Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea

Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea

Accepted Manuscript Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study i...

4MB Sizes 1 Downloads 68 Views

Accepted Manuscript Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea Joo Young Hong, Jin Yong Jeon PII:

S0360-1323(17)30479-1

DOI:

10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.10.021

Reference:

BAE 5134

To appear in:

Building and Environment

Received Date: 1 August 2017 Revised Date:

29 September 2017

Accepted Date: 16 October 2017

Please cite this article as: Hong JY, Jeon JY, Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology in a multifunctional urban area: A case study in Seoul, Korea, Building and Environment (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.10.021. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1

Relationship between spatiotemporal variability of soundscape and urban morphology

2

in a multifunctional urban area: a case study in Seoul, Korea

3

RI PT

4 Joo Young Hong a, b and Jin Yong Jeon a

6

a

Department of Architectural Engineering, Hanyang University, Seoul 04763, Korea

7

b

School of Electrical & Electronic Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, 639798,

8

Singapore

SC

5

M AN U

9 10

Running title: Spatiotemporal variability of soundscapes

11

Send correspondence to: Jin Yong Jeon ([email protected])

Architectural Acoustics Lab (Room 605-1)

13

Department of Architectural Engineering

14

Hanyang University

15

17 Haengdang-dong, Seongdong-gu

16

Seoul 04763, Korea

19 20 21

EP

18

Phone: +82 2 2220 1795 Fax: +82 2 2220 4794

AC C

17

TE D

12

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 22

Abstract This study presents a new perspective on interrelationships among spatiotemporal patterns

24

in soundscape, acoustic and urban morphological indicators. Spatiotemporal characteristics of

25

soundscapes in a multifunctional urban area of Seoul, Korea were analyzed. Physical acoustic

26

and perceived soundscape data were collected during three different daytime sampling

27

periods. Urban morphological indicators representing building, road, open public space, and

28

water feature components, were analyzed to quantify urban textures. Spatial and temporal

29

factors were found to play critical roles in urban soundscapes. Regarding spatial patterns in

30

soundscapes, urban soundscapes were characterized based on the main function of spaces.

31

The temporal variability of soundscapes in urban spaces depended on diurnal patterns in

32

perceived sound sources. Particularly, the perception of birdsong and sounds from human

33

activities showed considerable variation across urban spaces. In addition, significant

34

correlations were found among the spatiotemporal patterns in soundscape, acoustic and

35

morphological factors. Based on the acoustic and morphological indicators, pleasantness and

36

eventfulness models were proposed and the results indicated that the pleasantness model

37

explained 50% of the variance, while the eventfulness model only predicted 13% of the

38

variance. The results from the present study showed that urban morphological factors could

39

be useful indicators for better understanding soundscapes in urban environments.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

40

RI PT

23

41

Keywords: Soundscape, spatiotemporal variability, perceptual evaluation, urban acoustic

42

environment, soundscape map, urban morphology

43 44 45 2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 46

1. Introduction The acoustic environment is considered an important factor in creating sustainable and

48

healthy cities [1,2] because urban noise generated from transportation or industrial facilities

49

can adversely impact human health [3–5]. However, many studies reported that reducing

50

physical sound pressure levels (SPLs) does not guarantee desirable acoustic quality in urban

51

environments, since SPLs cannot differentiate the type of sound sources that have positive or

52

negative values [6]. As an alternative approach to managing sound environments,

53

soundscapes, which focus on human perception of acoustic environments, have attracted

54

attention [2,6,7].

M AN U

SC

RI PT

47

A soundscape is defined as the “acoustic environment as perceived or experienced and/or

56

understood by a person or people, in context [8]”. Therefore, soundscape studies investigate

57

perceptual descriptors of environmental sounds, so called soundscape descriptors, and related

58

physical environmental factors in certain contexts of place. Soundscape indicators are

59

measures used to predict the value of soundscape descriptors.

TE D

55

In soundscape studies, investigating the relationship between soundscape descriptors and

61

indicators is important [9,10]. Soundscape indicators are required to support not only acoustic

62

appraisal but also context appraisal [11]. In previous studies, diverse acoustic parameters

63

were used as soundscape indicators for acoustic appraisal [6]. A number of soundscape

64

studies have been conducted in various outdoor public spaces including parks [12–14],

65

squares [10,15,16], and streets [17–19]. In each urban setting, the perceived acoustic

66

environment has been assessed based on soundscape descriptors such as acoustic comfort,

67

tranquility, restoration, and appropriateness, and the relationships between the soundscape

68

descriptors and the acoustic indicators representing strength and the spectral and temporal

69

characteristics of the acoustic environment have then been explored in the previous studies.

AC C

EP

60

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT In particular, combinations of conventional noise indicators based on SPLs and

71

psychoacoustic parameters based on critical band have been applied to describe the

72

characteristics of soundscapes [20,12,21]. Psychoacoustic parameters (e.g., loudness,

73

sharpness, roughness, and fluctuation strength) covering several dimensions of basic auditory

74

sensation [22] can provide more accurate information on the relationship between the

75

acoustic environment (physical phenomenon) and the soundscape (perceptual construct) than

76

conventional SPL based noise indicators [23].

RI PT

70

According to ISO 12913-1, context, which includes all other non-acoustic factors of a place,

78

plays an important role in the perception of soundscape [8]. The contexts can be categorized

79

into the following four clusters: person, place, person-place interaction, and activity [24].

80

Hong and Jeon [25] showed that functions of places and activities might influence perceptual

81

construction of soundscapes in urban environments. The results indicate that studies focusing

82

on non-acoustic indicators are important to more accurately predict soundscape descriptors.

M AN U

SC

77

Urban morphology affects environmental factors in urban areas [26–31]. In addition, urban

84

morphology is related to urban activities [32–34]. In this context, relationships between urban

85

morphology and acoustic environment have been explored in previous studies [35–47]. In

86

those studies, various morphological parameters for buildings, roads, and green areas (e.g.,

87

height, width, distance, density, length, coverage ratio, façade and configuration) have been

88

applied to characterize the urban form. Some studies have investigated the effect of urban

89

morphology in urban areas on the spatial distribution of traffic sound levels [35–39], aircraft

90

[40], wind turbines [41] and birdsong [42] because sound propagation in urban built

91

environments is significantly influenced by urban morphology. However, those studies were

92

limited mainly to noise sources and their physical sound levels, which may not directly relate

93

to soundscape perception in urban areas. Relatively few studies focused on the relationships

AC C

EP

TE D

83

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT between perceived acoustic environment and urban morphologies [43–46]. Ge et al. [44]

95

collected objective and subjective data on acoustic environment in a case study area in Saga,

96

Japan and found significant correlations among soundscape descriptors, SPLs and the

97

morphological parameters related to buildings, land use, open space, water and green space;

98

however, in that study, temporal variation in soundscape components was not considered. Liu

99

et al. [43,45] and Mazaris et al. [46] conducted case studies to observe the spatiotemporal

100

patterns of perceived sound sources. They examined the relationships between landscape

101

indicators (e.g., density, green, and configuration of landscape) and temporal patterns in

102

sound sources concluding that spatiotemporal patterns in sound sources are closely associated

103

with landscape indices. However, their study focused on the dominance of various sound

104

sources, but not perceived soundscape descriptors such as pleasantness or tranquility.

105

Therefore, it is necessary to provide comprehensive views on the interrelationships among

106

spatiotemporal variations of soundscape components, acoustic indicators and urban

107

morphology.

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

94

The aims of the present study are to explore the spatiotemporal patterns of soundscapes in a

109

multifunctional urban area, to investigate interrelationships among soundscape descriptors,

110

physical acoustic environment, and urban morphology, and to develop soundscape prediction

111

model based on the interrelationships. For these purposes, physical acoustic and subjective

112

data on soundscapes were measured in an urban area. Subsequently, various morphological

113

indicators were analyzed using a geographic information system (GIS) and the relationships

114

with the measured soundscape data were explored.

AC C

EP

108

115 116

2. Methods

117

2.1 Case study area 5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT As shown in Fig. 1(a), the study was conducted in the north part of Seoul (Joong-gu and

119

Jongro-gu areas, 26,550 m2). The case study area was chosen to include various urban

120

settings with considerable variation in urban morphological characteristics. The case study

121

areas were classified into the following five groups regarding main function of urban spaces

122

and activities similar to a previous study [25]: high-density commercial area (e.g., Myeong-

123

dong street), low-density commercial area (e.g., Insa-dong street), residential areas (e.g.,

124

Sajik-dong), the central business district (CBD) consisting mainly of office buildings (e.g.,

125

Euljiro) and an urban recreation area including urban parks (e.g., Deoksugung park) and city

126

streams (e.g., Cheonggyecheon).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

118

Soundscape data were collected from 122 locations over the study area. To analyze the

128

morphological indicators, the case study area was divided based on the sampling locations as

129

shown in Fig. 1(b). The grids in the present study were modified to better reflect the

130

morphological characteristics from the grids used in a previous study [48]. The different grid

131

sizes were applied in the study area to reflect the spatial characteristics of the places; the main

132

grid size was 150 m × 150 m (110 meshes), while a 150 m × 75 m grid size was applied for

133

the city stream areas (e.g., Cheonggyecheon), which has long and narrow spaces (12 meshes).

134

In each grid, one sampling location, which was considered to represent the overall

135

soundscape quality of the grid, was chosen for collection of soundscape data.

137 138 139

EP

AC C

136

TE D

127

Figure 1

2.2 Data collection

140

Soundscape data were collected using a questionnaire as shown in Appendix A. The

141

dominance of the perceived sound sources were assessed using the question, “To what extent 6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT do you presently hear the following types of sounds?” and the following five-point scale: 1 =

143

not heard at all, 2 = heard a little, 3 = heard moderately, 4 = heard a lot and 5 = sound

144

dominates completely. Based on previous studies [25,45], the types of sound sources were

145

classified into five categories: traffic noise, sounds from human activities, water sounds,

146

birdsong and music, and other noises such as construction noise or mechanical noise. The

147

perceived affective quality was also assessed using the following eight adjectives: pleasant,

148

unpleasant, eventful, uneventful, exciting, monotonous, calm, and chaotic [9,49]. At each

149

location, the observers evaluated the degree to which an adjective attribute applied to their

150

perception of the soundscape quality with the question, “To what extent do you agree with the

151

eight attributes below on how you experience the present surrounding sound environment?”

152

and using a five-point scale with the following response alternatives: 1 = strongly disagree, 2

153

= slightly disagree, 3 = neither disagree nor agree, 4 = slightly agree, and 5 = strongly agree.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

142

Soundscape data over the study area were collected by a group of eight observers (6 males,

155

2 females, Mage = 25.6 years, SDage = 2.0 years) applying the same method used in a previous

156

study [48]. To obtain a homogenous group of observers, undergraduate or graduate students

157

from the department of architectural engineering were recruited as the observers. Using the

158

same soundscape data-collecting protocol, pilot training sessions in data collection

159

procedures were conducted for the observers under laboratory conditions to reduce

160

observation bias. Similar to a previous study [45], the observers practiced soundscape

161

evaluation methods using urban sound excerpts (1-min) with corresponding visual images.

162

Inter-rater variability and intraclass reliability were calculated based on the training results to

163

quantify the observation bias. Cronbach’s alpha values and the average measure of intraclass

164

correlation were greater than 0.9, indicating good reliability of agreement among observers’

165

judgments [50].

AC C

EP

TE D

154

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The observers assessed the soundscape during three different periods (period 1: 09:00–11:00,

167

period 2: 13:00–15:00, and period 3: 18:00–20:00) at each location. As the case study area is

168

too large to enable the collection of the soundscape data by the observers in a day, the study

169

area was divided into eight zones (A to I) as shown in Fig. 1(a). Each zone included

170

approximately 11–16 measurement locations. The observers used bicycles to move to each

171

measurement point within a zone in order to reduce the moving time. In each zone, the eight

172

observers recorded soundscape data across the three sampling periods within a day. Thus, a

173

total of 8 days were required to perform the soundscape measurements during clear weather

174

on weekdays in May. Weekends were not considered for the data collection in order to

175

minimize the errors across the measurement days because the soundscape on weekdays might

176

be significantly different from that on the weekend. Within each sampling period, both

177

perceptual soundscape data and a 5-min recording of the acoustic environment were

178

simultaneously acquired at each evaluation location using a binaural microphone (Type 4101,

179

B&K, Denmark) and a digital recorder (Zoom, H4n, Japan).

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

166

In total, 2928 responses (122 points × 8 observers × 3 sampling periods) were obtained from

181

each observer for eight measurement days. The responses from the eight observers were

182

averaged at each measurement point in order to obtain representative values at each point.

183

Thus, 366 datasets (122 points × 3 sampling periods) were used for performing the data

184

analyses and mappings in this study.

186

AC C

185

EP

180

2.3 Acoustic indicators

187

The physical acoustic indicators are summarized by three aspects: sound strength, the

188

spectral content, and the temporal structure of sounds [51]. The acoustic recording samples at

189

each grid were used to calculate SPLs and psychoacoustic parameters. A-weighted SPL (LAeq)

190

was calculated to represent sound strength of the acoustic environment. Regarding the 8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 191

spectral contents of acoustic environment, LCeq-Aeq, representing energy at low frequencies,

192

was calculated [25,49]. The differences between the 10 and 90 percentile levels were

193

calculated (L10-90) to quantify the temporal variability of the sound environment.

RI PT

194 In previous studies, Zwicker’s loudness and sharpness were shown to be useful indicators

196

for understanding soundscapes in urban environments among the psychoacoustic parameters.

197

Loudness represents the magnitude of sound based on an auditory sensation, and sharpness is

198

the sensation value of the amount of high-frequency content in the sound, which can be used

199

as a quantitative indicator for the spectral envelope [22]. Thus, among the psychoacoustic

200

parameters, the loudness and sharpness of the recording were analyzed using a B&K PULSE

201

Sound Quality software (Type 7698, B&K, Denmark) based on 1-min audio recordings; the

202

Zwicker’s loudness was calculated according to DIN 45631/ A1 (2010) [52] and the

203

sharpness was calculated according to DIN 45692, (2009) [53]

M AN U

TE D

204

SC

195

2.4 Urban morphological indices

206

Based on previous studies [36,42], various urban morphological parameters were used to

207

quantify the 2D and 3D characteristics of urban morphologies in the case study area. Table 1

208

presents descriptions and formulas of 16 morphological indices employed in this study. The

209

urban morphological indicators were classified into four groups in terms of variables related

210

to the following: 1) buildings, 2) green and open public areas (sum of green, square and city

211

stream areas), 3) exposed ground and road surfaces, and 4) water features. The 2D

212

characteristics of urban morphologies were quantified based on area and perimeter of the

213

features at the ground level. Ratios of the total surface areas of buildings to plan areas were

214

used to measure the 3D characteristics of building morphologies. The geographical

AC C

EP

205

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 215

information on area, perimeter and height of buildings in the study area were constructed

216

using GIS software, ArcGIS ver. 10.1 (ESRI, USA) and the urban morphology indices were

217

calculated in each grid.

218

RI PT

219

Table 1 2.5 Mapping process

The soundscape data collected in the study area during the three sampling periods were

221

visualized using GIS software, ArcGIS ver. 10.1 (ESRI, USA). The inverse distance

222

weighted (IDW) surface interpolation method was applied to create the sound source and the

223

perceived affective quality maps. IDW interpolation is based on the assumption that each

224

measured point has more local weight on the predicted values and the weights diminish as a

225

function of distance from the measurement point. In previous studies, IDW has been applied

226

because it can emphasize the locality of foreground sounds [46,54]. The collected soundscape

227

data obtained from eight observers were averaged at each measured location across the three

228

sampling periods for the IDW interpolation. The power value in the weight function was set

229

as “2,” and the standard search neighborhood defined by the ellipse parameters such as angle

230

and the major and minor semi-axis was used to create the interpolation surfaces.

M AN U

TE D

EP

231

SC

220

3. Results and discussion

233

3.1 Spatiotemporal patterns of soundscape

234

3.1.1 Perceived sound sources

AC C

232

235

Spatial distribution of the perceived sound sources including traffic, human, bird and water

236

sounds at three different sampling periods are shown in Fig. 2. In terms of spatial distribution

237

of sound sources, spatial patterns of the dominated sound source types differed across the

238

study area [48]. Overall, traffic sounds dominated near high-traffic roads. Sounds from 10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT human activities and music were primarily observed in urban parks or commercial areas. In

240

terms of natural sounds, birdsong was frequently observed in urban green areas and water

241

sounds were mainly heard near the water features such as fountains or city streams. The

242

temporal distribution of different types of sound sources also varied across the study area.

243

Small temporal variations were observed for traffic and water sounds because the sound

244

sources, traffic roads and water features are usually situated in fixed locations. In contrast, the

245

temporal distributions of sounds related to human activities and birdsong significantly varied

246

in the different sampling periods because the behavioral patterns of people and birds are

247

affected by time of day [43,45].

M AN U

SC

RI PT

239

248 249

Figure 2

250

The percentage of responses recorded as “heard a lot” or “dominates completely” for a

252

given source type were calculated to explore dominant sound sources for the three different

253

sampling periods and four categories of urban functions as shown in Fig. 3. The dominant

254

sound source varied based on the main functions of spaces and sampling period. Traffic

255

sounds were most and least dominant in business districts and recreational areas, respectively.

256

As plotted in Fig. 3a, the temporal variation in traffic sounds was relatively smaller than other

257

sounds implying that traffic noise was a typical background sound over the case study area.

258

Human sounds showed significantly different temporal patterns based on different urban

259

functional areas, as shown in Fig. 3b. In business districts, the dominance of human sounds

260

gradually increased from the morning to evening hours. Interestingly, in commercial districts,

261

different temporal tendencies in the dominance of human sound were observed in high- and

262

low-density commercial areas. In the commercial district with a high density of buildings, the

AC C

EP

TE D

251

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT prominence of human sounds dramatically increased from morning and peaked at 65.4% in

264

sampling period 3, indicating that people usually go to the high-density commercial district

265

(Myeong-dong shopping district), Seoul’s premier shopping destination, to enjoy their urban

266

lives after working hours, which may increase the sounds from human activities. In contrast,

267

the low-density commercial area (Insa-dong street), consisting of stores specializing in a wide

268

variety of goods representing Korean traditional culture and crafts, exhibited a sudden

269

decrease in the dominance of human sounds in sampling period 3. This result indicates that

270

people mainly visited this area during the day rather than evening or nighttime. In

271

recreational areas, dominance of the human sounds in urban recreational areas gradually

272

decreased from sampling period 1 to period 3. This result indicates that the frequency of

273

visitors in the recreational areas in the study area were concentrated during daytime. For

274

residential areas, human sounds were frequently heard during the commuting hours in the

275

morning (period 1) and evening (period 3). Temporal patterns for music according to main

276

space function were similar to the patterns of human sounds (Fig. 3c). These findings implied

277

that population flow and temporal visitation patterns differed by time of day depending on the

278

socio-spatial characteristics of urban space.

279

Bird and water sounds were most dominant in urban recreational areas as shown in Figs. 3d

280

and 3e, respectively. The dominance of bird sounds significantly declined in period 3 by

281

10.0%, whereas water sounds slightly increased by 23.3%. The observers could better

282

perceive water sounds in the evening (period 3) due to decreased human activities in urban

283

stream areas.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

263

284 285

Figure 3

286 12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 287 288

3.1.2 Perceived affective quality Principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted based on the responses for eight

290

semantic attributes to extract the perceived affective quality of soundscape using varimax

291

rotation method. Two components with eigenvalues larger than 1 were obtained. Components

292

1 (eventfulness) and 2 (pleasantness) explained 41.3% and 30.8% of the variance in the data

293

set, respectively. The Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin (KMO) measure of the sampling adequacy was

294

0.77 and the Bartlett's test of sphericity was also significant (χ2 (28) = 1725.53, and p < 0.01),

295

which indicates that the data set is appropriate for PCA.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

289

As listed in Table 2 for the factor loadings of the eight attributes, the PCA results showed

297

good agreement with the pleasantness-eventfulness model of soundscape perception proposed

298

in a previous study [49]. Pleasantness is associated with hedonic value of sound and

299

eventfulness is associated with variety of sounds or temporal structure of a soundscape. This

300

2D model of perceived soundscape quality has been widely used in soundscape studies

301

because it can provide comprehensive soundscape information for understanding

302

soundscapes.

305

EP

304

Table 2

AC C

303

TE D

296

306

To calculate the component scores for evaluation locations in the study area, the regression

307

method included in IBM SPSS 23 software was used and the component scores for

308

pleasantness and eventfulness were mapped over the study area for the three sampling

309

periods as shown in Fig. 4. The temporal variation for the pleasantness score (Fig. 4a) was

310

smaller than the eventfulness score (Fig. 4b) over the study area. 13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 311 312

Figure 4

313 Regarding the functions of urban spaces, higher pleasantness scores were observed in

315

recreational and residential areas, while the office district and high-density commercial areas

316

showed lower pleasantness scores (Fig. 5a). As shown in Fig. 5b, the high-density

317

commercial areas had the highest eventfulness score among the five types of urban functions

318

in the three sampling periods and peaked during period 3. The eventfulness scores in the low-

319

density commercial areas and urban recreational areas gradually decreased across the

320

sampling periods. Overall, the business districts and the residential areas were assessed as

321

uneventful soundscape for periods 1 and 2, but eventfulness score for the residential areas

322

slightly increased in period 3.

323

Figure 5

TE D

324

M AN U

SC

RI PT

314

Correlation coefficients between PCA component scores and perceived sound source types

326

were calculated as shown in Table 3. Perception of traffic sounds had strong and negative

327

correlation coefficients with the pleasantness scores for all sampling periods and no

328

significant correlations with eventfulness scores. The dominance of human activity sounds

329

was not associated with pleasantness score but showed significant and positive correlations

330

with eventfulness scores over the sampling periods. In addition, the correlation coefficients

331

for the sampling periods 2 (r=0.51, p<0.01) and 3 (r=0.55, p<0.01) were slightly larger than

332

for period 1 (r=0.42, p<0.01). Similar to the human sounds, perception of music sounds

333

showed positive correlation with eventfulness, but the correlation strength was less than

334

human sounds showing correlation coefficients of approximately 0.3 (p<0.01). There were

AC C

EP

325

14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT modest correlations between pleasantness scores and perception of water sounds over the

336

study area. The perceived dominance of birdsong showed significant correlations with

337

pleasantness scores. Interestingly, the correlations became weaker over the periods where the

338

highest correlation was 0.61 (p<0.01) in period 1 and the correlation declined by 0.38

339

(p<0.01) in period 3. This result was in good agreement with the temporal tendency of bird

340

sound dominance over the sampling periods.

342 3.1.3 Acoustic parameters

M AN U

343

Table 3

SC

341

RI PT

335

The calculated acoustic parameters in the three sampling periods for different urban spatial

345

functions are listed in Table 4. The mean value of LAeq ranged from 62.0 to 69.0 dB over the

346

study area, which fell below noise standards set by the Korean Ministry of Environment

347

(KMOE). Regarding the loudness of sound environment, the business district and commercial

348

areas showed higher LAeq and loudness values than the residential and recreational areas. The

349

LAeq and loudness values tended to decrease slightly from period 1 to period 3 except in high-

350

density commercial areas. The spectral characteristics of sound environment differed

351

according to the functions of space. The sound environment in the business districts and high-

352

density commercial areas showed higher LCeq-Aeq and lower sharpness values indicating that

353

main sound sources in the areas contained higher energy at low frequencies. In contrast, the

354

acoustic environment in the low-density commercial area had greater high-frequency content

355

with lower LCeq-Aeq and higher sharpness values. The residential and recreational areas

356

showed both relatively higher LCeq-Aeq and sharpness values compared with other urban areas.

357

Correlation coefficients among the acoustic parameters and identified sound sources were

358

calculated as listed in Table 5. Similar correlation tendencies between acoustic parameters

359

and perceived sound source types were found in a previous study [13]. Traffic noise was 15

AC C

EP

TE D

344

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT positively correlated with LAeq (r=0.55, p<0.01), loudness (r=0.53, p<0.01) and L10-90 (r=0.37,

361

p<0.01), whereas strong and negative relationships were found between traffic sound and

362

sharpness. This finding supports that road traffic sounds, containing significant low-

363

frequency energy, are the main sound source that increases the background noise level in

364

urban areas. The presence of human sounds (r=-0.31, p<0.01) and music (r=-0.26, p<0.01)

365

were negatively correlated to LCeq-Aeq indicating that sounds from human activities usually

366

consist of higher frequencies. In addition, human sounds showed weak correlation with LAeq

367

(r=0.17, p<0.01) and loudness (r=0.11, p<0.05) indicating that human sounds also contribute

368

to the increase in background sound levels during a certain period. Perception of bird sounds

369

was negatively correlated with overall loudness of the sound environment (LAeq and loudness)

370

and positively associated with sharpness. This finding demonstrates that urban green areas

371

with low traffic noise level could provide urban habitat for birds [55]. With respect to the

372

temporal variation, bird sounds showed large variations in the correlations with the acoustic

373

parameters according to sampling period; the correlation coefficients between the bird sounds

374

and the acoustic parameters decreased in period 3 due to the decreasing behavioral

375

frequencies in the period. This finding of temporal behavioral pattern of birds in urban areas

376

corresponds well with results of a previous study [43].

378 379 380

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

377

RI PT

360

Table 4 Table 5

381

3.2 Relationship between morphological indices and soundscape variables

382

3.2.1 Spatial variation of morphological indices

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT PCA was also conducted to characterize the 16 morphological variables. As in the previous

384

PCA, varimax rotation was applied to obtain the main components. The KMO measure of the

385

sampling adequacy was 0.53, and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (105) =

386

3952.25, and p < 0.01), which suggests that PCA with the 16 morphological indices is

387

feasible. Four components with Eigenvalues larger than 1 were found, as listed in Table 6.

388

The extracted components showed good agreement with our prior classification for 16

389

morphological indices. Component 1 (Open space) is highly related to variables representing

390

urban green and public areas. Component 2 (Building) represents morphological variables

391

associated with building areas and surfaces. Components 3 (Water feature) and 4 (Roads) are

392

associated with variables representing urban water features and road surfaces, respectively.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

383

393 394

Table 6

395

To understand the morphological characteristics of spaces with different functions, the

397

component scores for morphological variables were calculated using regression analysis. The

398

range of component scores varied by urban function as shown in Fig. 6. The business and

399

commercial areas in the study area can be characterized by higher building density than the

400

other urban functional areas. High-density commercial areas obtained higher component 2

401

and 3 scores than other areas indicating these areas consisted mainly of buildings and wide

402

roads. The low-density commercial areas showed relatively lower component 3 scores

403

implying these areas consisted of narrower roads. Residential areas showed small ranges and

404

neutral values for all component scores. Recreational areas showed higher values for

405

component 1 and lower values for component 2 than the other functional areas, indicating

406

recreational areas can be characterized as places with large open areas and low density of

AC C

EP

TE D

396

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 407

buildings. In addition, the range of component 3 values in the recreational areas was the

408

largest among the urban functions indicating roads are main morphological components in

409

urban recreational areas.

411

Figure 6

412 413

RI PT

410

3.2.2 Correlation between soundscape variables and morphological indices

The relationship between the identified sound source types and morphological indices were

415

explored by calculating the correlation coefficients (Table 7). As expected, traffic sounds

416

showed significant positive correlations with morphological factors related to road areas

417

(Rd(A) and RAF). Although the correlation was weaker than Rd(A) and RAF, negative

418

relationships were observed between perception of traffic sounds and morphological indices

419

for Open space (Op(A), Op(P), and OSR). Statistically significant correlations were not found

420

between traffic sounds and building factors.

TE D

M AN U

SC

414

Interestingly, human sounds mainly correlated with building-related components only for

422

period 3 and no significant relationships were observed for periods 1 and 2. In period 3, as

423

the morphological indices associated with the building area and perimeter increased, the

424

dominance of human sounds increased. This finding may be due to human activities

425

significantly increasing after working hours, especially in commercial areas, which mainly

426

consist of building components. This implies that the relationships between morphological

427

factors and human sounds depend on the temporal behavioral patterns of human activities.

428

Unlike human sounds, music sounds are consistently associated with building components

429

because music sounds are usually generated by shops in the commercial areas, which are

430

relatively independent from human activity.

AC C

EP

421

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Bird sounds were significantly associated with morphological variables for green and open

432

public spaces and had a negative correlation with road-related indices (Rd(A) and RAF).

433

Identification of water sounds was significantly correlated with the morphological indicators

434

related to water features (Wt(A) and Wt(P)). In addition, the variable associated with open

435

public space (Op(P) and OSR) strongly correlated with water sounds because water features

436

such as fountains are usually installed in urban public spaces such as urban squares. The

437

building-related indicators also had a negative relationship with water sounds; however, this

438

should be interpreted as an indirect relationship within the case study area because water

439

sounds were directly related to water features.

441

M AN U

440

SC

RI PT

431

442

3.2.3 Regression models using acoustic and morphological factors

443

3.2.3.1 Sound source models

Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to investigate the contributions of the

445

acoustic and urban morphological metrics prediction of the perceived sound sources in the

446

case study area. For the acoustic indicators, LAeq, LCeq-Aeq, L10-90 and sharpness values were

447

selected as independent variables. The loudness value was removed due to multicollinearity

448

with LAeq in the regression models. In terms of urban morphological indicators, four

449

component scores including Open public space, Buildings, Roads, and Water features were

450

used to reduce the number of morphological variables. The regression models were

451

developed using not only the total dataset but also datasets for each sampling period to

452

compare the temporal contributions of the input variables.

AC C

EP

TE D

444

453

The standardized regression coefficients (β) for perceived traffic, human, bird and water

454

sounds, and the coefficients of determination for the regression models are listed in Table 8. 19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT In terms of traffic noise, the regression model explained 44% to 55% of the variance in

456

perceived traffic noise. Among the acoustic indicators, the contributions of LAeq were the

457

greatest over the sampling periods. The contributions of the morphological components were

458

relatively smaller than of acoustic indicators. The Water feature component had a negative

459

relationship with traffic noise, whereas the Road component had positive contribution to the

460

perceived dominance of traffic noise; however, the strength was weak.

RI PT

455

The regression models for human sounds showed lower R2 values than other types of sounds

462

indicating that further efforts should be taken to develop better acoustic and morphological

463

indicators to accurately predict human sounds in urban areas. The regression model of period

464

1 explained approximately 14% of the variance in human sound, while R2 in periods 1 and 3

465

increased by 24% and 25%, respectively. Among the acoustic indicators, LCeq-Aeq,

466

representing the low-frequency sound content, had only a significant negative relationship

467

with human sounds in the sampling periods because human sounds usually contain higher

468

frequency energy. This finding was in good agreement with a previous study showing that

469

human-made sounds in urban environments were associated with frequency-related indicators

470

[12]. Regarding the morphological components, the Building component significantly

471

contributed to the prediction of human sounds only in sampling period 3. This is supported by

472

the observation that human activities were dramatically increased in the commercial districts

473

with high density of buildings in sampling period 3 in the case study area (Figs. 3 and 6). In

474

addition, the contribution of the Building component (β=0.35) was greater than LCeq-Aeq (β=-

475

0.26). This result also demonstrated that the contribution of urban morphological factors

476

depends on diurnal patterns of human sounds in urban environments.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

461

477

With respect to acoustic and morphological factors LAeq and Open space significantly

478

contributed to the explanation of perceived bird sounds in urban areas, respectively, in the 20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sampling periods. In contrast to the regression model for perceived human sounds, the R2

480

values of the regression models for bird sounds gradually decreased from 0.51 to 0.21 across

481

the three sampling periods. This result might be associated with the observed temporal

482

behavioral patterns of bird sounds in urban areas implying that temporal factors play

483

important roles in developing the prediction models, especially for biological sounds.

RI PT

479

The regression models for perceived water sounds explain more than 50% of the variance of

485

the identification of water sounds in the case study area. The acoustic indicators including

486

LAeq and sharpness showed significant relationships with perceived water sounds in period 1.

487

In terms of morphological factors, Water feature showed the most significant contribution for

488

predicting water sounds for all periods in the case study area. This finding supports the use of

489

morphological indicators for water features as reliable indicators for mapping the water

490

soundscape in urban areas [56,57] because in general, water sounds are generated from water

491

features such as fountains, rivers or sea, which are fixed at certain locations in urban areas.

494 495

M AN U

TE D

493

Table 8

EP

492

SC

484

3.2.3.2 Perceived affective quality models Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to predict pleasantness and eventfulness

497

scores using the acoustic and urban morphological indicators. Regression models for

498

pleasantness and eventfulness were developed and Table 9 summarizes the regression

499

analyses. The four acoustical and four urban morphological indicators were applied in the

500

same manner as in the perceived sound source model in the previous section.

AC C

496

501

Regarding the goodness of fit of the models, the pleasantness model showed significantly

502

higher R2 than the eventfulness model. The pleasantness model explained approximately 50%

503

of the variance, whereas the eventfulness model only predicted 13% of the variance. For the 21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT pleasantness model, LAeq was the most significant variable in terms of the acoustic

505

parameters. Regarding the morphological factors, the contribution of the water feature was

506

relatively stronger than the other morphological components even though the pleasantness

507

score showed higher correlation with perception of bird sounds than water sounds (Table 2).

508

Open space showed limited correlation with perceived bird sounds in the case study area

509

ranging from 0.32 to 0.40. Relationships between bird sounds and morphological indicators

510

related to urban green areas were also investigated in previous studies [42,43]; however, the

511

suggested indicators are limited to accurately predict birdsong in urban areas. Thus, more

512

reliable morphological indicators should be developed for bird sounds in the future.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

504

For the eventfulness model, the effects of LAeq and LCeq-Aeq were statistically significant

514

among the acoustic parameters, while the contribution of the Building component was only

515

significant among the morphological components. In general, eventfulness of soundscape was

516

closely associated with presence of human sounds, which was found dependent on temporal

517

patterns of human activity in urban areas. Reportedly, the eventfulness of soundscape plays a

518

vital role in judging appropriateness of soundscape in multifunctional urban spaces [9,25].

519

Thus, in the future, investigating relationships between soundscapes and the indicators

520

representing human activities, spatial configurations or building functions is necessary to

521

establish better eventfulness models.

523 524

EP

AC C

522

TE D

513

Table 9

525

Conclusion

526

In the present study, the relationships between the acoustic, urban morphological indicators

527

and spatiotemporal characteristics of soundscapes were explored in multifunctional urban 22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT areas in order to provide insight for soundscape planning and management in an urban

529

environment. Both physical acoustic data and perceptual soundscape data were collected by

530

observers at 122 locations over the three sampling periods from morning to evening in the

531

case study area. Morphological indicators relating to buildings, roads, open public spaces,

532

and water feature components were calculated in order to measure the urban forms.

RI PT

528

The soundscapes varied spatially and temporally in the urban area throughout a day. In

534

terms of spatial variations, urban soundscapes were influenced by the main function of the

535

spaces. The temporal variability of soundscapes in urban spaces was affected by diurnal

536

patterns in sound sources. Traffic and water sounds had relatively constant correlations with

537

acoustic and morphological indicators over the course of the day as sound sources such as

538

roads and water features are usually fixed in urban areas. In contrast, the dominance of

539

birdsong and sounds from human activities varied throughout the day owing to greater

540

temporal variation in biological and anthropogenic activity in urban areas.. This indicates that

541

temporal patterns in various sound sources can play critical roles in creating soundscapes in

542

urban areas.

TE D

M AN U

SC

533

Regression models for pleasantness and eventfulness were proposed based on acoustic and

544

morphological indicators, and the results revealed that the application of a combination of

545

acoustic and morphological factors could be a good approach for developing soundscape

546

prediction models in urban spaces. Pleasantness model was developed using LAeq, open space

547

and water feature components, which explain approximately 50% of the variance in the

548

dataset. However, the eventfulness model had some limitations in its explanation of the

549

variance of the collected soundscape data in this study, which indicates that further studies

550

will be required to investigate soundscape indicators for eventfulness. In particular,

551

soundscape indicators for bird and human activities in urban spaces, which are closely related

AC C

EP

543

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 552

to the pleasantness and eventfulness of soundscapes, respectively, should be explored in order

553

to improve soundscape prediction models. The present study has some inherent limitations. One limitation is the use of a small number

555

of observers in their 20s, whose observations may not be directly transferable to general

556

results. In order to generalize the results, large soundscape data collection from various age

557

groups is essential. Furthermore, the findings in this study are limited to the case study area

558

of Seoul, Korea. The urban density and morphology of typical western and eastern cities are

559

significantly different. Thus, cross-national comparative studies on relationships between

560

urban morphology and its soundscape should be conducted in the future. Despite these

561

limitations, this study has significantly enhanced our understanding of the relationships

562

between the urban morphology and soundscapes in urban areas, which will aid in urban

563

soundscape planning and design.

AC C

EP

TE D

564

M AN U

SC

RI PT

554

24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 565 566

References [1]

J. Corburn, Toward the Healthy City: People, Places, and the Politics of Urban Planning, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1468-

568

2427.2011.01040_3.x.

569

[2]

570

RI PT

567

J. Kang, B. Schulte-Fortkamp, Soundscape and the Built Environment, CRC Press, London, UK, 2015.

[3]

B. Berglund, T. Lindvall, D. Schwela, Guidelines for community noise, Geneva, 1999.

572

[4]

H.M.E. Miedema, Annoyance caused by environmental noise: Elements for evidence-

SC

571

based noise policies, J. Soc. Issues. 63 (2007) 41–57. doi:10.1111/j.1540-

574

4560.2007.00495.x.

575

[5]

M AN U

573

C. Marquis-Favre, E. Premat, D. Aubree, Noise and its Effects – A Review on Qualitative Aspects of Sound. Part I: Notions and Acoustic Ratings, Acta Acoust.

577

United With Acoust. 91 (2005) 613–625.

578

[6]

TE D

576

J. Kang, F. Aletta, T.T. Gjestland, L.A. Brown, D. Botteldooren, B. Schulte-fortkamp, P. Lercher, I. Van Kamp, K. Genuit, J. Luis, B. Coelho, L. Maffei, L. Lavia, Ten

580

questions on the soundscapes of the built environment, Build. Environ. 108 (2016)

581

284–294. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.08.011. [7]

583 584

Destiny Books, New York, 1977.

[8]

585 586

R. Schafer, The soundscape: our sonic environment and the tuning of the world,

AC C

582

EP

579

ISO 12913-1, Acoustics — Soundscape — Part 1 : Definition and conceptual

framework, Geneva, Switzerland, 2014.

[9]

F. Aletta, J. Kang, Ö. Axelsson, Soundscape descriptors and a conceptual framework

587

for developing predictive soundscape models, Landsc. Urban Plan. 149 (2016) 65–74.

588

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.02.001. 25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

590 591 592 593

[10] J. Kang, M. Zhang, Semantic differential analysis of the soundscape in urban open public spaces, Build. Environ. 45 (2010) 150–157. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2009.05.014. [11] B.M. Brooks, B. Schulte-Fortkamp, K.S. Voigt, A.U. Case, Exploring our sonic environment through soundscape research and theory, Acoust. Today. 10 (2014) 30–40.

RI PT

589

[12] J.Y. Jeon, J.Y. Hong, Classification of urban park soundscapes through perceptions of the acoustical environments, Landsc. Urban Plan. 141 (2015) 100–111.

595

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.005.

596

SC

594

[13] J. Liu, J. Kang, Soundscape design in city parks: exploring the relationships between soundscape composition parameters and physical and psychoacoustic parameters, J.

598

Environ. Eng. Landsc. Manag. 23 (2015) 102–112.

599

doi:10.3846/16486897.2014.998676.

602 603 604

Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 903–911.

TE D

601

[14] M. Nilsson, B. Berglund, Soundscape quality in suburban green areas and city parks,

[15] W. Yang, J. Kang, Soundscape and Sound Preferences in Urban Squares: A Case Study in Sheffield, J. Urban Des. 10 (2005) 61–80. doi:10.1080/13574800500062395. [16] L. Yu, J. Kang, Modeling subjective evaluation of soundscape quality in urban open

EP

600

M AN U

597

spaces: An artificial neural network approach., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 126 (2009) 1163–

606

1174. doi:10.1121/1.3183377.

607 608 609

AC C

605

[17] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Designing sound and visual components for enhancement of urban soundscapes, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 (2013) 2026–2036.

[18] Q. Meng, J. Kang, Effect of sound-related activities on human behaviours and acoustic

610

comfort in urban open spaces, Sci. Total Environ. 573 (2016) 481–493.

611

doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2016.08.130.

612

[19] Q. Meng, Y. Sun, J. Kang, Effect of temporary open-air markets on the sound 26

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 613

environment and acoustic perception based on the crowd density characteristics, Sci.

614

Total Environ. 601–602 (2017) 1488–1495. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2017.06.017.

616 617

[20] M. Yang, J. Kang, Psychoacoustical evaluation of natural and urban sounds in soundscapes., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134 (2013) 840–51. doi:10.1121/1.4807800. [21] M. Rychtáriková, G. Vermeir, Soundscape categorization on the basis of objective acoustical parameters, Appl. Acoust. 74 (2013) 240–247.

619

doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2011.01.004.

622 623

[22] H. Fastl, E. Zwicker, Psychoacoustics, Springer Berlin Heidelberg, New York, NY, 2006.

M AN U

621

SC

618

620

RI PT

615

[23] K. Genuit, A. Fiebig, Psychoacoustics and its benefit for the soundscape approach, Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 952–958.

[24] K. Herranz-Pascual, I. Aspuru, I. García, Proposed conceptual model of environmental

625

experience as framework to study the soundscape, in: Proc. Internoise, Lisbon,

626

Portugal, 2010: pp. 1–10.

TE D

624

[25] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Influence of urban contexts on soundscape perceptions: A

628

structural equation modeling approach, Landsc. Urban Plan. 141 (2015) 78–87.

629

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.004.

631 632 633

[26] L. Adolphe, A simplified model of urban morphology: Application to an analysis of

AC C

630

EP

627

the environmental performance of cities, Environ. Plan. B Plan. Des. 28 (2001) 183– 200. doi:10.1068/b2631.

[27] J.E. Nichol, High-Resolution Surface Temperature Patterns Related to Urban

634

Morphology in a Tropical City: A Satellite-Based Study, J. Appl. Meteorol. 35 (1996)

635

135–146. doi:10.1175/1520-0450(1996)035<0135:HRSTPR>2.0.CO;2.

636

[28] E. Ng, C. Yuan, L. Chen, C. Ren, J.C.H. Fung, Improving the wind environment in 27

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 637

high-density cities by understanding urban morphology and surface roughness: A

638

study in Hong Kong, Landsc. Urban Plan. 101 (2011) 59–74.

639

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.01.004.

641 642

[29] M. Hornikx, Ten questions concerning computational urban acoustics, Build. Environ. 106 (2016) 409–421. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.06.028.

RI PT

640

[30] C. Zhao, G. Fu, X. Liu, F. Fu, Urban planning indicators, morphology and climate indicators: A case study for a north-south transect of Beijing, China, Build. Environ.

644

46 (2011) 1174–1183. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2010.12.009.

[31] C. Yuan, E. Ng, L.K. Norford, Improving air quality in high-density cities by

M AN U

645

SC

643

646

understanding the relationship between air pollutant dispersion and urban

647

morphologies, Build. Environ. 71 (2014) 245–258.

648

doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2013.10.008.

649

[32] A. Gospodini, Urban Design , Urban Space Morphology , Urban Tourism : An Emerging New Paradigm Concerning, Eur. Plan. Stud. 9 (2001) 925–934.

651

doi:10.1080/0965431012007984.

653

Urban Morphology, Trans. GIS. 6 (2002) 295–309. doi:10.1111/1467-9671.00112. [34] A. Rapoport, Human aspects of urban form: towards a man: environment approach to

657

AC C

654

[33] B. Jiang, C. Claramunt, Integration of Space Syntax into GIS: New Perspectives for

EP

652

TE D

650

658

(2011) 66–76. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2011.10.018.

655 656

659 660

urban form and design, 1st ed., Pergamon press, New York, 1977.

[35] I.C.M. Guedes, S.R. Bertoli, P.H.T. Zannin, Influence of urban shapes on environmental noise: A case study in Aracaju - Brazil, Sci. Total Environ. 412–413

[36] H. Ryu, I.K. Park, B.S. Chun, S. Il Chang, Spatial statistical analysis of the effects of urban form indicators on road-traffic noise exposure of a city in South Korea, Appl. 28

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 661

Acoust. 115 (2017) 93–100. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2016.08.025. [37] E.M. Salomons, M. Berghauser Pont, Urban traffic noise and the relation to urban

663

density, form, and traffic elasticity, Landsc. Urban Plan. 108 (2012) 2–16.

664

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2012.06.017.

665

RI PT

662

[38] G. Rey Gozalo, J.M. Barrigón Morillas, J. Trujillo Carmona, D. Montes González, P. Atanasio Moraga, V. Gómez Escobar, R. Vílchez-Gómez, J.A. Méndez Sierra, C.

667

Prieto-Gajardo, Study on the relation between urban planning and noise level, Appl.

668

Acoust. 111 (2016) 143–147. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2016.04.018.

[39] B. Wang, J. Kang, Effects of urban morphology on the traffic noise distribution

M AN U

669

SC

666

670

through noise mapping: A comparative study between UK and China, Appl. Acoust.

671

72 (2011) 556–568. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2011.01.011.

672

[40] Y. Hao, J. Kang, Influence of mesoscale urban morphology on the spatial noise attenuation of flyover aircrafts, Appl. Acoust. 84 (2014) 73–82.

674

doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2013.12.001.

675

TE D

673

[41] F. Qu, J. Kang, Effects of built environment morphology in residential areas on resisting wind turbine noise on building façades, Renew. Energy. 107 (2017) 629–638.

677

doi:10.1016/j.renene.2017.02.037.

679 680

[42] Y. Hao, J. Kang, J.D. Krijnders, Integrated effects of urban morphology on birdsong

AC C

678

EP

676

loudness and visibility of green areas, Landsc. Urban Plan. 137 (2015) 149–162. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.01.006.

681

[43] J. Liu, J. Kang, H. Behm, Birdsong As an Element of the Urban Sound Environment:

682

A Case Study Concerning the Area of Warnemünde in Germany, Acta Acust. United

683

with Acust. 100 (2014) 458–466. doi:10.3813/AAA.918726.

684

[44] J. Ge, J. Lu, K. Morotomi, K. Hokao, Developing Soundscapegraphy for the Notation 29

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 685

of Urban Soundscape: Its Concept, Method, Analysis and Application, Acta Acust.

686

United with Acust. 95 (2009) 65–75. doi:10.3813/AAA.918128.

687

[45] J. Liu, J. Kang, T. Luo, H. Behm, T. Coppack, Spatiotemporal variability of soundscapes in a multiple functional urban area, Landsc. Urban Plan. 115 (2013) 1–9.

689

doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.03.008.

690

RI PT

688

[46] A.D. Mazaris, A.S. Kallimanis, G. Chatzigianidis, K. Papadimitriou, J.D. Pantis,

Spatiotemporal analysis of an acoustic environment: interactions between landscape

692

features and sounds, Landsc. Ecol. 24 (2009) 817–831. doi:10.1007/s10980-009-9360-

693

x.

M AN U

694

SC

691

[47] E. Margaritis, J. Kang, Relationship between green space-related morphology and

695

noise pollution, Ecol. Indic. 72 (2017) 921–933. doi:10.1016/j.ecolind.2016.09.032.

696

[48] J.Y. Hong, J.Y. Jeon, Exploring spatial relationships among soundscape variables in urban areas: A spatial statistical modelling approach, Landsc. Urban Plan. 157 (2017)

698

352–364. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.08.006.

TE D

697

[49] Ö. Axelsson, M.E. Nilsson, B. Berglund, A principal components model of soundscape

700

perception., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 128 (2010) 2836–2846. doi:10.1121/1.3493436.

EP

699

[50] G.G. Koch, Intraclass correlation coefficient, Encycl. Stat. Sci. 4 (1982) 213–217.

702

[51] B. De Coensel, D. Botteldooren, The quiet rural soundscape and how to characterize it,

704

[52] D. 45631/A1, Calculation of loudness level and loudness from the sound spectrum -

705

AC C

701

Zwicker method - Amendment 1: Calculation of the loudness of time-variant sound,

706

2008.

703

707 708

Acta Acust. United with Acust. 92 (2006) 887–897.

[53] 45692 DIN, Measurement Technique for the simulation of the auditory sensation of sharpness, 2009. 30

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 709

[54] Y. Gan, T. Luo, W. Breitung, J. Kang, T. Zhang, Multi-sensory landscape assessment:

710

The contribution of acoustic perception to landscape evaluation, J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

711

136 (2014) 3200–3210. doi:10.1121/1.4898424. [55] E. Fernandez-Juricic, J. Jokimäki, A habitat island approach to conserving birds in

RI PT

712 713

urban landscapes: case studies from southern and northern Europe, Biodivers. Conserv.

714

10 (2001) 2023–2043.

717 718

SC

716

[56] F. Aletta, J. Kang, Soundscape approach integrating noise mapping techniques: a case study in Brighton, UK, Noise Mapp. 2 (2015). doi:10.1515/noise-2015-0001. [57] F.M.A. Calarco, L. Galbrun, Soundscape design and over road traffic noise mapping of

M AN U

715

water features used, in: Internoise 2015, San Francisco, USA, 2015: pp. 1–11.

AC C

EP

TE D

719

31

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 1. Calculation of urban morphological parameters. (A) and (P) denote area and perimeter, respectively. Indicators Definition

Formula

Sum of building area

Bldg (P)

Sum of building perimeter

Bldsf (A)

Sum of building surface area

BPAF

The ratio of the plan area of buildings to the total surface area The summed area of roughness elements and exposed ground divided by the total surface area of the study region

0.00 – 144543.70



0.00 – 4819.35

M AN U

SC



Sum of green area

Gr (P)

Sum of green perimeter

Op (A)

Sum of open public area including urban squares, green and water areas

Op (P)

Sum of open perimeter including urban squares, green and water feature areas

TE D

Gr (A)

The ratio of the open area divided by the total surface area of the study region

Grd (A)

Sum of exposed ground area

Rd (A)

Sum of road area

EP

OSR



0.00 – 10387198

( ) ( )

0.00 – 0.64

( ) ( )

0.22 – 4.82

0.00 – 1575.57







0.00 – 557.03



0.00 – 15754.57





0.00 – 686.20

( ) ( )

0.00 – 0.70 630.29 – 17416.47





Wt (A)

Sum of water feature area

Wt (P)

Sum of water feature perimeter

AC C

RAF

The ratio of the exposed ground area divided by the total surface area of the study region The ratio of the road area to the study region

EGR



RI PT

Bldg (A)

CAR

Range

32.07 – 14765.52

( ) ( )

0.03 – 0.77

( ) ( )

0.00 – 0.66



32

!

0.00 – 2106.92



0.00 – 342.11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 2. Rotated component matrices of the PCA using semantic attribute responses (numbers in parentheses represent explained variance) Component 2 (30.8%)

0.13 0.00 -0.34 0.09

0.88 -0.92 0.80 -0.66

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

0.91 -0.84 -0.75 0.89

RI PT

Component 1 (41.3%)

SC

Attributes Pleasantness Pleasant Unpleasant Calm Chaotic Eventfulness Eventful Uneventful Monotonous Exciting

33

0.03 0.11 0.18 0.03

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 3. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and perceptual components of soundscapes for the three sampling periods (*p<0.05, **p<0.01) Water 0.17* 0.24** 0.29** 0.23** 0.11 0.08 0.04 0.08

M AN U TE D EP AC C 34

Bird 0.61** 0.54** 0.38** 0.51** -0.12 -0.10 0.06 -0.05

RI PT

Music 0.18* 0.03 -0.01 0.07 0.30** 0.37** 0.36** 0.34**

SC

Period Traffic Human ** Pleasantness P1 -0.65 -0.02 ** P2 -0.64 -0.07 P3 -0.65** -0.08 ** Total -0.64 -0.05 Eventfulness P1 -0.06 0.42** P2 0.01 0.51** P3 -0.07 0.55** Total -0.04 0.49** P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 4. Mean values of acoustic parameters of the three sampling periods

P2 P3 High-density

P1

Commercial P2 P3 Low-density Commercial

P1 P2 P3

Residential

P1

L10-90 [dB]

Loudness [sone]

Sharpness [acum]

Mean

67.88

9.91

6.90

25.03

1.69

SD Mean SD Mean SD

4.47 68.49 4.32 66.50 4.76

2.92 9.98 3.23 9.65 3.04

3.23 7.13 2.76 6.82 3.37

6.63 25.46 6.46 22.67 6.58

0.12 1.70 0.18 1.72 0.15

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

69.00 5.47 69.29 4.82 69.72 4.66

8.45 2.62 9.18 2.74 7.47 3.03

6.38 2.64 6.22 2.22 6.42 3.30

26.18 7.69 27.36 8.29 26.66 6.43

1.66 0.11 1.65 0.15 1.66 0.16

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

67.44 5.46 67.13 6.24 66.78 4.73

7.58 3.36 6.84 3.22 6.32 2.57

9.12 3.39 8.34 3.28 8.79 4.80

22.48 5.96 22.25 6.85 21.53 6.68

1.85 0.13 1.90 0.19 1.90 0.22

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

63.20 6.29 63.73 5.41 61.95 5.66

9.81 2.55 10.27 3.44 8.69 3.00

8.17 3.27 7.40 3.40 8.04 3.16

19.09 8.17 18.58 7.45 16.97 6.06

1.93 0.25 1.92 0.30 1.97 0.25

6.38 3.31 5.36 2.49 5.19 3.28

22.24 9.10 20.19 6.97 18.67 7.34

1.86 0.27 1.83 0.25 1.93 0.31

EP

P2

LCeq-Aeq [dB]

AC C

P3 Recreational

RI PT

P1

LAeq [dB]

SC

Business

Statistics

M AN U

Period

TE D

Functions

P1

Mean 65.62 9.59 SD 6.13 3.62 P2 Mean 64.08 9.36 SD 5.62 3.15 P3 Mean 62.96 8.43 SD 6.71 2.88 P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)

35

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 5. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and acoustic parameters (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)

M AN U

TE D

EP AC C 36

Sharpness -0.48** -0.52** -0.56** -0.52** 0.07 -0.11 -0.22* -0.09 0.00 -0.04 0.00 -0.01 0.58** 0.58** 0.30** 0.46** 0.34** 0.20* 0.14 0.21**

RI PT

Loudness 0.52** 0.56** 0.51** 0.53** 0.02 0.19* 0.13 0.11* -0.07 0.08 0.17 0.06 -0.54** -0.53** -0.31** -0.44** 0.10 0.11 -0.02 0.07

SC

Period LAeq LCeq-Aeq L10-90 ** Traffic P1 0.55 -0.13 0.34** ** P2 0.58 0.03 0.38** ** P3 0.52 0.16 0.41** Total 0.55** 0.03 0.37** Human P1 0.11 -0.34** 0.11 * P2 0.22 -0.35** 0.04 * ** P3 0.19 -0.24 0.04 Total 0.17** -0.31** 0.06 ** Music P1 0.02 -0.29 -0.03 P2 0.10 -0.25** -0.09 ** P3 0.15 -0.25 -0.08 Total 0.09 -0.26** -0.07 ** Bird P1 -0.55 0.12 -0.04 P2 -0.59** 0.01 -0.20* P3 -0.36** 0.13 -0.11 Total -0.47** 0.10 -0.10 Water P1 0.03 0.01 -0.28** P2 0.08 -0.08 -0.23* P3 -0.09 -0.06 -0.25** Total 0.01 -0.04 -0.25** P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 6. Rotated component matrices of the PCA using morphological indices (numbers in parentheses represent explained variance) 2 (19.44%)

3 (16.81%)

4 (10.54%)

0.89 0.84 0.90 0.77 0.89

-0.11 -0.16 -0.29 -0.40 -0.32

-0.04 -0.10 0.10 0.08 0.11

-0.09 -0.09 0.04 0.31 0.17

-0.18 -0.30 -0.09 -0.18 -0.36

0.91 0.87 0.75 0.94 0.81

-0.10 0.06 0.06 -0.11 -0.06

-0.19 -0.09 -0.14 -0.13 -0.01

-0.20 -0.21

0.10 0.09

0.96 0.96

-0.18 -0.17 -0.27 -0.25

0.90 -0.79 0.92 -0.85

-0.16 -0.30 0.12 -0.14

TE D

-0.23 -0.39 -0.22 -0.42

SC

M AN U

0.04 0.02

RI PT

1 (41.29%)

AC C

EP

Component Open space Gr (A) Gr (P) Op (A) Op (P) OSR Building Bldg (A) Bldsf (A) Bldg (P) BPAF CAR Water feature Wt (A) Wt (P) Road Rd (A) Grd (A) RAF EGR

37

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 7. Correlation coefficients between perceived sound sources and morphological indices (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)

P2 P3

0.05

0.04

-0.05

-0.09

-0.01 0.00 -0.12

0.03

0.03

0.01

-0.01

-0.05

P1

0.18*

0.06

0.21*

0.13

0.02

-0.19

*

-0.15 -0.12 -0.15

**

-0.01

Gr(P)

Op(A)

Op(P)

-0.15

-0.20

*

*

-0.22

*

-0.11 -0.11 -0.12

*

-0.06

-0.14 -0.18

**

-0.13

-0.20

OSR -0.22

*

Wt(A)

Wt(P)

Rd(A)

*

*

0.45

**

-0.27

**

-0.18 -0.22

*

**

-0.21*

-0.25

**

0.45

**

0.45

**

0.45

**

-0.11

-0.06 *

-0.20

-0.16

-0.21

**

-0.14

-0.23

**

-0.20

-0.21

*

**

-0.13

-0.20 -0.24

**

-0.22 -0.22

*

**

Grd(A)

RAF

EGR

0.36

**

-0.14

0.34

**

-0.13

0.37

**

-0.12

-0.06

0.36

**

-0.13*

0.13

-0.06 -0.05 -0.05

0.10

0.16

-0.26**

0.11

0.15

0.11

0.04

0.06

0.03

0.02

0.00

0.02

0.02

0.01

0.18

P3

0.32**

0.37**

0.30**

0.32**

0.32**

-0.14

-0.18*

-0.11

-0.14

-0.11

-0.06

-0.06

0.08

-0.20*

0.07

-0.23*

Total

0.22**

0.18**

0.22**

0.19**

0.13*

-0.03

-0.07

-0.08

-0.12*

-0.08

-0.06

-0.06

0.06

-0.09

0.04

-0.13*

P1

0.20*

0.18*

0.25**

0.20*

0.19*

-0.12

-0.14

-0.12

-0.08

-0.10

0.11

0.09

-0.19*

0.09

-0.18

0.09

P2

*

*

0.27

**

0.17

0.16

-0.11

-0.09

-0.07

-0.07

-0.06

0.02

0.01

-0.09

0.02

-0.09

0.00

0.47

**

-0.19

*

-0.10

-0.11

-0.05

-0.01

-0.11

-0.06

0.33

**

-0.12

*

-0.01

*

0.03

*

0.00

-0.42

**

0.09

-0.32

**

0.04

-0.25

**

-0.04

-0.33

**

0.04

P1 P2 P3

0.41

**

0.27

**

-0.02 -0.10 -0.03

Total

-0.05

P1

-0.36**

P2

-0.31

**

-0.33

**

-0.33

**

P3 Total

0.19 0.40

**

0.26

**

-0.18 -0.24

**

-0.13 -0.18

**

-0.01 0.04

0.40

**

0.26

**

-0.06 -0.14

-0.01

-0.04

0.01

-0.08

-0.30**

-0.27**

-0.35**

-0.24

**

**

**

-0.32

**

-0.28

**

-0.26

-0.21 -0.25

-0.28

0.36

**

-0.13

0.23

**

*

*

**

-0.31

**

-0.31

**

-0.12

M AN U

0.14

0.19

-0.21 -0.28

*

**

-0.15 -0.21

**

0.42

**

0.41

**

0.31

**

0.37

**

-0.27**

0.13

*

-0.17

**

*

-0.20

-0.14 0.37

**

0.36

**

0.20

0.32

*

**

0.16

-0.12 0.43

**

0.45

**

0.38

**

0.40

**

0.25

**

0.30

**

0.42 0.36

**

0.38

**

-0.02

-0.04

-0.28

0.08

-0.24**

0.24**

-0.15

0.04

-0.24

**

**

-0.16

-0.25

**

-0.24

**

-0.04 -0.01 -0.02

-0.06 -0.02 -0.04

0.30

**

*

0.69

**

0.68

**

0.36

**

0.73

**

0.74

**

0.34

**

0.68

**

0.68

**

0.11

-0.28

0.20

0.13

*

-0.25

**

0.13

*

*

0.23 0.17

**

0.20 0.33

**

0.25

**

-0.35

**

-0.28

**

-0.12

**

0.62**

0.07

-0.11

0.39

0.62**

*

38

0.34

**

0.00

**

0.25**

0.06

P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)

0.32

**

0.36**

**

0.12

-0.13

*

0.19*

*

-0.19

-0.24

**

-0.20

-0.10

*

P2

Total

Water

-0.05

0.00

Total

P3 Bird

-0.04

0.09

0.02

Gr(A)

TE D

Music

0.09

0.06

CAR

EP

Human

0.04

BPAF

RI PT

P1

Bldg(P)

AC C

Traffic

Bldsf(A)

SC

Bldg(A)

-0.22

*

**

-0.08 0.02

0.14 0.07 -0.02 0.07

0.23

0.14 0.20

**

-0.16 -0.15**

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 8. Standardized regression coefficients from multiple linear regression analysis for sound sources using acoustic and morphological indicators (*p<0.05, **p<0.01)

P1

P2

P3

Total

P1

P2

P3

Total

P1

P2

P3

Total

P1

P2

P3

0.47

0.44

0.49

0.55

0.15

0.14

0.24

0.25

0.32

0.51

0.46

0.21

0.51

0.62

0.51

0.57

0.45

0.36

0.48

0.47

0.03

0.03

0.05

-0.01

-0.37

-0.34

-0.53

-0.27

0.14

0.44

0.19

0.01

**

**

**

**

**

**

**

*

*

**

*

0.23

0.13

0.19

0.34

-0.33

-0.33

-0.41

-0.26

0.00

0.05

-0.20

0.10

-0.11

0.05

-0.11

-0.09

*

**

**

**

**

**

0.17

0.27

-0.09

-0.08

-0.10

-0.10

0.10

0.19

-0.03

0.11

-0.17

-0.20

-0.16

-0.13

*

**

**

*

Sharpness

-0.02

-0.02

0.12

0.59

0.06

0.00

**

**

0.13 ** -0.24

0.01

0.10

0.16

-0.20

-0.23

-0.21

**

**

**

**

0.08

0.15

0.08

0.04

0.19

0.14

**

-0.03

-0.16

Morphological

Road Water feature

-0.11

-0.06

0.02

*

-0.08

-0.04

-0.02

*

0.14

0.15

**

0.14 *

0.05

0.02

-0.18

0.17

**

**

-0.04 0.09

EP

Building

-0.07

0.12

0.11

AC C

Open space

-0.10

M AN U

L10-90

**

-0.23

-0.22

-0.23

-0.23

**

**

**

**

-0.14

TE D

LCeq-Aeq

Water

Total

Acoustic LAeq

Bird

-0.04

SC

R2

Human

RI PT

Traffic

-0.05

0.10 0.09

-0.19

0.03

*

-0.04 0.35

0.30

-0.04

-0.02

**

0.30

0.28

**

**

-0.01

0.00

0.29 ** -0.07

0.28 ** 0.06

**

0.24

0.16

-0.10

-0.09

-0.04

-0.06

*

-0.09

0.01

P1 (09:00-11:00), P2 (13:00-15:00) and P3 (18:00-20:00)

39

-0.01

-0.02

*

*

-0.01

-0.07

0.02

0.59

0.50

0.61

0.67

**

**

**

**

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 9. Standardized regression coefficients from multiple linear regression analysis of soundscape perception using acoustic and morphological indicators (*p<0.05, **p<0.01) Pleasantness Total

P1

P2

P3

Total

P1

P2

P3

0.49

0.54

0.53

0.50

0.13

0.08

0.22

0.21

-0.67

-0.45

-0.65

-0.81

0.22

**

**

**

**

**

-0.05

0.13

-0.10

-0.11

-0.17

-0.30

-0.24

**

*

*

-0.11

-0.09

-0.15

-0.09

-0.09

-0.05

-0.04

-0.01

0.17

-0.01

Acoustic LAeq LCeq-Aeq

0.12

L10-90

0.16

0.06

0.13

0.27

-0.07

-0.13

-0.03

**

Morphological Open space Building

0.12 0.00 0.00

Road

0.14

-0.01

0.05

0.04

-0.02

-0.06

-0.02

0.09

0.07

0.26

0.24

0.27

0.25

**

**

**

**

EP AC C 40

0.32

0.17

**

*

TE D

Water feature

0.12

**

-0.06

M AN U

Sharpness

0.02

SC

*

RI PT

R

2

Eventfulness

-0.04

*

0.11

-0.06

0.11

*

0.10 0.10

0.25 **

0.05

0.08

0.23 *

0.09

0.10

0.14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure captions Figure 1. Case study area in Seoul, Korea: (a) 122 sampling locations of the study area and sampling zone A-I for soundscape data collection; (b) morphological components.

RI PT

Figure 2. Spatiotemporal variation in perceived sound source types for three different sampling periods: (a) traffic, (b) human, (c) music, (d) bird, and (e) water sounds Figure 3. Temporal variation in the dominant sound source types for three different sampling periods and the main place functions: (a) traffic, (b) human, (c) music, (d) bird, and (e) water sounds. The perceived value of sound sources refer to the percentages of records that indicated “Heard a lot” or “Dominates completely” for a given sound source type.

SC

Figure 4. Spatiotemporal variation of: (a) pleasantness and (b) eventfulness for the three sampling periods

M AN U

Figure 5. Temporal variation of the mean PCA component scores for the three sampling periods and the main functions of places: (a) pleasantness and (b) eventfulness scores

AC C

EP

TE D

Figure 6. PCA component scores for morphological factors according to the main space functions

41

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Appendix A: Questionnaire for soundscape data collection (Translated from Korean) 1. To what extent do you presently hear the following types of sounds? Please tick off one response alternative per type of sound A little

Moderately













□ □ □ □ □

□ □ □ □ □

□ □ □ □ □

2. Sounds from human activities 3. Natural sounds - Water sounds - Bird sounds - Wind sounds 4. Music 5. Other sounds

Dominates completely









□ □ □ □ □

□ □ □ □ □

SC

1. Traffic noise

A lot

RI PT

Do not hear at all

M AN U

Type of sounds

2. To what extent do you agree with eight attributes below on how you experience the present surrounding sound environment? Please tick off one response alterative per attribute. Slightly Disagree

Neither disagree, nor agree

Slightly Agree

Strongly Agree



















































Monotonous











Calm





















Unpleasant Eventful Uneventful

AC C

Exciting

TE D

Pleasant

Strongly Disagree

EP

The sound environment is:

Chaotic

42

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

B

C

E

F

D

Seoul

Sampling zones: A-I (a)

TE D

H

I

EP

Residential area High-density commercial area Low density commercial area Central business area Urban public space

AC C

Sampling locations

G

M AN U

A

SC

Korea

(b)

Green / park

Square

City stream

Building

Ground

Road

Sampling period 1: 09-11

Sampling period 2: 13-15

Sampling period 3: 18-20

SC M AN U TE D EP AC C

(e) Water sounds

(d) Bird sounds

(c) Musicsounds sounds Human

(b) Human sounds

RI PT

(a) Traffic sounds

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

1.0-1.4 1.4-1.8 1.8-2.2 2.2-2.6 2.6-3.0 3.0-3.4 3.4-3.8 3.8-4.2 4.2-4.6 4.6-5.0

Not at all heard

Dominates completely

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

80

80

30 20

50 40 30 20 10

0

0 P1

P2 Sampling period

P3

P1 80

80

P2 Sampling period

60 50

50

30 20

P1

P2 Sampling period

P3

20

P1

P2 Sampling period

Low-density commercial Residential

40

Recreational

30

Sampling periods

20

P1(09:00-11:00) P2(13:00-15:00) P3(18:00-20:00)

0

0

30

High-density commercial

10

10

40

Business

AC C

40

60

50

Space functions

EP

Prominant sound source [%]

70

60

0 P3

(e) Water 70

70

10

TE D

(d) Bird

Prominant sound source [%]

40

RI PT

50

60

SC

60

(c) Music

70

M AN U

70

10

Prominant sound source [%]

80 (b) Human

Prominant sound source [%]

Prominant sound source [%]

(a) Traffic

P1

P2 Sampling period

P3

P3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Legend

Pleasant

Uneventful

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

Unpleasant

AC C

(b) Eventfulness score

Sampling period 3: 18-20

RI PT

Sampling period 2: 13-15

(a) Pleasantness score

Sampling period 1: 09-11

Eventful

(a)

1.0

1.0

(b)

SC

1.5

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

M AN U

0.0

Business Low-density commercial Recreational

P1

High-density commercial Residential

EP

-1.5

TE D

-0.5

-1.0

Eventfulness score

0.5

P2

AC C

Pleasantness score

0.5

Sampling period

P3

0.0

-0.5

-1.0

Business Low-density commercial Recreational

High-density commercial Residential

-1.5 P1

P2 Sampling period

P3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

RI PT

6 Open space Building Road

SC

Water feature

M AN U

4

TE D

2

-2

Business

AC C

EP

0

High-density commercial

Low-density commercial Space functions Main space functions

Residential

Recreational

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Highlights Spatiotemporal patterns in soundscapes were collected in an urban area. Intercorrelations were found among soundscape, acoustic and morphological factors.

RI PT

A combination of acoustic and morphological indices can be applied to describe urban

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

soundscapes.