Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
Rate and adaptation e¡ects on the auditory evoked brainstem response in human newborns and adults Robert E. Lasky * The University of Wisconsin-Madison Medical School, Department of Neurology, H6/528 Clinical Science Building, 600 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53792-5132, USA
Received 6 February 1997; revised 13 June 1997; accepted 17 June 1997
Abstract
Auditory evoked brainstem response (ABR) latencies increased and amplitudes decreased with increasing stimulus repetition rate for human newborns and adults. The wave V latency increases were larger for newborns than adults. The wave V amplitude decreases were smaller for newborns than adults. These differences could not be explained by developmental differences in frequency responsivity. The transition from the unadapted to the fully adapted response was less rapid in newborns than adults at short (= 10 ms) inter stimulus intervals (ISIs). At longer ISIs (= 20 ms) there were no developmental differences in the transition to the fully adapted response. The newborn transition occurred in a two stage process. The rapid initial stage observed in adults and newborns was complete by about 40 ms. A second slower stage was observed only in newborns although it has been observed in adults in other studies (Weatherby and Hecox, 1982; Lightfoot, 1991; Lasky et al., 1996). These effects were replicated at different stimulus intensities. After the termination of stimulation the return to the wave V unadapted response took nearly 500 ms in newborns. Neither the newborn nor the adult data can be explained by forward masking of one click on the next click. These results indicate human developmental differences in adaptation to repetitive auditory stimulation at the level of the brainstem. Keywords :
stimuli
Auditory brainstem evoked response; Human developmental di¡erence; Stimulus rate e¡ect; Adaptation to successive
1. Introduction
Auditory processing is a¡ected by prior and subsequent stimulation. Our knowledge of developmental di¡erences concerning temporal interactions among auditory stimuli is limited. Because auditory brainstem evoked responses (ABRs) are non-invasive and reliably measured, they can be used to investigate temporal interactions in the developing human. The rate paradigm has been the most frequently used evoked response paradigm to study human developmental di¡erences in temporal interactions. The same stimulus is presented repetitively at di¡erent rates. There are well documented developmental rate e¡ects on ABRs. Jewett and Romano (1972) in kittens and * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 (608) 263-9061; Fax +1 (608) 263-0412.
rats and Despland and Galambos (1980) in humans report repetition rates to which ABRs cannot be recorded in an immature organism but can be recorded in a more mature organism. Other developmental rate e¡ects have also been reported (Jewett and Romano, 1972 ; Salamy et al., 1978 ; Mair et al., 1979 ; Despland and Galambos, 1980 ; Shipley et al., 1980; Lasky and Rupert, 1982; Lasky, 1984, 1991 ; Donaldson and Rubel, 1990; Cone-Wesson et al., 1995 ; see Hall, 1992 for a review of the human literature). ABR latencies increase with increasing stimulus repetition rate. Latency prolongation is greater for more centrally generated waves, suggesting central as well as peripheral rate effects. Amplitude reduction is also associated with increasing repetition rate. Latency prolongation and the di¡erences between central and peripheral rate e¡ects decrease with age. A second paradigm which has been used to study
0378-5955 / 97 / $17.00 ß 1997 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved PII S 0 3 7 8 - 5 9 5 5 ( 9 7 ) 0 0 1 0 6 - 8
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
166
rate
e¡ects
volves uses
on
evoked
maximum
responses
length
cross-correlation
sequence
techniques
developmentally (MLS) to
stimuli
recover
the
in-
These adaptation e¡ects are in the order of hundreds of
and
milliseconds and suggest that full recovery from prior
re-
stimulation takes a surprisingly long time. Developmental data concerning the transition to and
corded response (Lasky et al., 1992 ; Weber and Roush, 1993, 1995 ; Burkard et al., 1996a,b). MLSs di¡er from
recovery
stimuli conventionally used in rate studies in that the
present study investigated rate e¡ects and the transition
from
intervals between the stimuli (inter stimulus intervals,
from
ISIs) in the sequences are not constant but vary pseu-
newborns.
the
the
adapted
unadapted
to
the
ABR
are
adapted
lacking.
ABR
in
The
human
dorandomly. Thus, the response to a MLS re£ects this variation in ISIs, while the conventional response re£ects the adapted response to constant ISI stimulation.
2. Methodology
Nonlinear systems identi¢cation techniques can be used to quantify the e¡ect of these temporal variations in MLSs on the 1978 ;
response
Shi, 1990 ;
(Marmarelis
2.1. Subjects
and Marmarelis, Two
Shi and Hecox, 1991 ; Lasky et al.,
groups
of subjects were
assessed.
One group
1992, 1995a,b ; Eggermont, 1993). Another advantage
consisted of normal hearing adults (age range, 16^32
of using the MLS paradigm is the presentation of arbi-
years) with no history of audiologic or neurologic ab-
trarily short ISI stimuli. The ISIs presented convention-
normalities. Normal hearing was de¢ned by pure tone
ally are limited by the length of the recorded response.
audiometry. All subjects had no greater than a 10 dB
If the interval between successive stimuli is shorter than
hearing loss at octave intervals from 0.25 to 8 kHz. The other group was full term, healthy newborns.
the duration of the response, overlapping of responses will occur as a by-product of averaging making inter-
These
pretation
physical and neurologic examinations by the pediatric
di¤cult.
MLS
stimuli
and
cross-correlation
newborns
(1)
were
judged
healthy
by
routine
and
house sta¡, (2) were not given antibiotics, loop diu-
Schreiner, 1982). Thus, rate e¡ects can be studied at
retics, or other medications, (3) had 5 min Apgar scores
techniques
much
overcome
faster
rates
this
than
limitation
presented
(Eysholdt
conventionally.
s
7, (4) weighed
s
2.5 kg at birth, (5) were between 38
the e¡ects apparent at slower conventional
and 41 weeks post conception according to the pediatric
rates persist at higher MLS rates in human newborns
house sta¡'s estimate of gestational age, and (6) had no
and
family history of congenital hearing loss. All newborns
Many of
kittens
(Lasky
et
al.,
1992 ;
Weber
and
Roush,
were tested between 24 and 72 h after birth.
1993, 1995 ; Burkard et al., 1996a,b). Temporal masking paradigms have also been used to study
temporal
interactions
developmentally
(Lasky
2.2. Apparatus and stimuli
and Rupert, 1982 ; Lasky, 1991, 1993). Forward maskThree
ing increases human newborn ABR latencies (and also
Grass
gold-plated
cup
electrodes
were
at-
ABR thresholds) more than adult ABR latencies. There
tached at the vertex (+), mastoid ipsilateral to the stim-
may be a direct relationship between rate e¡ects and
ulus (
forward masking e¡ects ; the former are often explained
tween any two less than 10 000
3
), and forehead (ground) with impedances be-
6
.
Two di¡erent apparatuses were used in the di¡erent
in terms of the latter (Lasky and Rupert, 1982 ; Burkard
experiments. For experiments I, II, IV and V the re-
and Hecox, 1983).
5
Several researchers (Thornton and Coleman, 1975 ;
sponses were ampli¢ed (10 ) and ¢ltered by a Grass
Don et al., 1977 ; Weatherby and Hecox, 1982 ; Light-
AC preampli¢er (P511J). The 1/2 amplitude frequency
foot, 1991 ; Lasky et al., 1996) have presented trains of
for the low and high cuto¡s were 100 and 3000 Hz. The
stimuli in order to record the transition from the una-
roll-o¡
dapted to the adapted ABR. Don et al. (1977) presented
frequency cuto¡s. A Nicolet 171/4 Signal Digitizer con-
adults a train of 10 clicks with 10 ms between clicks and
verted the analog response to digital form and rejected
an interval of 500 ms between click trains. By the fourth
signals greater than 10
or ¢fth click Don et al. reported that the ABR was fully
signals were averaged by a Nicolet 1174 Signal Averag-
adapted, i.e. ABR latencies in response to the fourth or
er.
¢fth click were the same as to clicks presented at a rate
50 000 Hz, and for experiments IV and V it was 7143
of 100/s. ABR results are similar to the results of elec-
Hz. Two independently averaged waveforms were re-
trocochleographic studies using a similar paradigm (Eg-
corded in response to each stimulus (2048 responses)
germont
for experiments I, II, and V. The two replicate wave-
and
Weatherby
and
Odenthal, Hecox
1974 ;
(1982),
Eggermont,
Lightfoot
1985).
(1991),
For
was
12
dB/octave
experiments
I
WV
and
at
both
the
low
and
high
peak-to-peak voltage. The
II
the
sampling
rate
was
and
forms were averaged, and that waveform was used in all
Lasky et al. (1996) report that the transition from the
measurements. For experiment IV a single waveform of
unadapted to the adapted wave V response may extend
2048 responses was recorded.
beyond the ¢rst few clicks in a train in some conditions.
For experiments III, VI, and VII a Nicolet Path¢nd-
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
167
er II was used to record the evoked responses. The ¢lter settings were 150^3000 Hz with a roll-o¡ of 12 dB/octave. The sampling rate was 51 200 Hz. Two independently averaged waveforms (2000 responses) were recorded to each stimulus. The latencies and amplitudes of waves I, III, and V were measured. The III-V complex was the most easily identi¢able portion of the ABR waveform (Lasky et al., 1987). The III-V complex was de¢ned by the rapid increment in amplitude to a peak (wave III) and the rapid decrement in amplitude following the last peak in the complex (wave V). Wave I was de¢ned as the highest peak prior to wave III. The amplitudes of waves I and III were measured from their peaks to the lowest trough between them. Using the trough after wave III to quantify wave III amplitude was less reliable. The amplitude of wave V was measured from its peak to the lowest trough after wave V. For experiments I, II, IV, and V the stimuli were generated by a WPI Anapulse Stimulator (model 302T). The intensity of the stimulus was controlled by Grason-Stadler ampli¢ers and attenuators. The stimuli were clicks, electronic pulses 100
Ws
in duration with instan-
taneous rise and fall times, which were fed into a shielded TDH-39 earphone. The intensity of the clicks was calibrated in dB nHL by calculating the mean threshold to the clicks for six adults who were assessed by pure tone audiometry to have no more than a 10 dB hearing loss at octave steps ranging from 0.25 to 8 kHz. The stimulus used to determine these adult thresholds was the 100
Ws
click at a rate of 10/s.
For experiments III, VI, and VII a Nicolet Path¢nder II was used to generate the stimuli, speci¢cally, the SM100 module in conjunction with the SM700 module. A TDH-50p earphone was used.
U
Fig. 1. Newborn and adult latency
rate functions for waves I, III,
and V. The error bars represent 1 S.D.
This study was reviewed by the Internal Review Boards at The University of Texas Health Science Cen-
long the latencies and reduce the amplitudes of new-
ter at Dallas and The University of Wisconsin-Madison
born ABRs may have little e¡ect on adult ABRs.
U
Figs. 1 and 2 present newborn and adult latency
Medical School. Informed consent was obtained from the adult subjects and the parents of the newborn sub-
rate and amplitude
jects prior to initiation of the testing.
V. 2 (developmental level)
U
U
U-
rate functions for waves I, III, and 3 (wave)
12 (rate) analy-
ses of variance were computed separately for latency and amplitude in order to investigate developmental di¡erences in rate e¡ects. As expected (see Hall, 1992,
3. Results
for a review), there were signi¢cant developmental level (F(1,18) = 64.11 ;
3.1. Experiment I
P
Although developmental rate e¡ects on the ABR
6
0.001),
and
P
6
0.001),
wave
rate
(F(11,198) = 41.09 ;
(F(2,36) = 2392.06 ;
U U
P
6
0.001)
main e¡ects for latency. Also as expected, there were
1992 for a review). In experiment I clicks at 12 di¡erent
rate (F(11,198) = 1.90 ; P = 0.042), developmental level wave (F(2,36) = 13.99 ; P 0.001), rate wave (F(22,396) = 5.15 ; P 0.001), and developmental level rate wave (F(22,396) = 2.13 ; P = 0.002) interactions for latency. Latency/rate
repetition rates were presented to 10 newborns and 10
e¡ects were similar for both developmental levels for
adults. Developmental di¡erences at slow repetition
wave I. For waves III and, especially, V latency/rate
rates were especially of interest. Slow rates which pro-
e¡ects were signi¢cantly greater for newborns. For am-
have been described in humans, only a relatively small number of repetition rates have been presented (Schulman-Galambos and Galambos, 1975 ; Salamy et al., 1978 ; Lasky and Rupert, 1982 ; Lasky, 1984 ; see Hall,
signi¢cant developmental level
6
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
U
U U
6
168
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
Fig. 2. Newborn and adult amplitudeUrate functions for waves I, III, and V. The error bars represent 1 S.D.
plitude the results also replicated previous work (Lasky, 1984). There were signi¢cant rate (F(11,198) =11.86; P 6 0.001) and wave (F(2,36) = 51.03; P 6 0.001) main e¡ects. The developmental levelUwave (F(2,36) =15.35; P 6 0.001) and the developmental levelUrateUwave (F(22,396) = 2.20; P = 0.002) interactions were signi¢cant. Amplitudes decreased with increasing rate. Ampli-
tude reduction was greatest for wave V, and the reduction was signi¢cantly greater for adults than newborns. Rate e¡ects for the slow and fast stimulus repetition rates were investigated separately. Analyses of variance like those described above were computed for the six rates between 10 and 20/s and for the six rates between 25 and 100/s. For latency at both rate ranges signi¢cant developmental level, rate, wave, and developmental levelUwave e¡ects were observed. At the slow rates the rate main e¡ect was signi¢cant (F(5,90) = 2.39; P = 0.044) and the developmental levelUrate interaction approached signi¢cance (F(5,90) =2.12; P =0.070). At these slow rates the only obvious rate e¡ects seemed to be for newborn wave V latencies, however, the lack of su¤cient power in the design precluded a more de¢nitive statement. At the fast rates the rate e¡ects were signi¢cant for both developmental levels and all three waves. There were signi¢cant rateUwave (F(10,180) = 4.30; P 6 0.001) and developmental levelUrateUwave (F(10,180) =2.26; P = 0.016) interactions. The greatest rate e¡ects were observed for wave V, and those e¡ects were greater for newborns than adults. For amplitude there were signi¢cant rate, wave, and developmental levelUwave interactions at both rate ranges. Amplitude decreased with increasing rate for both rate ranges and developmental levels and all three waves. The only other signi¢cant e¡ect was a signi¢cant developmental levelUrateUwave interaction (F(10,180) = 3.28; P = 0.001) for the fast but not the slow rates. The decrement in amplitude was greatest for wave V and that decrement was greater for adults than newborns. In order to provide comparative data, for each subject least square linear regression functions were computed expressing the relationship between the 12 rates and the latencies and amplitudes of the three waves. For latency linear functions provided good ¢ts of the data for wave V and poorer ¢ts for waves I and III (see Table 1). These functions are similar to those previously reported for term newborns and adults (Lasky, 1984). The linear amplitudeUrate functions did not provide very adequate ¢ts of the data for either developmental level for any of the three waves (see Table 2). No non-
Table 1 Linear regressions characterizing the relationship between stimulus rate and ABR latency for newborns and adults Mean (S.D.) % of Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.) slope P-value of Age Wave No. of signi¢cant variance explainedb intercept (ms) (Ws/decade) t-testc regressions (out of 10)a Newborn I 4 31 (27) 3.0 (0.5) 62 (40) 6 0.001 III 6 40 (26) 5.4 (0.4) 47 (26) 6 0.001 V 10 85 (8) 7.6 (0.5) 139 (19) 6 0.001 Adult I 7 43 (25) 2.0 (0.3) 61 (57) 6 0.001 III 6 41 (32) 4.3 (0.2) 42 (23) 6 0.001 V 10 76 (19) 6.1 (0.2) 79 (33) 6 0.001 a Number of subjects whose stimulus rateUlatency linear regression function di¡ered signi¢cantly (P 6 0.05) from zero. b Mean (S.D.) percentage of the variance in latency of the ABR explained by stimulus rate. c Two tailed t-test that the slopes of the stimulus rateUlatency linear regression functions for the 10 subjects at each age were zero.
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
169
linear functions consistently provided a better ¢t of the data. 3.2. Experiment II
Di¡erent latency and amplitude waveforms are elicited by di¡erent frequency stimuli (see Stapells et al., 1994 for a review). In normal hearing adults, ABRs to clicks at low stimulus rates re£ect stimulation of the 2^4 kHz region of the basilar membrane. At high rates of stimulation other frequencies may dominate the ABR. Thus, developmental di¡erences concerning the stimulus frequencies that generate the ABR at di¡erent rates could explain developmental rate e¡ects. A high pass masking paradigm (Teas et al., 1962) was used to restrict the stimuli eliciting the ABRs to explore this possibility. Ten healthy, term newborns were tested in experiment II. A 55 dB nHL, 100 Ws click was presented at four di¡erent rates, 20, 40, 70, and 100 clicks/s. A continuous broadband masker was presented simultaneously with the click. The masker was produced by a Grason-Stadler Noise Generator (model 455B) which was fed into two Krohn-Hite 48 dB/octave ¢lters (model 3700) in series. The output from the ¢lters was ampli¢ed, attenuated, and mixed with the click by a series of Grason-Stadler components. The intensity of the broadband masker was increased until an ABR was no longer elicited. That masker intensity was held constant for the remaining stimuli presented to that newborn. The four click rates were presented in the presence of one high pass masker with a low cut-o¡ frequency equal to 4 kHz. The di¡erence waveform resulting from subtracting the ABR in the presence of the masker containing frequencies s 4 kHz from the ABR in the presence of no masker yielded the ABR contributed by frequencies s 4 kHz. For the 10 newborns the slopes of the latencyUrate and amplitudeUrate functions were similar for the unmasked and the frequency restricted responses (see Fig. 3). The slopes did not signi¢cantly (P 6 0.05) di¡er from each other by 2 (frequency range)U4 (rate)U3 (wave) analysis of variance computed separately for la-
Fig. 3. Wave V latencies and amplitudes as a function of rate and frequency range (broadband or restricted). The error bars represent 1 S.D.
tency and amplitude (i.e. there were no signi¢cant frequency rangeUrate or frequency rangeUrateUwave interactions). However, the amplitudes of the unmasked responses were signi¢cantly greater than the amplitudes of the frequency restricted responses (i.e. there was a signi¢cant frequency range main e¡ect). 3.3. Experiment III
Experiment I described developmental di¡erences in rate e¡ects on the ABR. The response recorded to each rate represented the adapted response to that rate. In
Table 2 Linear regressions characterizing the relationship between stimulus rate and ABR amplitude for newborns and adults Mean (S.D.) % of Mean (S.D.) Mean (S.D.) P-value of Age Wave No. of signi¢cant variance explainedb intercept (WV) slope (WV/decade) t-testc regressions (out of 10)a Newborn I 6 40 (22) 0.20 (0.07) 30.012 (0.006) 6 0.001 III 3 26 (28) 0.24 (0.05) 30.007 (0.007) 0.007 V 3 23 (26) 0.26 (0.11) 30.006 (0.008) 0.034 Adult I 1 19 (17) 0.14 (0.05) 30.007 (0.006) 0.008 III 5 38 (30) 0.21 (0.05) 30.010 (0.006) 6 0.001 V 7 36 (20) 0.41 (0.08) 30.018 (0.011) 6 0.001 a Number of subjects whose stimulus rateUamplitude linear regression function di¡ered signi¢cantly (P 6 0.05) from zero. b Mean (S.D.) percentage of the variance in amplitude of the ABR explained by stimulus rate. c Two tailed t-test that the slopes of the stimulus rateUlatency linear regression functions for the 10 subjects at each age were zero.
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
170
experiment III a train of stimuli was presented in order to investigate developmental di¡erences in the transition from the unadapted to the fully adapted response. Don et al. (1977) presented adults a train of 10 clicks with 10 ms between clicks and an interval of 500 ms between click trains. By the fourth or ¢fth click Don et al. reported that the ABR was fully adapted, i.e. the ABR in response to the fourth or ¢fth click was the same as to clicks presented at a rate of 100/s. Other researchers
have
reported
similar
results
(Thornton
and Coleman, 1975). These ABR results are similar to electrocochleographic
results
(Eggermont
and
Oden-
thal, 1974 ; Eggermont, 1985). Experiment III was conducted to investigate developmental di¡erences in the transition from the unadapted to the fully adapted ABR. The inter stimulus interval (ISI) between clicks in a train (10 or 20 ms) was varied. Nine healthy, term newborns and 11 normal hearing adults served as subjects in this experiment. Each subject was presented ¢ve di¡erent stimuli. The ¢rst stimulus was a 50 dB nHL, 100
Ws
click presented at a rate
of 10/s. The second and third stimuli were the same click at the same intensity but presented at rates of 50/s and 100/s. The ¢nal two stimuli were trains of twenty-three 50 dB nHL, 100
Ws
clicks. The clicks in
one of these click trains were separated by 10 ms ISIs. The clicks in the other click train were separated by 20 ms ISIs. There was a 250 ms inter train interval (ITI)
Fig. 4. Percentage of the wave V latency adapted response as a function of developmental level, number of click in the train, and the ISI characterizing the train. The error bars represent 1 S.D.
between click trains in both the 10 and 20 ms ISI stimuli. Describing developmental di¡erences in the transi-
sponse).
Responses
that
were
signi¢cantly
di¡erent
tion from the adapted to the unadapted response in
from neither the unadapted nor the adapted responses
terms of the change in
were too variable given the power in the design to in-
Ws or WV of the ABR is mislead-
ing because the dynamic range of the response in new-
terpret meaningfully. Similar analyses were conducted
borns and adults di¡ers. Consequently, changes in la-
to assess the transition in ABR amplitudes from the
tency
for
unadapted to the adapted response. Wave V latency
newborns and adults by the measured dynamic range.
measurements for both newborns and adults were inter-
For latency, dynamic range was de¢ned by the mean
pretable. The other measurements were not consistently
di¡erence in ABR latency to the 100/s or 50/s stimulus
interpretable and will not be reported.
and
amplitude
were
adjusted
separately
minus the ABR latency to the 10/s stimulus. For am-
Fig. 4 presents the percentage of the dynamic range
plitude, dynamic range was the mean di¡erence in am-
for wave V latency as a function of the number of the
plitude between the 10/s stimulus minus the 100/s or 50/
click in the train, the ISI de¢ning the train, and the
s stimulus. For latency, the dependent variable was the
developmental level of the subject. The adult wave V
percentage of the dynamic range accounted for by the
latency to the ¢rst click in the 20 ms ISI stimulus did
latency to the eliciting click minus the latency to the 10/
not signi¢cantly di¡er from the unadapted response.
s stimulus. For amplitude, the dependent variable was
The newborn and adult responses to all other clicks
the percentage of the dynamic range accounted for by
did. Thus, the ITI in this study (250 ms) was not su¤-
the amplitude to the 10/s click minus the amplitude to
ciently long for full recovery for the newborns in re-
the eliciting click.
sponse to either stimulus or for the adults when the
t-tests
determine
ISI was 10 ms. By the second click in the 20 ms ISI
whether the latency of the response to the click in ques-
stimulus newborn and adult responses did not signi¢-
tion was consistently longer than the response to the 10/
cantly di¡er from each other and were not signi¢cantly
t-tests
di¡erent from the fully adapted response. Thus, the
were also computed to determine whether the latency to
transition to the adapted response was achieved rapidly
the click in question was consistently shorter than to
and similarly at both developmental levels in response
the 100/s or the 50/s stimulus (the fully adapted re-
to the 20 ms ISI stimulus.
One
sample
were
computed
to
s stimulus (the unadapted response). One sample
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
Fig. 5. Percentage of the newborn wave V latency adapted response as a function of number of click in a 25 click train. The error bars represent 1 S.D.
Although the response to the ¢rst click of the 10 ms ISI stimulus was signi¢cantly longer in latency than the unadapted response and did not di¡er between the two developmental levels, there was a signi¢cant developmental level e¡ect in the response to subsequent clicks. Newborn wave V latencies were generally signi¢cantly shorter than their adapted (100/s) wave V latencies. In contrast, that was only true for the ¢rst several clicks in the stimulus train for adults (replicating Don et al.'s results). The signi¢cant developmental level e¡ect was con¢rmed statistically by a 2 (developmental level)U23 (click) repeated measures analyses of variance (F(1,18) = 4.84; P 6 0.041). There was no signi¢cant developmental level e¡ect for the 20 ms ISI stimulus. 3.4. Experiment IV
Experiment III demonstrated that the transition from the unadapted to the fully adapted state was less rapid and less complete in newborns than adults in response to some stimuli. Experiment IV presented a 25 click train stimulus in an e¡ort to replicate this result in newborns. Furthermore, the ITI was increased to 500 ms since the ¢rst click in the 23 click train was not fully recovered after a 250 ms ITI (experiment III). A total of eight healthy, term newborns served as subjects in this study. Each subject was presented three stimuli. One stimulus was the 55 dB nHL, 100 Ws click presented at a rate of 10/s. A second stimulus was the same click at the same intensity but presented at a rate of 100/s. The ¢nal stimulus was a train of twenty-¢ve 55 dB nHL, 100 Ws clicks each separated by 10 ms of silence. There was a 500 ms ITI between click trains.
171
The ABR to the 10/s stimulus was used to estimate the unadapted response. The ABR to the 100/s stimulus was used to estimate the fully adapted response. The transition from the unadapted to the adapted response was de¢ned in terms of the percentage of the dynamic range explaining the di¡erence between the unadapted and adapted responses (see Fig. 5). Waves I and III latency and the amplitude data will not be presented due to the variability of the data. One sample t-tests were computed to determine whether the recorded latencies for wave V to each click signi¢cantly di¡ered from the wave V latencies to the unadapted (10/s) and the adapted (100/s) stimuli. Wave V to the ¢rst click was not signi¢cantly di¡erent from the unadapted response. By the third click and all clicks thereafter, it was. The transition from the unadapted response to 60% of the adapted response occurred by the ¢fth click. The remaining transition was quite gradual. It was not until the 20th click that the recorded wave V latency was no longer signi¢cantly di¡erent from the adapted response. Indeed, with a larger sample it is possible that the responses to even the 25th stimulus would not be fully adapted. 3.5. Experiment V
Experiments III and IV indicated that the fully adapted response in newborns to some stimuli (those of short ISI) takes a surprisingly long time to develop. Forward masking has been used to explain rate e¡ects (Lasky and Rupert, 1982; Burkard and Hecox, 1983). Experiment V was conducted to determine the relationship between forward masking and the rate and transition e¡ects reported in experiments I^IV. The vt between two clicks was systematically varied, and the vt at which forward masking e¡ects were no longer observed was determined. That vt de¢nes the length of time that a single click can in£uence the response to a subsequent click. If the auditory system were a second order nonlinear temporal system (i.e. one and only one stimulus a¡ects the response to a preceding or following stimulus), then that vt should de¢ne the maximum ISI (or the slowest rate) a¡ecting the ABR. It should also de¢ne the time between the ¢rst click in a train and the time the fully adapted response was ¢rst recorded. Experiment I indicated that for wave V latency, rate effects were observed at rates as slow as 25/s (ISI = 40 ms) for adults and slower than that for newborns. If the results of this two click paradigm fail to predict the results of experiments I^IV, those results are probably a consequence of higher order temporal nonlinearities (i.e. the interactions of three or more clicks). Eight adults and eight healthy, term newborns served as subjects. Five stimuli were presented to each subject. One stimulus was a 55 dB nHL, 100 Ws click presented at a rate of 10/s. A second stimulus was the same click
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
172
the same click at the same intensity but presented at a rate of 100/s. The remaining two stimuli were trains of eight 50 dB nHL, 100
Ws clicks each separated by 10 ms
of silence. The only di¡erence in these two stimuli was the ITI between click trains (ITI = 304 or 475 ms). The ABR to the 10/s stimulus was used to estimate the unadapted response. The ABR to the 100/s stimulus was used to estimate the fully adapted response. As for experiments III and IV the transition from the unadapted to the adapted response was de¢ned in terms of the percentage of the di¡erence between the unadapted and adapted responses (see Fig. 6). t-tests were comFig. 6. Percentage of the wave V latency adapted response as a
puted to determine whether the recorded wave V laten-
function of click number and the ITI separating the click trains.
cies to each click were signi¢cantly di¡erent than the
The error bars represent 1 S.D.
latencies
to
the
unadapted
(10/s)
and
the
adapted
(100/s) stimuli. Wave V to the ¢rst click was not sigpresented at a rate of 100/s. The other three stimuli
ni¢cantly di¡erent (although longer in latency) than the
consisted of trains of two 55 dB nHL, 100
clicks
unadapted response at an ITI = 475 ms, however, it was
with 100 ms separating the o¡set of the second click
signi¢cantly longer in latency than the unadapted re-
of the train and the onset of the ¢rst click of the next
sponse at an ITI = 304 ms. The e¡ect of the shorter
train. The
between the two clicks of the trains dif-
ITI was to increase the latency of all clicks in the train
fered, they were 10, 20, and 30 ms for the three stimuli.
similar to the results reported by Lasky et al. (1996) for
vts
Ws
For both developmental levels all wave V latencies signi¢cantly (P
6
0.05) di¡ered from the 100/s response
t-test).
(by a one sample
Only wave V to the second
click of the pair separated by a
vt = 10
ms was signi¢-
cantly di¡erent from the 10/s response. There seemed to
adults. However, 2 (ITI)
U
8 (click) repeated measures
analysis of variance computed on the wave V latencies indicated that this di¡erence as a function of ITI was not statistically signi¢cant. The variability in the data prevented unquali¢ed interpretation of this di¡erence.
be little e¡ect of forward masking of one click on another at a no
vt = 30
ms. Furthermore, there seemed to be
developmental
di¡erences
as
a
function
vt
of
although the statistical power associated with this state-
U
ment warrants quali¢cation. These statements were sup-
U
ported by the results of a 2 (developmental level) (click
number)
of
variance.
P
0.001) and
6
fects
and
P = 0.007)
3
The
v
( t)
repeated
click
number
measures
2
analysis
(F(1,14) = 24.44 ;
vt (F(2,28) = 3.51 ; P = 0.044) main efthe click number vt (F(2,28) = 6.02 ;
U
3.7. Experiment VII Experiment VII was conducted to determine the effect of varying the intensity of the clicks on the transition from the unadapted to adapted wave V responses in human newborns. Seven healthy, term newborns served as subjects. Six stimuli were presented to each subject. One stimulus was a 60 dB nHL, 100
Ws
click presented at a rate of
interaction were signi¢cant. None of the ef-
10/s. A second stimulus was the same click at the same
fects involving developmental level were signi¢cant. The
intensity presented at a rate of 100/s. Two other stimuli
other ABR measurements will not be reported because of the variability associated with those measurements.
3.6. Experiment VI Experiment VI was conducted to investigate the effect of ITI on the recovery of the ABR to the unadapted state. Newborns were presented trains of 8 clicks. These trains of clicks were separated by two di¡erent ITIs (304 and 475 ms). In experiment III an ITI = 250 ms was insu¤cient for full recovery to the unadapted
state,
whereas
in
experiment
IV
an
ITI = 500 ms was su¤cient for full recovery. Nine healthy, term newborns served as subjects in this experiment. Each subject was presented four di¡erent stimuli. One stimulus was the 50 dB nHL, 100
Ws
click presented at a rate of 10/s. A second stimulus was
Fig. 7. Percentage of the wave V latency adapted response as a function of click number and the intensity of the click trains. The error bars represent 1 S.D.
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
173
were identical to the ¢rst two stimuli except that they
sponses attenuate and lower frequency longer latency
were presented at 45 dB nHL. The ¢nal two stimuli
responses
were trains of sixteen 100
sponse.
Ws
clicks with an ISI = 10
One sample
t-tests
sition
were computed to compare the
characterize
the
recorded
re-
Experiments III, IV, VI, and VII concerned the tran-
ms and an ITI = 250 ms. One of these click trains was presented at 60 dB nHL, the other at 45 dB nHL.
increasingly
from
sponse.
the
The
unadapted
transition
to
from
the the
fully
adapted
unadapted
to
rethe
wave V latencies to each click to the unadapted (10/s)
adapted response (wave V latency) was more rapid in
and the adapted (50/s or the 100/s) wave V latencies.
adults than newborns at a short ISI (10 ms). Similarly,
The results were similar for the two intensities (see Fig.
in experiment I the range of rates which a¡ected wave
7). For the 60 dB nHL train, the 11th and 13th^16th
V
responses did not signi¢cantly di¡er from the response
Speci¢cally, newborn wave V latencies were prolonged
to the 100/s click. For the 45 dB nHL train, the eighth,
at ISIs which no longer a¡ected adult wave V latencies.
ninth and 11th^16th responses did not signi¢cantly dif-
At a longer ISI (20 ms) there were no developmental
fer from the response to the 100/s click. All other re-
di¡erences in the transition to the adapted response.
U
latencies
was
narrower
for
adults
than
newborns.
The results of experiment V indicate that a simple
sponses were signi¢cantly shorter latency than the wave 16 (click)
second order temporal nonlinearity cannot adequately
repeated measures analysis of variance was computed
account for rate and adaptation e¡ects in newborns and
for
adults. Speci¢cally, it was di¤cult to demonstrate an
V latency to the 100/s click. A 2 (intensity)
the
percentage
of
wave
V
latency
prolongation.
e¡ect of one click on another at a temporal separation
Only the click main e¡ect was signi¢cant.
(30 ms) that from rate studies there should be an e¡ect for both newborns and adults. Furthermore, there were no
4. Discussion
striking
developmental
di¡erences
in
the
forward
masking of one click on another click. Thus, di¡erences In experiment I developmental rate e¡ects reported
between newborn and adult rate results seem to be a
by a number of investigators were replicated (Jewett
consequence of temporal nonlinearities of greater than
and Romano, 1972 ; Schulman-Galambos and Galam-
second order (i.e. due to the interactions of three or
bos, 1975 ; Starr et al., 1977 ; Despland and Galambos,
more clicks).
1980 ; Lasky, 1984 ; see Hall, 1992 for a review). Two
For 10 ms ISI trains newborn transitions seemed to
results which have not been emphasized were noted.
occur in two stages, an initial rapid stage spanning the
The ¢rst is the likelihood that newborn ABR latencies
¢rst four or ¢ve stimuli and a slower second stage that
are
extended
a¡ected at
slow rates
which have
little
e¡ect on
for a considerable time. It is interesting to
adult ABR latencies. The data support this statement
note that in terms of milliseconds of wave V latency
although there was insu¤cient power in the study to be
prolongation (not percent of the adapted response as
de¢nitive. The second result concerns di¡erences in rate
presented
e¡ects on latencies and amplitudes. Amplitude reduc-
and the adult transition were similar, however, the sec-
tion was observed with increasing stimulus rate for all
ond stage was not observed in adults. The second stage
three waves and at both developmental levels. In con-
in newborns indicates that the responses to clicks in a
trast to the latency data, this e¡ect was signi¢cantly
train with 10 ms ISIs were a¡ected by clicks presented
greater for the wave Vs of adults than newborns. The
up to several hundred ms earlier. The temporal inter-
adult wave V was the largest amplitude measurement
actions responsible for the second stage in the transition
recorded which may explain this result. Greater ampli-
to the adapted response may distinguish newborns and
tude reduction is possible the larger the wave. The dif-
adults in terms of their responses to repetitive stimula-
ferences between the e¡ects of rate on latency and am-
tion.
plitude
underscore
interpreting duced
the
need
developmental
latency
rate
prolongation
and
to
exercise
care
in
di¡erences.
Rate
in-
amplitude
reduction
in
this
study)
the
¢rst
stage
of
newborns
Three studies (Weatherby and Hecox, 1982 ; Lightfoot, 1991 ;
Lasky et al., 1996) indicate that even in
adults the transition from the unadapted to the adapted
do
wave V response may be much less rapid in some con-
not necessarily imply more general developmental dif-
ditions than reported in this study and by Don et al.
ferences in rate e¡ects.
(1977). With a larger sample of subjects than used in
di¡er
with
developmental
level.
Those
di¡erences
Experiment II indicated that rate e¡ects in newborns
s
other studies, Lightfoot reported that none of the re-
and
sponses to clicks in eight click trains presented at an
broadband responses. Thus, it is unlikely that the great-
ISI = 11.1 ms were fully adapted. The greater statistical
er
power of Lightfoot's design may have allowed him to
were
similar
latency
for
frequency
prolongation
of
restricted
newborn
(
4
kHz)
responses
to
in-
creasing rate is a consequence of frequency e¡ects. Spe-
observe
ci¢cally, it does not seem to be the case that as rate in
Weatherby and Hecox replicated the results of Don et
newborns is increased high frequency short latency re-
al. (1977) with click stimuli but reported a much pro-
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
di¡erences
missed
by
other
investigators.
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
174
longed two stage transition to the adapted response
click intensity while maintaining a constant click to
with noise burst stimuli. Lasky et al. (1996) recently
masker level (this result did not hold at the softest click
replicated the two stage transition to the adapted re-
levels presented) did not change the forward masking
sponse reported by Weatherby and Hecox and Light-
latency functions while changing masker level but main-
foot. These studies suggest that the transition from the
taining a constant click level did (experiment V, Lasky
unadapted to the adapted response may involve the
and Rupert, 1982 ; experiment II, Lasky, 1993). Among
same two stage function for newborns and adults and
other things, this result indicates that age related di¡er-
that stimulus parameters and developmental level a¡ect
ences in outer and middle ear resonances which do af-
the transition. The presence of the second stage of this
fect the level of the input to the cochlea probably do
function depends on whether the auditory system in
not account for the observed results.
question has su¤cient time to recover from prior stim-
Studies using a variety of paradigms consistently re-
ulation. In this study, for a 10 ms ISI the adult auditory
port di¡erences between human newborns and adults in
system seemed to be able to recover from prior stimu-
their brainstem responses to stimuli separated in time.
lation more rapidly than the newborn auditory system.
This study contributes to those results by indicating
For 20 ms trains both developmental levels seemed to
that the transition between the adapted and unadapted
be able recover so that their responses were only af-
response di¡ers in newborns and adults. Caution is warranted in interpreting the developmen-
fected by a few prior clicks. Another indication that long time constants de¢ne
tal di¡erences reported in this study. Statistical artifact
the limits of temporal interactions among clicks was
may explain some of those di¡erences. Because the la-
the
research
tency prolongation associated with adaptation is much
(Thornton and Coleman, 1975 ; Don et al., 1977 ; Lasky
greater in newborns than adults, the statistical power in
et al., 1996) on adults indicates that an ITI between 200
our experimental paradigms is greater for newborns
and 500 ms is needed in order to insure that the ¢rst
than adults. This di¡erence in statistical power does
response in a train is fully unadapted. Experiments III^
not seem to explain all of the results.
e¡ect
of
ITI
on
the
responses.
Other
VI indicate that the su¤cient ITI for full recovery of
Relating the results to neural mechanisms is specu-
the ABR is at least as long in newborns. In fact, experi-
lative. Single unit research at the level of the cochlear
ment III suggests that adults return to the unadapted
nerve suggests three adaptation stages to continuously
state more rapidly than newborns for the same ITI.
presented pure tones : the ¢rst lasting a few millisec-
Lasky and Rupert (1982) reported similar results con-
onds, the second lasting tens of milliseconds (Wester-
cerning forward masking with a broadband masker.
man and Smith, 1984 ; Yates et al., 1985 ; Rhode and
Thus, the memory of the auditory system at this level
Smith, 1985), and the third lasting in the order of tens
is such that the response to a click is altered depending
of seconds (Kiang et al., 1965 ; Young and Sachs, 1973).
on whether of not there was auditory input to the sys-
After stimulation, recovery of the spontaneous ¢ring
tem nearly 500 ms prior to the click.
rate takes up to 30 s (Kiang et al., 1965 ; Young and
The contrast between the two click results from ex-
Sachs, 1973).
periment V and the return to the unadapted response
How these processes are re£ected in volume con-
following a train of clicks from experiments III, IV, VI,
ducted potentials from many levels of the auditory
and VII indicate that a train of clicks has a larger and
pathway to repetitively presented transients is unclear.
longer lasting e¡ect on a subsequent click than a single
Relkin and Doucet (1991) demonstrated that low and
click. Similar results are reported from studies employ-
high spontaneous rate (SR) type I auditory neurons
ing temporal masking paradigms (Lasky and Rupert,
have di¡erent rates of recovery to a 80 dB SPL, 100
1982 ; Lasky, 1993). Long duration forward maskers
ms conditioning tone. Speci¢cally, low SR neurons re-
prolong
duration
cover 10 times slower than the 200 ms recovery time
maskers (experiment IV, Lasky and Rupert, 1982 ; ex-
characteristic of high SR neurons. The study of Relkin
periments I^III, Lasky, 1993) and that di¡erence re-
et al. (1995) demonstrated long term recovery of the
mains as the separation between masker and signal in-
compound action potential (CAP) on a time scale char-
creases (experiment IV, Lasky and Rupert, 1982). These
acteristic of low SR neurons. This suggests that the
studies also indicate that forward masking has a greater
CAP may be used to assess low SR neurons. The results
e¡ect on newborn than adult ABR latencies. Not sur-
of their study are similar to the CAP study in cats of
prisingly, there are similarities between adaptation and
Abbas and Gorga (1981). These results indicate that
forward masking e¡ects.
adaptation
ABR
latencies
more
than
short
and
recovery
involve
several
processes,
Varying the intensity of the train did not alter sig-
probably re£ecting di¡erent mechanisms and cell types,
ni¢cantly the transition from the unadapted to the
some of which last a surprisingly long time. Further-
adapted latency function (experiment VII). Again, Las-
more, the e¡ects of these processes can be observed in
ky and Rupert (1982 ; experiment VI) reported similar
compound action potentials. Similarly, the data from
results for forward masking latency functions. Varying
this study suggest that ABRs can be used to study sig-
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
ni¢cant developments in adaptation time constants at the level of the brainstem in humans. Paradigms varying the temporal intervals between stimuli demonstrate that the auditory system is a time varying, nonlinear system with memory. The time constants de¢ning the memory of the auditory system change as a function of stimulation (e.g. the di¡erences between the responses to the 10 and 20 ms ISI trains in experiment III). Failing to address the time varying nature of the auditory system may contribute to the general lack of success of nonlinear systems identi¢cation approaches in modeling the auditory system (Eggermont, 1993; Lasky et al., 1995b). Furthermore, it suggests that time varying processes are fundamental to understanding how the auditory system processes sound. Consequently, they must be incorporated into models of the auditory system. Lasky et al. (1995b) have emphasized this point by demonstrating that temporal variations that di¡erentially a¡ect the ABR when presented separately (e.g. rate studies) fail to do so when presented one after the other (e.g. studies using stimuli with pseudorandomly varying ISIs). Those temporal variations may evoke similar responses due to the temporal inertia of the system. Some human newborn and adult auditory responses at the level of the brainstem di¡er. Volume conducted evoked responses to rate are one example. The experiments reported in this study begin to detail human developmental di¡erences in rate e¡ects at the level of the brainstem. Some auditory stimuli seem to a¡ect the newborn brainstem response to subsequent stimuli for a longer time than the adult response. Lasky and Spiro (1980) and Cowan et al. (1982) have reported that human infant behavioral responses are a¡ected by prior stimuli at inter stimulus intervals too long to a¡ect adult responses. Both studies investigated backward masking and one (Lasky and Spiro, 1980) vision. Like the present study, these studies indicate that stimuli may interact at temporally greater intervals with decreasing age of the subject. This may be a general developmental phenomenon across di¡erent sensory modalities and paradigms.
Acknowledgments This work was supported by NIH Grant #R01 NS2 0647-01A1.
References Abbas, P.J., Gorga, M.P., 1981. AP responses in forward masking paradigms and their relationship to responses of auditory nerve ¢ber. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 69, 492^499. Burkard, R., McGee, J., Walsh, E.J., 1996a. E¡ects of stimulus rate
175
on the feline brain-stem auditory evoked response during development. I. Peak latencies. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 978^990. Burkard, R., McGee, J., Walsh, E.J., 1996b. E¡ects of stimulus rate on the feline brain-stem auditory evoked response during development. I. Peak amplitudes. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 991^1002. Burkard, R., Hecox, K.E., 1983. The e¡ect of broadband noise on the human brainstem auditory evoked response. I. Rate and intensity e¡ects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 74, 1204^1213. Cone-Wesson, B.K., Hill, K.G., Liu, G.B., 1995. E¡ect of click rate on ABR latency and amplitude in developing tammar wallaby (Macropus eugenii). Abstr. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 18, 443. Cowan, N., Suomi, K., Morse, P., 1982. Echoic storage in infant perception. Child Dev. 53, 984^990. Despland, P.A., Galambos, R., 1980. The auditory brainstem response (ABR) is a useful diagnostic tool in the intensive care nursery. Pediatr. Res. 14, 154^158. Don, M., Allen, A., Starr, A., 1977. E¡ect of click rate on the latency of auditory brainstem responses in humans. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 86, 196. Donaldson, G., Rubel, E., 1990. E¡ects of stimulus repetition rate on ABR threshold, amplitude and latency in neonatal and adult Mongolian gerbil. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 77, 458^470. Eggermont, J.J., 1985. Peripheral auditory adaptation and fatigue: A model oriented review. Hear. Res. 18, 57^71. Eggermont, J.J., 1993. Wierner and Volterra analyses applied to the auditory system. Hear. Res. 66, 177^201. Eggermont, J.J., Odenthal, D.W., 1974. Electrophysiological investigation of the human cochlea: recruitment, masking and adaptation. Audiology 13, 1^22. Eysholdt, U., Schreiner, C., 1982. Maximum length sequences ^ A fast method for measuring brain-stem-evoked responses. Audiology 21, 242^250. Hall, J.W., 1992. Handbook of Auditory Evoked Responses. Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA, pp. 136^141. Jewett, D., Romano, M., 1972. Neonatal development of auditory system potentials averaged from the scalp of rat and cat. Brain Res. 36, 89^100. Kiang, N.Y.S., Watanabe, T., Thomas, C., Clark, L.F., 1965. Discharge Patterns of Single Fibers in the Cat's Auditory Nerve. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Lasky, R.E., 1984. A developmental study on the e¡ect of stimulus rate on the auditory evoked brain-stem response. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 59, 411^419. Lasky, R.E., 1991. The e¡ects of rate and forward masking on human adult and newborn auditory evoked brainstem response thresholds. Dev. Psychobiol. 24, 51^64. Lasky, R.E., 1993. The e¡ect of forward masker duration, rise/fall time and pressure on auditory evoked brainstem responses in human newborns and adults. Ear Hear. 14, 95^103. Lasky, R.E., Maier, M., Laughlin, N., Hecox, K., 1995a. Auditory evoked brainstem and middle latency responses in Macaca mulatta and humans. Hear. Res. 89, 212^225. Lasky, R.E., Maier, M., Hecox, K., 1996. Auditory evoked brain stem responses to trains of stimuli in human adults. Ear Hear. 17, 544^ 551. Lasky, R.E., Perlman, J., Hecox, K.E., 1992. Maximum length sequence auditory evoked brainstem responses in human newborns and adults. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 3, 383^389. Lasky, R.E., Rupert, A., 1982. Temporal masking of auditory evoked brainstem responses in human newborns and adults. Hear. Res. 6, 315^334. Lasky, R.E., Rupert, A., Waller, M., 1987. Reproducibility of auditory brainstem evoked responses as a function of the stimulus, scorer, and subject. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 68, 45^57. Lasky, R.E., Spiro, D., 1980. The processing of tachistoscopically presented visual stimuli by ¢ve-month-old infants. Child Dev. 51, 1292^1294.
HEARES 2861 11-11-97
R.E. Lasky / Hearing Research 111 (1997) 165^176
176
Lasky, R.E., Van Veen, B.D., Maier, M.M., Hecox, K.E., 1995b. Nonlinear
functional
modeling
of
surface
recorded
auditory
evoked responses in human adults. Abstr. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 18, 551.
Shipley, C., Buchwald, J., Norman, R., Guthrie, D., 1980. Brain stem auditory evoked response development in the kitten. Brain Res. 182, 313^326. Stapells, D.R., Picton, T.W., Durieux-Smith, A., 1994. Electrophys-
Lightfoot, G., 1991. The E¡ects of Click Repetition Rate on the Auditory Brainstem Response. Ph.D. Thesis.
iologic measures of frequency-speci¢c auditory function. In : Jacobson,
Mair, I., Elverland, H., Laukli, E., 1979. Early auditory-evoked responses in the cat : Rate e¡ects. Audiology 18, 265^278.
J.T.
(Ed.),
Principles
and
Applications
in
Auditory
Evoked Potentials. Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA, pp. 251^283. Starr, A., Amlie, R.N., Martin, W.H., Sanders, S., 1977. Development
Marmarelis, P.Z., Marmarelis, V.Z., 1978. Analysis of Physiological Systems. Plenum Press, New York.
of auditory function in newborn infants revealed by auditory brainstem potentials. Pediatrics 60, 831^839.
Relkin, E.M., Doucet, J.R., 1991. Recovery from prior stimulation. I :
Teas, D., Eldredge, D., Davis, H., 1962. Cochlear responses to acous-
Relationship to spontaneous ¢ring rates of primary auditory neu-
tic transients : An interpretation of whole nerve action potentials.
rons. Hear. Res. 55, 215^222.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 34, 1438^1459.
Relkin, E.M., Doucet, J.R., Sterns, A., 1995. Recovery of the com-
Thornton, A.R.D., Coleman, M.J., 1975. The adaptation of cochlear
pound action potential following prior stimulation : Evidence for a
and brainstem auditory evoked potentials in humans. Electroen-
slow component that re£ects recovery of low spontaneous-rate auditory neurons. Hear. Res. 83, 183^189.
ceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39, 399^406. Weatherby, L., Hecox, K., 1982. Adaptation of brainstem evoked
Rhode, W.S., Smith, P.H., 1985. Characteristics of tone-pip response patterns in relationship to spontaneous rate in cat auditory nerve ¢bers. Hear. Res. 18, 159^168.
response. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 71 (Suppl. 1), 599. Weber, B., Roush, P., 1993. Application of maximum length sequence analysis to auditory brainstem response testing of premature new-
Salamy, A., McKean, C., Pettett, G., Mendelson, T., 1978. Auditory brainstem recovery processes from birth to adulthood. Psychophysiology 15, 214^220.
borns. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 4, 157^162. Weber, B., Roush, P., 1995. Use of maximum length sequence analysis in newborn hearing testing. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 6, 187^190.
Schulman-Galambos, C., Galambos, R., 1975. Brainstem auditory evoked responses in premature infants. J. Speech Hear. 18, 456^ 465.
Westerman, L.A., Smith, R.L., 1984. Rapid and short-term adaptation in auditory nerve responses. Hear. Res. 15, 249^260. Yates, G.K., Robertson, D., Johnstone, B.M., 1985. Very rapid adap-
Shi, Y., 1990. M-pulse Sequences in the Study of Nonlinear Behavior of Auditory Evoked Potentials. Ph.D. Thesis.
tation in the guinea pig auditory nerve. Hear. Res. 17, 1^12. Young, E., Sachs, M.B., 1973. Recovery from sound exposure in
Shi, Y., Hecox, K., 1991. Nonlinear system identi¢cation by m-pulse
auditory nerve ¢bers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80, 163^176.
sequences : Application to brainstem auditory evoked responses. IEEE Biomed. 38, 834^845.
HEARES 2861 11-11-97