Accepted Manuscript Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea Joo Young Hong, Jin Yong Jeon PII:
S0360-1323(17)30479-1
DOI:
10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.10.021
Reference:
BAE 5134
To appear in:
Building and Environment
Received Date: 1 August 2017 Revised Date:
29 September 2017
Accepted Date: 16 October 2017
Please cite this article as: Hong JY, Jeon JY, Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea, Building and Environment (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.10.021. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology
2
in a multifunctional urban area: a case study in Seoul, Korea
3
RI PT
4 Joo Young Hong a, b and Jin Yong Jeon a
6
a
Department of Architectural Engineering, Hanyang University, Seoul 04763, Korea
7
b
School of Electrical & Electronic Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, 639798,
8
Singapore
SC
5
M AN U
9 10
Running title: Spatiotemporal variability of soundscapes
11
Send correspondence to: Jin Yong Jeon (
[email protected])
Architectural Acoustics Lab (Room 605-1)
13
Department of Architectural Engineering
14
Hanyang University
15
17 Haengdang-dong, Seongdong-gu
16
Seoul 04763, Korea
19 20 21
EP
18
Phone: +82 2 2220 1795 Fax: +82 2 2220 4794
AC C
17
TE D
12
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 22
Abstract This study presents a new perspective on interrelationships among spatiotemporal patterns
24
in soundscape, acoustic and urban morphological indicators. Spatiotemporal characteristics of
25
soundscapes in a multifunctional urban area of Seoul, Korea were analyzed. Physical acoustic
26
and perceived soundscape data were collected during three different daytime sampling
27
periods. Urban morphological indicators representing building, road, open public space, and
28
water feature components, were analyzed to quantify urban textures. Spatial and temporal
29
factors were found to play critical roles in urban soundscapes. Regarding spatial patterns in
30
soundscapes, urban soundscapes were characterized based on the main function of spaces.
31
The temporal variability of soundscapes in urban spaces depended on diurnal patterns in
32
perceived sound sources. Particularly, the perception of birdsong and sounds from human
33
activities showed considerable variation across urban spaces. In addition, significant
34
correlations were found among the spatiotemporal patterns in soundscape, acoustic and
35
morphological factors. Based on the acoustic and morphological indicators, pleasantness and
36
eventfulness models were proposed and the results indicated that the pleasantness model
37
explained 50% of the variance, while the eventfulness model only predicted 13% of the
38
variance. The results from the present study showed that urban morphological factors could
39
be useful indicators for better understanding soundscapes in urban environments.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
40
RI PT
23
41
Keywords: Soundscape, spatiotemporal variability, perceptual evaluation, urban acoustic
42
environment, soundscape map, urban morphology
43 44 45 2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 46
1. Introduction The acoustic environment is considered an important factor in creating sustainable and
48
healthy cities [1,2] because urban noise generated from transportation or industrial facilities
49
can adversely impact human health [3–5]. However, many studies reported that reducing
50
physical sound pressure levels (SPLs) does not guarantee desirable acoustic quality in urban
51
environments, since SPLs cannot differentiate the type of sound sources that have positive or
52
negative values [6]. As an alternative approach to managing sound environments,
53
soundscapes, which focus on human perception of acoustic environments, have attracted
54
attention [2,6,7].
M AN U
SC
RI PT
47
A soundscape is defined as the “acoustic environment as perceived or experienced and/or
56
understood by a person or people, in context [8]”. Therefore, soundscape studies investigate
57
perceptual descriptors of environmental sounds, so called soundscape descriptors, and related
58
physical environmental factors in certain contexts of place. Soundscape indicators are
59
measures used to predict the value of soundscape descriptors.
TE D
55
In soundscape studies, investigating the relationship between soundscape descriptors and
61
indicators is important [9,10]. Soundscape indicators are required to support not only acoustic
62
appraisal but also context appraisal [11]. In previous studies, diverse acoustic parameters
63
were used as soundscape indicators for acoustic appraisal [6]. A number of soundscape
64
studies have been conducted in various outdoor public spaces including parks [12–14],
65
squares [10,15,16], and streets [17–19]. In each urban setting, the perceived acoustic
66
environment has been assessed based on soundscape descriptors such as acoustic comfort,
67
tranquility, restoration, and appropriateness, and the relationships between the soundscape
68
descriptors and the acoustic indicators representing strength and the spectral and temporal
69
characteristics of the acoustic environment have then been explored in the previous studies.
AC C
EP
60
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT In particular, combinations of conventional noise indicators based on SPLs and
71
psychoacoustic parameters based on critical band have been applied to describe the
72
characteristics of soundscapes [20,12,21]. Psychoacoustic parameters (e.g., loudness,
73
sharpness, roughness, and fluctuation strength) covering several dimensions of basic auditory
74
sensation [22] can provide more accurate information on the relationship between the
75
acoustic environment (physical phenomenon) and the soundscape (perceptual construct) than
76
conventional SPL based noise indicators [23].
RI PT
70
According to ISO 12913-1, context, which includes all other non-acoustic factors of a place,
78
plays an important role in the perception of soundscape [8]. The contexts can be categorized
79
into the following four clusters: person, place, person-place interaction, and activity [24].
80
Hong and Jeon [25] showed that functions of places and activities might influence perceptual
81
construction of soundscapes in urban environments. The results indicate that studies focusing
82
on non-acoustic indicators are important to more accurately predict soundscape descriptors.
M AN U
SC
77
Urban morphology affects environmental factors in urban areas [26–31]. In addition, urban
84
morphology is related to urban activities [32–34]. In this context, relationships between urban
85
morphology and acoustic environment have been explored in previous studies [35–47]. In
86
those studies, various morphological parameters for buildings, roads, and green areas (e.g.,
87
height, width, distance, density, length, coverage ratio, façade and configuration) have been
88
applied to characterize the urban form. Some studies have investigated the effect of urban
89
morphology in urban areas on the spatial distribution of traffic sound levels [35–39], aircraft
90
[40], wind turbines [41] and birdsong [42] because sound propagation in urban built
91
environments is significantly influenced by urban morphology. However, those studies were
92
limited mainly to noise sources and their physical sound levels, which may not directly relate
93
to soundscape perception in urban areas. Relatively few studies focused on the relationships
AC C
EP
TE D
83
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT between perceived acoustic environment and urban morphologies [43–46]. Ge et al. [44]
95
collected objective and subjective data on acoustic environment in a case study area in Saga,
96
Japan and found significant correlations among soundscape descriptors, SPLs and the
97
morphological parameters related to buildings, land use, open space, water and green space;
98
however, in that study, temporal variation in soundscape components was not considered. Liu
99
et al. [43,45] and Mazaris et al. [46] conducted case studies to observe the spatiotemporal
100
patterns of perceived sound sources. They examined the relationships between landscape
101
indicators (e.g., density, green, and configuration of landscape) and temporal patterns in
102
sound sources concluding that spatiotemporal patterns in sound sources are closely associated
103
with landscape indices. However, their study focused on the dominance of various sound
104
sources, but not perceived soundscape descriptors such as pleasantness or tranquility.
105
Therefore, it is necessary to provide comprehensive views on the interrelationships among
106
spatiotemporal variations of soundscape components, acoustic indicators and urban
107
morphology.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
94
The aims of the present study are to explore the spatiotemporal patterns of soundscapes in a
109
multifunctional urban area, to investigate interrelationships among soundscape descriptors,
110
physical acoustic environment, and urban morphology, and to develop soundscape prediction
111
model based on the interrelationships. For these purposes, physical acoustic and subjective
112
data on soundscapes were measured in an urban area. Subsequently, various morphological
113
indicators were analyzed using a geographic information system (GIS) and the relationships
114
with the measured soundscape data were explored.
AC C
EP
108
115 116
2. Methods
117
2.1 Case study area 5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT As shown in Fig. 1(a), the study was conducted in the north part of Seoul (Joong-gu and
119
Jongro-gu areas, 26,550 m2). The case study area was chosen to include various urban
120
settings with considerable variation in urban morphological characteristics. The case study
121
areas were classified into the following five groups regarding main function of urban spaces
122
and activities similar to a previous study [25]: high-density commercial area (e.g., Myeong-
123
dong street), low-density commercial area (e.g., Insa-dong street), residential areas (e.g.,
124
Sajik-dong), the central business district (CBD) consisting mainly of office buildings (e.g.,
125
Euljiro) and an urban recreation area including urban parks (e.g., Deoksugung park) and city
126
streams (e.g., Cheonggyecheon).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
118
Soundscape data were collected from 122 locations over the study area. To analyze the
128
morphological indicators, the case study area was divided based on the sampling locations as
129
shown in Fig. 1(b). The grids in the present study were modified to better reflect the
130
morphological characteristics from the grids used in a previous study [48]. The different grid
131
sizes were applied in the study area to reflect the spatial characteristics of the places; the main
132
grid size was 150 m × 150 m (110 meshes), while a 150 m × 75 m grid size was applied for
133
the city stream areas (e.g., Cheonggyecheon), which has long and narrow spaces (12 meshes).
134
In each grid, one sampling location, which was considered to represent the overall
135
soundscape quality of the grid, was chosen for collection of soundscape data.
137 138 139
EP
AC C
136
TE D
127
Figure 1
2.2 Data collection
140
Soundscape data were collected using a questionnaire as shown in Appendix A. The
141
dominance of the perceived sound sources were assessed using the question, “To what extent 6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT do you presently hear the following types of sounds?” and the following five-point scale: 1 =
143
not heard at all, 2 = heard a little, 3 = heard moderately, 4 = heard a lot and 5 = sound
144
dominates completely. Based on previous studies [25,45], the types of sound sources were
145
classified into five categories: traffic noise, sounds from human activities, water sounds,
146
birdsong and music, and other noises such as construction noise or mechanical noise. The
147
perceived affective quality was also assessed using the following eight adjectives: pleasant,
148
unpleasant, eventful, uneventful, exciting, monotonous, calm, and chaotic [9,49]. At each
149
location, the observers evaluated the degree to which an adjective attribute applied to their
150
perception of the soundscape quality with the question, “To what extent do you agree with the
151
eight attributes below on how you experience the present surrounding sound environment?”
152
and using a five-point scale with the following response alternatives: 1 = strongly disagree, 2
153
= slightly disagree, 3 = neither disagree nor agree, 4 = slightly agree, and 5 = strongly agree.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
142
Soundscape data over the study area were collected by a group of eight observers (6 males,
155
2 females, Mage = 25.6 years, SDage = 2.0 years) applying the same method used in a previous
156
study [48]. To obtain a homogenous group of observers, undergraduate or graduate students
157
from the department of architectural engineering were recruited as the observers. Using the
158
same soundscape data-collecting protocol, pilot training sessions in data collection
159
procedures were conducted for the observers under laboratory conditions to reduce
160
observation bias. Similar to a previous study [45], the observers practiced soundscape
161
evaluation methods using urban sound excerpts (1-min) with corresponding visual images.
162
Inter-rater variability and intraclass reliability were calculated based on the training results to
163
quantify the observation bias. Cronbach’s alpha values and the average measure of intraclass
164
correlation were greater than 0.9, indicating good reliability of agreement among observers’
165
judgments [50].
AC C
EP
TE D
154
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The observers assessed the soundscape during three different periods (period 1: 09:00–11:00,
167
period 2: 13:00–15:00, and period 3: 18:00–20:00) at each location. As the case study area is
168
too large to enable the collection of the soundscape data by the observers in a day, the study
169
area was divided into eight zones (A to I) as shown in Fig. 1(a). Each zone included
170
approximately 11–16 measurement locations. The observers used bicycles to move to each
171
measurement point within a zone in order to reduce the moving time. In each zone, the eight
172
observers recorded soundscape data across the three sampling periods within a day. Thus, a
173
total of 8 days were required to perform the soundscape measurements during clear weather
174
on weekdays in May. Weekends were not considered for the data collection in order to
175
minimize the errors across the measurement days because the soundscape on weekdays might
176
be significantly different from that on the weekend. Within each sampling period, both
177
perceptual soundscape data and a 5-min recording of the acoustic environment were
178
simultaneously acquired at each evaluation location using a binaural microphone (Type 4101,
179
B&K, Denmark) and a digital recorder (Zoom, H4n, Japan).
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
166
In total, 2928 responses (122 points × 8 observers × 3 sampling periods) were obtained from
181
each observer for eight measurement days. The responses from the eight observers were
182
averaged at each measurement point in order to obtain representative values at each point.
183
Thus, 366 datasets (122 points × 3 sampling periods) were used for performing the data
184
analyses and mappings in this study.
186
AC C
185
EP
180
2.3 Acoustic indicators
187
The physical acoustic indicators are summarized by three aspects: sound strength, the
188
spectral content, and the temporal structure of sounds [51]. The acoustic recording samples at
189
each grid were used to calculate SPLs and psychoacoustic parameters. A-weighted SPL (LAeq)
190
was calculated to represent sound strength of the acoustic environment. Regarding the 8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 191
spectral contents of acoustic environment, LCeq-Aeq, representing energy at low frequencies,
192
was calculated [25,49]. The differences between the 10 and 90 percentile levels were
193
calculated (L10-90) to quantify the temporal variability of the sound environment.
RI PT
194 In previous studies, Zwicker’s loudness and sharpness were shown to be useful indicators
196
for understanding soundscapes in urban environments among the psychoacoustic parameters.
197
Loudness represents the magnitude of sound based on an auditory sensation, and sharpness is
198
the sensation value of the amount of high-frequency content in the sound, which can be used
199
as a quantitative indicator for the spectral envelope [22]. Thus, among the psychoacoustic
200
parameters, the loudness and sharpness of the recording were analyzed using a B&K PULSE
201
Sound Quality software (Type 7698, B&K, Denmark) based on 1-min audio recordings; the
202
Zwicker’s loudness was calculated according to DIN 45631/ A1 (2010) [52] and the
203
sharpness was calculated according to DIN 45692, (2009) [53]
M AN U
TE D
204
SC
195
2.4 Urban morphological indices
206
Based on previous studies [36,42], various urban morphological parameters were used to
207
quantify the 2D and 3D characteristics of urban morphologies in the case study area. Table 1
208
presents descriptions and formulas of 16 morphological indices employed in this study. The
209
urban morphological indicators were classified into four groups in terms of variables related
210
to the following: 1) buildings, 2) green and open public areas (sum of green, square and city
211
stream areas), 3) exposed ground and road surfaces, and 4) water features. The 2D
212
characteristics of urban morphologies were quantified based on area and perimeter of the
213
features at the ground level. Ratios of the total surface areas of buildings to plan areas were
214
used to measure the 3D characteristics of building morphologies. The geographical
AC C
EP
205
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 215
information on area, perimeter and height of buildings in the study area were constructed
216
using GIS software, ArcGIS ver. 10.1 (ESRI, USA) and the urban morphology indices were
217
calculated in each grid.
218
RI PT
219
Table 1 2.5 Mapping process
The soundscape data collected in the study area during the three sampling periods were
221
visualized using GIS software, ArcGIS ver. 10.1 (ESRI, USA). The inverse distance
222
weighted (IDW) surface interpolation method was applied to create the sound source and the
223
perceived affective quality maps. IDW interpolation is based on the assumption that each
224
measured point has more local weight on the predicted values and the weights diminish as a
225
function of distance from the measurement point. In previous studies, IDW has been applied
226
because it can emphasize the locality of foreground sounds [46,54]. The collected soundscape
227
data obtained from eight observers were averaged at each measured location across the three
228
sampling periods for the IDW interpolation. The power value in the weight function was set
229
as “2,” and the standard search neighborhood defined by the ellipse parameters such as angle
230
and the major and minor semi-axis was used to create the interpolation surfaces.
M AN U
TE D
EP
231
SC
220
3. Results and discussion
233
3.1 Spatiotemporal patterns of soundscape
234
3.1.1 Perceived sound sources
AC C
232
235
Spatial distribution of the perceived sound sources including traffic, human, bird and water
236
sounds at three different sampling periods are shown in Fig. 2. In terms of spatial distribution
237
of sound sources, spatial patterns of the dominated sound source types differed across the
238
study area [48]. Overall, traffic sounds dominated near high-traffic roads. Sounds from 10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT human activities and music were primarily observed in urban parks or commercial areas. In
240
terms of natural sounds, birdsong was frequently observed in urban green areas and water
241
sounds were mainly heard near the water features such as fountains or city streams. The
242
temporal distribution of different types of sound sources also varied across the study area.
243
Small temporal variations were observed for traffic and water sounds because the sound
244
sources, traffic roads and water features are usually situated in fixed locations. In contrast, the
245
temporal distributions of sounds related to human activities and birdsong significantly varied
246
in the different sampling periods because the behavioral patterns of people and birds are
247
affected by time of day [43,45].
M AN U
SC
RI PT
239
248 249
Figure 2
250
The percentage of responses recorded as “heard a lot” or “dominates completely” for a
252
given source type were calculated to explore dominant sound sources for the three different
253
sampling periods and four categories of urban functions as shown in Fig. 3. The dominant
254
sound source varied based on the main functions of spaces and sampling period. Traffic
255
sounds were most and least dominant in business districts and recreational areas, respectively.
256
As plotted in Fig. 3a, the temporal variation in traffic sounds was relatively smaller than other
257
sounds implying that traffic noise was a typical background sound over the case study area.
258
Human sounds showed significantly different temporal patterns based on different urban
259
functional areas, as shown in Fig. 3b. In business districts, the dominance of human sounds
260
gradually increased from the morning to evening hours. Interestingly, in commercial districts,
261
different temporal tendencies in the dominance of human sound were observed in high- and
262
low-density commercial areas. In the commercial district with a high density of buildings, the
AC C
EP
TE D
251
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT prominence of human sounds dramatically increased from morning and peaked at 65.4% in
264
sampling period 3, indicating that people usually go to the high-density commercial district
265
(Myeong-dong shopping district), Seoul’s premier shopping destination, to enjoy their urban
266
lives after working hours, which may increase the sounds from human activities. In contrast,
267
the low-density commercial area (Insa-dong street), consisting of stores specializing in a wide
268
variety of goods representing Korean traditional culture and crafts, exhibited a sudden
269
decrease in the dominance of human sounds in sampling period 3. This result indicates that
270
people mainly visited this area during the day rather than evening or nighttime. In
271
recreational areas, dominance of the human sounds in urban recreational areas gradually
272
decreased from sampling period 1 to period 3. This result indicates that the frequency of
273
visitors in the recreational areas in the study area were concentrated during daytime. For
274
residential areas, human sounds were frequently heard during the commuting hours in the
275
morning (period 1) and evening (period 3). Temporal patterns for music according to main
276
space function were similar to the patterns of human sounds (Fig. 3c). These findings implied
277
that population flow and temporal visitation patterns differed by time of day depending on the
278
socio-spatial characteristics of urban space.
279
Bird and water sounds were most dominant in urban recreational areas as shown in Figs. 3d
280
and 3e, respectively. The dominance of bird sounds significantly declined in period 3 by
281
10.0%, whereas water sounds slightly increased by 23.3%. The observers could better
282
perceive water sounds in the evening (period 3) due to decreased human activities in urban
283
stream areas.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
263
284 285
Figure 3
286 12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 287 288
3.1.2 Perceived affective quality Principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted based on the responses for eight
290
semantic attributes to extract the perceived affective quality of soundscape using varimax
291
rotation method. Two components with eigenvalues larger than 1 were obtained. Components
292
1 (eventfulness) and 2 (pleasantness) explained 41.3% and 30.8% of the variance in the data
293
set, respectively. The Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin (KMO) measure of the sampling adequacy was
294
0.77 and the Bartlett's test of sphericity was also significant (χ2 (28) = 1725.53, and p < 0.01),
295
which indicates that the data set is appropriate for PCA.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
289
As listed in Table 2 for the factor loadings of the eight attributes, the PCA results showed
297
good agreement with the pleasantness-eventfulness model of soundscape perception proposed
298
in a previous study [49]. Pleasantness is associated with hedonic value of sound and
299
eventfulness is associated with variety of sounds or temporal structure of a soundscape. This
300
2D model of perceived soundscape quality has been widely used in soundscape studies
301
because it can provide comprehensive soundscape information for understanding
302
soundscapes.
305
EP
304
Table 2
AC C
303
TE D
296
306
To calculate the component scores for evaluation locations in the study area, the regression
307
method included in IBM SPSS 23 software was used and the component scores for
308
pleasantness and eventfulness were mapped over the study area for the three sampling
309
periods as shown in Fig. 4. The temporal variation for the pleasantness score (Fig. 4a) was
310
smaller than the eventfulness score (Fig. 4b) over the study area. 13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 311 312
Figure 4
313 Regarding the functions of urban spaces, higher pleasantness scores were observed in
315
recreational and residential areas, while the office district and high-density commercial areas
316
showed lower pleasantness scores (Fig. 5a). As shown in Fig. 5b, the high-density
317
commercial areas had the highest eventfulness score among the five types of urban functions
318
in the three sampling periods and peaked during period 3. The eventfulness scores in the low-
319
density commercial areas and urban recreational areas gradually decreased across the
320
sampling periods. Overall, the business districts and the residential areas were assessed as
321
uneventful soundscape for periods 1 and 2, but eventfulness score for the residential areas
322
slightly increased in period 3.
323
Figure 5
TE D
324
M AN U
SC
RI PT
314
Correlation coefficients between PCA component scores and perceived sound source types
326
were calculated as shown in Table 3. Perception of traffic sounds had strong and negative
327
correlation coefficients with the pleasantness scores for all sampling periods and no
328
significant correlations with eventfulness scores. The dominance of human activity sounds
329
was not associated with pleasantness score but showed significant and positive correlations
330
with eventfulness scores over the sampling periods. In addition, the correlation coefficients
331
for the sampling periods 2 (r=0.51, p<0.01) and 3 (r=0.55, p<0.01) were slightly larger than
332
for period 1 (r=0.42, p<0.01). Similar to the human sounds, perception of music sounds
333
showed positive correlation with eventfulness, but the correlation strength was less than
334
human sounds showing correlation coefficients of approximately 0.3 (p<0.01). There were
AC C
EP
325
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT modest correlations between pleasantness scores and perception of water sounds over the
336
study area. The perceived dominance of birdsong showed significant correlations with
337
pleasantness scores. Interestingly, the correlations became weaker over the periods where the
338
highest correlation was 0.61 (p<0.01) in period 1 and the correlation declined by 0.38
339
(p<0.01) in period 3. This result was in good agreement with the temporal tendency of bird
340
sound dominance over the sampling periods.
342 3.1.3 Acoustic parameters
M AN U
343
Table 3
SC
341
RI PT
335
The calculated acoustic parameters in the three sampling periods for different urban spatial
345
functions are listed in Table 4. The mean value of LAeq ranged from 62.0 to 69.0 dB over the
346
study area, which fell below noise standards set by the Korean Ministry of Environment
347
(KMOE). Regarding the loudness of sound environment, the business district and commercial
348
areas showed higher LAeq and loudness values than the residential and recreational areas. The
349
LAeq and loudness values tended to decrease slightly from period 1 to period 3 except in high-
350
density commercial areas. The spectral characteristics of sound environment differed
351
according to the functions of space. The sound environment in the business districts and high-
352
density commercial areas showed higher LCeq-Aeq and lower sharpness values indicating that
353
main sound sources in the areas contained higher energy at low frequencies. In contrast, the
354
acoustic environment in the low-density commercial area had greater high-frequency content
355
with lower LCeq-Aeq and higher sharpness values. The residential and recreational areas
356
showed both relatively higher LCeq-Aeq and sharpness values compared with other urban areas.
357
Correlation coefficients among the acoustic parameters and identified sound sources were
358
calculated as listed in Table 5. Similar correlation tendencies between acoustic parameters
359
and perceived sound source types were found in a previous study [13]. Traffic noise was 15
AC C
EP
TE D
344
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT positively correlated with LAeq (r=0.55, p<0.01), loudness (r=0.53, p<0.01) and L10-90 (r=0.37,
361
p<0.01), whereas strong and negative relationships were found between traffic sound and
362
sharpness. This finding supports that road traffic sounds, containing significant low-
363
frequency energy, are the main sound source that increases the background noise level in
364
urban areas. The presence of human sounds (r=-0.31, p<0.01) and music (r=-0.26, p<0.01)
365
were negatively correlated to LCeq-Aeq indicating that sounds from human activities usually
366
consist of higher frequencies. In addition, human sounds showed weak correlation with LAeq
367
(r=0.17, p<0.01) and loudness (r=0.11, p<0.05) indicating that human sounds also contribute
368
to the increase in background sound levels during a certain period. Perception of bird sounds
369
was negatively correlated with overall loudness of the sound environment (LAeq and loudness)
370
and positively associated with sharpness. This finding demonstrates that urban green areas
371
with low traffic noise level could provide urban habitat for birds [55]. With respect to the
372
temporal variation, bird sounds showed large variations in the correlations with the acoustic
373
parameters according to sampling period; the correlation coefficients between the bird sounds
374
and the acoustic parameters decreased in period 3 due to the decreasing behavioral
375
frequencies in the period. This finding of temporal behavioral pattern of birds in urban areas
376
corresponds well with results of a previous study [43].
378 379 380
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
377
RI PT
360
Table 4 Table 5
381
3.2 Relationship between morphological indices and soundscape variables
382
3.2.1 Spatial variation of morphological indices
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT PCA was also conducted to characterize the 16 morphological variables. As in the previous
384
PCA, varimax rotation was applied to obtain the main components. The KMO measure of the
385
sampling adequacy was 0.53, and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (105) =
386
3952.25, and p < 0.01), which suggests that PCA with the 16 morphological indices is
387
feasible. Four components with Eigenvalues larger than 1 were found, as listed in Table 6.
388
The extracted components showed good agreement with our prior classification for 16
389
morphological indices. Component 1 (Open space) is highly related to variables representing
390
urban green and public areas. Component 2 (Building) represents morphological variables
391
associated with building areas and surfaces. Components 3 (Water feature) and 4 (Roads) are
392
associated with variables representing urban water features and road surfaces, respectively.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
383
393 394
Table 6
395
To understand the morphological characteristics of spaces with different functions, the
397
component scores for morphological variables were calculated using regression analysis. The
398
range of component scores varied by urban function as shown in Fig. 6. The business and
399
commercial areas in the study area can be characterized by higher building density than the
400
other urban functional areas. High-density commercial areas obtained higher component 2
401
and 3 scores than other areas indicating these areas consisted mainly of buildings and wide
402
roads. The low-density commercial areas showed relatively lower component 3 scores
403
implying these areas consisted of narrower roads. Residential areas showed small ranges and
404
neutral values for all component scores. Recreational areas showed higher values for
405
component 1 and lower values for component 2 than the other functional areas, indicating
406
recreational areas can be characterized as places with large open areas and low density of
AC C
EP
TE D
396
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 407
buildings. In addition, the range of component 3 values in the recreational areas was the
408
largest among the urban functions indicating roads are main morphological components in
409
urban recreational areas.
411
Figure 6
412 413
RI PT
410
3.2.2 Correlation between soundscape variables and morphological indices
The relationship between the identified sound source types and morphological indices were
415
explored by calculating the correlation coefficients (Table 7). As expected, traffic sounds
416
showed significant positive correlations with morphological factors related to road areas
417
(Rd(A) and RAF). Although the correlation was weaker than Rd(A) and RAF, negative
418
relationships were observed between perception of traffic sounds and morphological indices
419
for Open space (Op(A), Op(P), and OSR). Statistically significant correlations were not found
420
between traffic sounds and building factors.
TE D
M AN U
SC
414
Interestingly, human sounds mainly correlated with building-related components only for
422
period 3 and no significant relationships were observed for periods 1 and 2. In period 3, as
423
the morphological indices associated with the building area and perimeter increased, the
424
dominance of human sounds increased. This finding may be due to human activities
425
significantly increasing after working hours, especially in commercial areas, which mainly
426
consist of building components. This implies that the relationships between morphological
427
factors and human sounds depend on the temporal behavioral patterns of human activities.
428
Unlike human sounds, music sounds are consistently associated with building components
429
because music sounds are usually generated by shops in the commercial areas, which are
430
relatively independent from human activity.
AC C
EP
421
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Bird sounds were significantly associated with morphological variables for green and open
432
public spaces and had a negative correlation with road-related indices (Rd(A) and RAF).
433
Identification of water sounds was significantly correlated with the morphological indicators
434
related to water features (Wt(A) and Wt(P)). In addition, the variable associated with open
435
public space (Op(P) and OSR) strongly correlated with water sounds because water features
436
such as fountains are usually installed in urban public spaces such as urban squares. The
437
building-related indicators also had a negative relationship with water sounds; however, this
438
should be interpreted as an indirect relationship within the case study area because water
439
sounds were directly related to water features.
441
M AN U
440
SC
RI PT
431
442
3.2.3 Regression models using acoustic and morphological factors
443
3.2.3.1 Sound source models
Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to investigate the contributions of the
445
acoustic and urban morphological metrics prediction of the perceived sound sources in the
446
case study area. For the acoustic indicators, LAeq, LCeq-Aeq, L10-90 and sharpness values were
447
selected as independent variables. The loudness value was removed due to multicollinearity
448
with LAeq in the regression models. In terms of urban morphological indicators, four
449
component scores including Open public space, Buildings, Roads, and Water features were
450
used to reduce the number of morphological variables. The regression models were
451
developed using not only the total dataset but also datasets for each sampling period to
452
compare the temporal contributions of the input variables.
AC C
EP
TE D
444
453
The standardized regression coefficients (β) for perceived traffic, human, bird and water
454
sounds, and the coefficients of determination for the regression models are listed in Table 8. 19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT In terms of traffic noise, the regression model explained 44% to 55% of the variance in
456
perceived traffic noise. Among the acoustic indicators, the contributions of LAeq were the
457
greatest over the sampling periods. The contributions of the morphological components were
458
relatively smaller than of acoustic indicators. The Water feature component had a negative
459
relationship with traffic noise, whereas the Road component had positive contribution to the
460
perceived dominance of traffic noise; however, the strength was weak.
RI PT
455
The regression models for human sounds showed lower R2 values than other types of sounds
462
indicating that further efforts should be taken to develop better acoustic and morphological
463
indicators to accurately predict human sounds in urban areas. The regression model of period
464
1 explained approximately 14% of the variance in human sound, while R2 in periods 1 and 3
465
increased by 24% and 25%, respectively. Among the acoustic indicators, LCeq-Aeq,
466
representing the low-frequency sound content, had only a significant negative relationship
467
with human sounds in the sampling periods because human sounds usually contain higher
468
frequency energy. This finding was in good agreement with a previous study showing that
469
human-made sounds in urban environments were associated with frequency-related indicators
470
[12]. Regarding the morphological components, the Building component significantly
471
contributed to the prediction of human sounds only in sampling period 3. This is supported by
472
the observation that human activities were dramatically increased in the commercial districts
473
with high density of buildings in sampling period 3 in the case study area (Figs. 3 and 6). In
474
addition, the contribution of the Building component (β=0.35) was greater than LCeq-Aeq (β=-
475
0.26). This result also demonstrated that the contribution of urban morphological factors
476
depends on diurnal patterns of human sounds in urban environments.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
461
477
With respect to acoustic and morphological factors LAeq and Open space significantly
478
contributed to the explanation of perceived bird sounds in urban areas, respectively, in the 20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sampling periods. In contrast to the regression model for perceived human sounds, the R2
480
values of the regression models for bird sounds gradually decreased from 0.51 to 0.21 across
481
the three sampling periods. This result might be associated with the observed temporal
482
behavioral patterns of bird sounds in urban areas implying that temporal factors play
483
important roles in developing the prediction models, especially for biological sounds.
RI PT
479
The regression models for perceived water sounds explain more than 50% of the variance of
485
the identification of water sounds in the case study area. The acoustic indicators including
486
LAeq and sharpness showed significant relationships with perceived water sounds in period 1.
487
In terms of morphological factors, Water feature showed the most significant contribution for
488
predicting water sounds for all periods in the case study area. This finding supports the use of
489
morphological indicators for water features as reliable indicators for mapping the water
490
soundscape in urban areas [56,57] because in general, water sounds are generated from water
491
features such as fountains, rivers or sea, which are fixed at certain locations in urban areas.
494 495
M AN U
TE D
493
Table 8
EP
492
SC
484
3.2.3.2 Perceived affective quality models Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to predict pleasantness and eventfulness
497
scores using the acoustic and urban morphological indicators. Regression models for
498
pleasantness and eventfulness were developed and Table 9 summarizes the regression
499
analyses. The four acoustical and four urban morphological indicators were applied in the
500
same manner as in the perceived sound source model in the previous section.
AC C
496
501
Regarding the goodness of fit of the models, the pleasantness model showed significantly
502
higher R2 than the eventfulness model. The pleasantness model explained approximately 50%
503
of the variance, whereas the eventfulness model only predicted 13% of the variance. For the 21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT pleasantness model, LAeq was the most significant variable in terms of the acoustic
505
parameters. Regarding the morphological factors, the contribution of the water feature was
506
relatively stronger than the other morphological components even though the pleasantness
507
score showed higher correlation with perception of bird sounds than water sounds (Table 2).
508
Open space showed limited correlation with perceived bird sounds in the case study area
509
ranging from 0.32 to 0.40. Relationships between bird sounds and morphological indicators
510
related to urban green areas were also investigated in previous studies [42,43]; however, the
511
suggested indicators are limited to accurately predict birdsong in urban areas. Thus, more
512
reliable morphological indicators should be developed for bird sounds in the future.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
504
For the eventfulness model, the effects of LAeq and LCeq-Aeq were statistically significant
514
among the acoustic parameters, while the contribution of the Building component was only
515
significant among the morphological components. In general, eventfulness of soundscape was
516
closely associated with presence of human sounds, which was found dependent on temporal
517
patterns of human activity in urban areas. Reportedly, the eventfulness of soundscape plays a
518
vital role in judging appropriateness of soundscape in multifunctional urban spaces [9,25].
519
Thus, in the future, investigating relationships between soundscapes and the indicators
520
representing human activities, spatial configurations or building functions is necessary to
521
establish better eventfulness models.
523 524
EP
AC C
522
TE D
513
Table 9
525
Conclusion
526
In the present study, the relationships between the acoustic, urban morphological indicators
527
and spatiotemporal characteristics of soundscapes were explored in multifunctional urban 22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT areas in order to provide insight for soundscape planning and management in an urban
529
environment. Both physical acoustic data and perceptual soundscape data were collected by
530
observers at 122 locations over the three sampling periods from morning to evening in the
531
case study area. Morphological indicators relating to buildings, roads, open public spaces,
532
and water feature components were calculated in order to measure the urban forms.
RI PT
528
The soundscapes varied spatially and temporally in the urban area throughout a day. In
534
terms of spatial variations, urban soundscapes were influenced by the main function of the
535
spaces. The temporal variability of soundscapes in urban spaces was affected by diurnal
536
patterns in sound sources. Traffic and water sounds had relatively constant correlations with
537
acoustic and morphological indicators over the course of the day as sound sources such as
538
roads and water features are usually fixed in urban areas. In contrast, the dominance of
539
birdsong and sounds from human activities varied throughout the day owing to greater
540
temporal variation in biological and anthropogenic activity in urban areas.. This indicates that
541
temporal patterns in various sound sources can play critical roles in creating soundscapes in
542
urban areas.
TE D
M AN U
SC
533
Regression models for pleasantness and eventfulness were proposed based on acoustic and
544
morphological indicators, and the results revealed that the application of a combination of
545
acoustic and morphological factors could be a good approach for developing soundscape
546
prediction models in urban spaces. Pleasantness model was developed using LAeq, open space
547
and water feature components, which explain approximately 50% of the variance in the
548
dataset. However, the eventfulness model had some limitations in its explanation of the
549
variance of the collected soundscape data in this study, which indicates that further studies
550
will be required to investigate soundscape indicators for eventfulness. In particular,
551
soundscape indicators for bird and human activities in urban spaces, which are closely related
AC C
EP
543
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 552
to the pleasantness and eventfulness of soundscapes, respectively, should be explored in order
553
to improve soundscape prediction models. The present study has some inherent limitations. One limitation is the use of a small number
555
of observers in their 20s, whose observations may not be directly transferable to general
556
results. In order to generalize the results, large soundscape data collection from various age
557
groups is essential. Furthermore, the findings in this study are limited to the case study area
558
of Seoul, Korea. The urban density and morphology of typical western and eastern cities are
559
significantly different. Thus, cross-national comparative studies on relationships between
560
urban morphology and its soundscape should be conducted in the future. Despite these
561
limitations, this study has significantly enhanced our understanding of the relationships
562
between the urban morphology and soundscapes in urban areas, which will aid in urban
563
soundscape planning and design.
AC C
EP
TE D
564
M AN U
SC
RI PT
554
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 565 566
References [1]
J. Corburn, Toward the Healthy City: People, Places, and the Politics of Urban Planning, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1468-
568
2427.2011.01040_3.x.
569
[2]
570
RI PT
567
J. Kang, B. Schulte-Fortkamp, Soundscape and the Built Environment, CRC Press, London, UK, 2015.
[3]
B. Berglund, T. Lindvall, D. Schwela, Guidelines for community noise, Geneva, 1999.
572
[4]
H.M.E. Miedema, Annoyance caused by environmental noise: Elements for evidence-
SC
571
based noise policies, J. Soc. Issues. 63 (2007) 41–57. doi:10.1111/j.1540-
574
4560.2007.00495.x.
575
[5]
M AN U
573
C. Marquis-Favre, E. Premat, D. Aubree, Noise and its Effects – A Review on Qualitative Aspects of Sound. Part I: Notions and Acoustic Ratings, Acta Acoust.
577
United With Acoust. 91 (2005) 613–625.
578
[6]
TE D
576
J. Kang, F. Aletta, T.T. Gjestland, L.A. Brown, D. Botteldooren, B. Schulte-fortkamp, P. Lercher, I. Van Kamp, K. Genuit, J. Luis, B. Coelho, L. Maffei, L. Lavia, Ten
580
questions on the soundscapes of the built environment, Build. Environ. 108 (2016)
581
284–294. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.08.011. [7]
583 584
Destiny Books, New York, 1977.
[8]
585 586
R. Schafer, The soundscape: our sonic environment and the tuning of the world,
AC C
582
EP
579
ISO 12913-1, Acoustics — Soundscape — Part 1 : Definition and conceptual
framework, Geneva, Switzerland, 2014.
[9]
F. Aletta, J. Kang, Ö. Axelsson, Soundscape descriptors and a conceptual framework
587
for developing predictive soundscape models, Landsc. Urban Plan. 149 (2016) 65–74.
588
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.02.001. 25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
590 591 592 593
[10] J. Kang, M. Zhang, Semantic differential analysis of the soundscape in urban open public spaces, Build. Environ. 45 (2010) 150–157. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2009.05.014. [11] B.M. Brooks, B. Schulte-Fortkamp, K.S. Voigt, A.U. Case, Exploring our sonic environment through soundscape research and theory, Acoust. Today. 10 (2014) 30–40.
RI PT
589
[12] J.Y. Jeon, J.Y. Hong, Classification of urban park soundscapes through perceptions of the acoustical environments, Landsc. Urban Plan. 141 (2015) 100–111.
595
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.005.
596
SC
594
[13] J. Liu, J. Kang, Soundscape design in city parks: exploring the relationships between soundscape composition parameters and physical and psychoacoustic parameters, J.
598
Environ. Eng. Landsc. Manag. 23 (2015) 102–112.
599
doi:10.3846/16486897.2014.998676.
602 603 604
Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 903–911.
TE D
601
[14] M. Nilsson, B. Berglund, Soundscape quality in suburban green areas and city parks,
[15] W. Yang, J. Kang, Soundscape and Sound Preferences in Urban Squares: A Case Study in Sheffield, J. Urban Des. 10 (2005) 61–80. doi:10.1080/13574800500062395. [16] L. Yu, J. Kang, Modeling subjective evaluation of soundscape quality in urban open
EP
600
M AN U
597
spaces: An artificial neural network approach., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126 (2009) 1163–
606
1174. doi:10.1121/1.3183377.
607 608 609
AC C
605
[17] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Designing sound and visual components for enhancement of urban soundscapes, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 (2013) 2026–2036.
[18] Q. Meng, J. Kang, Effect of sound-related activities on human behaviours and acoustic
610
comfort in urban open spaces, Sci. Total Environ. 573 (2016) 481–493.
611
doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2016.08.130.
612
[19] Q. Meng, Y. Sun, J. Kang, Effect of temporary open-air markets on the sound 26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 613
environment and acoustic perception based on the crowd density characteristics, Sci.
614
Total Environ. 601–602 (2017) 1488–1495. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2017.06.017.
616 617
[20] M. Yang, J. Kang, Psychoacoustical evaluation of natural and urban sounds in soundscapes., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 (2013) 840–51. doi:10.1121/1.4807800. [21] M. Rychtáriková, G. Vermeir, Soundscape categorization on the basis of objective acoustical parameters, Appl. Acoust. 74 (2013) 240–247.
619
doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2011.01.004.
622 623
[22] H. Fastl, E. Zwicker, Psychoacoustics, Springer Berlin Heidelberg, New York, NY, 2006.
M AN U
621
SC
618
620
RI PT
615
[23] K. Genuit, A. Fiebig, Psychoacoustics and its benefit for the soundscape approach, Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 952–958.
[24] K. Herranz-Pascual, I. Aspuru, I. García, Proposed conceptual model of environmental
625
experience as framework to study the soundscape, in: Proc. Internoise, Lisbon,
626
Portugal, 2010: pp. 1–10.
TE D
624
[25] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Influence of urban contexts on soundscape perceptions: A
628
structural equation modeling approach, Landsc. Urban Plan. 141 (2015) 78–87.
629
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.004.
631 632 633
[26] L. Adolphe, A simplified model of urban morphology: Application to an analysis of
AC C
630
EP
627
the environmental performance of cities, Environ. Plan. B Plan. Des. 28 (2001) 183– 200. doi:10.1068/b2631.
[27] J.E. Nichol, High-Resolution Surface Temperature Patterns Related to Urban
634
Morphology in a Tropical City: A Satellite-Based Study, J. Appl. Meteorol. 35 (1996)
635
135–146. doi:10.1175/1520-0450(1996)035<0135:HRSTPR>2.0.CO;2.
636
[28] E. Ng, C. Yuan, L. Chen, C. Ren, J.C.H. Fung, Improving the wind environment in 27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 637
high-density cities by understanding urban morphology and surface roughness: A
638
study in Hong Kong, Landsc. Urban Plan. 101 (2011) 59–74.
639
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.01.004.
641 642
[29] M. Hornikx, Ten questions concerning computational urban acoustics, Build. Environ. 106 (2016) 409–421. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.06.028.
RI PT
640
[30] C. Zhao, G. Fu, X. Liu, F. Fu, Urban planning indicators, morphology and climate indicators: A case study for a north-south transect of Beijing, China, Build. Environ.
644
46 (2011) 1174–1183. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2010.12.009.
[31] C. Yuan, E. Ng, L.K. Norford, Improving air quality in high-density cities by
M AN U
645
SC
643
646
understanding the relationship between air pollutant dispersion and urban
647
morphologies, Build. Environ. 71 (2014) 245–258.
648
doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2013.10.008.
649
[32] A. Gospodini, Urban Design , Urban Space Morphology , Urban Tourism : An Emerging New Paradigm Concerning, Eur. Plan. Stud. 9 (2001) 925–934.
651
doi:10.1080/0965431012007984.
653
Urban Morphology, Trans. GIS. 6 (2002) 295–309. doi:10.1111/1467-9671.00112. [34] A. Rapoport, Human aspects of urban form: towards a man: environment approach to
657
AC C
654
[33] B. Jiang, C. Claramunt, Integration of Space Syntax into GIS: New Perspectives for
EP
652
TE D
650
658
(2011) 66–76. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2011.10.018.
655 656
659 660
urban form and design, 1st ed., Pergamon press, New York, 1977.
[35] I.C.M. Guedes, S.R. Bertoli, P.H.T. Zannin, Influence of urban shapes on environmental noise: A case study in Aracaju - Brazil, Sci. Total Environ. 412–413
[36] H. Ryu, I.K. Park, B.S. Chun, S. Il Chang, Spatial statistical analysis of the effects of urban form indicators on road-traffic noise exposure of a city in South Korea, Appl. 28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 661
Acoust. 115 (2017) 93–100. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2016.08.025. [37] E.M. Salomons, M. Berghauser Pont, Urban traffic noise and the relation to urban
663
density, form, and traffic elasticity, Landsc. Urban Plan. 108 (2012) 2–16.
664
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2012.06.017.
665
RI PT
662
[38] G. Rey Gozalo, J.M. Barrigón Morillas, J. Trujillo Carmona, D. Montes González, P. Atanasio Moraga, V. Gómez Escobar, R. Vílchez-Gómez, J.A. Méndez Sierra, C.
667
Prieto-Gajardo, Study on the relation between urban planning and noise level, Appl.
668
Acoust. 111 (2016) 143–147. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2016.04.018.
[39] B. Wang, J. Kang, Effects of urban morphology on the traffic noise distribution
M AN U
669
SC
666
670
through noise mapping: A comparative study between UK and China, Appl. Acoust.
671
72 (2011) 556–568. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2011.01.011.
672
[40] Y. Hao, J. Kang, Influence of mesoscale urban morphology on the spatial noise attenuation of flyover aircrafts, Appl. Acoust. 84 (2014) 73–82.
674
doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2013.12.001.
675
TE D
673
[41] F. Qu, J. Kang, Effects of built environment morphology in residential areas on resisting wind turbine noise on building façades, Renew. Energy. 107 (2017) 629–638.
677
doi:10.1016/j.renene.2017.02.037.
679 680
[42] Y. Hao, J. Kang, J.D. Krijnders, Integrated effects of urban morphology on birdsong
AC C
678
EP
676
loudness and visibility of green areas, Landsc. Urban Plan. 137 (2015) 149–162. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.01.006.
681
[43] J. Liu, J. Kang, H. Behm, Birdsong As an Element of the Urban Sound Environment:
682
A Case Study Concerning the Area of Warnemünde in Germany, Acta Acust. United
683
with Acust. 100 (2014) 458–466. doi:10.3813/AAA.918726.
684
[44] J. Ge, J. Lu, K. Morotomi, K. Hokao, Developing Soundscapegraphy for the Notation 29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 685
of Urban Soundscape: Its Concept, Method, Analysis and Application, Acta Acust.
686
United with Acust. 95 (2009) 65–75. doi:10.3813/AAA.918128.
687
[45] J. Liu, J. Kang, T. Luo, H. Behm, T. Coppack, Spatiotemporal variability of soundscapes in a multiple functional urban area, Landsc. Urban Plan. 115 (2013) 1–9.
689
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.03.008.
690
RI PT
688
[46] A.D. Mazaris, A.S. Kallimanis, G. Chatzigianidis, K. Papadimitriou, J.D. Pantis,
Spatiotemporal analysis of an acoustic environment: interactions between landscape
692
features and sounds, Landsc. Ecol. 24 (2009) 817–831. doi:10.1007/s10980-009-9360-
693
x.
M AN U
694
SC
691
[47] E. Margaritis, J. Kang, Relationship between green space-related morphology and
695
noise pollution, Ecol. Indic. 72 (2017) 921–933. doi:10.1016/j.ecolind.2016.09.032.
696
[48] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Exploring spatial relationships among soundscape variables in urban areas: A spatial statistical modelling approach, Landsc. Urban Plan. 157 (2017)
698
352–364. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.08.006.
TE D
697
[49] Ö. Axelsson, M.E. Nilsson, B. Berglund, A principal components model of soundscape
700
perception., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 128 (2010) 2836–2846. doi:10.1121/1.3493436.
EP
699
[50] G.G. Koch, Intraclass correlation coefficient, Encycl. Stat. Sci. 4 (1982) 213–217.
702
[51] B. De Coensel, D. Botteldooren, The quiet rural soundscape and how to characterize it,
704
[52] D. 45631/A1, Calculation of loudness level and loudness from the sound spectrum -
705
AC C
701
Zwicker method - Amendment 1: Calculation of the loudness of time-variant sound,
706
2008.
703
707 708
Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 887–897.
[53] 45692 DIN, Measurement Technique for the simulation of the auditory sensation of sharpness, 2009. 30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 709
[54] Y. Gan, T. Luo, W. Breitung, J. Kang, T. Zhang, Multi-sensory landscape assessment:
710
The contribution of acoustic perception to landscape evaluation, J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
711
136 (2014) 3200–3210. doi:10.1121/1.4898424. [55] E. Fernandez-Juricic, J. Jokimäki, A habitat island approach to conserving birds in
RI PT
712 713
urban landscapes: case studies from southern and northern Europe, Biodivers. Conserv.
714
10 (2001) 2023–2043.
717 718
SC
716
[56] F. Aletta, J. Kang, Soundscape approach integrating noise mapping techniques: a case study in Brighton, UK, Noise Mapp. 2 (2015). doi:10.1515/noise-2015-0001. [57] F.M.A. Calarco, L. Galbrun, Soundscape design and over road traffic noise mapping of
M AN U
715
water features used, in: Internoise 2015, San Francisco, USA, 2015: pp. 1–11.
AC C
EP
TE D
719
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1. Calculation of urban morphological parameters. (A) and (P) denote area and perimeter, respectively. Indicators Definition
Formula
Sum of building area
Bldg (P)
Sum of building perimeter
Bldsf (A)
Sum of building surface area
BPAF
The ratio of the plan area of buildings to the total surface area The summed area of roughness elements and exposed ground divided by the total surface area of the study region
0.00 – 144543.70
0.00 – 4819.35
M AN U
SC
Sum of green area
Gr (P)
Sum of green perimeter
Op (A)
Sum of open public area including urban squares, green and water areas
Op (P)
Sum of open perimeter including urban squares, green and water feature areas
TE D
Gr (A)
The ratio of the open area divided by the total surface area of the study region
Grd (A)
Sum of exposed ground area
Rd (A)
Sum of road area
EP
OSR
0.00 – 10387198
( ) ( )
0.00 – 0.64
( ) ( )
0.22 – 4.82
0.00 – 1575.57
0.00 – 557.03
0.00 – 15754.57
0.00 – 686.20
( ) ( )
0.00 – 0.70 630.29 – 17416.47
Wt (A)
Sum of water feature area
Wt (P)
Sum of water feature perimeter
AC C
RAF
The ratio of the exposed ground area divided by the total surface area of the study region The ratio of the road area to the study region
EGR
RI PT
Bldg (A)
CAR
Range
32.07 – 14765.52
( ) ( )
0.03 – 0.77
( ) ( )
0.00 – 0.66
32
!
0.00 – 2106.92
0.00 – 342.11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 2. Rotated component matrices of the PCA using semantic attribute responses (numbers in parentheses represent explained variance) Component 2 (30.8%)
0.13 0.00 -0.34 0.09
0.88 -0.92 0.80 -0.66
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
0.91 -0.84 -0.75 0.89
RI PT
Component 1 (41.3%)
SC
Attributes Pleasantness Pleasant Unpleasant Calm Chaotic Eventfulness Eventful Uneventful Monotonous Exciting
33
0.03 0.11 0.18 0.03
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 3. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and perceptual components of soundscapes for the three sampling periods (*p<0.05, **p<0.01) Water 0.17* 0.24** 0.29** 0.23** 0.11 0.08 0.04 0.08
M AN U TE D EP AC C 34
Bird 0.61** 0.54** 0.38** 0.51** -0.12 -0.10 0.06 -0.05
RI PT
Music 0.18* 0.03 -0.01 0.07 0.30** 0.37** 0.36** 0.34**
SC
Period Traffic Human ** Pleasantness P1 -0.65 -0.02 ** P2 -0.64 -0.07 P3 -0.65** -0.08 ** Total -0.64 -0.05 Eventfulness P1 -0.06 0.42** P2 0.01 0.51** P3 -0.07 0.55** Total -0.04 0.49** P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 4. Mean values of acoustic parameters of the three sampling periods
P2 P3 High-density
P1
Commercial P2 P3 Low-density Commercial
P1 P2 P3
Residential
P1
L10-90 [dB]
Loudness [sone]
Sharpness [acum]
Mean
67.88
9.91
6.90
25.03
1.69
SD Mean SD Mean SD
4.47 68.49 4.32 66.50 4.76
2.92 9.98 3.23 9.65 3.04
3.23 7.13 2.76 6.82 3.37
6.63 25.46 6.46 22.67 6.58
0.12 1.70 0.18 1.72 0.15
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
69.00 5.47 69.29 4.82 69.72 4.66
8.45 2.62 9.18 2.74 7.47 3.03
6.38 2.64 6.22 2.22 6.42 3.30
26.18 7.69 27.36 8.29 26.66 6.43
1.66 0.11 1.65 0.15 1.66 0.16
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
67.44 5.46 67.13 6.24 66.78 4.73
7.58 3.36 6.84 3.22 6.32 2.57
9.12 3.39 8.34 3.28 8.79 4.80
22.48 5.96 22.25 6.85 21.53 6.68
1.85 0.13 1.90 0.19 1.90 0.22
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
63.20 6.29 63.73 5.41 61.95 5.66
9.81 2.55 10.27 3.44 8.69 3.00
8.17 3.27 7.40 3.40 8.04 3.16
19.09 8.17 18.58 7.45 16.97 6.06
1.93 0.25 1.92 0.30 1.97 0.25
6.38 3.31 5.36 2.49 5.19 3.28
22.24 9.10 20.19 6.97 18.67 7.34
1.86 0.27 1.83 0.25 1.93 0.31
EP
P2
LCeq-Aeq [dB]
AC C
P3 Recreational
RI PT
P1
LAeq [dB]
SC
Business
Statistics
M AN U
Period
TE D
Functions
P1
Mean 65.62 9.59 SD 6.13 3.62 P2 Mean 64.08 9.36 SD 5.62 3.15 P3 Mean 62.96 8.43 SD 6.71 2.88 P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)
35
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 5. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and acoustic parameters (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)
M AN U
TE D
EP AC C 36
Sharpness -0.48** -0.52** -0.56** -0.52** 0.07 -0.11 -0.22* -0.09 0.00 -0.04 0.00 -0.01 0.58** 0.58** 0.30** 0.46** 0.34** 0.20* 0.14 0.21**
RI PT
Loudness 0.52** 0.56** 0.51** 0.53** 0.02 0.19* 0.13 0.11* -0.07 0.08 0.17 0.06 -0.54** -0.53** -0.31** -0.44** 0.10 0.11 -0.02 0.07
SC
Period LAeq LCeq-Aeq L10-90 ** Traffic P1 0.55 -0.13 0.34** ** P2 0.58 0.03 0.38** ** P3 0.52 0.16 0.41** Total 0.55** 0.03 0.37** Human P1 0.11 -0.34** 0.11 * P2 0.22 -0.35** 0.04 * ** P3 0.19 -0.24 0.04 Total 0.17** -0.31** 0.06 ** Music P1 0.02 -0.29 -0.03 P2 0.10 -0.25** -0.09 ** P3 0.15 -0.25 -0.08 Total 0.09 -0.26** -0.07 ** Bird P1 -0.55 0.12 -0.04 P2 -0.59** 0.01 -0.20* P3 -0.36** 0.13 -0.11 Total -0.47** 0.10 -0.10 Water P1 0.03 0.01 -0.28** P2 0.08 -0.08 -0.23* P3 -0.09 -0.06 -0.25** Total 0.01 -0.04 -0.25** P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 6. Rotated component matrices of the PCA using morphological indices (numbers in parentheses represent explained variance) 2 (19.44%)
3 (16.81%)
4 (10.54%)
0.89 0.84 0.90 0.77 0.89
-0.11 -0.16 -0.29 -0.40 -0.32
-0.04 -0.10 0.10 0.08 0.11
-0.09 -0.09 0.04 0.31 0.17
-0.18 -0.30 -0.09 -0.18 -0.36
0.91 0.87 0.75 0.94 0.81
-0.10 0.06 0.06 -0.11 -0.06
-0.19 -0.09 -0.14 -0.13 -0.01
-0.20 -0.21
0.10 0.09
0.96 0.96
-0.18 -0.17 -0.27 -0.25
0.90 -0.79 0.92 -0.85
-0.16 -0.30 0.12 -0.14
TE D
-0.23 -0.39 -0.22 -0.42
SC
M AN U
0.04 0.02
RI PT
1 (41.29%)
AC C
EP
Component Open space Gr (A) Gr (P) Op (A) Op (P) OSR Building Bldg (A) Bldsf (A) Bldg (P) BPAF CAR Water feature Wt (A) Wt (P) Road Rd (A) Grd (A) RAF EGR
37
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 7. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and morphological indices (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)
P2 P3
0.05
0.04
-0.05
-0.09
-0.01 0.00 -0.12
0.03
0.03
0.01
-0.01
-0.05
P1
0.18*
0.06
0.21*
0.13
0.02
-0.19
*
-0.15 -0.12 -0.15
**
-0.01
Gr(P)
Op(A)
Op(P)
-0.15
-0.20
*
*
-0.22
*
-0.11 -0.11 -0.12
*
-0.06
-0.14 -0.18
**
-0.13
-0.20
OSR -0.22
*
Wt(A)
Wt(P)
Rd(A)
*
*
0.45
**
-0.27
**
-0.18 -0.22
*
**
-0.21*
-0.25
**
0.45
**
0.45
**
0.45
**
-0.11
-0.06 *
-0.20
-0.16
-0.21
**
-0.14
-0.23
**
-0.20
-0.21
*
**
-0.13
-0.20 -0.24
**
-0.22 -0.22
*
**
Grd(A)
RAF
EGR
0.36
**
-0.14
0.34
**
-0.13
0.37
**
-0.12
-0.06
0.36
**
-0.13*
0.13
-0.06 -0.05 -0.05
0.10
0.16
-0.26**
0.11
0.15
0.11
0.04
0.06
0.03
0.02
0.00
0.02
0.02
0.01
0.18
P3
0.32**
0.37**
0.30**
0.32**
0.32**
-0.14
-0.18*
-0.11
-0.14
-0.11
-0.06
-0.06
0.08
-0.20*
0.07
-0.23*
Total
0.22**
0.18**
0.22**
0.19**
0.13*
-0.03
-0.07
-0.08
-0.12*
-0.08
-0.06
-0.06
0.06
-0.09
0.04
-0.13*
P1
0.20*
0.18*
0.25**
0.20*
0.19*
-0.12
-0.14
-0.12
-0.08
-0.10
0.11
0.09
-0.19*
0.09
-0.18
0.09
P2
*
*
0.27
**
0.17
0.16
-0.11
-0.09
-0.07
-0.07
-0.06
0.02
0.01
-0.09
0.02
-0.09
0.00
0.47
**
-0.19
*
-0.10
-0.11
-0.05
-0.01
-0.11
-0.06
0.33
**
-0.12
*
-0.01
*
0.03
*
0.00
-0.42
**
0.09
-0.32
**
0.04
-0.25
**
-0.04
-0.33
**
0.04
P1 P2 P3
0.41
**
0.27
**
-0.02 -0.10 -0.03
Total
-0.05
P1
-0.36**
P2
-0.31
**
-0.33
**
-0.33
**
P3 Total
0.19 0.40
**
0.26
**
-0.18 -0.24
**
-0.13 -0.18
**
-0.01 0.04
0.40
**
0.26
**
-0.06 -0.14
-0.01
-0.04
0.01
-0.08
-0.30**
-0.27**
-0.35**
-0.24
**
**
**
-0.32
**
-0.28
**
-0.26
-0.21 -0.25
-0.28
0.36
**
-0.13
0.23
**
*
*
**
-0.31
**
-0.31
**
-0.12
M AN U
0.14
0.19
-0.21 -0.28
*
**
-0.15 -0.21
**
0.42
**
0.41
**
0.31
**
0.37
**
-0.27**
0.13
*
-0.17
**
*
-0.20
-0.14 0.37
**
0.36
**
0.20
0.32
*
**
0.16
-0.12 0.43
**
0.45
**
0.38
**
0.40
**
0.25
**
0.30
**
0.42 0.36
**
0.38
**
-0.02
-0.04
-0.28
0.08
-0.24**
0.24**
-0.15
0.04
-0.24
**
**
-0.16
-0.25
**
-0.24
**
-0.04 -0.01 -0.02
-0.06 -0.02 -0.04
0.30
**
*
0.69
**
0.68
**
0.36
**
0.73
**
0.74
**
0.34
**
0.68
**
0.68
**
0.11
-0.28
0.20
0.13
*
-0.25
**
0.13
*
*
0.23 0.17
**
0.20 0.33
**
0.25
**
-0.35
**
-0.28
**
-0.12
**
0.62**
0.07
-0.11
0.39
0.62**
*
38
0.34
**
0.00
**
0.25**
0.06
P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)
0.32
**
0.36**
**
0.12
-0.13
*
0.19*
*
-0.19
-0.24
**
-0.20
-0.10
*
P2
Total
Water
-0.05
0.00
Total
P3 Bird
-0.04
0.09
0.02
Gr(A)
TE D
Music
0.09
0.06
CAR
EP
Human
0.04
BPAF
RI PT
P1
Bldg(P)
AC C
Traffic
Bldsf(A)
SC
Bldg(A)
-0.22
*
**
-0.08 0.02
0.14 0.07 -0.02 0.07
0.23
0.14 0.20
**
-0.16 -0.15**
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 8. Standardized regression coefficients from multiple linear regression analysis for sound sources using acoustic and morphological indicators (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)
P1
P2
P3
Total
P1
P2
P3
Total
P1
P2
P3
Total
P1
P2
P3
0.47
0.44
0.49
0.55
0.15
0.14
0.24
0.25
0.32
0.51
0.46
0.21
0.51
0.62
0.51
0.57
0.45
0.36
0.48
0.47
0.03
0.03
0.05
-0.01
-0.37
-0.34
-0.53
-0.27
0.14
0.44
0.19
0.01
**
**
**
**
**
**
**
*
*
**
*
0.23
0.13
0.19
0.34
-0.33
-0.33
-0.41
-0.26
0.00
0.05
-0.20
0.10
-0.11
0.05
-0.11
-0.09
*
**
**
**
**
**
0.17
0.27
-0.09
-0.08
-0.10
-0.10
0.10
0.19
-0.03
0.11
-0.17
-0.20
-0.16
-0.13
*
**
**
*
Sharpness
-0.02
-0.02
0.12
0.59
0.06
0.00
**
**
0.13 ** -0.24
0.01
0.10
0.16
-0.20
-0.23
-0.21
**
**
**
**
0.08
0.15
0.08
0.04
0.19
0.14
**
-0.03
-0.16
Morphological
Road Water feature
-0.11
-0.06
0.02
*
-0.08
-0.04
-0.02
*
0.14
0.15
**
0.14 *
0.05
0.02
-0.18
0.17
**
**
-0.04 0.09
EP
Building
-0.07
0.12
0.11
AC C
Open space
-0.10
M AN U
L10-90
**
-0.23
-0.22
-0.23
-0.23
**
**
**
**
-0.14
TE D
LCeq-Aeq
Water
Total
Acoustic LAeq
Bird
-0.04
SC
R2
Human
RI PT
Traffic
-0.05
0.10 0.09
-0.19
0.03
*
-0.04 0.35
0.30
-0.04
-0.02
**
0.30
0.28
**
**
-0.01
0.00
0.29 ** -0.07
0.28 ** 0.06
**
0.24
0.16
-0.10
-0.09
-0.04
-0.06
*
-0.09
0.01
P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)
39
-0.01
-0.02
*
*
-0.01
-0.07
0.02
0.59
0.50
0.61
0.67
**
**
**
**
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 9. Standardized regression coefficients from multiple linear regression analysis of soundscape perception using acoustic and morphological indicators (*p<0.05, **p<0.01) Pleasantness Total
P1
P2
P3
Total
P1
P2
P3
0.49
0.54
0.53
0.50
0.13
0.08
0.22
0.21
-0.67
-0.45
-0.65
-0.81
0.22
**
**
**
**
**
-0.05
0.13
-0.10
-0.11
-0.17
-0.30
-0.24
**
*
*
-0.11
-0.09
-0.15
-0.09
-0.09
-0.05
-0.04
-0.01
0.17
-0.01
Acoustic LAeq LCeq-Aeq
0.12
L10-90
0.16
0.06
0.13
0.27
-0.07
-0.13
-0.03
**
Morphological Open space Building
0.12 0.00 0.00
Road
0.14
-0.01
0.05
0.04
-0.02
-0.06
-0.02
0.09
0.07
0.26
0.24
0.27
0.25
**
**
**
**
EP AC C 40
0.32
0.17
**
*
TE D
Water feature
0.12
**
-0.06
M AN U
Sharpness
0.02
SC
*
RI PT
R
2
Eventfulness
-0.04
*
0.11
-0.06
0.11
*
0.10 0.10
0.25 **
0.05
0.08
0.23 *
0.09
0.10
0.14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure captions Figure 1. Case study area in Seoul, Korea: (a) 122 sampling locations of the study area and sampling zone A-I for soundscape data collection; (b) morphological components.
RI PT
Figure 2. Spatiotemporal variation in perceived sound source types for three different sampling periods: (a) traffic, (b) human, (c) music, (d) bird, and (e) water sounds Figure 3. Temporal variation in the dominant sound source types for three different sampling periods and the main place functions: (a) traffic, (b) human, (c) music, (d) bird, and (e) water sounds. The perceived value of sound sources refer to the percentages of records that indicated “Heard a lot” or “Dominates completely” for a given sound source type.
SC
Figure 4. Spatiotemporal variation of: (a) pleasantness and (b) eventfulness for the three sampling periods
M AN U
Figure 5. Temporal variation of the mean PCA component scores for the three sampling periods and the main functions of places: (a) pleasantness and (b) eventfulness scores
AC C
EP
TE D
Figure 6. PCA component scores for morphological factors according to the main space functions
41
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Appendix A: Questionnaire for soundscape data collection (Translated from Korean) 1. To what extent do you presently hear the following types of sounds? Please tick off one response alternative per type of sound A little
Moderately
□
□
□
□
□
□
□ □ □ □ □
□ □ □ □ □
□ □ □ □ □
2. Sounds from human activities 3. Natural sounds - Water sounds - Bird sounds - Wind sounds 4. Music 5. Other sounds
Dominates completely
□
□
□
□
□ □ □ □ □
□ □ □ □ □
SC
1. Traffic noise
A lot
RI PT
Do not hear at all
M AN U
Type of sounds
2. To what extent do you agree with eight attributes below on how you experience the present surrounding sound environment? Please tick off one response alterative per attribute. Slightly Disagree
Neither disagree, nor agree
Slightly Agree
Strongly Agree
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
Monotonous
□
□
□
□
□
Calm
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
□
Unpleasant Eventful Uneventful
AC C
Exciting
TE D
Pleasant
Strongly Disagree
EP
The sound environment is:
Chaotic
42
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
B
C
E
F
D
Seoul
Sampling zones: A-I (a)
TE D
H
I
EP
Residential area High-density commercial area Low density commercial area Central business area Urban public space
AC C
Sampling locations
G
M AN U
A
SC
Korea
(b)
Green / park
Square
City stream
Building
Ground
Road
Sampling period 1: 09-11
Sampling period 2: 13-15
Sampling period 3: 18-20
SC M AN U TE D EP AC C
(e) Water sounds
(d) Bird sounds
(c) Musicsounds sounds Human
(b) Human sounds
RI PT
(a) Traffic sounds
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1.0-1.4 1.4-1.8 1.8-2.2 2.2-2.6 2.6-3.0 3.0-3.4 3.4-3.8 3.8-4.2 4.2-4.6 4.6-5.0
Not at all heard
Dominates completely
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
80
80
30 20
50 40 30 20 10
0
0 P1
P2 Sampling period
P3
P1 80
80
P2 Sampling period
60 50
50
30 20
P1
P2 Sampling period
P3
20
P1
P2 Sampling period
Low-density commercial Residential
40
Recreational
30
Sampling periods
20
P1(09:00-11:00) P2(13:00-15:00) P3(18:00-20:00)
0
0
30
High-density commercial
10
10
40
Business
AC C
40
60
50
Space functions
EP
Prominant sound source [%]
70
60
0 P3
(e) Water 70
70
10
TE D
(d) Bird
Prominant sound source [%]
40
RI PT
50
60
SC
60
(c) Music
70
M AN U
70
10
Prominant sound source [%]
80 (b) Human
Prominant sound source [%]
Prominant sound source [%]
(a) Traffic
P1
P2 Sampling period
P3
P3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Legend
Pleasant
Uneventful
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
Unpleasant
AC C
(b) Eventfulness score
Sampling period 3: 18-20
RI PT
Sampling period 2: 13-15
(a) Pleasantness score
Sampling period 1: 09-11
Eventful
(a)
1.0
1.0
(b)
SC
1.5
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
M AN U
0.0
Business Low-density commercial Recreational
P1
High-density commercial Residential
EP
-1.5
TE D
-0.5
-1.0
Eventfulness score
0.5
P2
AC C
Pleasantness score
0.5
Sampling period
P3
0.0
-0.5
-1.0
Business Low-density commercial Recreational
High-density commercial Residential
-1.5 P1
P2 Sampling period
P3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
RI PT
6 Open space Building Road
SC
Water feature
M AN U
4
TE D
2
-2
Business
AC C
EP
0
High-density commercial
Low-density commercial Space functions Main space functions
Residential
Recreational
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights Spatiotemporal patterns in soundscapes were collected in an urban area. Intercorrelations were found among soundscape, acoustic and morphological factors.
RI PT
A combination of acoustic and morphological indices can be applied to describe urban
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
soundscapes.