Roaring function in male goitered gazelles

Roaring function in male goitered gazelles

Accepted Manuscript Title: ROARING FUNCTION IN MALE GOITERED GAZELLES Author: D.A. Blank K. Ruckstuhl W. Yang PII: DOI: Reference: S0376-6357(14)0013...

271KB Sizes 0 Downloads 36 Views

Accepted Manuscript Title: ROARING FUNCTION IN MALE GOITERED GAZELLES Author: D.A. Blank K. Ruckstuhl W. Yang PII: DOI: Reference:

S0376-6357(14)00130-2 http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.05.009 BEPROC 2839

To appear in:

Behavioural Processes

Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:

4-1-2014 13-4-2014 31-5-2014

Please cite this article as: Blank, D.A., Ruckstuhl, K., Yang, W.,ROARING FUNCTION IN MALE GOITERED GAZELLES., Behavioural Processes (2014), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.05.009 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1   

ROARING FUNCTION IN MALE GOITERED GAZELLES. D.A. Blank a,b*, K. Ruckstuhl c, W. Yang a a

Key Laboratory of Biogeography and Bioresource in Arid Land, Xinjiang Institute of Ecology and Geography, the Chinese Academy of Sciences, Urumqi, China;

6  7  8 

b

Institute of Zoology, Kazakh Academy of Sciences, Alma-Ata, Kazakhstan. Department of Biological Sciences, University Calgary, Canada.



*Corresponding author: David Blank – [email protected], Fax +86-991-7885320

c

cr

10 

ip t

1  2  3  4  5 

11 

Abstract.

12 

very close distance. The goitered gazelle is a rare exception to this rule, and during the rutting

13 

period territorial males of this species are among the noisiest antelopes. Rutting vocalization is

14 

such an essential part the rutting behaviour in goitered gazelle that adult males have a hypertrophic

15 

larynx, the muscle tissues of which increase considerably in size during the rut. We were

16 

interested in the frequency and variance with which male goitered gazelles emit their calls

17 

depending on an animal’s age, reproductive status and time of the year in order to understand the

18 

main function of the rutting vocalizations. We found that roaring was mostly related to courting

19 

displays, while vocalizations during aggressive displays were less frequent in male-male

20 

interactions. Acoustic signals likely enhance courtship displays and also may aid in accelerating

21 

female ovulation, promoting synchronization of breeding cycles during the mating and birthing

22 

periods, which last only several days for most of the females in our population. We discuss the

24  25  26  27  28  29 

us

an

M

d

te

Ac ce p

23 

Most of the vocalizations of Antilopinae males are soft and usually only heard from a

potential benefits of such behaviours and compare it to other species living in similarly extreme

environments.

Key words: Aggressive displays, courting behaviour, goitered gazelle, male vocalization, rutting roaring.

INTRODUCTION

30 

 

31 

and acoustic signals are particularly widespread in communication and used by individuals of all

32 

ages and sexes in different social contexts (Vannoni et al., 2005). Bovids represent an interesting

33 

group, which show a wide variations of vocal signals and examples of convergent evolution with

34 

cervids and humans (Fitch and Reby, 2001; Frey et al., 2007, 2008 a,b). Adult males of many

In ungulates, as in other mammals, communication plays a crucial role in social interactions,

Page 1 of 24

2   

ungulate species are known to give distinct loud calls just during the rutting season (Minami and

36 

Kawamichi, 1992). Most of these vocalizations even during the rut uttered by Antilopinae males,

37 

for example, are rather soft and usually can be heard only from a very close distance from animals

38 

in captivity (Walther et al., 1983). These species vocalize mainly through the nose, and only

39 

optionally also through the mouth (Walther, 1984). Only a few Antilopinae species, such as the

40 

Mongolian gazelle (Procapra gutturosa – Frey et al., 2008b), and goitered gazelle (Gazella

41 

subgutturosa – Blank, 1998), are exceptions from this rule in that territorial males of these species

42 

are the noisiest of all Antilopinae species. Rutting adult males of Mongolian and goitered gazelles

43 

vocalize through widely opened mouths, while the females of these species produce both oral and

44 

nasal calls (Frey et al., 2008 a,b).

It is well known that gazelles use their vocalizations both for courting and aggressive

46 

displays (Walther et al., 1983), but there are variations in use among species. For example, Grant’s

47 

gazelle males (Nanger granti) rarely roar, but when they do, it is always connected to courting

48 

females, while in Thomson’s gazelles (Eudorcas thomsonii) rutting calls are most commonly heard

49 

during aggressive displays and more rarely while courting females (Walther et al., 1983). Roaring

50 

during the rut is a very typical phenomenon in goitered gazelles and vocalizations are an integral

51 

part of courtship and aggressive displays by territorial males (Blank, 1998). Goitered gazelle

52 

males are also distinctive in that they have an anatomical adaptation for uttering these relatively

53 

58  59 

“real” rut in November-December and a “false” rut in April-May, do many adult males establish

60 

individual territories and increase the frequency of their courting and aggressive displays by many

61 

times. Goitered gazelles mate only during the “real” rut, and females give birth only once a year,

62 

in May (Blank and Fedosenko, 1983; Blank, 1998). The second “false” rut in goitered gazelles has

63 

been hypothesized to be an ancestral trait from warmer geological periods, when this species had

64 

the environmental conditions and ability to breed twice a year (Sludskiy 1956; Tsapluk 1972),

54  55  56  57 

te

d

M

an

45 

Ac ce p

us

cr

ip t

35 

loud rutting calls, namely, an enlarged and descended larynx, the muscle tissues of which considerably increase in size during the rut (Blank, 1998; Frey et al., 2011). The goitered gazelle is a medium-sized ungulate, which originally was spread widely all over

of Middle and Central Asia, Iran, Afghanistan, Turkey, and the Caucasus (Kingswood and Blank, 1996). This species typically lives in small groups of 2-3 individuals (Blank et al., 2012b). In male goitered gazelles, courting and aggressive displays are seen all year round, but only during the

Page 2 of 24

3   

65 

though this so called ancestral behaviour certainly needs further investigations. In spite of the fact that the morphology of the vocal apparatus and acoustical features of

67 

goitered gazelle vocalizations have been described well in recent reports (Efremova et al., 2011

68 

a,b; Frey et al., 2011; Volodin et al., 2011), the behavioural aspects of their vocalizations are still

69 

poorly understood. This paper describes the behavioural components the rutting vocalizations and

70 

quantifies the characteristics of goitered gazelle roaring depending on season, social status, and the

71 

age of displaying males. We suggest several hypotheses for the occurrence of rut roaring in

72 

goitered gazelles.

In polygynous cervids and bovids, vocal features were found to be linked to the physical

74 

quality of rutting males and to play a decisive role in the reproductive success of the caller (Reby

75 

and McComb, 2003; Reby et al., 2005; Charlton et al., 2007, 2008). Since in many Antilopinae

76 

species adult males use their vocalizations most often while courting females or expelling rivals

77 

from their territories (Walther et al., 1983), we expected to find significant seasonal fluctuations in

78 

male roaring rates, with adult males vocalizing most often during the rutting period in

79 

November-December, less often during the “false” rut in April-May, and least often during the rest

80 

of the year. In addition, due to a considerable increase in the size of the larynx muscle tissues

81 

during the rut (Frey et al., 2011), we expected that vocalizations would be louder and better heard

82 

during the rut than outside the rut.

83 

88  89 

In other words, the frequency of vocalizations would increase with an increase in the number of

90 

potential individuals to whom the adult male would display either courting or aggressive

91 

behaviours.

84  85  86  87 

te

d

M

an

us

73 

Ac ce p

cr

ip t

66 

It also was found that the rutting vocalizations of adult male goitered gazelle have low

amplitude and consequently a poor propagation rate through the environment; therefore adult males must approach a recipient to perform their courting or aggressive displays at close range (Frey et al., 2011). So, we predicted that adult males would produce calls more often in the presence of other males or females than when they were alone, and that vocalization frequency would grow with the size of the female or male groups that intruded into the adult male’s territory.

92 

Territorial males are the most active individuals during the rutting period (Walther et al.,

93 

1983), so we predicted that territorial males would vocalize more often than sub-adult males and

94 

non-territorial males in groups, especially during the rut in November-December.

Page 3 of 24

4   

The goitered gazelles’ retractable larynx and vocal tract have evolved in such a way that

96 

during the rut, adult males, as compared to other ruminant species, can emit rutting calls even

97 

while running at a full gallop (Frey et al., 2011). We therefore predicted that adult males would run

98 

mainly at a high (galloping) speed during their vocalized courting or aggressive displays, because

99 

of their intensified arousal and rutting displays, while outside the rut they would vocalize at

100 

ip t

95 

usually lower speeds.

104 

MATERIAL AND METHODS

The term “roaring” is commonly used for vocalizations of large animals, such as lions

us

102  103 

cr

101 

(Panthera leo), leopards (P. pardus), elephant seals (Mirounga angustirostris), red deer and musk

106 

oxen (Ovibos moschatus), but we have used this term for the goitered gazelle’s vocalizations based

107 

on the more general definition of Weissengruber et al. (2002), who suggested that a roar is

108 

specified as a call type with a low fundamental frequency and lowered formant frequencies.

M

Observations for our study were conducted in the Ili depression (southeastern Kazakhstan) during a 6-year period from 1981 to 1986. We mostly made focal animal observations,

111 

concentrating on only one randomly selected group of gazelles at a time (usually the closest), and

112 

continuously sampled separate group members for up to 9 hours while recording the courting and

113 

aggressive displays both accompanied by calls and without calls. With aggressive displays, every

114 

119  120 

occurred. In most cases, gazelles were observed from distances of 50-100 m, with observation

121 

posts usually established on elevations from where we could see 30-50 gazelles at the same time.

122 

During the rut, the size of a male’s territory was 50-80 ha (Blank, 1985), and we typically could

123 

observe 5-6 individual territories simultaneously from our elevated observation point. And since

124 

females were distributed unevenly among territories (Blank, 1998), we chose a male closest to our

125 

observation point that had at least one female in his territory. To avoid pseudo-replication, we

115  116  117  118 

te

d

110 

Ac ce p

109 

an

105 

agonistic interaction between two rivals was calculated as a single incident, and the same for each vocalized aggressive display. Only one rival (territorial male) used his roaring against the male-intruder, which retreated usually without vocalization, each of such interaction also was calculated as a single case. Vocalizations were rarely observed during conflicts between territorial

males.

All behaviours were recorded in the order in which they occurred and each time they

Page 4 of 24

5   

changed our observation point every day and tried to observe in different parts of the study area

127 

(over ~4600 km2), establishing 20 different observation sites. The total size of the study

128 

population in the Ili depression was estimated to range from 1.5 thousand in 1981 up to 5 thousand

129 

in 1986 (Blank, 1990). For focal observations we used binoculars (magnification 8x) and a

130 

telescope (magnification 30x- 60x). We conducted 181 hours of focal observations in April, 470

131 

hours in May, 374 hours in June, 173 hours in July, 224 hours in November and 148 hours in

132 

December over the 6-year period.

cr

Goitered gazelles vocalized while courting females and chasing other males when performing aggressive displays, so we made a distinction between courting and aggressive vocalizations.

135 

Courting territorial males demonstrated their roaring mostly while demonstrating the low-stretch

136 

and high-stretch postures; the description (ethogram) of these displays is presented in Table 1.

137 

Among aggressive displays, adult males vocalized mostly while in dominance and threat postures,

138 

and rarely during battles (horn-pressing) (Table 1). Frey et al. (2011), using auditory and video

139 

recording techniques, found that rutting calls of adult male goitered gazelles consist of three types

140 

of sounds: roars, growls and grunts. Since we were using only our ears and eyes, we did not

141 

distinguish these types of calls, and in this paper called rutting vocalization “roaring”, keeping in

142 

mind that these vocalizations consisted in fact of the three types of calls mentioned above. In

143 

addition, Frey et al. (2011) pointed out that male goitered gazelles produced their rutting

144 

149  150 

January, February, March, and September. For our investigations into differences in goitered

151 

gazelle vocalized and non-vocalized courting and aggressive displays based on sex, age, season,

152 

and territorial status, we used two main indices: the absolute frequency of vocalization during

153 

courting or aggressive displays (number of behavioural acts per hour of observation) and relative

154 

frequency (proportion of observations with vocalizations out of the total number of all

155 

observations). The first index just stated whether the frequency of vocalized displays increased or

145  146  147  148 

te

d

M

an

us

134 

Ac ce p

133 

ip t

126 

vocalizations only through widely opened mouths, while our observations showed that during slow gaits (walking, trotting), the male’s nostrils also may participate in the producing rutting sounds (Table 1).

We investigated the frequency changes of vocalizations according to the breeding cycle and

distinguished three biological seasons for the goitered gazelle: rutting season (mid-November to mid-December), “false” rut (April-May) and outside of the rut (June-August); data were absent for

Page 5 of 24

6   

156 

decreased over months, while the second index gave an idea of whether the proportion of

157 

vocalized versus non-vocalized displays increased. It was possible to individually identify separate territorial males, since they stayed in the

159 

same area for several weeks. Most other individuals, though, were not individually recognizable,

160 

although we were able to differentiate gazelles according to sex (horned males and hornless

161 

females) and age (adult and sub-adult males and adult and sub-adult females). The age of all

162 

goitered gazelles observed was differentiated based on the descriptions of Zhevnerov (1984): adult

163 

males (older than 2 years old – horns 1.5 times longer than ears and absence of the black spot on

164 

the front of the muzzle), adult females (older than 2 years old – hornless and absence of black spot

165 

on the front of the muzzle), sub-adult males (1-2 years old – horns are equal or shorter than ears

166 

and presence of a black spot on the muzzle front), sub-adult females (1-2 years old – hornless and

167 

black spot on the muzzle front), young (up to 1 year old, body size 2-3 times smaller than in adult

168 

gazelles and a black spot on the muzzle front).

We used One-Way ANOVA followed by LSD Post-Hoc test and a General Linear Model

170 

(GLM, Type 3 Sum of Squares) to test for the effect of month and age on absolute and relative

171 

frequencies of vocalization acts. These data were normally distributed according to Shapiro-Wilk

172 

and Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests. We used Log-linear models to compare proportions of separate

173 

behavioural acts and Spearman’s Correlation for a possible correlation between frequency of

174 

180  181 

increasing in occurrence 6.1 times in April, 36.4 times in November and 37.5 times in December

182 

compared to July (Post-Hoc LSD test, P < 0.05, 0.002 and 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 1). The

183 

frequency of aggressive patterns also increased during these months, but overall changes were

184 

insignificant (One-Way ANOVA, F = 1.974, df1 = 7, df2 = 83, P = 0.068). Furthermore, the roaring

185 

was soft and difficult to detect in most months at distances more than 50 m and only during the

186 

rutting period in November and December were these sounds relatively loud and clearly heard

175  176  177  178  179 

te

d

169 

Ac ce p

M

an

us

cr

ip t

158 

vocalized displays and group size. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 17.0 software package.

RESULTS

We found that adult male goitered gazelles’ vocalized courting patterns were varied and

fluctuated significantly over months (One-Way ANOVA, F = 12.242, df1 = 7, df2 = 141, P < 0.001),

Page 6 of 24

7   

from distances up to 300-400 m. The relative frequency of courting displays also fluctuated

188 

significantly over months (One-Way ANOVA, F = 3.514, df1 = 7, df2 = 132, P = 0.002), but in fact

189 

only in November was this value significantly larger compared to other months (Post-Hoc LSD

190 

test, P < 0.01), and insignificant at all other times (P > 0.05). The relative frequency of aggressive

191 

displays fluctuated insignificantly (One-Way ANOVA, F = 0.133, df1 = 6, df2 = 74, P > 0.05).

In adult males, the yearly average frequency of vocalized courting displays exceeded 2.4

193 

times the aggressive patterns with vocalizations (GLM test, F = 8.869, df = 1, P = 0.003), and only

194 

in July, were aggressive displays with calls observed more often than courting patterns (1.8 times)

195 

(GLM test, F = 9.039, df = 7, P < 0.001). The relative frequency showed the same trend, with

196 

courting displays’ exceeding 1.2 times the aggressive patterns (GLM test, F = 5.248, df = 1, P =

197 

0.023). In addition, our analysis showed that maximal frequency of vocalization by an adult male

198 

was demonstrated when he stayed together with one female (26%, n = 903), while he was roaring

199 

slightly less often with two or three females (21-22%) (Log-linear model test, Z = -2.111-2.574,

200 

P = 0.035 and 0.01), and much less when he had a larger female group or when alone (1-7%)

201 

(Z = -3.631-5.577, P < 0.001). However, the frequency of vocalizing of males in female groups of

202 

different sizes was closely correlated with the proportion of available groups (Spearman’s

203 

Correlation = 0.982, N = 203, P < 0.001). Furthermore, territorial single males vocalized

204 

significantly more often while courting females, than did non-territorial males in groups

205 

210  211 

from the rut (Z = -4.337-5.002, P < 0.001), and adult males vocalized more often in male groups

212 

(up to 11 individuals) than when they had only one rival. However, frequency of vocalizing males

213 

in groups of different size was also closely correlated with the proportion of available groups

214 

(Spearman’s Correlation = 0.999, N = 185, P < 0.001).

206  207  208  209 

te

d

M

an

us

cr

192 

Ac ce p

ip t

187 

(Log-linear model test, Z = 4.494, P < 0.001), with the maximum difference in November and December (Z = 7.812 and 4.219, P < 0.001), a smaller difference in April (Z = 3.055, P = 0.002), and no difference between single and group males in other months (P > 0.05). During the aggressive displays in the rut (November-December), adult males vocalized most often when they had one rival, and considerably less often when they chased male groups of 2 or more rivals (Z = -4.293, P < 0.001). Outside the rut, the distribution of vocalization frequency was different

215 

Sub-adult males started to show vocalized courting displays in April, with some increase in

216 

frequency during May-July (One-Way ANOVA, F = 7.382, df1 = 3, df2 = 19, P = 0.002); in other

Page 7 of 24

8   

months such behaviour was not observed. The relative frequency fluctuated insignificantly during

218 

April-July (One-Way ANOVA, F = 1.093, df1 = 3, df2 = 15, P > 0.05). The average frequency of

219 

calls during aggressive displays in sub-adult males was insignificantly higher than their calls

220 

during courting displays (GLM test, F = 1.328, df =1, P > 0.05). In April, vocalized courting

221 

displays in sub-adult males were observed much more rarely (3.4%, n = 58) compared to adult

222 

males. Further, this proportion increased from 15.3% (n = 98) in May and up to 40.9% (n = 44) in

223 

June and again decreased by as much as 12.5% (n = 8) in July. The proportion of vocalized

224 

aggressive displays in sub-adult males was even lower than in adult males and fluctuated between

225 

3-4% (n = 59) in April-May and 10% (n = 10) in June.

In any month, adult males produced calls usually when courting females, and especially

227 

frequently in November-December (98% cases, n = 911) and April-May (94%). They rarely

228 

vocalized before starting a chase (Log-linear model test, Z = 9.325, P < 0.001), though in July they

229 

called before chasing more frequently (up to 30% cases). Adult males commonly vocalized in the

230 

low-stretch or high-stretch postures, especially often in November-December (99.6%, n = 913),

231 

and even outside the rut this proportion only decreased to 88%; rarely did adult males vocalize in a

232 

neutral posture (Z = - 6.250, P < 0.001). Vocalizations were performed most often with an elevated

233 

tail (in a horizontal position), especially in November-December (89-96%, n = 859) and

234 

April-May (95-98%) (Log-linear model test, Z = 6.194, P < 0.001). More rarely they vocalized

235 

240  241 

(Z = -7.023, P < 0.001) (Fig. 2). And November did not differ from December (Z = 1.556,

242 

P > 0.05). In contrast, outside the rut adult males produced calls mostly while moving at a slow

243 

pace (walking or trotting – 30-55%), and in July-August even in a standing position (11-38%)

244 

(Log-linear model test, Z = -5.807-7.922, P < 0.001). With aggressive behaviour, territorial males

245 

typically vocalized while driving intruders-bachelors (non-territorial males) out of their territories,

246 

and rarely used their calls during conflicts with other territorial males (Log-linear model test, Z =

236  237  238  239 

te

d

M

an

226 

Ac ce p

us

cr

ip t

217 

with the tail in a vertical or down position, but increasing the proportion of the last pattern by up to 20% outside the rut (August). While calling, the auricles were usually pressed backward toward the neck (70-99%, n = 753) and more rarely in a neutral position (Z = 9.242-11.781, P < 0.001). In November-December, roaring adult males chased females most often while galloping

(33-41%, n = 697) (Log-linear model test, Z = 2.458, P = 0.014), less frequently when walking (31-39%) or trotting (24-27%), and least often while standing (1-1.5%) and/or jerking (1-2.6%)

Page 8 of 24

9   

2.638, P = 0.008). All year round, roaring adult males mostly walked (up to 75%) when chasing of

248 

other males, while trotting and galloping were observed considerably less often (15-30% and

249 

6-28%, respectively) than walking (Z = -3.307-4.315, P < 0.002). However during the rut, the

250 

territorial males chased their rivals more often using faster gaits (trot or gallop), resulting in a

251 

decrease in the portion of walking pursuits (45-50%, n = 601); conversely, walking pursuits

252 

increased outside the rut (53-75%, n = 330) (Z = -3.307-7.490, P < 0.001). Calls made by adult

253 

males while standing and/or jerking occurred least often (1-10%).

When courting females, territorial males produced calls periodically, while constant roaring

255 

during the entire chase sequence was rarely observed. During the most intensive courting period

256 

in November, however, the portion of constant roaring increased by up to 28% (Chi squire of fit

257 

test, Chi squire = 106.681, df = 1, P < 0.001). Most often during aggressive chases in the rut,

258 

territorial bucks used single, infrequent, and relatively long-term (2-3s) calls while expelling

259 

non-territorial males from their territories (46-54%, n = 74), as well as when following passing

260 

bachelor males (45-51%, n = 72) (Log-linear model test, Z = - 0.439, P > 0.05); outside the rut

261 

adult males followed each other mostly with bouts of short (less than 1s) and frequently repeated

262 

calls (Z = - 4.536, P < 0.001). There was no difference in the proportion of these calls between

263 

November and December (Z = -1.628, P > 0.05), but these two months differed significantly from

264 

other months (Z = -4.055-4.581, P < 0.001).

265 

271  272 

and relative frequencies of vocalization during courtship significantly increased during the real rut.

273 

Hence, adult males roared more frequently per hour of observation not only because of an increase

274 

in the total number of courting displays during the rut, but also because of an increase in the

275 

proportion of vocalized versus non-vocalized displays. In contrast, during aggressive displays,

276 

adult males showed insignificant increases in vocalized displays compared to the non-rutting

277 

period. In addition, goitered gazelles vocalized considerably more often during courting displays

266  267  268  269  270 

te

d

M

an

us

254 

Ac ce p

cr

ip t

247 

DISCUSSION

Adult males showed considerable seasonal fluctuations in their vocalization; roaring during

their courtship and aggressive displays were observed all year round, with some increasing frequency during the “false” rut in April-May and maximum frequency during the real rutting period in November and December, compared to other months of the year. And both the absolute

Page 9 of 24

10   

than aggressive demonstrations, and only in July, when testicle physiological activity was minimal

279 

and most adult males gathered in large groups (Tsapluk, 1972; Blank et al., 2012a), did the

280 

vocalized aggressive interactions exceed the courting displays. Thus, roaring in goitered gazelles

281 

is more related to courting displays than to aggressive patterns, with males accompanying up to

282 

half of their courting displays by roaring (Blank, 1998).

ip t

278 

The same phenomenon was found in red deer (Cervus elaphus), where the stag’s roar is

284 

speeding up ovulation, which improves mating success with frequent calling (Fedosenko, 1980;

285 

McComb, 1987, 1991; Charlton et al., 2007). Similarly in fallow bucks (Dama dama) time

286 

invested in vocal display is linked to mating success (McElligott et al., 1999). Among Antilopinae

287 

species, Grant’s gazelles only rarely emit relatively loud roars, except when courting females

288 

(Walther et al., 1983). It is possible that territorial male rutting roars in goitered gazelles serve the

289 

same function as in deer, namely to promote ovulation.

an

us

cr

283 

Goitered gazelle rutting roars were loud enough to be clearly heard from a distance of several

291 

hundred meters, while outside the rut adult males’ calls were difficult to hear from a distance of

292 

more than 50 m. Furthermore, during the rut males produced long-term, usually single or

293 

infrequent calls, while outside of the rut adult males followed each other mostly with bouts of

294 

short and frequently repeated calls, especially during aggressive displays.

d

te

Acoustic signals can be sexually selective if they generate information about the signaler and

301 

Ac ce p

295 

M

290 

302 

male goitered gazelle is much larger and rests lower in the neck (Frey et al., 2011; Volodin et al.,

303 

2011). An increase in larynx size and the vocal tract elongation leads to the acoustic effect of

304 

producing calls with lower frequency and resonance, which are unexpected low for an animal the

305 

size of the goitered gazelles (Frey et al., 2011). The same phenomenon is found in three

306 

mammalian taxa: among bovids, which apart from goitered gazelles, is also present in Mongolian

307 

gazelle; among cervids, such as fallow deer and red deer; and in humans (Bigalke, 1972; Walther,

296  297  298  299  300 

allow increasing reproductive success through mate competition or mate choice (Espmark et al., 2000). It is likely that vocalizations in goitered gazelle are a result of the same processes, and rutting calls in adult males are such an important and essential component (Blank, 1998), that males have a large and very prominent larynx or “Adam’s apple” (Kingswood and Blank, 1996) with muscle tissue that noticeably increases in size (up to 50%) during the rut (Frey et al., 2011). Compared to domestic bovids of similar size, and even females of conspecifics, the larynx of the

Page 10 of 24

11   

308 

1981; Fitch and Reby, 2001; McElligott et al., 2006; Frey et al., 2008 a,b). As predicted, adult goitered gazelle males showed their vocalization most often when they

310 

had females inside their territories or with rivals for aggressive displays, while they vocalized

311 

rarely when they were alone. The significantly enlarged larynx of goitered gazelle males is

312 

adapted for producing low-amplitude close-range calls (Frey et al., 2011). Therefore having

313 

relatively large-sized individual territories, goitered gazelle usually started to call after

314 

approaching females or rivals and rarely vocalized over long distances in response to activities of

315 

other males in neighboring territories (Blank 1998). The same behaviour also was observed in

316 

Mongolian gazelles (Frey et al., 2011).

Contrary to our predictions, the calling rate didn’t increase with group size, and vocalizations

318 

of adult males were noted frequently when courting of small female groups (2-4 individuals),

319 

while cases of vocalized courting displays for larger groups were observed significantly less often.

320 

A similar behaviour was observed for aggressive displays during the rut, when adult males

321 

produced calls most often for one rival (single bachelors), and considerably less often, when

322 

chasing male groups of any size. In contrast, outside the rut, males addressed their vocalized

323 

aggressive displays more often to male groups than to a single male. Our analyses showed,

324 

however, that this was not a result of males’ preference to call for groups of a certain size, but was

325 

a consequence of the goitered gazelles’ social structure, where small female groups and single

326 

331  332 

adult males or bachelors, which may gather into groups, sometimes also court females in mixed

333 

groups, or when a bachelor group occasionally meets a herd of females. In this case, several males

334 

will run after the same female, which then usually results in fighting. A territorial male is regularly

335 

chasing male groups, attempting to separate the non-territorial males away from his females

336 

(Walther et al., 1983). Since territorial males are constantly defending their territory from rivals,

337 

they have a high rate of aggressive displays, while bachelors without territories only occasionally

327  328  329  330 

te

d

M

an

317 

Ac ce p

us

cr

ip t

309 

males were more common during the rut than outside of it (Blank et al., 2012b). Territorial males vocalized during courting and aggressive displays significantly more often

than bachelor bucks in groups and sub-adult males. Territorial males, and territoriality as a whole, have the greatest impact on reproductive success, since in many Antilopinae species, including goitered gazelles, reproduction is almost exclusively done by territorial males (Walther et al., 1983). When females arrive in their territories, territorial males are busy courting. Non-territorial

Page 11 of 24

12   

have conflicts within their groups. We found the frequency of vocalized courting and aggressive

339 

displays to be closely correlated with the total frequency of all displays, including non-vocalized

340 

ones; therefore territorial males vocalized significantly more often than bachelors in groups.

341 

During the “false” rut (April-May), only some adult males became territorial, while the majority

342 

stayed in groups (Blank, 1998); therefore the behavioural difference between single (territorial)

343 

males and bachelors in groups was less pronounced. During the non-rut period, territoriality in

344 

males was absent and the proportion of single males was at a minimum (Blank et al., 2012a), so

345 

differences in the frequency of vocalization between single males and group males were

346 

negligible.

The vocalizations of young males started to differentiate from females’ as early as the second

348 

week of life (Efremova et al., 2011a). Young males become interested in females by the age of 6

349 

months, but just follow the females and sniff their hindquarters, demonstrating only a neutral

350 

posture (Blank, 1998). Vocalized courting and aggressive displays of sub-adult males started in

351 

April at the age of one year. Later (May-July), the frequency of vocalized display increased, while

352 

during the real rut in November-December, sub-adult males did not perform vocalized courting or

353 

aggressive displays at all. Adult males had a sharp decrease in activity in June-July, starting at the

354 

end of the “false” rut in May, while proportion of vocalized displays of sub-adults increased

355 

during the same period compared to adult males. This behavior of sub-adult males corresponds to

356 

361  362 

while adult males vocalized courting displays significantly more often than aggressive displays.

357  358  359  360 

te

d

M

an

347 

Ac ce p

us

cr

ip t

338 

the cyclical physiology of their testicle development, which begins to function for the first time in April, when the “false” rut starts in adult males; sub-adult testicular activity continues through May-July then demonstrates a distinctive waning in November. In contrast, the activity of adult male testicles is at its maximum during the rut in November-December, less during the “false” rut in April-May and fades during non-rutting periods (Tsapluk, 1972). It is interesting to note that sub-adult males used calls with the same frequency during both courting and aggressive displays,

363 

As observed in the non-rutting period, adult males following females preferred to make calls

364 

while moving at a slow gaits (walking and trotting). During the rut, however, roaring territorial

365 

males chased females most often when galloping – a phenomenon made possible by the

366 

anatomical construction of the goitered gazelle’s vocal apparatus (Frey et al., 2011). The point is

367 

that during the rutting period females don’t remain continually within an individual male’s

Page 12 of 24

13   

territory, but instead just crosses through it on their way to their daytime resting sites (Blank,

369 

1998). The territorial male, then, tries to keep the female group within his territory as long as

370 

possible, herding and courting, usually at a gallop, one female after the other in an attempt to drive

371 

them into the center of his territory (Walther et al. 1983). While engaged in this chasing, males

372 

utter their roars very often (Blank, 1998). In contrast, territorial males used their vocalized

373 

aggressive displays almost exclusively against bachelors-intruders while expelling them from their

374 

territories, while they did not use their roaring for solving territorial conflicts with their neighbors.

375 

They used their calls without physical conflict, because the male-intruders retreated without

376 

resistance and without returning the calls - it was always a one-sided display. Roaring territorial

377 

males used their calls to demonstrate their territorial status to bachelors (Walther, 1981), but they

378 

did not vocalize when addressing their aggressive displays to their neighbors, possibly because all

379 

neighbors were familiar with each other individually. During the rut territorial males mostly

380 

expelled intruders-bachelors from their territories while walking, because the males-trespassers

381 

(bachelors) usually retreated by walking or more rarely trotting. Outside the rut, adult males also

382 

chased each other using a rather slow gaits (mostly walking) as opposed to a faster ones

383 

(galloping), because aggressive interactions between members within the same male group always

384 

had playful characteristics and serious conflicts were rarely observed (Blank 1992).

cr

us

an

M

d

te

Adult males produced their roaring calls while posed in erected postures (low-stretch and

391 

Ac ce p

385 

ip t

368 

392 

So why are goitered gazelles a rare example of mouth-produced rutting vocalizations, which are

393 

louder than in most other Antilopinae species? Walther (1981) emphasized the exclusive

394 

importance of rutting vocalizations for males of all territorial antelope species as an advertisement

395 

of their territorial social status, both when courting females and when chasing away bachelors.

396 

However, only a few have relatively loud rutting calls (Walther et al., 1983). Our explanation is

397 

that most tropical antelope species have extended or perennial breeding periods, thus maintaining

386  387  388  389  390 

high-stretch), when they vocalized with an extended throat region. An elevated head tends to decrease formant frequencies by a slight elongation of the length of the vocal tract and is, therefore, producing sounds of a lower tone (timbre). These stretched postures are also typical for roaring of red deer stags and Mongolian gazelles (Frey and Gebler, 2010). Many antelope species make rutting vocalizations when chasing females, though their

nasally-produced sounds are usually soft and difficult to detect at a distance (Walther et al. 1983).

Page 13 of 24

14   

continual territoriality (Estes, 1991). In contrast, goitered gazelles have a very short, exacting

399 

overall autumn rutting period (mid-November-mid-December), with most matings occurring

400 

within a 4-6 day period (Blank, 1998). As a result, most females give birth within a short period

401 

(just several days) at the end of May that coincides with the most favorable time of year, which is

402 

very limited in the Central Asiatic deserts (Blank et al., 2012a). To achieve such a high

403 

synchronization in birthing, goitered gazelles have to perform very short, vigorous rutting period

404 

(one month) with intensive rutting vocalizations with low-formant calls, which are particularly

405 

attractive for females, and are likely accelerating their ovulation and synchronization their

406 

ovulatory cycles. Mongolian gazelles, which live in conditions comparable to those in Middle Asia

407 

(Western Central Asia) with cold snowy winters and hot dry summers, have similar anatomical

408 

and behavioural adaptations (Leimgruber et al., 2001; Frey et al., 2011).

Saiga antelope (Saiga tatarica) has another anatomical adaptation (trunk-like nose) for

410 

producing low-formant rutting calls (nasal roaring), which are also attractive for females (Frey et

411 

al., 2007); but similar to goitered and Mongolian gazelles, saiga almost constantly express their

412 

vocalizations during their short and vigorous rutting period (Volodin et al., 2009) to also achieve a

413 

very high synchronization in birthing (5-8 days – Bekenov et al., 1998). Other ungulates, such as

414 

chiru (Pantholops hodsonii) and Tibetan gazelle (Procapra picticaudata), living in the

415 

high-elevated cold deserts of the Tibetan Plateau, also use relatively loud, deep-throated roars

416 

421  422 

vocalizations (Pohle, 1974).

417  418  419  420 

te

d

M

409 

Ac ce p

an

us

cr

ip t

398 

(chiru) and grunting roars (Tibetan gazelle) frequently during their vigorous and brief rutting season to likely achieve a high synchronization in births, which last approximately a week (Schaller, 1998). The anatomical adaptations of these species still have not been investigated,

though it is known that chiru males have distinctive walnut-sized bulges of their nostrils that enlarge their nasal passages and that they use both nasal and mouth-produced calls for rutting vocalizations (Schaller, 1998). Tibetan gazelle has proboscis-like noses, which are inflated during

423 

Finally, the springbok, a gazelle-like antelope, lives in a completely different environment in

424 

southern Africa. It has been called both a seasonal and a non-seasonal breeder (Bigalke, 1972;

425 

Smithers, 1983), but male springboks, which make relatively loud vibration rutting calls through

426 

their widely opened nostrils, are also the nosiest among Antilopinae (Walther, 1981). This seems

427 

contradict our supposition that relatively loud rutting vocalizations are important for

Page 14 of 24

15   

synchronizing mating and birthing cycles in females. In fact, springboks also demonstrate

429 

synchronization of oestrous and the subsequent clumping of births (Skinner et al., 1992), though

430 

this species gives birth in an opportunistic, pulse-like manner (in different months) because birth

431 

peaks occur at the beginning of the rainy season, the timing and duration of which is unpredictable

432 

in the arid environment of southern Africa (Skinner and Louw, 1996). In addition, the spring

433 

birthing season of springboks in Cape Province and Namibia varies from year to year by up to two

434 

months, and a second autumn birth peak occurs only during favorable years (Estes, 1991).

All these species have different anatomical adaptations for producing rutting vocalizations,

436 

but each uses its relatively loud rutting roars to intensify courting and synchronize breeding cycles.

437 

This point is attributable to the fact that most females give birth during the most favorable time of

438 

year for rearing young, which is an especially narrow time window in the northern Asiatic deserts,

439 

including the cold deserts of the Tibetan Plateau. Furthermore, the function of rutting vocalizations

440 

as an advertisement of territorial status (Walther 1981) is not inconsistent in fact to our

441 

explanations. Rutting vocalizations, as a cue to territorial (high social) status, make a territorial

442 

male particularly attractive to females, so the main function of rutting vocalizations in adult male

443 

goitered gazelles seems to be in courting females to promote synchronization of the mating and

444 

birthing cycles. Females mate exclusively with territorial males, even though non-territorial males

445 

are also fertile (Skinner and Louw, 1996). This, then, defines vocalizations as an extremely

446  447  448  449  450  451 

te

d

M

an

us

435 

Ac ce p

cr

ip t

428 

valuable part of a male’s breeding success, while vocalized aggressive displays are less often observed and likely play a minor role in male-male interactions.

Acknowledgments

We thank the National Natural Science Foundation of China (U1303301), Chinese Academy

452 

of Sciences (CAS) for granting our work (Visiting Professorships for Senior International

453 

Scientists – 2011T1Z42), West Light Foundation (CAS – Y336171) for creating all conditions for

454 

writing this paper. We are very grateful to the Institute of Zoology, former Academy of Sciences of

455 

Kazakhstan, which has given us the possibility for investigations of goitered gazelles in their

456 

natural environment during 10 years. We thank Mrs. Patricia Johnston, who did the constant help

457 

in editing this manuscript.

Page 15 of 24

16   

458  459 

466  467  468  469  470  471 

ip t

465 

Bigalke, R.C., 1972. Observations on the behavior and feeding habits of the springbok, Antidorcas marsupialis. Zoologica Africana 7, 333-359.

cr

464 

the Saiga antelope in Kazakhstan. Mammal Review 28(1): 1-52.

Blank, D.A., 1985. Peculiarities of social and reproductive behaviour of Gazella subgutturosa in the Ili Hollow. Zool. Zhurnal 64, 1059-1070 (in Russian with English abstract).

us

463 

Bekenov, A.B., Grachev, IU.A., and Milner-Gulland, E.J. 1998. The ecology and management of

Blank, D.A., 1990. Persian gazelle, in: Kovshar, A.F. (Ed.), Rare animals of desert regions. Nauka Kazakh SSR, Alma-Ata, pp. 56-80. (in Russian).

an

462 

REFERENCES

Blank, D.A., 1998. Mating behaviour of the Persian gazelle (Gazella subgutturosa Guldenstaedt, 1780). Mammalia, 62(4), 499-519.

M

460  461 

Blank, D.A., Fedosenko, A.K., 1983. False rut in Persian gazelle, in: Zakharov, A.A.(Ed.),

473 

Proceedings of the third all-union conference on animal behaviour, Vol. 2. Nauka of the

474 

Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Moscow, pp. 258-259 (in Russian).

subgutturosa Guldenstaedt 1780). Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 24, 306-321.

482 

Ac ce p

476 

Blank. D.A., Ruckstuhl, R.T., Yang, W., 2012a. Social organization in goitered gazelle (Gazella

te

475 

d

472 

483 

Efremova, K.O., Volodin, I.A., Volodina, E.V., Frey, R., Soldatova, N.V., Lapshina, E.N., Makarov,

484 

I.S., and Gorbunov, K.S., 2011a. Effect of sex and age on structural features of nasal calls and

485 

body size in fawns of the goitered gazelle, Gazella subgutturosa (Artiodactyla, Bovidae). Biol.

486 

Bull. 38(7), 736-746.

477  478  479  480  481 

Blank, D.A., Yang W., Xia, C., and Xu, W. 2012b. Grouping pattern of the goitered gazelle, Gazella subgutturosa (Cetartiodactyla: Bovidae) in Kazakhstan. Mammalia 76: 149-155.

Charlton, B.D., Reby, D., McComb, K., 2007. Female red deer prefer the roars of larger males. Biol. Let. 3, 382-385.

Charlton, B.D., Reby, D., McComb, K., 2008. Effect of combined source (F0) and filter (formant) variation on red deer hind responses to male roars. J. Acoustic. Soc. Amer. 123, 2936-2943.

487 

Efremova, K.O., Volodin, I.A., Volodina, E.V., Frey, R., Lapshina, E.N., Soldatova, N.V., 2011b.

488 

Developmental changes of nasal and oral calls in the goitred gazelle Gazella subgutturosa, a

Page 16 of 24

17   

489 

nonhuman mammal with a sexually dimorphic and descended larynx. Naturwissenschaften,

490 

98, 919-931.

494  495  496  497  498 

Estes, R.D., 1991. The behavior guide to African mammals. University of California Press,

ip t

493 

animal communication, Trondhein, Norway: Tapir Academic Press.

Berkeley.

Fedosenko, A.K., 1980. Maral: Ecology, behavior, and use, Kapitonov V.I. (Ed.). Nauka Press of

cr

492 

Espmark, Y., Amundsen, T., Rosenqvist, G., 2000. Animal signals: Signalling and signal design in

Kazakh SSR, Alma-Ata (in Russian).

Fitch, W.T., Reby, D., 2001. The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proc.Royal Soc.,

us

491 

London B 268, 1669-1675.

Frey, R., Gebler, A., 2010. Mechanisms and evolution of roaring-like vocalization in mammals, in:

500 

Brudzynski S.M.(Ed.), Handbook of Mammalian Vocalization. An Integrative Neuroscience

501 

Approach. Eslsevier, Amsterdam – Tokyo.

M

an

499 

Frey, R., Gebler, A., Olson, K.A., Odonkhuu, D., Fritsch, G., Batsaikhan, N, Stuermer, I.W., 2008a.

503 

Mobile larynx in Mongolian gazelle: retraction of the larynx during rutting barks in male

504 

Mongolian gazelle (Procapra gutturosa Pallas, 1777). J. Morphol. 269, 1223-1237.

d

502 

Frey, R., Gebler, A., Olson, K.A., Odonkhuu, D., Fritsch, G., Batsaikhan, N, Stuermer, I.W., 2008b.

506 

Head anatomy of male and female Mongolian gazelle – striking example of sexual

508  509  510  511  512  513 

Ac ce p

507 

te

505 

dimorphism, in: Endo, H., Frey, R. (Eds.), Anatomical Imaging – Towards a New

Morphology. Springer, Tokyo, pp. 1-13.

Frey, R., Volodin, I., Volodina, E., 2007. A nose that roars: anatomical specializations and behavioural features of rutting male saiga. J. Anat. 211, 717-736.

Frey, R., Volodin, I., Volodina, E., Soldatova, N.V., Juldaschev, E.T., 2011. Descended and mobile larynx, vocal tract elongation and rutting roars in male goitered gazelles (Gazella

subgutturosa Guldenstaedt, 1780). J. Anat. 218(5), 566-585.

514 

Kingswood, S.C., Blank, D.A., 1996. Gazella subgutturosa. Mamm. Spec. 518; 1-10 pp.

515 

Leimgruber, P., McShea, W., Brookes, C., Lhamsuren, B., Wemmer, C., 2001. Spatial patterns in

516 

relative primary productivity and gazelle migration in the Eastern Steppes of Mongolia. Biol.

517 

Con. 102(2), 205-212.

518 

McComb, K., 1987. Roaring by red deer stags advances the date of oestrus in hinds. Nature 330,

Page 17 of 24

18   

520  521  522  523 

648-649. McComb, K.E., 1991. Female choice for high roaring rate in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Anim. Beh. 41, 79-88. McElligott, A.G., O’Nell, K.P., Hayden, T.J., 1999. Cumulative long-term investment in vocalization of fallow bucks, Dama dama. Anim. Beh. 57, 1159-1167.

ip t

519 

McElligott, A.G., Birrer, M., Vannoni, E., 2006. Retraction of the mobile descended larynx during

525 

groaning enables fallow bucks (Dama dama) to lower their formants frequencies. J. Zool.

526 

270, 340-345.

530  531  532 

us

529 

nippon. J. Mamm. Soc. Japan, 17(2), 71-94.

Pohle, C. 1974. Haltung und Zucht der saiga-Antelope (Saiga tatarica) im Tierpark Berlin. Zool.

an

528 

Minami, M., Kawamichi, T., 1992. Vocal repertoires and classification of the sika deer Cervus

Garten 61: 263-266.

Reby, D., McComb, K., 2003. Anatomical constraints generate honesty: acoustic cues to age and

M

527 

cr

524 

weight in the roars of red deer stags. Anim. Beh. 65, 519-530. Reby, D., McComb, K., Cargnelutti, B, Darwin, C.J., Fitch, W.T. and Clutton-Brock, T.H., 2005.

534 

Red deer stags use formants as assessment cues during intrasexual agonistic interactions.

535 

Proc. Royal Soc., London B 272, 941-947.

536 

Schaller, G.B. 1998. Wildlife of the Tibetan steppe. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois.

537 

Ac ce p

te

d

533 

538  539  540  541  542  543  544  545 

Skinner, J.D., Jackson, T.P., and Marais, A.L. 1992. The ‘ram-effect” in three species of African ungulates. In: Spitz, F., Janeau, G., Gonzales, G., and Aulager, S., eds. Ungulates 91.

SFEPM-IRGM, Toulouse.

Skinner, J.D. and Louw, G.N. 1996. The Springbok (Antidorcas marsupialis – Zimmermann, 1780). Pretoria: Transvaal Museum.

Sludskiy, A.A. 1956. The breeding in goitered gazelle. Proceedings of the Institute of Zoology, Academy of Sciences, Kazakh SSR 6: 78-108 (in Russian).

Smithers, R.H.N. 1983. The Mammals of the Southern African Subregion. Pretoria: Univ. of Pretoria.

546 

Tsapluk, O.E., 1972. Age and seasonal dynamics of sexual activity in Gazella subgutturosa Gueld.

547 

Proceedings of the Academy of Sciences Kazakh SSR, Biological series 3: 39-46 (in Russian).

548 

Vannoni, E., Torriani, M.V.G., McElligott, A.G., 2005. Acoustuc signaling in cervids: a

Page 18 of 24

19   

549 

methodological approach for measuring vocal communication in fallow deer. Cogn.Brain,

550 

Beh. 9(3), 551-565. Volodin, I.A., Lapshina, E.N., Volodina, E.V., Frey, R., Soldatova, N.V., 2011. Nasal and oral calls

552 

in juvenile goitered gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa) and their potencial to encode sex and

553 

identity. Ethology 117, 294-308.

ip t

551 

Volodin, I.A., Volodina, E.V., and Efremova, K.O., 2009. Antelope, calling through the nose:

555 

structure of sounds and effect of sexual selection on the vocal behavior of the saiga (Saiga

556 

tatarica). Zoological Journal 88(1): 113-124 (In Russian, English abstract).

560  561  562 

us

559 

1790. D. Zoologicher Garten 51, 81-103.

Walther, F.R., 1984. Communication and expression in hoofed animals. Indiana University Press,

an

558 

Walther, F.R., 1981. Remarks on behavior of springbok, Antidorcas marsupialis Zimmermann

Bloomington.

Walther, F.R., Mungall, E.C., Grau, G.H., 1983. Gazelles and their relatives. Noyes Publications,

M

557 

cr

554 

Park Ridge, New Jersey.

Weissengruber, G.E., Forstenpointer, G., Peters, G., Kubber-Heiss, A., Fitch, W.T., 2002. Hyoid

564 

apparatus and pharynx in the lion (Panthera leo), jaguar (Panthera onca), tiger (Panthera

565 

tigris), cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) and domestic cat (Felis silvestris f.catus). J. Anat. 201,

566 

195-209.

568  569  570  571  572  573  574  575  576  577  578 

te

Ac ce p

567 

d

563 

Zhevnerov, V.V., 1984. Goitered gazelle of the Barsa-Kelmes Island. Nauka Press of the Kazakh SSR, Alma-Ata (in Russian).

Figure Legends:

Fig. 1. Frequency (act/h) fluctuations of vocalized courting (Court) and aggressive (Agress) displays of adult males over months. Totally, it was observed 204 cases of vocalized aggressive and 955 cases of vocalized courting behaviours during 1690 hours of focal observations. Fig. 2. Relative proportions of vocalized courting (A) and aggressive displays (B) in adult males at different paces over months.

Page 19 of 24

20   

cr

ip t

                     

us

2.5 2

an

1.5

M

1 0.5 0 Jun

Jul Aug Months

Aggres

Court

Oct

Nov

Dec

 

Ac ce p

589  590 

May

te

Apr

d

Display frequency (act/h)

578  579  580  581  582  583  584  585  586  587  588 

Page 20 of 24

21   

  A. 

Mar

May

Jun

Standing

592    B.   

Jul Aug Months

Walking

Trotting

Oct

Nov

Dec

Galloping

 

M

593  594  595 

Apr

us

cr

ip t

100% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0%

an

Proportion

590  591 

d te

70% 60% 50% 40% 30%

Ac ce p

Proportion

100% 90% 80%

20% 10% 0%

Mar

596 

Apr

May

Standing

Jun

Jul Aug Months

Walking

Trotting

Oct

Nov

Galloping

Dec

 

597 

Page 21 of 24

22   

Table  1.  Description  of  the  recorded  courting  displays  and  roaring  (ethogram)  in  adult  males  goitered gazelle during the rut according to Walther et al. (1983) and Blank (1998).    Courting postures 

Description  Stretching the head and neck forward horizontally with the nose pointing  horizontally forward. Auricles are directed backward and usually pressed to  the  head  in  a  position  either  parallel  to  the  ground  or  slightly  downward  with the aperture inward. Rarely auricles lay back, pressed to each other,  or  held  one  apart  of  the  other  at  a  45°  angle.  Tail  is  lifted  in  different  degrees, up to vertical position. 

  High‐stretch or  nose‐lift or  head‐up   

  Head  and  neck  are  stretched  forward  and  upward  up  to  45°  above  the  horizontal  level,  the  top  of  the  muzzle  lifted  forward  and  upward,  and  auricles pressed to the head. 

        Roaring 

Roaring  consists  of  gruff,  wheezing  or  hissing  sounds  resembling  gurgling  or sneezing. In producing these sounds, the male opens his mouth widely,  lifts his tongue slightly and takes a deep breath, then passes air through his  partly closed mouth and possibly as well as his nostrils. During a long and  fast gallop, the male’s mouth is open and his tongue out.  Posture:  Muzzle  is  raised  to  an  angle  of  up  to  45°  from  a  horizontal  position. Auricles are directed backward and tightly pressed, more or less,  to  the  head  by  apertures  inside.  Abruptly,  the  male  jerks  his  head  and  whole  body  forward  and  upward,  and  his  protruding  thyroid  cartilage  moves down and up. During the run, the roaring is intermittent.   

Aggressive postures  Dominance 

te

d

M

an

us

cr

ip t

    Low‐stretch or  neck‐stretch 

Description 

Rivals  stand  in  lateral  T‐position,  parallel  or,  most  commonly,  reserve‐parallel  orientation.  The  erected  posture,  when  the  neck  is  vertically  erected  upwards  “proudly”.  The  entire  body  stays  as  high  as  possible due to stretching all  four legs upwards. The nose is pressed  to a  neck and horns are directed forward and the head is turned in a more or  less pronounced fashion away from opponent. Tail is uplifted and in waving  movements  from  one  side  to  another;  ears  are  apart  horizontally  and  directed  sidewise  by  tops,  and  their  holes  are  directed  forward  or  downward. All walking movements are unusually slow. 

Ac ce p

597  598  599 

Threat 

Horns  are  directed  forward.  Ears  are  directed  forward‐sideward;  tail  is  normal position. 

Horn‐pressing   

The rivals approach each other with lowered heads to the ground without  preliminary displays of threat and lean  the frontal surfaces of their  horns  against  each  other.  They  fight  while  standing  on  all  four  legs  with  the  forelegs widely spread, and hindlegs in normal position or relatively close  together.  Each  of  them  tries  to  press  the  horns  of  the  opponent  to  the  ground  and  move  him  back.  Then  everyone  hits  the  revival’s  head  with  maximum  power  and  presses  the  head  to  the  ground  and  back  again, 

Page 22 of 24

23   

marking jerks. The horns almost invariably interlock (horn of one between  horns  of  another).  This  fight  can  transfer  to  fight‐circling,  when  rivals  do  circles  with  locked  horns.  In  this  situation,  they  pivot  with  their  hindquarters while keeping their heads in the circle center. During fighting,  the ears are directed sidewise by top horizontally or backward sometimes  and tail is pressed. This fight is common for non‐territorial males.       

te

d

M

an

us

cr

ip t

 

Ac ce p

600  601 

Page 23 of 24

24   

te

d

M

an

us

cr

ip t

Highlights  The  quantitative  characteristics  of  vocalized  male  displays  in  goitered  gazelle  have  never  been  considered before now.    Male  vocalizations  were  mostly  related  to  courting  females  and  less  often  observed  during  aggressive displays.    Aggressive calls of territorial male were used primarily for expelling male‐intruders.  Rutting male vocalizations likely promote synchronization of breeding cycles for most females in  the population.         

Ac ce p

601  602  603  604  605  606  607  608  609 

Page 24 of 24