Reduced fronto-temporal connectivity is associated with frontal gray matter density reduction and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia

Reduced fronto-temporal connectivity is associated with frontal gray matter density reduction and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia

Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Schizophrenia Research j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l ...

606KB Sizes 0 Downloads 34 Views

Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Schizophrenia Research j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / s c h r e s

Reduced fronto-temporal connectivity is associated with frontal gray matter density reduction and neuropsychological deficit in schizophrenia Ilaria Spoletini a, Andrea Cherubini a, Margherita Di Paola a, Giulia Banfi a, Nicolas Rüsch a, Giovanni Martinotti b, Pietro Bria b, Ivo Alex Rubino c, Alberto Siracusano c, Carlo Caltagirone a,c, Gianfranco Spalletta a,c,⁎ a b c

IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Institute of Psychiatry, Rome, Italy “Tor Vergata” University, Dept. of Neuroscience, Rome, Italy

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Received 29 July 2008 Received in revised form 13 October 2008 Accepted 8 November 2008 Available online 18 December 2008 Keywords: Schizophrenia Diffusion tensor imaging Voxel-based morphometry Neuropsychology

a b s t r a c t Objectives: A “disconnectivity model” of schizophrenia has been proposed, but it is still unclear if white matter abnormalities are associated with gray matter changes and if they may be the anatomic substrate of cognitive impairment, which is a core symptom of the disorder. The first objective was to detect if white matter microstructure alterations in schizophrenia are associated with or independent of gray matter change, using an optimized method for white matter (Tract-Based Spatial Statistics) and gray matter analyses (whole-brain voxel-wise approach). The second objective was to identify the neuropsychological correlates of white matter abnormalities in the schizophrenic group, using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery. Methods: In this case-control study 43 schizophrenic patients and 43 healthy volunteers were consecutively enrolled and matched for age and gender. Results: Fractional anisotropy reduction was found in 6 fronto-temporal clusters (corrected p-values b0.05) in schizophrenic group in comparison with healthy volunteers, and 3 clusters showed fractional anisotropy increase (corrected p-values b 0.05). Two of the clusters showing reduced fractional anisotropy were associated with reduced gray matter density in neuroanatomically-related regions in schizophrenic subjects (p-values ranging from 0.001 to 0.026). Executive, constructionalpraxis, and working memory deficits were significant predictors of fractional anisotropy reduction in 4 clusters in the schizophrenic group (p-values ranging from b 0.0001 to 0.0017). Conclusions: Our data support the disconnectivity hypothesis in schizophrenia, enlightening a link between reduced fronto-temporal connectivity and “frontal” cognitive deficits. Reduced gray matter density may be involved primarily in the pathogenesis of some of these disconnected areas. © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Schizophrenia has been considered as a disconnection syndrome (Andreasen et al., 1998; Crow, 1998b; Davis et al., 2003; McGuire and Frith, 1996; Spalletta et al., 2003), with a ⁎ Corresponding author. IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Laboratory of Clinical and Behavioural Neurology, Via Ardeatina, 306. 00179 Rome, Italy. Tel.: +39 06 51501575; fax: +39 06 51501575. E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Spalletta). 0920-9964/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.011

deficit in white matter (WM) integrity leading to a defective communication among brain regions. This hypothesis has been supported by post-mortem studies showing abnormalities in the myelin sheath (Uranova et al., 2001) and oligodendroglia (Hof et al., 2003) and by evidence of a decreased expression of myelination-related genes in schizophrenia (Hakak et al., 2001; Jungerius et al., 2007; Karoutzou et al., 2008; McIntosh et al., 2007). An indicator of WM integrity is Fractional Anisotropy (FA) obtained from Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) (Assaf and

58

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Pasternak, 2008; Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996), which roughly represents the degree to which water diffusion is directionally hindered (anisotropic) (Ashtari et al., 2007; Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005; Ben Bashat et al., 2005), providing information related to fiber density, diameter, myelination, and intra-tract coherence (Le Bihan et al., 2001). Abnormal WM myelination during the development has been hypothesized to lead to aberrant cortical folding pattern, such as gyral asymmetry abnormalities, in frontal (Narr et al., 2004, 2001; Wisco et al., 2007) and temporal (Narr et al., 2004, 2001) regions in schizophrenic patients. A decrease in WM integrity in schizophrenia could be due to different mechanisms, such as a disrupted axonal myelination, or the axonal elimination resulting from neuronal death. According to the latter hypothetical mechanism, WM integrity reduction should be associated with gray matter (GM) reduction (Farrow et al., 2005) which is well-described in schizophrenia (Kreczmanski et al., 2007), with predominance in prefrontal regions (Selemon et al., 2002; Shenton et al., 2001; Suzuki et al., 2005) and in association with cognitive impairment (i.e. executive functioning, working memory and verbal learning) (Goldman-Rakic and Selemon, 1997; Rusch et al., 2007). Similarly, a lower FA has been found with a whole brain analysis in schizophrenic patients (Ardekani et al., 2003; Lim et al., 1999; Minami et al., 2003) mainly in frontal and temporal lobe regions (Davidson and Heinrichs, 2003; Davis et al., 2003) in association with clinical symptoms (Hyde et al., 1992; Shin et al., 2006) and cognitive deficits (e.g. executive function (Kubicki et al., 2003), working memory (Karlsgodt et al., 2008; Nestor et al., 2004, 2008) verbal learning and episodic memory (Lim et al., 2006; Nestor et al., 2004, 2008; Szeszko et al., 2008)). In particular, considering the “frontal” cognitive deficits, which are greatly impaired in schizophrenia (Crespo-Facorro and Barbadillo, 2007), DTI studies have found associations between reduced verbal working memory performance and lower FA in the left Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (Karlsgodt et al., 2008), and between reduced executive function and lower FA in several fibers (i.e. cingulum bundle, anterior cingulate cortex, corpus callosum, basal ganglia, parahippocampal WM) (Kubicki et al., 2003; Nestor et al., 2004, 2008). Thus, both WM and GM abnormalities appear to be prevailing in the frontal and temporal lobes and linked to specific neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Nevertheless, though WM microstructural abnormalities in schizophrenia have been described, it remains unclear if these abnormalities are associated, or on the contrary, independent of GM changes, and to what extent they are associated with cognitive impairment. This issue has been very under-explored in literature, with only one study, to our knowledge, investigating WM along with GM abnormalities in adolescents with schizophrenia (Douaud et al., 2007), revealing a widespread FA reduction neuroanatomically associated with localized reduced GM in frontal regions (such as right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right anterior cingulate cortex, left Broca's area), and in one parietal (i.e. the parietal operculum) and one temporal region (i.e. Heschl's gyrus). The Authors speculate that these changes represent the structural substrate for cognitive impairment in domains such as language and working memory. With these considerations in mind, the first aim of this study was to confirm differences in WM microstructure integrity between adult schizophrenic patients and healthy

comparison (HC) subjects, using the recent Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) method, which increases the sensitivity and the interpretability of the results compared with voxel-based approaches based purely on non-linear registration (Smith et al., 2006). On the basis of previous work (Davidson and Heinrichs, 2003; Davis et al., 2003), we expected that schizophrenic patients would demonstrate lower FA compared to HC mainly in frontal and temporal WM fibers. Secondly, using an optimised Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) protocol, we tested the patient sample in order to detect areas in which GM density values are related to WM microstructure abnormalities. In order to obtain these results, we combined two whole brain analysis methods: TBSS for WM analysis and VBM for GM analysis. Both these methods are not biased to a particular structure and provide a comprehensive assessment of anatomical differences throughout the brain (Good et al., 2001). On the basis of Douaud et al.'s (2007) study we expected fronto-temporal FA reduction to be neuroanatomically associated with reduced GM mainly localized in frontal regions. Finally, we aimed to investigate the presence of neuropsychological correlates of WM microstructure abnormalities in schizophrenic patients. Since prior DTI studies in schizophrenia have described associations between lower FA in fronto-temporal fibers and reduced working memory (Karlsgodt et al., 2008) and executive function performances (Kubicki et al., 2003; Nestor et al., 2004, 2008), we hypothesized that lower FA in frontal and temporal fiber tracts would be associated with worse executive and working memory functions. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects Sixty consecutive patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia according to DSM-IV (APA, 1994) were initially recruited from two outpatient clinics in central Italy. Diagnosis of schizophrenia was independently performed by three clinicians, who had been trained until an interrater reliability level of k ≥ 0.80 was reached. Specifically, two clinicians (G.M., I.A.R.), who treated the patients and knew their clinical history, used DSMIV criteria to make a preliminary diagnosis of schizophrenia. The two clinicians were blind to the prior diagnosis of the patient and to the aims of the study. All diagnoses were then confirmed or not on the day of image acquisition by one senior clinical psychiatrist (G.S.) using the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID-P) (First et al., 1997b). The senior psychiatrist also screened the patients according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria of the study. In the case of disagreement between the senior psychiatrist and the two clinicians who had made the preliminary diagnosis, more data were requested to help resolve the differences, and the diagnostic process continued until a final consensus diagnosis was assigned. If the three diagnosticians could not reach an agreement, the patient was removed from the sample. Severity of schizophrenia was assessed by using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Kay et al., 1987) (mean total PANSS score ± SD = 96.93 ± 20.77; mean positive symptoms score±SD=22.26±6.23; mean negative symptoms

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

score±SD=23.77±7.61; mean general psychopathology score± SD=50.91±11.70). Age at onset was defined as age at first hospitalization or, where possible, age at onset of positive or negative symptoms preceding the first hospitalization. All the patients included were between 18 and 66 years of age, were right-handed, and had completed at least 5 years of schooling. Exclusion criteria were: history of alcohol or drug dependence or traumatic head injury; any past or present major medical or neurological illness; any additional psychiatric disorder; any brain pathology identified on T2- or FLAIR-scans; and mental retardation. All patients were receiving stable oral doses of one or more atypical antipsychotic drug such as risperidone, quetiapine, and olanzapine. Antipsychotic dosages were converted to estimated equivalent dosages of olanzapine. Out of the 60 patients in the initial sample, 17 patients were excluded according to the above-mentioned exclusion criteria. Specifically, seven patients had history of alcohol and/or psychoactive drug dependence, two had traumatic head injury, one suffered from mental retardation, three met criteria for other psychotic disorders according to the diagnosis made by the psychiatrist who administered the SCID-P (one delusional disorder, two schizoaffective disorder), and four patients were removed from subsequent analyses due to significant movement artefacts during the MRI procedure. Thus, the final sample consisted of 43 schizophrenic patients. Forty-three HC subjects were recruited in the same geographic area and rigorously matched with the schizophrenic patients for age and gender (Table 1). All the HC were carefully screened for a current or past diagnosis of any axis I or II disorder using SCID-I and SCID-II (First et al., 1997a,b). Schizophrenia or any other mental disorder diagnosis among first-degree relatives was an exclusion criterion as well as the other above-mentioned exclusion criteria for patients. Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of 43 patients with schizophrenia and 43 healthy comparison subjects Schizophrenic Comparison subjects patients (N = 43) (N = 43) Characteristics

Mean ± SD

Mean ± SD

t

df

Age (years) Educational level (years) Age at the onset of the illness (years) Duration of the illness (years) Olanzapine Equivalents (mg/day)

40.23 ± 11.97 11.63 ± 3.29

41.30 ± 21.46 14.46 ± 3.14

0.285 4.091

84 0.776 84 b 0.0001

24.14 ± 8.90









16.09 ± 12.28









20.95 ± 34.44









N (%) Gender (male) DSM-IV 27 (63) subtypes: Paranoid 24 (56) schizophrenia Disorganized 3 (7) schizophrenia Undifferentiated 8 (18.5) schizophrenia Residual schizophrenia 8 (18.5)

p

N (%)

χ2

27 (63)

0.000 1

N 0.9999

































SD = standard deviation; df = degrees of freedom.

df

p

59

The study was approved and undertaken in accordance with the guidance of our local Ethics Committee and written consent was obtained from all participants after a full explanation of the procedures of the study. 2.2. Image acquisition The 86 participants underwent the same imaging protocol with a whole-brain T1-weighted and diffusion-weighted scanning using a 3 T Allegra MR imager (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard quadrature head coil. Whole-brain T1-weighted images were obtained in the sagittal plane using a modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform (MDEFT) (Deichmann et al., 2004) sequence (TE/ TR = 2.4/7.92 ms, flip angle 15°, voxel-size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). Diffusion-weighted images were obtained using echoplanar imaging (SE-EPI, TE/TR = 89/8500 ms, 52 axial slices, bandwidth = 1860 Hz/vx, voxel size 1.5 × 1.5× 3.0 mm3) with 12 isotropically distributed orientations for the diffusion-sensitising gradients at a b-value of 1000 s·mm− 2 and 2 b = 0 images. Image distortions induced by eddy currents and head motion in the DTI data were corrected by applying a full affine (mutual information cost function) alignment of each image to the mean no diffusion weighted image. After distortion corrections, DTI data were averaged and concatenated into thirteen (1 b0 + 12 b1000) volumes. We minimized movement by stabilizing the head with cushions and tape before scanning. Scans were assessed by a trained radiologist, and any scan with significant artefacts was repeated. 2.3. Image analysis 2.3.1. White matter preprocessing All FA maps were generated using DTIFit within the FMRIB Diffusion Toolbox (part of FSL) (Smith, 2004). Voxel-wise differences in DTI scalar indices were assessed using TBSS (also part of FSL), a recent approach which increases the sensitivity and the interpretability of the results compared with voxel-based approaches based purely on non-linear registration (Smith et al., 2006). Ventricular enlargement caused by the pathophysiological process may for instance considerably mislead the interpretation of the voxel-based results. TBSS aims to solve the problematic issues of standard voxel-wise methods via the use of a carefully tuned non-linear registration, followed by the projection of the nearest maximum FA values onto a skeleton derived from a mean FA image. This projection step aims to remove the effect of cross-subject spatial variability that remains after the non-linear registration. 2.3.2. Gray matter preprocessing As we aimed to investigate the correlation between FA values and GM density in schizophrenic patients across the whole brain, we used a practical and well-known methodology for the automated segmentation and registration of the brain to obtain a GM tissue map for each patient. A density map reflects the voxel values of GM segments, i.e. the local presence, or concentration, of GM. The MDEFT images were processed following an ‘optimized’ VBM protocol (Good et al., 2001) using the standard segmentation and registration tools available in the statistical parametric mapping software (SPM5, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm)

60

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

(Ashburner and Friston, 2005). To optimize brain extraction and tissue segmentation, each Magnetic Resonance Image (MRI) brain density map was positioned along the anterior– posterior commissure line and was rotated so that the septum pellucidum and at least a large part of the falx would be visible in the sagittal plane. Then the unified segmentation step in SPM5 was run. This function in SPM5 can be used for bias correction, spatial normalization (using a combination of linear and non-linear functions), and segmentation of the images in GM and WM partitions and in cerebrospinal fluid (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). Finally, all GM images were smoothed with a 12 mm3 FWHM Gaussian kernel. Statistical analysis was performed only on the GM tissue images. 2.4. Cognitive examination Two trained neuropsychologists (I.S. and G.B.), who were blind to the aims of the study, conducted the cognitive assessment, which was performed within fifteen days of MRI. Acceptable interrater reliability level for the present study was defined as of k N 0.80. Thirty-seven patients completed the entire neuropsychological battery and 6 refused. To obtain a global index of cognitive impairment, the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used (Folstein et al., 1975). Furthermore, we selected the following tests from the Mental Deterioration Battery (MDB) (Carlesimo et al., 1996) in order to provide information about the functionality of different cognitive domains such as: verbal memory (MDB Rey's 15-word Immediate Recall (RIR) and Delayed Recall (RDR)); short term visual memory (MDB Immediate Visual Memory (IVM)); logical reasoning (MDB Raven's Progressive Matrices' 47 (PM47)); language (MDB Phonological Verbal Fluency (PVF), and MDB Sentence Construction (SC)). We also added the Category Fluency (CF) test (Lucas et al., 1998) in order to assess semantic fluency ability. As “executive functioning” denotes a set of different cognitive abilities which are involved in complex, goal-directed thought and behaviour (Kerns et al., 2008), the following executive dimensions were assessed: a) attentional control; b) set-shifting; c) perceptual organization and planning; d) working memory. In order to assess abilities of attentional control and inhibition we administered the Stroop test (ST) (Stroop, 1935). The ST consists of three parts: in the ‘word reading’ task, participants were asked to read as quickly as possible words indicating colours (Italian words for ‘blue’, ‘red’, ‘green’, and ‘yellow’) that were printed in black ink on a white sheet. For the second part (‘colour naming’), participants were shown a sequence of blue, red, green, and yellow dots and asked to name the colours as quickly as possible. The third part of the test (‘interference condition’) consisted of words indicating colours but printed with a coloured ink not correspondent to the colour word (e.g., the word ‘blue’ printed in red ink). Participants were requested to name the ink colour of the word, as quickly as possible (in the example, to say “red”). In the case of a reading error the patient was stopped and ordered to go back to the previous word. Time of performance during the interference subtest was chosen as measure. Set-shifting or cognitive flexibility, that is the ability to alter a behavioural response mode in the face of changing

contingencies, was assessed using the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting test (MWCST) (Heaton et al., 1993). In this test, subjects were asked to sort cards according to a criterion (colour, number, or form) and then to shift to a new criterion following the feedback of the examiner. The number of completed categories and the number of perseverative errors were chosen as measures. We also administered the Rey–Osterrieth complex picture test. It is a measure of visual perception and constructional praxis (Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941) which also provides assessment of a number of other cognitive processes including perceptual organizational skills, executive function (i.e. planning), and problem-solving strategies (Knight and Kaplan, 2003). For these reasons, the Rey–Osterrieth complex picture test is considered to be a useful tool for the evaluation of frontal lobe function, and has been used to identify organization/planning deficits in patients with frontal lobe lesions (Pillon, 1981). Different versions of this test exist. We administered two test conditions: the copy of Rey– Osterrieth complex picture (CROP) and its delayed recall (ROPR). In the first condition (CROP), subjects were given the stimulus card, and then asked to draw the same figure. Participants were asked to copy the figure on the sheet of paper, as carefully as they could. Subjects were not allowed to erase or to change page orientation. The ROCF was scored according to the Taylor criteria, with scores ranging from 0 to 36 (Spreen and Strauss,1991). In the second condition (ROCFR), starting after 10 min in which only non-visuospatial trials must be administered, participants were asked to draw what they remembered, providing information about long-term visuospatial memory. In order to measure verbal, spatial, and visual working memory we administered the “n-back” tasks (Braver et al., 1997). The n-back test has been widely used in previous research (Oliveri et al., 2001; Owen et al., 1998; Smith and Jonides, 1997; Spalletta et al., 2008). In this test subjects are required to monitor continuously a sequence of verbal/spatial/visual stimuli (a total of twentytwo items for each task, visually presented on a screen) and to select items that appeared n-items back in any sequence (this sequence was randomly generated using locally written software installed on a Pentium® 4 IBM computer). In the verbal task, stimuli consist of a series of words; in the spatial task there is a white box which is differentially located among black boxes; finally, in the visual task, stimuli are a series of abstract pictures. The item selection was done by the participants using a keyboard with three keys, one for each stimulus. Also, the computer software automatically generated a file with results of the task with corrected–uncorrected responses. We administered the n-back subtasks (i.e. verbal/spatial/visual) at three different levels of difficulty. At the “n-1 level” subjects were required to select an item that appeared one item back in a sequence, at the “n-2 level” to select an item that appeared two items back in a sequence, and at the “n-3 level” to select an item that appeared three items back in a sequence. The number of correct responses (“accuracy score”) was generally considered as index of working memory performance. All participants were trained to obtain their maximal performance score, using the n-1 back paradigm by means of verbal and written explanations. Training consisted in having patients practice the n-1 back sequence three times before starting the experimental procedure. Accuracy scores as memory load at the n-2 level for

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68 Table 2 WM clusters showing reduced FA in the schizophrenic group (N = 43) in comparison with comparison subjects group (N = 43) N cluster

Brain gyrus

White matter fiber

Side

9 8 7 6 5 4

ITG ITG SFG IFG SFG SFG

ILF ILF SLF III UF SLF III EmC

Right Right Right Right Right Left

Coordinates MNI x

y

z

44 36 41 24 49 −21

−36 −65 19 21 28 50

−11 −10 21 −6 17 16

Cluster size 286 122 107 103 106 85

Cluster p-value corrected b 0.05. ITG: Inferior Temporal Gyrus; SFG: Superior Frontal Gyrus; IFG: Inferior Frontal Gyrus; ILF: Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus; SLF III: Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus subcomponent III; UF: Uncinate Fasciculus; EmC: Estreme Capsula.

the three subtasks were chosen as measures in the statistical analyses, as patients scored at chance for the n-3 task and at ceiling for the n-1 task.

61

sessed by using a series of stepwise multiple regression analyses, with a forward procedure and an F to enter of 4. Preselection of variables to include in the stepwise regression models was performed by using correlation analyses (Pearson's r) and Fisher's r to z transformation in order to determine the significance of correlations for continuous variables. In the stepwise multivariate models only variables with p b 0.05 in the pre-selection analyses were included. All tests were twotailed, and the level of statistical significance was defined as an alpha less than 0.05. 3. Results From the 60 patients of the initial sample, 17 schizophrenic patients were excluded following the exclusion criteria (see the Methods section). The final sample of 43 schizophrenic patients and the sample of 43 HC did not differ significantly in age and gender, whereas they did differ in educational attainment (Table 1). 3.1. White matter results

2.5. Statistical analyses Comparisons between the two groups on sociodemographic characteristics (i.e. age and educational level) were performed using t-tests. Comparison between the two groups on gender variable was performed using the chi-square test. 2.5.1. WM comparisons between schizophrenia and HC groups To achieve accurate inference including full correction for multiple comparisons over space, we used permutation-based non-parametric inference within the framework of the general linear model (Nichols and Holmes, 2002) to investigate changes in the distribution of FA between both groups (5000 permutations). Results were all considered significant for p b 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons at the whole brain voxel level. Regions of interest (ROI) for subsequent correlation analysis were obtained considering the clusters showing significant changes in FA between patients and HC. The mean FA value from these clusters was extracted from each patient, to be used in the subsequent correlation analysis with whole-brain GM. 2.5.2. Correlation between WM clusters and GM tissue density map We performed single regression analyses to test for any covariation between the mean FA value and the GM map density in schizophrenic patient group. Each regression model included all patients' GM maps density and each WM cluster that showed a significant reduction or increase in patients compared to HC in the previous statistical analysis. The statistical threshold was set to Family Wise Error (FWE) p b 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons. We looked at the whole GM map. No ROI was selected. The stereotaxic coordinates are reported in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (Cocosco et al., 1997). 2.5.3. Correlation between WM clusters and neuropsychological scores The neuropsychological predictors of FA value of each WM cluster in the schizophrenic subset of 37 patients were as-

TBSS mapping of anisotropy differences between schizophrenic patients and HC demonstrated a significant decrease of FA in the right hemisphere of patients in the Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (ILF), in the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus subcomponent III (SLF III), and in the Uncinate Fasciculus (UF); in the left hemisphere of patients the decrease of FA was confined to the Extreme Capsule (EmC) (see Table 2 for details and coordinates). We found a relative increase in FA of patients in Internal Capsule, posterior limb (ICp) bilaterally (see Table 3 for details and coordinates; Fig. 1). 3.1.1. Correlation between WM clusters and GM tissue maps We found that 2 out of 6 WM clusters of decreased FA of patients correlated positively with GM tissue map density (i.e. FA value in a given WM cluster correlated with GM density in neuroanatomically related regions) (see Table 4 for details and coordinates). In particular, the FA value in cluster 7, identified as right SLF III, correlated with GM density in the Right Middle Frontal Gyrus (BA 6), Right Pars Triangularis (BA 45) and Left Middle Frontal Gyrus (BA 6). Cluster 6, identified as right UF, correlated with Right Anterior Cingulum (BA 32) and Right Cerebellum. None of the increased FA clusters showed correlations with GM tissue maps (Fig. 2).

Table 3 WM clusters showing increased FA in schizophrenic patients (N = 43) in comparison with control subjects (N = 43) N cluster

White matter

Side

3 2 1

ICp ICp ICp

Right Left Left

Cluster p-value corrected b 0.05. ICp: Internal Capsule, posterior limb.

Coordinates MNI x

y

z

15 −12 − 26

0 −2 −13

4 0 −4

Cluster size 286 122 109

62

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Fig. 1. White matter fractional anisotropy differences in 43 schizophrenic patients compared to 43 comparison subjects. Red shows where Fractional Anisotropy (FA) is significantly lower in the schizophrenic sample compared to healthy subjects. Blue shows where FA is significantly higher in the schizophrenic sample compared to healthy subjects. The clusters obtained from the FA skeleton are inflated for display purposes, and overlaid on the average FA map obtained from all 86 participants. L: Left; R: Right; EmC: Extreme Capsule; SLF III: Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus subcomponent III; UF: Uncinate Fasciculus; ICp: Internal Capsule, posterior limb; ILF: Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

3.1.2. Cognitive predictors of WM abnormalities in schizophrenia Thirty-seven patients completed the entire neuropsychological assessment. This subset did not differ from the whole patient sample for age (subset of patients: mean ±S.D.=41.8±11.71; whole patient sample: 40.2±11.97), education (subset of patients: mean±S.D.=11.7±3.22; whole patient sample: 11.6±3.29), gender (subset of patients: male=22 (60%); whole patient sample: male=27 (63%)), illness duration (subset of patients: mean±S.D.= 16.8±12.56; whole patient sample: 16.1±12.28), and neuroleptic treatment dosages measured as equivalents of olanzapine (subset of patients: mean ± S.D. = 21.2 ± 36.97; whole patient sample: 20.9 ± 34.44). In order to clarify the relationship between neuropsychological deficit and WM abnormality in the schizophrenic subset of 37 patients, we performed a series of stepwise multiple regression analyses with each WM cluster as dependent variable. The pre-selected neuropsychological variables that in the correlation analysis were related (p b 0.05) with the dependent variable (i.e. WM cluster) were included as independent variables. Illness duration was included in all the stepwise multiple regression analyses as independent variable, in order to exclude a confounding role of this factor on WM microstructure integrity.

The analyses showed that significant predictor of cluster 4 (i.e. left EmC) FA score was CROP score. The resulting equation was significant (F = 11.577; df = 1.35; p = 0.0017) and explained 24.9% (r2) of the overall variance of cluster 4 FA score. In particular, lower CROP scores predicted the lower FA values in the left EmC. Significant predictors of cluster 6 FA values, corresponding to the right UF, were CROP scores and the number of completed categories at MWCST. The resulting equation was significant (F = 12.722; df = 2.34; p b 0.0001) and explained 42.8% (r2) of the overall variance of cluster 6 FA values. In particular, lower CROP scores and lower number of completed categories at MWCST predicted the lower FA values in the right UF. The same FA value predictors (i.e. CROP scores and the number of completed categories at MWCST) were found for cluster 9, corresponding to the right ILF. The resulting equation was significant (F = 12.514; df = 2.34; p b 0.0001) and explained 42.4% (r2) of the overall variance of cluster 9 FA score. In particular, lower CROP scores and lower number of completed categories at MWCST predicted the lower FA values in the right ILF. Significant predictors of cluster 7 (i.e. right SLF III) FA values were CROP and n-back verbal n-2 task scores. The

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68 Table 4 Regions of correlation between WM clusters and the whole GM in 43 patients with schizophrenia N cluster

White matter fiber

Cluster Right 7 SLF III

Cluster Right 6 UF

Gray matter

Coordinates MNI x

y

Cluster p value⁎ size z

Right middle frontal 28 14 62 gyrus (BA 6) Right pars 50 36 14 Triangularis (BA 45) Right pars 54 24 0 Triangularis (BA 45) Right pars 56 26 16 Triangularis (BA 45) Left middle frontal −56 −4 34 gyrus (BA 6) Right anterior 10 36 16 cingulum (BA 32) Right cerebellum 28 − 80 −38

46

0.001

21

0.008

26

0.008

36

0.012

26

0.016

17

0.005

15

0.026

⁎p value corrected Family Wise Error. SLF III: Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus subcomponent III; UF: Uncinate Fasciculus.

resulting equation was significant (F = 13.455; df = 2.34; p b 0.0001) and explained 44.2% (r2) of the overall variance of cluster 7 FA values. In particular, lower CROP scores and lower n-back verbal n-2 task scores predicted the lower FA values in the right SLF III. None of the increased FA clusters was predicted by neuropsychological performance. 4. Discussion TBSS analysis revealed that patients with schizophrenia in comparison to HC subjects are characterized by decreased WM integrity within several fiber tracts linked to frontal and/ or temporal cortices and most of them right-lateralized. Furthermore, in the schizophrenic sample, 2 out of the 6 WM clusters of decreased FA are associated with a reduced GM density in related regions. Finally, specific neuropsychological deficits are significant predictors of reduced WM microstructure integrity in the subset of 37 patients which completed the cognitive assessment. 4.1. White matter abnormalities in schizophrenia and related cognitive deficits Our findings of a reduced FA in the right ILF, the right SLF III, the right UF, and the left EmC indicate a decreased connectivity between brain regions linked to frontal and temporal circuits. These results are consistent with evidence from other DTI (Jones et al., 2006; Kanaan et al., 2005; Kubicki et al., 2005, 2002; Seal et al., 2008; Skelly et al., 2008), histological (Benes et al., 1986, 2001; Bouras et al., 2001), and functional studies (Carter et al., 2001, 1998; Cohen et al., 1998; Nordahl et al., 2001), supporting the hypothesis of an inefficient communication between frontal and temporal lobes in schizophrenia (Winterer et al., 2003). Furthermore, we found significant cognitive predictors of almost all the WM clusters showing reduced FA. Particularly, a complex constructive-praxis task (CROP) is significant predictor of all the reduced FA clusters,

63

whereas verbal working memory deficit, measured by verbal n-2 task (n-back test), is predictor of FA reduction in SLF III, and the executive ability of set-shifting, as assessed by MWCST, predicts FA values in ILF and UF. We explain these results considering the anatomical connections of these fiber tracts and their functional meaning. The EmC divides into a superior ramus that lies in the WM of the inferior frontal lobe, and an inferior ramus that lies beneath the claustrum on the floor of the orbital cortex, laterally adjacent to the fibers of the UF. Considering EmC's connections, it has been hypothesized (Crosby and Section, 1982) that the rostral prefrontal cortex exerts its control, via the EmC, on the most highly integrated cortical information, and its disconnections may be the anatomical basis for an impairment in abstract information processing (Christoff et al., 2003) and in the integration of multiple cognitive operations (Petrides, 2005; Ramnani and Owen, 2004) in schizophrenia. We found that significant predictor of EmC FA values is CROP score. Indeed, the Rey–Osterrieth Picture is an abstract and complex stimulus, whose copy requires strategic planning and organizing abilities (Pillon, 1981). Specifically, reproducing the Rey–Osterrieth Picture involves the ability to organize the figure into a meaningful perceptual unit (Pillon, 1981) and to integrate different cognitive information, a multiple cognitive process which could be underpinned by EmC's connections. Similarly, the right ILF showed a reduced FA in our schizophrenic sample compared to HC sample, consistently with several DTI studies (Buchsbaum et al., 1998; Lim et al., 1999; Minami et al., 2003; Skelly et al., 2008). The ILF links the ventral and lateral occipital regions with the ventral temporal cortices, which are important for object recognition, and the posterior parahippocampal gyrus, which is engaged in memory and discrimination tasks (Squire and Zola-Morgan, 1991; Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982). We found that significant predictors of FA value in right ILF are CROP score and number of completed MWCST categories, which tests are both composed of visual stimuli involving discrimination and object recognition abilities. Thus, a decrease in WM connectivity in this fiber tract may explain these neuropsychological deficits. TBSS analysis also revealed a reduced FA in two other fiber tracts linked to the frontal cortex and whose disconnectivity has been found to be implicated in schizophrenia: the UF and the SLF III. These are relatively long associative fibers, among the major connections to the frontal cortex (Crosby et al., 1962), whose growth and extended maturation in the brain during fetal life and childhood make them plausible targets for the substrate of a neurodevelopmental disorder (Jones et al., 2006). Particularly, the UF is a ventral limbic pathway that links the rostral superior temporal gyrus, the rostral inferior temporal gyrus, the medial temporal area (entorhinal, perirhinal, parahippocampal gyri), and the orbital, medial and prefrontal cortices. We found that significant predictors of lower FA in UF were CROP score and number of MWCST categories. Thus, UF's connections are likely to underlie the cognitive processes involved in both the above-mentioned neuropsychological tasks. As well as the UF, the SLF III is implicated in schizophrenia (Burns et al., 2003). We found that significant predictors of FA in SLF III were CROP and n-back verbal scores. These results are consistent with the anatomy of this fiber bundle, which links the rostral inferior parietal lobule, the supramarginal

64

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Fig. 2. Correlation between white matter fractional anisotropy and gray matter density in 43 schizophrenic patients. Correlations between white matter microstructure and gray matter density are shown in Panel A and B. Panel A: a decrease of FA in right UF (in blue) was significantly associated with a decrease of gray matter density in the right Anterior Cingulum and the right Cerebellum (in red). Panel B: a decrease of FA in right SLF III (in blue) was significantly associated with a decrease of gray matter density in the Right MFG, the right Pars Triangularis, and the Left MFG (in red). All the results were overlaid on the template for a better visualisation of the regions involved. L: Left; R: Right; SLF III: Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus subcomponent III; UF: Uncinate Fasciculus; MFG: Middle Frontal Gyrus. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

gyrus (an intramodal association area concerned with high order somatosensory information), the pars opercularis, and the ventral prefrontal area, which is engaged in working memory. Furthermore, the finding that verbal working memory is a significant predictor of SLF III reduced WM integrity is consistent with a recent DTI study (Karlsgodt et al., 2008) as well as with evidence of frontal parietal activation deficit during working memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia (Barch and Csernansky, 2007). In their entirety, these results suggest that abnormalities in WM microstructure integrity in the above-mentioned fiber tracts are linked to frontal cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Thus, we may speculate that the anisotropy alterations related to the functional changes in brain connectivity may be involved in the neuropathology of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. 4.1.1. Right hemisphere hypo-connectivity in schizophrenia All the reduced FA clusters, with the exception of the EmC (i.e. ILF, SLF III, and UF), are right-lateralized, seemingly in contrast with part of the existing literature (Burns et al., 2003; Kubicki et al., 2002). Nevertheless, as discussed by Mitchell and Crow (2005), the importance of the right hemisphere

in schizophrenia has been disregarded for a long time, and previous authors have argued that the right hemisphere plays a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (Cutting, 1994; David, 1994). In fact, many nuclear symptoms of schizophrenia have been found to be associated with right hemisphere dysfunction, e.g. auditory hallucinations (Jaynes, 1979; Nasrallah, 1985) or thought insertion and social withdrawal, which are linked to activation and deactivation in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Crow, 1998a, 2004a). Disorganization of speech has been understood to be a product of the interactions between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and occipito-temporo-parietal cortex in the right hemisphere (Crow, 1997, 2004b). Also emotional communication skills (i.e. prosody) and the interpretation of others' emotional state in order to direct social behaviour (affect recognition, a social cognitive ability which is impaired in schizophrenia) (Pinkham et al., 2007) are lateralized in the right hemisphere (Mitchell and Crow, 2005). Finally, from a neuropsychological point of view, alertness, a fundamental component of attention (Callejas et al., 2005; Posner and Petersen, 1990) is thought to be subserved by a mostly rightlateralized frontal, parietal, thalamic and brainstem network (Sturm and Willmes, 2001).

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Thus, our result of a mainly right-lateralized WM integrity reduction and its above-mentioned cognitive correlates provides additional evidence of a key role of the right hemisphere in schizophrenia. 4.1.2. Increased FA in Internal Capsule, posterior limb Our finding of an increased FA in the ICp bilaterally is a novel result. Despite the neuropathological counterpart of an increased FA is not fully understood, it could probably be underpinned by abnormal axonal migration causing alterations in axonal packing density. From research on autism we know that an increased WM volume in primary motor and premotor areas is linked to motor impairment and to deficits in socialization and communication (Mostofsky et al., 2007), thus we may hypothesize that an increased anisotropy in the ICp, because of its connections to those areas, could be linked to abnormal motor and/or communicative behaviours in schizophrenia. However, exploring this issue was not among the aims of this study, and further research is needed to validate this hypothesis. 4.2. Anatomically related gray and white matter abnormalities The VBM-style analysis indicates that WM in 2 out of the 6 clusters of reduced FA is positively correlated with reduced GM density in neuroanatomically-related regions. Particularly, FA values in the right UF correlated with GM density in the right anterior cingulum and the right cerebellum; and FA values in the SLF III correlated with GM density in the right pars triangularis and the middle frontal gyrus bilaterally. We explain these findings considering the anatomical connections of these structures and their functional meaning. The UF comprises the cingulum bundle, which is the most prominent fiber tract within the limbic system, and its anterior– agranular, motor-related cortex is interconnected to amygdala, nucleus accumbens, medial dorsal thalamus, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The anterior cingulate cortex has been found to be implicated in schizophrenia (Carter et al.,1998; Kiehl et al., 2000), and linked to cognitive deficits (i.e. error detection processing, decision monitoring, and attention) (Cohen et al., 1998; Nestor et al., 2007), positive symptoms (i.e. hallucinations) (Frith, 1995), and social cognitive processing (i.e. affect recognition) (Tamminga et al., 2000). Previous studies have described a reduced GM volume in the anterior cingulate gyrus in schizophrenia (Farrow et al., 2005; Jayakumar et al., 2005; Job et al., 2002; Kubicki et al., 2002; Salgado-Pineda et al., 2003). Furthermore, a disruption in the limbic system network integrity, and in particular in the cingulate gyrus, has been hypothesized to be involved in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, explaining some of the psychiatric and cognitive symptoms (Cohen et al., 1999). The cerebellum has been found to show a reduced GM in schizophrenia (Farrow et al., 2005; Jayakumar et al., 2005; Salgado-Pineda et al., 2003) and to play an important role in cognitive processes, neurological soft signs, dyscoordination, abnormal posture and proprioception, impaired eyeblink conditioning (Picard et al., 2008). Furthermore, enlarged cerebellar ventricles, including lateral and third ventricles, are associated with deficits in attention, executive, and premorbid cognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients (Crespo-Facorro and Barbadillo, 2007), consistently with the above-described neuropsychological predictors of reduced FA

65

in UF in this study. Thus, it could be hypothesized that the reduced WM integrity in the UF is underpinned by structural changes in these two cingular and cerebellar regions. More research is needed in order to substantiate this hypothesis. Similarly, the topography of GM atrophy in the middle frontal gyri bilaterally and the right pars triangularis suggests a structural substrate for the impairment in WM integrity in the right SLF III. The pars triangularis is a triangular-shaped aspect of the gyral structure in the lateral and inferior part of frontal lobe situated between pars opercularis and pars orbitalis. We previously found a reduction in GM volume in left pars triangularis in schizophrenia (Spalletta et al., 2003). The middle frontal gyrus, which is located on the lateral surface of the temporal lobe ventral to the superior temporal gyrus, has been found to show a reduced GM in schizophrenia (Jayakumar et al., 2005; Onitsuka et al., 2004), and to be involved in several cognitive processes (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000). Thus, we speculate that the reduced right SLF III WM integrity may be underpinned by structural changes in the right pars triangularis and bilateral middle frontal gyri, representing the basis for an impairment in neuropsychological abilities, such as working memory and complex constructive-praxis tasks, as previously argued. 4.3. Strengths and limitations of the study and conclusions The main strength of this study is the use of a refined VBMlike approach for GM analysis and the method of TBSS for WM analysis, performed on the same sample of schizophrenic patients. However, some limitations need to be taken into account. Firstly, results of differences in anisotropy within WM fibers are open to a number of interpretations, as the data would be consistent with either the presence of differences in number and/or architecture of the axons (which are expressed by one out of the two FA constituent parameters, i.e. Axial Diffusivity, indicating parallel diffusion), or in myelination of tracts and/or alteration in the axonal cytoskeleton (expressed by the FA perpendicular constituent parameter, i.e. Radial Diffusivity), or a combination of such processes (Beaulieu, 2002). From a very recent DTI study in schizophrenic patients (Seal et al., 2008) using TBSS, we know that FA reduction in SLF, UF and other WM tracts, is accounted for by an increase in Radial Diffusivity but not in Axial Diffusivity, thus it is likely to be underpinned by reduced axonal myelination rather than gross axonal damage. Further studies should aim to clarify the nature of the WM microstructure abnormalities in schizophrenia. Secondly, it is not possible to specify if WM reduction is caused by GM reduction or vice versa, as neither the directionality nor the pathophysiology of the associations between lower WM integrity and lower GM density is fully understood. More research is needed in order to clarify this issue. Thirdly, our sample is represented by non-first-onset schizophrenic patients, and there is evidence of a positive association between reduced WM integrity and illness duration in schizophrenia patients (Walterfang et al., 2006). However, illness duration was included in all the stepwise multiple regression analyses and was not a significant predictor of any FA clusters in this study. Fourthly, all schizophrenic patients in our study received cumulative doses of atypical antipsychotics, and an influence of chronic treatment on the results should be taken into

66

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

consideration (Bartzokis et al., 2007; McCormick et al., 2005; Scherk and Falkai, 2006). However, we performed a series of univariate correlation analyses which did not show any significant association between olanzapine equivalents and FA value for each cluster (data available upon request). Therefore, we exclude a significant confounding effect of medication on our sample results. Finally, as schizophrenic patients and HC differed for educational attainment, one may wonder if education could represent a confounding variable on the results. However, performing a series of univariate correlation analyses we failed to find any significant association between education and FA value for each cluster (data available upon request). Thus, we also exclude a confounding effect of educational attainment on our sample results. Further research is necessary to deepen all these issues. In conclusion, we demonstrate a significant reduction in WM integrity in fronto-temporal fiber tracts and their neuropsychological predictors in schizophrenic patients. The topographic distribution of the WM differences is similar to those reported previously in literature but more right-lateralized, consistently with Mitchell and Crow's hypothesis (Mitchell and Crow, 2005). Furthermore, some of these WM abnormalities are associated with reduced GM density in neuroanatomically related regions. Considered comprehensively, our data further support the disconnectivity hypothesis in schizophrenia (McGlashan and Hoffman, 2000; Stephan et al., 2006), suggesting a link between reduced fronto-temporal connectivity and cognitive deficits in typically impaired domains in schizophrenia, as well as structural GM reduction. These results help to clarify structural and connectivity disturbances and their relation with frontal cognitive symptoms in schizophrenia, leading to a more comprehensive neuroanatomical characterization of this complex disorder. Role of the funding source This research has been supported by the RC 05/A grant from the Italian Ministry of Health. Contributors Gianfranco Spalletta, MD, PhD, wrote the protocol, designed the study, took part at the data collection process and undertook the statistical analyses. Ilaria Spoletini, PhD, wrote the first draft of the manuscript and took part at the data collection process. Andrea Cherubini, PhD, and Margherita di Paola, BsC, managed the literature searches and analyses. Giulia Banfi, BsC, Nicolas Rüsch, MD, Giovanni Martinotti, MD, Ivo Alex Rubino, MD, and Alberto Siracusano, MD, took part at the data collection process. Carlo Caltagirone, MD, and Pietro Bria, MD, designed the study. All authors have contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Dr. Gianfranco Spalletta has full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Conflict of interest The authors have no conflict of interest. Acknowledgements No acknowledgements.

References Andreasen, N.C., Paradiso, S., O'Leary, D.S., 1998. “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcorticalcerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull. 24 (2), 203–218. APA, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington.

Ardekani, B.A., Nierenberg, J., Hoptman, M.J., Javitt, D.C., Lim, K.O., 2003. MRI study of white matter diffusion anisotropy in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 14 (16), 2025–2029. Ashburner, J., Friston, K.J., 2005. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26 (3), 839–851. Ashtari, M., Cervellione, K.L., Hasan, K.M., Wu, J., McIlree, C., Kester, H., Ardekani, B.A., Roofeh, D., Szeszko, P.R., Kumra, S., 2007. White matter development during late adolescence in healthy males: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 35 (2), 501–510. Assaf, Y., Pasternak, O., 2008. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: A review. J. Mol. Neurosci. 34 (1), 51–61. Barch, D.M., Csernansky, J.G., 2007. Abnormal parietal cortex activation during working memory in schizophrenia: verbal phonological coding disturbances versus domain-general executive dysfunction. Am. J. Psychiatry 164 (7), 1090–1098. Barnea-Goraly, N., Menon, V., Eckert, M., Tamm, L., Bammer, R., Karchemskiy, A., Dant, C.C., Reiss, A.L., 2005. White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb. Cortex 15 (12), 1848–1854. Bartzokis, G., Lu, P.H., Nuechterlein, K.H., Gitlin, M., Doi, C., Edwards, N., Lieu, C., Altshuler, L.L., Mintz, J., 2007. Differential effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on brain myelination in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 93 (1–3), 13–22. Basser, P.J., Pierpaoli, C., 1996. Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J. Magn. Reson., B. 111 (3), 209–219. Beaulieu, C., 2002. The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—a technical review. NMR Biomed. 15 (7–8), 435–455. Ben Bashat, D., Ben Sira, L., Graif, M., Pianka, P., Hendler, T., Cohen, Y., Assaf, Y., 2005. Normal white matter development from infancy to adulthood: comparing diffusion tensor and high b value diffusion weighted MR images. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 21 (5), 503–511. Benes, F.M., Davidson, J., Bird, E.D., 1986. Quantitative cytoarchitectural studies of the cerebral cortex of schizophrenics. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 43 (1), 31–35. Benes, F.M., Vincent, S.L., Todtenkopf, M., 2001. The density of pyramidal and nonpyramidal neurons in anterior cingulate cortex of schizophrenic and bipolar subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 50 (6), 395–406. Bouras, C., Kovari, E., Hof, P.R., Riederer, B.M., Giannakopoulos, P., 2001. Anterior cingulate cortex pathology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Neuropathol. 102 (4), 373–379. Braver, T.S., Cohen, J.D., Nystrom, L.E., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Noll, D.C., 1997. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5 (1), 49–62. Buchsbaum, M.S., Tang, C.Y., Peled, S., Gudbjartsson, H., Lu, D., Hazlett, E.A., Downhill, J., Haznedar, M., Fallon, J.H., Atlas, S.W., 1998. MRI white matter diffusion anisotropy and PET metabolic rate in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 9 (3), 425–430. Burns, J., Job, D., Bastin, M.E., Whalley, H., Macgillivray, T., Johnstone, E.C., Lawrie, S.M., 2003. Structural disconnectivity in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Psychiatry 182, 439–443. Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., 2000. Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (1), 1–47. Callejas, A., Lupianez, J., Funes, M.J., Tudela, P., 2005. Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Exp. Brain Res. 167 (1), 27–37. Carlesimo, G.A., Caltagirone, C., Gainotti, G., 1996. The mental deterioration battery: normative data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. The Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery. Eur. Neurol. 36 (6), 378–384. Carter, C.S., Perlstein, W., Ganguli, R., Brar, J., Mintun, M., Cohen, J.D., 1998. Functional hypofrontality and working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 155 (9), 1285–1287. Carter, C.S., MacDonald III, A.W., Ross, L.L., Stenger, V.A., 2001. Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 158 (9), 1423–1428. Christoff, K., Ream, J.M., Geddes, L.P., Gabrieli, J.D., 2003. Evaluating selfgenerated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behav. Neurosci. 117 (6), 1161–1168. Cocosco, C.A., Kollokian, V., Kwan, R., Evans, A.C., 1997. Brainweb: online interface to a 3D MRI simulated brain database. Neuroimage 5, S425. Cohen, R.M., Nordahl, T.E., Semple, W.E., Andreason, P., Pickar, D., 1998. Abnormalities in the distributed network of sustained attention predict neuroleptic treatment response in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 19 (1), 36–47. Cohen, R.A., Kaplan, R.F., Zuffante, P., Moser, D.J., Jenkins, M.A., Salloway, S., Wilkinson, H., 1999. Alteration of intention and self-initiated action associated with bilateral anterior cingulotomy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 11 (4), 444–453.

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68 Crespo-Facorro, B., Barbadillo, L., 2007. Neuropsychological functioning and brain structure in schizophrenia. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 19 (4), 325–336. Crosby, E.C., Section, C., 1982. White matter of hemispheres. In: Crosby, E.C., Schnitzlein, H.N. (Eds.), Comparative Correlative Neuroanatomy of the Vertebrate Telencephalon. Macmillan, New York, pp. 317–337. Crosby, E.C., Humphrey, T., Lauer, E.W., 1962. Correlative Anatomy of the Nervous System. Macmillian, New York. Crow, T.J., 1997. Schizophrenia as failure of hemispheric dominance for language. Trends Neurosci. 20 (8), 339–343. Crow, T.J., 1998a. Nuclear schizophrenic symptoms as a window on the relationship between thought and speech. Br. J. Psychiatry 173, 303–309. Crow, T.J., 1998b. Schizophrenia as a transcallosal misconnection syndrome. Schizophr. Res. 30 (2), 111–114. Crow, T.J., 2004a. Auditory hallucinations as primary disorders of syntax: an evolutionary theory of the origins of language. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 9 (1–2), 125–145. Crow, T.J., 2004b. Directional asymmetry is the key to the origin of modern homo sapiens (the Broca–Annett axiom). Response to Lesley Rogers. Laterality 9, 233–242. Cutting, J.C., 1994. Evidence for right hemisphere dysfunction in schizophrenia. In: David, A.S., Cutting, J.C. (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Psychology Press, Hove, pp. 231–244. David, A.S., 1994. The neuropsychological origin of auditory hallucinations. In: David, A.S., Cutting, J.C. (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Psychology Press, Hove, pp. 269–313. Davidson, L.L., Heinrichs, R.W., 2003. Quantification of frontal and temporal lobe brain-imaging findings in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 122 (2), 69–87. Davis, K.L., Stewart, D.G., Friedman, J.I., Buchsbaum, M., Harvey, P.D., Hof, P.R., Buxbaum, J., Haroutunian, V., 2003. White matter changes in schizophrenia: evidence for myelin-related dysfunction. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60 (5), 443–456. Deichmann, R., Schwarzbauer, C., Turner, R., 2004. Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T. Neuroimage 21 (2), 757–767. Douaud, G., Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Behrens, T., Johansen-Berg, H., Vickers, J., James, S., Voets, N., Watkins, K., Matthews, P.M., James, A., 2007. Anatomically related grey and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130 (Pt 9), 2375–2386. Farrow, T.F., Whitford, T.J., Williams, L.M., Gomes, L., Harris, A.W., 2005. Diagnosis-related regional gray matter loss over two years in first episode schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 58 (9), 713–723. First, M.B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B.W., Benjamin, L.S., 1997a. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). American Psychiatric Press, Washington DC. First, M.B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J.B., 1997b. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IVAxis I Disorders (SCID-I), Clinician Version. American Psychiatric Press, Washington DC. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R., 1975. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12 (3), 189–198. Frith, C.D., 1995. The cognitive abnormalities underlying the symptomatology and the disability of patients with schizophrenia. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 10 (Suppl 3), 87–98. Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Selemon, L.D., 1997. Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 23 (3), 437–458. Good, C.D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.S., 2001. Computational neuroanatomy: new perspectives for neuroradiology. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 157 (8–9 Pt 1), 797–806. Hakak, Y., Walker, J.R., Li, C., Wong, W.H., Davis, K.L., Buxbaum, J.D., Haroutunian, V., Fienberg, A.A., 2001. Genome-wide expression analysis reveals dysregulation of myelination-related genes in chronic schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98 (8), 4746–4751. Heaton, R.K., Chelune, G.J., Talley, J.L., Kay, G.C., Curtiss, G., 1993. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL. Hof, P.R., Haroutunian, V., Friedrich Jr., V.L., Byne, W., Buitron, C., Perl, D.P., Davis, K.L., 2003. Loss and altered spatial distribution of oligodendrocytes in the superior frontal gyrus in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 53 (12), 1075–1085. Hyde, T.M., Ziegler, J.C., Weinberger, D.R., 1992. Psychiatric disturbances in metachromatic leukodystrophy. Insights into the neurobiology of psychosis. Arch. Neurol. 49 (4), 401–406. Jayakumar, P.N., Venkatasubramanian, G., Gangadhar, B.N., Janakiramaiah, N., Keshavan, M.S., 2005. Optimized voxel-based morphometry of gray matter volume in first-episode, antipsychotic-naive schizophrenia. Prog. Neuro-psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 29 (4), 587–591. Jaynes, J., 1979. The Origins of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Houghton-Mifflin Co., Boston.

67

Job, D.E., Whalley, H.C., McConnell, S., Glabus, M., Johnstone, E.C., Lawrie, S.M., 2002. Structural gray matter differences between first-episode schizophrenics and normal controls using voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 17 (2), 880–889. Jones, D.K., Catani, M., Pierpaoli, C., Reeves, S.J., Shergill, S.S., O'Sullivan, M., Golesworthy, P., McGuire, P., Horsfield, M.A., Simmons, A., Williams, S.C., Howard, R.J., 2006. Age effects on diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging tractography measures of frontal cortex connections in schizophrenia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27 (3), 230–238. Jungerius, B.J., Hoogendoorn, M.L., Bakker, S.C., Van't Slot, R., Bardoel, A.F., Ophoff, R.A., Wijmenga, C., Kahn, R.S., Sinke, R.J., 2007. An association screen of myelin-related genes implicates the chromosome 22q11 PIK4CA gene in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 13 (11), 1060–1068. Kanaan, R.A., Kim, J.S., Kaufmann, W.E., Pearlson, G.D., Barker, G.J., McGuire, P.K., 2005. Diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 58 (12), 921–929. Karlsgodt, K.H., van Erp, T.G., Poldrack, R.A., Bearden, C.E., Nuechterlein, K.H., Cannon, T.D., 2008. Diffusion tensor imaging of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and working memory in recent-onset schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 63 (5), 512–518. Karoutzou, G., Emrich, H.M., Dietrich, D.E., 2008. The myelin-pathogenesis puzzle in schizophrenia: a literature review. Mol. Psychiatry 13 (3), 245–260. Kay, S.R., Fiszbein, A., Opler, L.A., 1987. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 13 (2), 261–276. Kerns, J.G., Nuechterlein, K.H., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., 2008. Executive functioning component mechanisms and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64 (1), 26–33. Kiehl, K.A., Liddle, P.F., Hopfinger, J.B., 2000. Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 37 (2), 216–223. Knight, J.A., Kaplan, E., 2003. The handbook of Rey–Osterrieth complex figure usage: clinical and research applications. Psychological Assessment Resources. Kreczmanski, P., Heinsen, H., Mantua, V., Woltersdorf, F., Masson, T., Ulfig, N., Schmidt-Kastner, R., Korr, H., Steinbusch, H.W., Hof, P.R., Schmitz, C., 2007. Volume, neuron density and total neuron number in five subcortical regions in schizophrenia. Brain 130 (Pt 3), 678–692. Kubicki, M., Westin, C.F., Maier, S.E., Mamata, H., Frumin, M., ErsnerHershfield, H., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A., McCarley, R., Shenton, M.E., 2002. Diffusion tensor imaging and its application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 10 (6), 324–336. Kubicki, M., Westin, C.F., Nestor, P.G., Wible, C.G., Frumin, M., Maier, S.E., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2003. Cingulate fasciculus integrity disruption in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 54 (11), 1171–1180. Kubicki, M., Park, H., Westin, C.F., Nestor, P.G., Mulkern, R.V., Maier, S.E., Niznikiewicz, M., Connor, E.E., Levitt, J.J., Frumin, M., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2005. DTI and MTR abnormalities in schizophrenia: analysis of white matter integrity. Neuroimage 26 (4), 1109–1118. Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.F., Poupon, C., Clark, C.A., Pappata, S., Molko, N., Chabriat, H., 2001. Diffusion tensor imaging: concepts and applications. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 13 (4), 534–546. Lim, K.O., Hedehus, M., Moseley, M., de Crespigny, A., Sullivan, E.V., Pfefferbaum, A., 1999. Compromised white matter tract integrity in schizophrenia inferred from diffusion tensor imaging. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56 (4), 367–374. Lim, K.O., Ardekani, B.A., Nierenberg, J., Butler, P.D., Javitt, D.C., Hoptman, M.J., 2006. Voxelwise correlational analyses of white matter integrity in multiple cognitive domains in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 163 (11), 2008–2010. Lucas, J.A., Ivnik, R.J., Smith, G.E., Bohac, D.L., Tangalos, E.G., Graff-Radford, N.R., Petersen, R.C., 1998. Mayo's older Americans normative studies: category fluency norms. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 20 (2), 194–200. McCormick, L., Decker, L., Nopoulos, P., Ho, B.C., Andreasen, N., 2005. Effects of atypical and typical neuroleptics on anterior cingulate volume in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 80 (1), 73–84. McGlashan, T.H., Hoffman, R.E., 2000. Schizophrenia as a disorder of developmentally reduced synaptic connectivity. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57 (7), 637–648. McGuire, P.K., Frith, C.D., 1996. Disordered functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 26 (4), 663–667. McIntosh, A.M., Baig, B.J., Hall, J., Job, D., Whalley, H.C., Lymer, G.K., Moorhead, T.W., Owens, D.G., Miller, P., Porteous, D., Lawrie, S.M., Johnstone, E.C., 2007. Relationship of catechol-O-methyltransferase variants to brain structure and function in a population at high risk of psychosis. Biol. Psychiatry 61 (10), 1127–1134. Minami, T., Nobuhara, K., Okugawa, G., Takase, K., Yoshida, T., Sawada, S., HaKawa, S., Ikeda, K., Kinoshita, T., 2003. Diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging of disruption of regional white matter in schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 47 (3), 141–145.

68

I. Spoletini et al. / Schizophrenia Research 108 (2009) 57–68

Mitchell, R.L., Crow, T.J., 2005. Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere? Brain 128 (Pt 5), 963–978. Mostofsky, S.H., Burgess, M.P., Gidley Larson, J.C., 2007. Increased motor cortex white matter volume predicts motor impairment in autism. Brain 130 (Pt 8), 2117–2122. Narr, K.L., Thompson, P., Sharma, T., Moussai, J., Zoumalan, C., Rayman, J., Toga, A., 2001. Three-dimensional mapping of gyral shape and cortical surface asymmetries in schizophrenia: gender effects. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 244–255. Narr, K.L., Bilder, R.M., Kim, S., Thompson, P.M., Szeszko, P., Robinson, D., Luders, E., Toga, A.W., 2004. Abnormal gyral complexity in first-episode schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 859–867. Nasrallah, H.A., 1985. The unintegrated right cerebral hemispheric consciousness as alien intruder: a possible mechanism for Schneiderian delusions in schizophrenia. Compr. Psychiatry 26 (3), 273–282. Nestor, P.G., Kubicki, M., Gurrera, R.J., Niznikiewicz, M., Frumin, M., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2004. Neuropsychological correlates of diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 18 (4), 629–637. Nestor, P.G., Kubicki, M., Spencer, K.M., Niznikiewicz, M., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2007. Attentional networks and cingulum bundle in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 90 (1–3), 308–315. Nestor, P.G., Kubicki, M., Niznikiewicz, M., Gurrera, R.J., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2008. Neuropsychological disturbance in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuropsychology 22 (2), 246–254. Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15 (1), 1–25. Nordahl, T.E., Carter, C.S., Salo, R.E., Kraft, L., Baldo, J., Salamat, S., Robertson, L., Kusubov, N., 2001. Anterior cingulate metabolism correlates with stroop errors in paranoid schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 25 (1), 139–148. Oliveri, M., Turriziani, P., Carlesimo, G.A., Koch, G., Tomaiuolo, F., Panella, M., Caltagirone, C., 2001. Parieto-frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 11 (7), 606–618. Onitsuka, T., Shenton, M.E., Salisbury, D.F., Dickey, C.C., Kasai, K., Toner, S.K., Frumin, M., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A., McCarley, R.W., 2004. Middle and inferior temporal gyrus gray matter volume abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia: an MRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 161 (9), 1603–1611. Osterrieth, P.A., 1944. Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Arch. Psychol. 30, 206–356. Owen, A.M., Stern, C.E., Look, R.B., Tracey, I., Rosen, B.R., Petrides, M., 1998. Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95 (13), 7721–7726. Petrides, M., 2005. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360 (1456), 781–795. Picard, H., Amado, I., Mouchet-Mages, S., Olie, J.P., Krebs, M.O., 2008. The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: an update of clinical, cognitive, and functional evidences. Schizophr. Bull. 34 (1), 155–172. Pillon, B., 1981. [Visuo-constructive problems and methods of compensation. Results for 85 patients with brain lesions]. Neuropsychologia 19, 375–383. Pinkham, A.E., Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., 2007. Affect recognition deficits in schizophrenia: neural substrates and psychopharmacological implications. Exp. Rev. Neurother. 7 (7), 807–816. Posner, M.I., Petersen, S.E., 1990. The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42. Ramnani, N., Owen, A.M., 2004. Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 (3), 184–194. Rey, A., 1941. L'examen psychologie dan les cas d'encéphalopathie traumatique (Les problèmes). Arch. Psychol. 28, 286–340. Rusch, N., Spoletini, I., Wilke, M., Bria, P., Di Paola, M., Di Iulio, F., Martinotti, G., Caltagirone, C., Spalletta, G., 2007. Prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar gray matter networks and executive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 93 (1–3), 79–89. Salgado-Pineda, P., Baeza, I., Perez-Gomez, M., Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Bargallo, N., Bernardo, M., 2003. Sustained attention impairment correlates to gray matter decreases in first episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage 19 (2 Pt 1), 365–375. Scherk, H., Falkai, P., 2006. Effects of antipsychotics on brain structure. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19 (2), 145–150.

Seal, M.L., Yücel, M., Fornito, A., Wood, S.J., Harrison, B.J., Walterfang, M., Pell, G.S., Pantelis, C., 2008. Abnormal white matter microstructure in schizophrenia: a voxelwise analysis of axial and radial diffusivity. Schizophr. Res. 101 (1–3), 106–110. Selemon, L.D., Kleinman, J.E., Herman, M.M., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 2002. Smaller frontal gray matter volume in postmortem schizophrenic brains. Am. J. Psychiatry 159 (12), 1983–1991. Shenton, M.E., Dickey, C.C., Frumin, M., McCarley, R.W., 2001. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 49 (1–2), 1–52. Shin, Y.W., Kwon, J.S., Ha, T.H., Park, H.J., Kim, D.J., Hong, S.B., Moon, W.J., Lee, J.M., Kim, I.Y., Kim, S.I., Chung, E.C., 2006. Increased water diffusivity in the frontal and temporal cortices of schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage 30 (4), 1285–1291. Skelly, L.R., Calhoun, V., Meda, S.A., Kim, J., Mathalon, D.H., Pearlson, G.D., 2008. Diffusion tensor imaging in schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms. Schizophr. Res. 98 (1–3), 157–162. Smith, S.M., 2004. Overview of fMRI analysis. Br. J. Radiol. 77, S167–175 Spec No 2. Smith, E.E., Jonides, J., 1997. Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cogn. Psychol. 33 (1), 5–42. Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T.E., Mackay, C.E., Watkins, K.E., Ciccarelli, O., Cader, M.Z., Matthews, P.M., Behrens, T.E., 2006. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31 (4), 1487–1505. Spalletta, G., Tomaiuolo, F., Marino, V., Bonaviri, G., Trequattrini, A., Caltagirone, C., 2003. Chronic schizophrenia as a brain misconnection syndrome: a white matter voxel-based morphometry study. Schizophr. Res. 64 (1), 15–23. Spalletta, G., Tomaiuolo, F., Di Paola, M., Trequattrini, A., Bria, P., Macaluso, E., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Caltagirone, C., 2008. The neuroanatomy of verbal working memory in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometry study. Clin. Schizophr. Relat. Psych. 2 (1), 79–87. Spreen, O., Strauss, E., 1991. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Oxford University Press, New York. Squire, L.R., Zola-Morgan, S., 1991. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. 253 (5026), 1380–1386. Stephan, K.E., Baldeweg, T., Friston, K.J., 2006. Synaptic plasticity and dysconnection in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 59 (10), 929–939. Stroop, J.R., 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. Sturm, W., Willmes, K., 2001. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 14 (1 Pt 2), S76–84. Suzuki, M., Zhou, S.Y., Takahashi, T., Hagino, H., Kawasaki, Y., Niu, L., Matsui, M., Seto, H., Kurachi, M., 2005. Differential contributions of prefrontal and temporolimbic pathology to mechanisms of psychosis. Brain 128 (Pt 9), 2109–2122. Szeszko, P.R., Robinson, D.G., Ashtari, M., Vogel, J., Betensky, J., Sevy, S., Ardekani, B.A., Lencz, T., Malhotra, A.K., McCormack, J., Miller, R., Lim, K.O., Gunduz-Bruce, H., Kane, J.M., Bilder, R.M., 2008. Clinical and neuropsychological correlates of white matter abnormalities in recent onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33 (5), 976–984. Tamminga, C.A., Vogel, M., Gao, X., Lahti, A.C., Holcomb, H.H., 2000. The limbic cortex in schizophrenia: focus on the anterior cingulate. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 31 (2–3), 364–370. Ungerleider, L.G., Mishkin, M., 1982. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle, D.J., Goodale, M.A., Mansfield, R.W.J. (Eds.), Analysis of Visual Behavior. MIT Press, pp. 549–586. Uranova, N., Orlovskaya, D., Vikhreva, O., Zimina, I., Kolomeets, N., Vostrikov, V., Rachmanova, V., 2001. Electron microscopy of oligodendroglia in severe mental illness. Brain Res. Bull. 55 (5), 597–610. Walterfang, M., Wood, S.J., Velakoulis, D., Pantelis, C., 2006. Neuropathological, neurogenetic and neuroimaging evidence for white matter pathology in schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30 (7), 918–948. Winterer, G., Coppola, R., Egan, M.F., Goldberg, T.E., Weinberger, D.R., 2003. Functional and effective frontotemporal connectivity and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 54 (11), 1181–1192. Wisco, J.J., Kuperberg, G., Manoach, D., Quinn, B.T., Busa, E., Fischl, B., Heckers, S., Sorensen, A.G., 2007. Abnormal cortical folding patterns within Broca's area in schizophrenia: evidence from structural MRI. Schizophr. Res. 94 (1–3), 317–327.