Scene-based and viewer-centered representations for comparing shapes

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations for comparing shapes

CognitioPl,30 (198$) l-35 viewer-centered (i.e., heod- onses also regressedtowa the first author was supported by grant87-2-36Jromthe Alfred P. Sl...

4MB Sizes 0 Downloads 22 Views

CognitioPl,30 (198$) l-35

viewer-centered (i.e., heod-

onses also regressedtowa

the

first author was supported by grant87-2-36Jromthe Alfred P. Sloan Foundation. The -xcond author P.B-;*;*,~Y q+n*n ..--MPTQc?%lQ for _gU‘“.” ._,.I ..M from A.F. Sitian Fciuii ational Institute of Health. We thank NRSA Fellowship F32 anonymous reviewer for he and, Steve Pinker, Roger should be addressedto: LawrenceM. Parsons,Departmentof Fsycholo Hell 330, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, U.S.A.

sevier Science

2

GE. Hindonand L.

rsom

‘An alternative, but bss common, hypothesis is that spatial structure is encoded by complex relational features such as “contains three points that form a triangle with angles of 30,60,90 degrees” or contains “5 line endings.”

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

3

resentation of an object is the easiest to vantage that it changes with ev ‘ect-based frames are both us

ects, some of which may ect-based frame for the

e v~ksal system ne

,

to store where ea

of objects, the stored e orientation, and size) ca

all errors to accu

4

GE. Hinton and L.

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

5

sof~eu~ous for ea

1cortex is r&m-

re relative to variou

tion of the fixati ead-based (or scene-based) found such neurons in area

tated into alignment with

tion that is changing is relative to a scene-based ntation relative revity, we refer

‘When the relation of the viewer to the sceneis very rapidly changing,it is conceivablethat the overhead of updating the scene-basedframe would make it more practicalto use only two frames, the object-basedand viewer-basedframes.

6

at was near see

rientation that regre

each object to perceive each object with respect to a body-based

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

e

of reference. en subjects

In

xperiment 5, we fou the same pattern of alignco aring objects with quite different of

S.

respect to the viewer-centered identical views of each obje

Figure 2.

7

line-drawing illustrations of the two experimental stimuli.

8

GE.

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

of viewer-centered

9

scene-based a&aments when obdegrees apart. (a) left at 315 degrees, tered a~i~n~nt; and (c) scene-based

GE. Hinton and L.

rsons

rocedure

The was to entical or mirror were te mke

ing

a trial su c~~~a~is~n object

Scene-based and Gewcr-cewrred representations

ent no more

Yk

12

GE. Hinton and L.

le 1.

Eugene 1: son object

~ien~atio~ to

Direction of subjects’ rotation

Orientation of stan 315 degrees

Nora. Based on 12 subjects and a to in Figure 3.

observations. Degrees refer to compass directions

&cause the comparison object was never presented in perfect aiignment with the viewer-centered frame, when identical objects differed in orientation by 90 degrees, a clockwise rotation was always the shortest angle to turn the object to achieve alignment in the scene-based frame. As expected, subjects chose the shortest angle of rotation 83 percent of the time in thes:: cases.

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

13

Scene-based and vietier-centered representations

Table 2.

v&ion oforientath

15

to which subjects

em angular difference Parallel

180

270

179.9 (4.05)

269.6 (2.90)

160

200

340

175.6 (8.16)

215.6 (23.43)

357.4 (10.45)



135

225

315

67.91(26.31)

159.73 (7.71)

248.09 (19.71)

336.46 (16.03)

0

90 1.0 (2.52)

~~~-P~el

70 f!k

Sk

(7.32)

91.7 (2.19)

on 11 subjects. Degrees refer to the compass directions in F&we 3.

tatio

on s

.75 (2.13)

14.95 (11.63)

23.05 (14.59)

16

GE. Hinton mui L.

rsons

Scene-based and viewer-centered representWorn

clt

been

in any

related

ex

17

ents,

ent 2 that used

eanm difference

0.33 (2.40)

90.67 (2.11)

M.ll(2.33)

271.22 (2.82)

22 (11.25) 343.22(20.13) Skewed

Note. B

45

135

225

74.22 (28.83)

167.56 (14.25)

245.

on 9 subjects.

Of

ali

grees refer to the compass dkmtions in

315

3.

G.G3(2.31 j

23.83 (21.88>

Scene-based and viewer-centeredrepresentations

Table 4.

19

-based ~ig~~~t, perceived perfect scene&ued aligm dard &v&ion oforietzmion to which subjectsrotated ‘k-a!compa.r&m object Meananghr difference 0 0.1s (2.13) 70

45 58.11(8.86)

90 9-0.11(2.18)

180

270

180.67 (2.31)

270.79 (2.53)

160

0.42 (.7)

340

169.11(19.33)

223.00(13.15)

344.44 (22.11)

135

225

315

239.89 (12.66)

333.56 (8.47)

(13.34)

Nore. Based on 9 subjects. Degrees refer to the compass diiom

in Qure 3.

10.1%(18.05)

15.64 (11.07)

20

G. E. Hinton and L.

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

tical to those i

21

t to objecrs i&ion of orientation to

ble 5.

hallel

0

.II.__ 1.33 (1.97)

1

270

181.0 (1.29)

271.67 (2.38)

.” ---

21.29 (14.80)

.33(17.1) 45 .5 (18.70) ased on 6 subjects.

1.33 (1.54)

135

22.5

162.83 (16.77)

235.17 (16.

315

gmes refer to the compass directions in Figure 3.

26.71(14.!8)

Scene-based an$ viewer-centered representations

23

t 4: IS&Degree Separation, observer’s fines ofsight to two objects rii devia~n of orientation to

Meanaqular difference IParallel

0

90 1.5 (1.89)

Skc&?&

92.17 (3.34)

--_

180

270

181.5 (.%)

271.0

(1.83)

70

160

200

340

93.67 (2: 5)

199.17 (19.36)

24567 (16.4)

385.667 (12.66)

45

!35

225

315

94.00 (24.93)

188.0 (17.36)

272.5 (32.54)

366.67 (29.97)

ased on 6 subjects. Degrees refer to the compass diions

in Figure 3.

l.s4(1.75)

38.54 (17.83;

X29(26.76)

24

rsoras

0

had not bee

in any

relate

Scene-based and viewer-centemdrepresentations

25

tzler & Shepard Objects. Mean and whichsubjects rotatedcomparisonob-

Mean angular difference Parallel

0

90 1.43 (2.60)

Nearly-Parallel

Skewed

90.75 (2.59)

d on 8 subj

270

181.38(2.99)

271.00(1.80)

70

160

200

340

$2.63 (6.87)

174.13(27.61)

232.88 (12.92)

356.50 (13.35)

45

135

225

315

69.13 (8.95) 159.00(18.65) py.^-NC&?.

180

refer to the corn

-_

1.19 (2.56)

19.03(20.65)

247.75 (5.Ei4) 343.38 (23.37) 24.G (iS.Gj __I___I_c____I__I_~-_.~..--.-“III__IIIW-.

and L.M. Parsons

nton

Table 8.

and standard devotion of orientation to n objecrs ean angular differeiice

Parallel

0

90 2.00 (2.92)

Nearly-Parallel

91.13(1.76)

180

270

181.00 (2.45)

272.50 (1.50) ___ ~-

70

160

200

340

75.75(11.20)

175.88(13.73)

231.13(21.77)

336.00(28.54)

45

135 ---_~-~

225

3325

----_ Skewed ~----------_-_

1.66 (2.31)

12.19 (23.81)

_---_-.

79.13(18.09) 156.25 (17.89) 249.75 (9.86) 332.63 (21.52) 24.44 (17.28) ^---Ix.-~..-_xII-“.~_l._l____ll-_-__~.-~_,lllllllll~ .DIxIII~__. _______--_ - _.-. ~~~I^--“_~ -___ ~“. Nofe. Based on 8 subjects. Degrees rcfcr to the compass directions in Figure 3.

Scene-based and viewer-centeredrepresentations

ali an

27

e discrimination of mirror i was less than .5 percent.

d to correct for the

a scene-based fra

ise between scenemere are three observations of reliable effects of absolute orientation on the extent of compromise between scent-based and viewer-centered alignments in the Nearly-Parallel and Skewed conditions. In two cases,this effect occurred in the replication of the Shape-Comparisontask from Experiment 2. An ANQVA of responsesin each condition was conductedon the data from all performancesof this task in Experiments 2-5. This analysisshoweda reliable effect of absoluteorientation only in the Neariy-Parallel condition (F(3.66) = 5.86, p < .oOl). The effect occurredbecausethe responsesin the 70 degreeorientation were relatively near scene-basedalignment and those in the 200 degreeorientation were relatively far from scene-basedalignment: the overall means were 10.85, 19.67,27.81, and 18.57 for the 70, 160,200, and 340 degree orientations. The only other effect of absoluteorientations was in the Nearly-Parallel condition in the Helix task (in Experiment 5). The range of compromiseobservedthere varied more widely than usualacrossall the absoluteorientations. It remains a mysterywhy these variations in compromisealignmentsoccurredfor different absoluteorienta. _-_ A :- ^.._ ru-P1a...r*rl .a,..:,... tions. ihcse effect5couid hnve ken due to uuc of the hiGiiiS iioi CGiiiiiMGihiCEu 418 VUI wbpuwr;wm wagu. far example, either the order of trials (all subjectssaw the samesequenceof trials) or the interaction between prefercncc at which it was presented(e utc 0 handed object shapeand necessaryto unde~t~nd thi her r he object from some an

28

igure 5.

Scene-based and viewer-centered representations

29

30

Scene-based and viewer-centeredrepremmtims

31

Andersen, R.A., Essick, G.K., Br Siegel, R.M. (1984). The role of eye position on the visual response of neurons in area 7a. So&~ for PleuroscienceAbstracts, 10.934. Anderson, R.A., Essick, G.K., & Siegel, R.M. (1985). The encoding of spatial location by posteriorparietal neurons. Science, 230,456-458. Arnold, V.I. (1984). crrsnophe ti@o9. Berlin: Springer-Veriag. . Triangles as ambiguous figures. American Journal of Psychology, 81,447-453. Representation of physicalspace. In A.W. Melton & E.J. Martin(Eds.), Codingprmesses in human nrcmory. Washington, DC: Winston. Attneave, F., & Arnoult, M.D. (1956). The quantitativestudy of shape and pattern perception. Psychological Bulletin, 53,452471.

Attneave, F., % Farrar,P. (lm).

The visual world behind the head. American Journal of Psychology, 90. Discriminabilityof stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation. 74, M!H57. a pointer into perceived and imaginedspace.

chology,

Vo~unta~ control of frame of referenceand slope equivalence under head rrhre~l Fsychdogy, 78, 153-159. : Structureand function. The Behavioral and Brain sciemes, 9,67-120.

32

G.E. Hinton and L.

Rirsons

y tilt on apparentverticality,apparentbody position, and their relation. Bauermeister,M. (1964). Effect of Jowt& of fiperimed Psychology,67.142-147. Biederman, I. (19gl). On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In . lwovy UL J. Pomerantz (Eds.), Petwpawl Orgtw&ation. HilWe, NS: Erlbaum. Belation between cognitive and motor-oriented Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heir, 8.. L Nagie, M. (1 Ps@to&gy: Human Perceptionand systems of visual position perception. Jo& of Pe@ormufKe,5.692-700. of IEEE hitmnatiod CaMan, J., & Weiss, Ii. (1985). A model

II W -

atadspace in vision.Psycldogicd Review,7.325-343.

H.L. Boitblat, T.G. 975). Meotal rotation of random

nsional shapes. Ci?gniiivePsydaokbgy,7,2W3.

ExpcrimrnralPsychology:Human R.N. (1973). Chrommextic studiesof the rotation of mental images. In W.G.

CO co

.C., Nagourney. B.A., Shetzer, L.I., & Stefanatos, 6. (1918). Mental rotation under head tiitz Factorsinfluencing the location of subjective reference frame. Pemption d Psychophyk~.~, 24,263273. tection of symmetryas a fun&on of angularorientation. loud ?erception& I, 221-m. C.E. (1976 in mentai rotation? Pemeptiomdt

Day, R.H., (StWade, NJ. (1969). Mechanismsinvolved in visualorientation constancy. J%ycMogicofBuWn, 71,33-42. Giiinsky, A. (1955). The effect of attitude on the perception of size. AmericanJownd of Psychol@~,68, 173-192. Girgus, J.S., Gellman, LH., & Ho&erg, J. (19gtJ).The effect of spatial order on piecemeal shape recognition: A developmentalstudy. Perception& Psychophysics, 2&133-138. Gross,C.G., Bocha randa, CE,, & Bender, D.B. (t972). Visual propertiesof neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the macaque.Jod of Aletmphysiofogy, 35.96-111. Hinton, GE. (1981a). A parallelcomputation that assignscanonical object-based frames of reference. Proceedingsof the 7thlntematiod Joint Conferenceon Artijkialhueliigence.Vancouver, BC. Hinton, G.E. (1981b). Shape representation in parallel systems. Proceedingsof the 7th ham&ma/ J&t Conferenceon Adfxial Intelligence.Vancouver, BC. Hinton, G.E., & Parsons, L.M. (1981). Frames of reference and mental imagery. In A.D. Baddeley and 1. Long (Eds.) Attentionand Perfomtance(Vol. 9, pp. 241-277). Hiiisdaie, NJ: Erlbaum. Hochberg, J. (i96g). in the mind’seye. in &ii. Haber (Ed, j, &nfemporasrjl &oq & ~smmh h ii&& perception.New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Hochberg, J. (1982). How big is a stimulus? In J. Beck (Ed.), Orguttizution and representation in perceptiot~ (pp. 191-217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Scene-based and viewer-centeredrepresentations

33

Hock, H., & TromIey, CL. (1978). Mental rotation and perceptualuprightness.Perception & Psychophysics, 24.529-533.

Howard, I.P. (lM6). The perception of posture, self motion, and the visual ve&al. In K.R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J.P. Thomas(Eds.), Hadbookofperceptionandhumanperfomance (Vol. I: sensory processes dpem@on). New Yorkz Wiley. Hub& D.H., & V&M, T.H. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction, and functhal architectureiz the cat’s v&x! cortex. Journal of Physioiogj @atdon), 140,10&154. Hub& D.H., & Whel, T.H. (1974). Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: A parallel relationshipbetween field size, ~LWT, znd ma@ication factor. Journal of Gwnpadve Neurology, IS8,295-302. Just, MA.. & Carpcmter,P.A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems. Psychologicd Review, 92,137-17~1. KaH, P., B P~EXMU,L.M. (1981). optical informationand practicein the disuimhation of 3-D mirror-refhted objects. Percepiion, IO, 545-562. Keeml, J. 8~ M~are, R.E. (1979). Memory for images of concealed objects: A reexamination of Neisser aiid . Jouii of Expdimd Psychokqy: Hmazii Learning and Kemory, 5,374-W. KaedlilIk. J.J. (1984). m internal rrpnsentacion of solid shape and visual expIoration.In L. Spihan & RR. Woott--c??~ Sensory eqxrhce, udqtu&=, ~&pemeption (pp.132142). HiMale. NJ: Erlbaum. Koeoderink. J-J., 8t Doom, A.J. van (1976). Siies of the visual mapping. Bidogid Cybrmclicp, 24, 51-59. Kaenderink, I. J., dprIhmm, A.J. van (1979). The internal representationof solid shape with respectto vision. Biokqid Cybmetics, 32.211-216. Kosslyn. SM. (1980). hage and mind. Cambridge,EUIA: bard University Press. Lynch, J.C. (1.980).The MonaI organht~n of posterior parietalassociation cortex. 77~ Behuviorulcurd Brain Sciences, 3,~S34. Mach, E. (1897). ?%eMolysY of seamdons. English translation(1959), New York: Dover. Marr, D., & Nii, H.K. (1978). Representation and recagaition of the spatial organization of threedimensional shapes. procbcdingsof the Royal Society, Series B, 200,~294. McCroq. C. (1980). Profiru of su&ces. Covenuy: Uoiversity of Wtick. McDermott, D., & Davis, E. (1984). planning routes through uncertain territory. Ar@ci&ffnteUigence,22, 101-156. Met&r, J. (1973). Chgnihe anahgues of the m&don of thnr-dimmsionol objects. Unpublished doctoraI dissertation, Stanford University. Met&r, J., & Shepard, R.N. (1974). Transformationalstudies of the internal representationof thedimensional objects. In R. S&o (Ed.), llrrories in cognitivepsychok?~: The Loyofa Symposium (pp. 174201). Hills&k, NJ: Erlbaum. Mishkin, M., Ungcrleider, L.G., & a&o, K.A. (1983). object vi&u! ad spatia! vision: ‘TLvocortical pathways. Trrndrin Neuroscience%6,414417. Minsky, ML., & Papert, S. (1969). Percepfrons, Cambridge,MA: MIT Press. or-m, M-J., Findlay, J.M., & Watt, R.J. (1982). Aperture viewing: A review and a synthesis. Qtierly Journal of Experime~ Psychology, 34A, 211-233. Noda, II., & Warabi, T. (1986). Dies of Purkinjecells in monkey’s floaxdus duringpursuit eye movements and visual stimulus movements. wrimentul Neurofugy, 93,39O-i03. GIson, R.K., % Attaeave, F. (1970). What variables produce similarity grouping? American Jowl of &ycholo#y, 83, l-21. Palmer, S.E. (1975). Visual roeption and world knowledge: Notes on a model of sensory cognitive interaction. In D.A. Norman 8t D.E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Expio~tions in cognition. San Francisco: WM. Prec!. Palmer, S.E. (1977). Hiera&ical structurein perceptual represeMation.Cogdive Psycho@Y, %441--4% rr SE., BrBuchet, N. (1981). Configuraleffects in perceived pointing of ambiguoustria@as. Journal P ofi!lqmhenurl P8ydwlogy: Human Pemeptio+ad P6yformanm, 7,~114.

doweusevhal

Van lEssen,D.C. (1985).Functionalorgmixationof primate visual aster. In A. Peters 8 E.G. Jones, l7te iX&hilGXitTX

(Vd

3;. SW

Y&Z

Fi~iBiifti &SS.

Scene-hased

viewer-centeredrepresentations

35