Br J Sports Med 2010; 44:1112-1117.
19. Maria MPS, Pinkston JB, Miller SR, Gouvier WD. Stability of postconcussion symptomatology differs between high and low responders and by gender but not by
20. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008; 4:1-32.
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 465 466 467
21. Mehl MR, Conner TS, eds. Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. 1st ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2011. 22. Dillman DA, Smyth JD, Christian LM, eds. Internet, Mail, and Mixed-Mode Surveys: The Tailored Design Method. 3rd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Publishing;
469
2008.
470
RI PT
468
23. McCrory P, Meeuwisse WH, Johnston K, et al. Consensus statement on concussion in sport 3rd International conference on concussion in sport held in Zurich,
472
November 2008. Clin J Sport Med 2009; 19:185-200.
24. Valovich McLeod TC, Bay RC, Lam KC, Chhabra A. Representative baseline
M AN U
473
SC
471
474
values on the Sport Concussion Assessment Tool 2 (SCAT2) in adolescent athletes
475
vary by gender, grade, and concussion history. Am J Sports Med 2012; 40:927-933.
476
25. Cohen S, Williamson G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In Spacapan S, Oskamp S, eds. The social psychology of health. Newbury
478
Park, CA: Sage; 1988:31-67.
481 482 483 484 485 486 487
Scand 1983; 67:361-370.
EP
480
26. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr
27. Stewart AL, Hays RD, Ware JEJ. Heath perceptions, energy/fatigue and health distress measures. In Stewart AL, Ware JEJ, eds. Measuring Functioning and Well-
AC C
479
TE D
477
Being: The Medical Outcomes Study Approach. Durham, NC: Duke University Press; 1992:55-377.
28. Chalder T, Berelowitz G, Pawlikowska T, et al.. Development of a fatigue scale. J Psychosom Res 1993; 37:147-153. 29. Hormuth SE. The sampling of experiences in situ. J Pers 1986; 54:262-293.
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 488 489 490
30. Conner Christensen T, Barrett LF, Bliss-Moreau E, Lebo K, Kaschub C. A practical guide to experience-sampling procedures. J Happiness Stud 2003; 4:53-78. 31. Kernis MH, Cornell DP, Sun CR, Berry A, Harlow T. There's more to self-esteem than whether it is high or low: the importance of stability of self-esteem. J Pers Soc
492
Psychol 1993; 65:1190-1204.
497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511
SC
NY: Routledge; 2010.
M AN U
496
33. Hox JJ, ed. Multilevel analysis: Techniques and Applications. 2nd ed. New York,
34. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, eds. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2003. 35. Westfall PH, Young SS, eds. Resampling-Based Multiple Testing: Examples and Methods for P-Value Adjustment. 1st ed. New York, NY: Wiley; 2003.
TE D
495
SPSS. 1st ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2013.
36. R Core Team (R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing). Version 3.0.2. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. 37. Amaducci Cde M, Mota DD, Pimenta CA. Fatigue among nursing undergraduate
EP
494
32. Heck RH, Thomas SL, Tabata LN. Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling with IBM
students. Rev Esc Enferm USP 2010; 44:1052-1058. 38. Oginska H, Pokorski J. Fatigue and mood correlates of sleep length in three age-
AC C
493
RI PT
491
social groups: School children, students, and employees. Chronobiol Int 2006; 23:1317-1328.
39. Valpiani EM, Brown RF, Thorsteinsson EB. Poor sleep quality mediates between depression to fatigue in a university student sample. Psychol Educ 2011; 48:59-71. 40. Scollon C, Kim-Prieto C, Diener E. Experience Sampling: Promises and Pitfalls, Strengths and Weaknesses. J Happiness Stud 2003; 4:5-34.
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 512
41. Collie A, Maruff P, Darby DG, McStephen M. The effects of practice on the
513
cognitive test performance of neurologically normal individuals assessed at brief
514
test-retest intervals. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003; 9:419-428. 42. Valovich McLeod TC, Perrin DH, Guskiewicz KM, Shultz SJ, Diamond R,
RI PT
515
Gansneder BM. Serial administration of clinical concussion assessments and
517
learning effects in healthy young athletes. Clin J Sport Med 2004; 14:287-295.
518
43. Andrews B, Wilding JM. The relation of depression and anxiety to life-stress and
519
SC
516
achievement in students. Br J Psychol 2004; 95:509-521.
44. Said D, Kypri K, Bowman J. Risk factors for mental disorder among university
521
students in Australia: findings from a web-based cross-sectional survey. Soc
522
Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2013; 48:935-944.
523
M AN U
520
45. Gallagher RP. National survey of college counseling, [The International Association of Counseling Services, Inc], 2012. Available at
525
http://www.collegecounseling.org/wp-content/uploads/NSCCD_Survey_2012.pdf.
526
Accessed June 6, 2015.
528
general population data. Aust Psychol 2010; 45:249-257. 47. Moreno E, Girón FJ. On the frequentist and Bayesian approaches to hypothesis
532
AC C
529
46. Stallman HM. Psychological distress in university students: A comparison with
EP
527
TE D
524
533
Br J Sports Med 2015; 49:499-505
530 531
testing. SORT 2006; 30:3-28.
48. Lee H, Sullivan SJ, Schneiders AG, et al. Smartphone and tablet apps for concussion road warriors (team clinicians): a systematic review for practical users.
534 535
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT TABLES
536 537
Table 1
538
Univariate Linear Mixed-Effects Analyses Predicting the TSSchange and SSSchange
RI PT
Univariate linear mixed-effects analysesa TSSchange Predictor variable
p value
SC
p value
SSSchange
.90
.85
Time zones of day
.29
.25
.01
.06
.02
.007
.47
.66
Physical activity status in the last 2 hours
.48
.28
Trouble sleeping the previous night
.001
.02
Alcohol consumption in the last 2 hours
.05
.70
Caffeine consumption in the last 2 hours
.76
.76
Stress
.001
.001
.001
.001
.001
.001
Mental fatigue
.001
.001
Physical fatigue
.001
.001
Life stressors (e.g. exam stress)
.004
.001
Location of the participant Primary activity
EP
Anxiety
TE D
Type of interactant (person)
M AN U
Gender
AC C
Depression
539
Note. TSS=total symptom score; SSS=symptom severity score
540
a
p < .20 was set as a criterion to filter the predictors from the univariate model
541 542
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Multiple Linear Mixed-Effects Analyses Predicting the TSSchange Predictor variable
Estimate
Time (days)
-0.29***
[-0.40, -0.18]
University
1.06**
[0.36, 1.75]
Flat/college
0.38
[-0.29, 1.05]
Café/Restaurant
2.19***
[0.84, 3.55]
M AN U
Location of participanta
Sports/recreation centre
0.30
[-0.83, 1.45]
Other place
4.04***
[3.08, 5.01]
-0.98***
[-1.21, -0.74]
-0.53***
[-0.77, -0.29]
Physical fatigue
TE D
Mental fatigue 545
Note. CI=confidence interval
546
a
547
**p < .01, ***p < .001
549 550 551
EP
Other place (reference category)
AC C
548
95% CI
RI PT
544
Table 2
SC
543
552 553 554
25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 555
Table 3
556
Post-hoc Analyses for the Predictor Variable of Location of the Participant Estimate
University vs Other place
1.06 *
[0.11, 2.00]
Flat/college vs Other place
0.38
[-0.53, 1.30]
Café/Restaurant vs Other place
2.19 *
-0.67 *
[-1.29, -0.05]
Café/Restaurant vs University
1.13
[-0.62, 2.90]
Sports/recreation centre vs University
-0.75
[-2.15, 0.64]
Café/Restaurant vs Flat/college
1.81 *
[0.07, 3.55]
Café/Restaurant vs Sports/recreation centre
-1.88
[-1.43, 1.29]
-0.07
[-4.02, 0.24]
TE D
Sports/recreation centre vs Flat/college Note. CI=confidence interval
558
*p < .05
AC C
562
EP
557
561
SC
[-1.25, 1.87]
Flat/college vs University
560
[0.35, 4.04]
0.30
M AN U
Sports/recreation centre vs Other place
559
95% CI
RI PT
Predictor variable
563 564 565
26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 566
Table 4
567
Multiple Linear Mixed-Effects Analyses Predicting the SSSchange
-0.53**
[-0.86, -0.19]
-0.98
[-2.76, 0.78]
Anxietya From time to time,
A great deal of time A lot of the time Only occasionally Physical fatigue
568
Note. CI=confidence interval
569
a
570
**p < .01, ***p < .001
572 573 574
1.17
[-1.28, 3.62]
13.21***
[10.02, 16.40]
-2.73 ***
[-3.44, -2.02]
-1.73***
[-2.47, -0.99]
EP
Only occasionally (reference category)
AC C
571
[3.63, 10.01]
TE D
Mental fatigue
6.82***
M AN U
but not too often
95% CI
RI PT
Time (days)
Estimate
SC
Predictor variable
575 576
27
Table 5
578
Post-hoc Analysis for the Predictor Variable of Anxiety Estimate
From time to time, but not too often vs Only occasionally A great deal of time vs Only occasionally
1.17
[-2.00, 4.35]
7.81*** [4.10, 11.52]
TE D
A great deal of time vs From time to time, but not too often
2.16
A lot of the time vs A great deal of time
-5.65*** [-9.15, -2.15]
580
***p < .001
AC C
Note. CI=confidence interval
[-0.46, 4.79]
EP
A lot of the time vs From time to time, but not too often
579
582
[-3.28, 1.30]
6.82*** [2.69, 10.90]
A lot of the time vs Only occasionally
581
-0.98
95% CI
M AN U
Predictor variable
SC
577
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
583
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
585
Table S1
586
Demographic Comparison of Compliant and Non-Compliant Participants
RI PT
SUPPLEMENTARY TABLES
584
Outside the time
framea
a
588
b
Females
n
n
Group 1
19.7 (2.1)
5
Group 2
19.7 (2.6)
Group 3
Males
Females
Combined
n
n
n
n
20
25
1
4
5
6
13
19
4
7
11
20.4 (2.7)
5
7
12
3
15
18
Group 4
21.5 (2.4)
4
11
15
6
9
15
Group 5
20.5 (2.6)
2
20
22
1
7
8
Group 6
20.7 (1.5)
4
13
17
2
11
13
TE D
M (SD)
Combined
Number of participants who completed the study within 4 hours of sending the text message
AC C
589
Males
EP
587
Age (Years)
frameb
M AN U
Groups
SC
Within the time
Number of participants who did not comply with the study criterion
590
29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table S2
592
Percentages and Frequencies for the Categorical Predictor Variables (2 levels) at Baseline (t0)
593
Participants 594
Females (n=84)
595
Yes
No
Yes
% (n)
% (n)
% (n)
597
Variable 598 599
Physical activity status
26.9 (7)
73.1 (19)
Trouble sleeping
38.5 (10)
Alcohol consumption Caffeine consumption
% (n)
% (n)
76.4 (84)
61.5 (16)
40.5 (34)
59.5 (50)
40.0 (44)
60.0 (66)
0.0 (0)
100.0 (26)
2.4 (2)
97.6 (82)
1.8 (2)
98.2 (108)
26.9 (7)
73.1 (19)
16.7 (14)
83.3 (70)
19.1 (21)
80.9 (89)
TE D
EP AC C
606
No
23.6 (26)
602
605
Yes
77.4 (65)
601
604
% (n)
Combined (N=110)
22.6 (19)
600
603
No
M AN U
596
SC
Males (n=26)
RI PT
591
607
30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table S3
609
Percentages and Frequencies for the Categorical Predictor Variables (>2 levels) at Baseline (t0)
RI PT
608
Participants
65.4 (17) 26.9 (7) 3.8 (1) 3.8 (1)
35.7 (30) 48.8 (41) 4.8 (4) 10.7 (9)
42.7 (47) 43.6 (48) 4.5 (5) 9.1 (10)
53.8 (14) 3.8 (1) 34.6 (9) 3.8 (1) 0.0 (0) 3.8 (1) 0.0 (0)
29.8 (25) 9.5 (8) 11.9 (10) 10.7 (9) 4.8 (4) 2.4 (2) 31.0 (26)
35.5 (39) 8.2 (9) 17.3 (19) 9.1 (10) 3.6 (4) 2.1 (3) 23.6 (26)
AC C
EP
TE D
Location of the participant University/classroom/labs/library Flat/college (residential) Sports/recreation facility Other Primary activity of the participant Studying Watching TV Activity in classroom/laboratory Sports/leisure activities Sleeping Working Other
Females (n=84) Combined (N=110) % (n) % (n)
SC
Males (n=26) % (n)
M AN U
Variable
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624
Other issues None
7.7 (2) 11.5 (3) 0.0 (0) 3.8 (1) 7.7 (2)
3.6 (3) 10.7 (9) 2.4 (2) 10.7 (9) 15.5 (13)
4.5 (5) 10.9 (12) 1.8 (2) 9.1 (10) 13.6 (15)
57.1 (48)
60.0 (66)
26.9 (7)
47.6 (40)
27.3 (30)
19.2 (5) 7.7 (2)
19.0 (16) 11.9 (10)
42.7 (47) 19.1 (21)
46.2 (12)
21.4 (18)
10.9 (12)
42.3 (11) 15.4 (4) 7.7 (2) 34.6 (9)
52.4 (44) 15.5 (13) 7.1 (6) 25.0 (21)
50.0 (55) 15.5 (17) 7.3 (8) 27.3 (30)
69.2 (18)
Anxiety From time to time, but not too often A great deal of time A lot of the time Only occasionally Depression Sometimes Very often Nearly all the time Not at all
RI PT
34.5 (38) 2.7 (3) 19.1 (21) 43.6 (48)
SC
614
36.9 (31) 2.4 (2) 15.5 (13) 45.2 (38)
M AN U
613
26.9 (7) 3.8 (1) 30.8 (8) 38.8 (10)
TE D
612
EP
611
Type of interactant of the participant Friends/acquaintances Co-workers Classmates/flat mates Alone Life stressors Exam related issues Study-related (e.g., assignments) Family issues Health-related
AC C
610
625
32
626
Table S4
627
Descriptive Results for the Continuous Predictor Variables
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Participants
Females (n=84)
Day 1 score Day 7 score Difference
M AN U
Day 1 score Day 7 score Difference
SC
Males (n=26)
scorea M (SD)
-.12 (.15)
3.34 (1.18) 3.55 (1.35)
.21 (.17)
2.99 (1.20) 3.16 (1.35)
.17 (.15)
(Max score=5)
Physical fatigue
M (SD)
2.35 (2.01) 2.23 (2.16)
(Max score=8)
Mental fatigue
M (SD)
628
Note. M=mean; SD=standard deviation.
629
a
Day 7 score minus Day 1 score
2.65 (1.73) 2.65 (1.92) 3.00 (1.41) 3.06 (1.50) 2.48 (1.28) 2.78 (1.43)
Day 1 score Day 7 score Difference
scorea M (SD)
scorea M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
.00 (.19)
2.58 (1.79) 2.55 (1.98)
-.03 (.19)
.06 (.09)
3.08 (1.36) 3.19 (1.44)
.11 (.08)
.30 (.15)
2.60 (1.27) 2.87 (1.42)
.27 (.15)
AC C
(Max score=5)
M (SD)
TE D
Stress
M (SD)
EP
Variable
Combined (N=110)
630
33