Accepted Manuscript Screening for cognitive and behavioural impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: Frequency of abnormality and effect on survival
Zhouwei Xu, Ashwag Rafea S. Alruwaili, Robert David Henderson, Pamela Ann McCombe PII: DOI: Reference:
S0022-510X(17)30156-9 doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2017.02.061 JNS 15196
To appear in:
Journal of the Neurological Sciences
Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:
15 November 2016 1 February 2017 24 February 2017
Please cite this article as: Zhouwei Xu, Ashwag Rafea S. Alruwaili, Robert David Henderson, Pamela Ann McCombe , Screening for cognitive and behavioural impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: Frequency of abnormality and effect on survival. The address for the corresponding author was captured as affiliation for all authors. Please check if appropriate. Jns(2017), doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2017.02.061
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Screening for Cognitive and Behavioral Impairment in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis: frequency of abnormality and effect on survival
Zhouwei Xu1,*, Ashwag Rafea S Alruwaili1, Robert David Henderson2, Pamela Ann McCombe1
PT
1. The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Centre for Clinical Research, Queensland, Australia
SC
RI
2. Department of Neurology, Royal Brisbane & Women’s Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
* Corresponding author
NU
Email:
[email protected]
MA
Address: Level 4 UQCCR building 71/918, RBWH, Herston, Brisbane, Australia.
AC C
EP T
ED
Telephone number: 07-33466022
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract: Objective: To screen for cognitive and behavioral impairment in people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and controls with neuromuscular disease and to correlate these with clinical features. Methods: 108 people with ALS and 60 controls with other neuromuscular diseases
PT
were recruited and assessed with the Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination-III (ACE-
RI
III), the Frontal assessment battery (FAB), and the executive function component of
SC
the Edinburgh cognitive and behavioral ALS screen (ECAS). The Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis-Frontotemporal dementia questionnaire (ALS- FTD-Q) and the Motor
NU
Neuron Disease Behavioral instrument (MiND-B) were administered to the caregivers of people with ALS. The prevalence of abnormalities was determined and correlated
MA
with clinical features and survival. In 37 people with ALS, serial studies were
ED
performed.
Results: The frequencies of cognitive impairment based on the ACE-III and FAB
EP T
were 30.0% and 14.0%, in ALS and 11.7% and 3.3% in controls, respectively. Age and years of education influence the results of the ACE-III and ECAS executive
AC C
function. In ALS, the frequencies of behavioral impairment based on ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B were 32.1% and 39.4 %, respectively. There is significant correlation of ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B with the ALSFRS-R score. ALS participants with cognitive impairment measured with ACE-III had significantly shorter survival time than those without. ALS participants with behavioral impairment measured with ALS-FTD-Q had worse prognosis than those without. No significant difference was found between the first two serial cognitive tests based on ACE-III and FAB by using generalized estimating equation.
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Conclusion: There is a greater frequency of cognitive impairment in people with ALS than in patients with other neuromuscular diseases. The cognitive and behavioral tests are potential biomarkers of the prognosis of ALS. The results of cognitive tests are
lateral sclerosis,
cognitive
SC
Keywords: Amyotrophic
RI
PT
stable over 6 months and possibly longer.
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
impairment, survival, longitudinal study
3
impairment,
behavioral
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Introduction Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis is defined by progressive loss of upper and lower motor neurones [1] . Some people with ALS also have frontotemporal dementia (FTD) while others have lesser degrees of cognitive impairment [2]. Some of the overlap between ALS and FTD is associated with the presence of a C9orf72 gene mutation [3,
PT
4]. Different approaches have been used to study the frequency of cognitive
RI
impairment. Full neuropsychological testing is time-consuming. Therefore a variety of simple tools have been used to screen for cognitive impairment in ALS. These
SC
include Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination–III (ACE-III), an updated version of
NU
the ACE-R [5-8], the Frontal assessment battery (FAB) [9-11], the Edinburgh cognitive and behavioral ALS screen (ECAS), which includes tests of social cognition,
MA
[12-16] and the ALS cognitive behavioral screen (ALS CBS) [17]. Table 1 shows the elements tested in the different tools. Other scales have been developed to detect
ED
behavioral dysfunction in ALS [18, 19]. These include the Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis-Frontotemporal dementia questionnaire (ALS-FTD-Q) and the Motor
EP T
Neurone Disease Behavioural instrument (MiND-B), which are administered to carers. With such screening tools, the incidence of the cognitive impairment in ALS
AC C
ranges from 30 to 75% [20-23] and the rate of behavioral impairment ranges from 14% to 40% [12, 23-25] (Table 2).
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ACE-III
FAB
ECAS
ALS CBS
Attention
Yes*
No
No
Yes
Memory
Yes*
No
Yes
No
Verbal fluency
Yes*
Yes*
Yes
Yes
Language or conceptualization
Yes*
Yes*
Yes
No
Visuospatial
Yes*
No
RI
No
Prehension behaviour
No
Yes*
No
No
Executive function
No
Yes*
Yes*
Yes
Social cognition (as part of executive function)
No
Yes*
No
PT
Cognitive Domain
NU
Table 1 Summary of domains tested by screening tools used in ALS
SC
No
MA
No
AC C
EP T
ED
ALS CBS: ALS cognitive behavioural screen ECAS: Edinburgh cognitive and behavioral ALS screen ACE-R: Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination revised FAB: Frontal assessment battery *Test used in the present study
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 2 Summary of the recent papers in cognitive and behavioral impairment Measurement
Country/year
No of patients
Percentage of Percentage of cognitive behavioral impairment impairment ALS CBS USA/2016 274 54% 14% ECAS China/2016 84 36 % 27 % ECAS Italy/2016 107 37% 54% ALS CBS USA/2015 86 62% 37% FAB Greece/2015 34 50% N/A ALS CBS Spain/2015 50 46% 18% ACE-R China/2014 145 30% N/A FAB Canada/2014 44 21% N/A MoCA Canada/2014 39 53% N/A FAB and FBI China/2014 126 32% 46% ECAS Germany/2014 136 22% 30% FAB USA/2009 16 50% N/A ALS CBS: ALS cognitive behavioral screen ECAS: Edinburgh cognitive and behavioral ALS screen ACE-R: Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination revised FAB: Frontal assessment battery FBI: Frontal behavior inventory MoCA: Montreal cognitive assessment
Reference
T P
Murphy et al.[23] Ye et al.[13] Poletti et al.[16] Bock et al.[26] Nidos et al.[27] Turon-sans et al.[28] Wei et al.[22] Osborne et al.[29] Osborne et al.[29] Wei et al.[30] Lule et al.[14] Oskarsson et al.[31]
I R
C S
U N
D E
A M
T P
E C
C A
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Current evidence suggest that some people with ALS have cognitive and behavioral impairment, and others do not. The cause of this is unclear. As already mentioned, carriage of a repeat expansion in C9orf72 is a cause of cognitive impairment in ALS, but there could be other factors. There have been attempts to determine whether clinical features are correlated with the presence of cognitive and
PT
behavioral testing. Some studies have found cognitive impairment to be more common
RI
in people with bulbar onset than limb onset of ALS [32]. However, other studies
35]. Therefore further information is required.
SC
suggest that site of onset has no influence on the presence of cognitive impairment [33-
NU
There have also been studies of whether the presence of cognitive or behavioral impairment affects prognosis, as might be the case if the presence of cognitive
MA
impairment indicates more severe or widespread disease. Some studies have suggested that the presence of cognitive impairment [36] or behavioral abnormality is associated
ED
with worse prognosis [37]. However, other studies suggest that cognitive impairment
EP T
has no influence on survival [35]. Therefore further information is required. Another possibility is that cognitive impairment, and other extramotor features of
AC C
ALS, might develop in all people with ALS over time, as disease spreads more widely [38]. If this were the case, cognitive impairment would correlate with disease severity, but there is little information about this. There is conflicting information about whether there is later development of cognitive impairment in patients who are cognitively normal at onset of ALS [39-42] . Further information is required about this issue as well. The first aim of the present study is to measure cognitive and behavioral impairment in the ALS patients by using 5 different clinic based questionnaires (ACE-
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT III, FAB, the executive component of the ECAS, ALS-FTD-Q, MiND-B), and to compare with controls with other neuromuscular disease, with the hypothesis that cognitive and behavioral impairment is greater than in neuromuscular disease. The rationale for this study is that people with neurological disorders would be more suitable controls than healthy people, because people with neuromuscular disease
RI
the same difficulties as people with ALS attending our clinic.
PT
attending the same department would have similar background and experience many of
SC
The second aim is to determine whether the presence of cognitive and behavioral impairment is related to the clinical features of the patient, to test the hypothesis, based
NU
on previous reports, that bulbar onset of ALS is correlated with cognitive impairment, and that age and education would influence cognition in ALS, as is the case in healthy
MA
people [43] and that cognitive impairment is related to disease severity as would be the case if cognitive impairment occurs with more severe and widespread disease. The
ED
rationale for this was to provide further information about this unresolved issue. The
EP T
third aim is to evaluate the effects of cognitive and behavioral impairment on survival, with the hypothesis that the presence of cognitive impairment indicates more severe
AC C
and wide spread disease, and would be expected to worsen prognosis. The final aim is to determine if there is deterioration in cognition over time. The reasoning behind this study was to investigate whether patients who are cognitively normal at onset develop dysfunction over time, or whether ALS can be divided into people with cognitive issues and people without cognitive issues. Methodology
1.1 Subjects: People with probable or definite ALS [1] were recruited
from the
multidisciplinary MND clinic at Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital (RBWH). The 8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT caregivers of the patients were also asked to complete questionnaires about the behaviour of the person with ALS. There was no systematic genetic screening; however, 5 people with ALS are known to have causative mutations (3 with C9orf72 repeat expansions, one with transactive response DNA binding protein (TARDBP) mutation and one with superoxide dismutase 1(SOD1) mutation). Of 81 people with
PT
ALS for whom there was information about the family history of ALS, 10 had familial
SC
with ALS, of whom 10 had a positive family history.
RI
ALS. Information about a family history of dementia was available from 54 people
The controls were people with neuromuscular diseases with no evidence of disease
NU
of the central nervous system. Informed written consent was obtained from all
committee (HREC/15/QRBW/68).
1.2 Clinical details:
MA
participants and caregivers. This research project was approved by the RBWH ethics
ED
The age, gender, the date of onset of symptoms and the site of onset of symptoms
EP T
were recorded. The revised ALS Functional Rating Scale (ALS-FRS-R) [44] was recorded at each visit. When applicable, the date of death was recorded and the length
AC C
of survival from onset of disease to death or censoring was calculated.
1.3 Cognitive and behavioral tests All participants were tested with the ACE-III, which has a maximum possible score of 100. A score of less than 88 was defined as possible cognitive impairment [45]. The FAB, with a maximum possible score of 18, was also administered to all participants. A score of less than 12 was regarded as high possibility of cognitive impairment [10]. The executive function section of the ECAS (which includes reverse digit span, alternation, sentence completion and social cognition) was added to the testing panel 9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT later in the study. It was administered to 41 people with ALS and 30 controls. The cutoff value for cognitive impairment is 33 of 48 [12]. Some people with ALS were unable to complete the tests due to physical disability. This was the case for 19 people for the ACE-III and for 3 for the FAB. To deal with this, we calculated the percentage of correct responses of the items that were tested, and then estimated the final score as
PT
this percentage of the total possible. This approach is an approximation, but has been
RI
used by others in this field [46].
abnormalities.
SC
Carers were tested with ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B to detect behavioral The MiND-B evaluates disinhibition, apathy and stereotypical
NU
behaviour. The cutoff values for ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B were 29 and 34, respectively [18, 19]. The relationship between the carer and the person with ALS was
MA
also recorded.
ED
To evaluate any changes over time, serial cognitive tests, at intervals of 6 months or more, were obtained from 37 patients.
EP T
1.4 Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS 20 software. To compare groups,
AC C
ANOVA was used, if the variables were normally distributed. Otherwise, the data were compared with the Mann-Whitney U test for two independent groups. To compare the proportions of groups, the χ2 test was used. To determine whether cognitive and behavioral function were influenced by clinical features, multiple logistic regression was used to evaluate the effects of age, gender, disease duration at the time of testing, years of education, site of onset and relationship of the carer to the person with ALS, To determine whether there was an association between disease severity measured by ALSFRS-R and scores of cognitive and behavioral testing, we used the Spearman rank test. To assess the effects of cognition and behavior on survival, Kaplan-Meier curve 10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT and log rank test were utilized to compare survival between patients with normal and those with abnormal results. A Cox proportional regression model was used to report the hazard ratio and 95% confidence interval for each cofactor affecting prognosis. A generalized estimating equation model was used to test the longitudinal data. The model based estimator was chosen in the covariance matrix. Unstructured working
PT
correlation matrix was selected. P<0.05 was considered as significant.
RI
Results
SC
1.1 Clinical features
Clinical features of patients and controls, and numbers of participants for each test,
MA
in Error! Reference source not found..
NU
are given in Table 3. The diagnoses of the people with neuromuscular disease are given
Table 3 Clinical features of the patients and controls Other neuromuscular disease 60
P value#
68/40
35/25
0.5550
63.8±10.6
62.3±12.1
0.4025
Years of school education( median, range)
11 (5,30)
10 (7,19)
0.1160
No tested with ALSFRS-R
88
N/A
No tested with ACE-III
108
60
No needing adjustment of ACE-III*
19
N/A
No tested with FAB
108
60
No needing adjusting of FAB* No with executive section of ECAS
3
N/A
41
30
ED
ALS
Males: females
AC C
Age (mean, SD)
108
EP T
Total number
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
No tested with ALS-FTDQ
84
N/A
No tested with MiND-B
66
N/A
RI
Table 4 Diagnoses of controls
PT
*In these patients, some test items were omitted and the score was adjusted
SC
Disease category Peripheral nerve disorder
NU
Idiopathic neuropathy Carpel tunnel syndrome
MA
Diabetic neuropathy Guillain–Barré syndrome
ED
Pelvic neurofibroma
No
15 2 2 1 1 5
Anterior interosseous syndrome
1
Anti Myelin-associated glycoprotein (MAG) neuropathy
1
Brachial plexus injury
1
AC C
EP T
Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease
Cramp fasciculation syndrome
1
Chronic inflammatory demyelinating polyneuropathy
3
Schwannoma posterior tibial nerve
1
Small fibre neuropathy
2
Trigeminal neuropathy
1
Multifocal motor neuropathy
2
Muscle disease Mitochondrial myopathy
2
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Pompe disease
1
Inclusion body myositis
3
Facioscapulohumeral muscular dystrophy
5
Myopathy
1
Idiopathic hyperCKemia
1
PT
Anterior horn cell non-ALS Spinal muscular atrophy type 2
1
RI
Myasthenia gravis
60
SC
Total
7
NU
1.2 What is the prevalence of cognitive and behavioral impairment? The frequency distributions of ACE-III and FAB scores in disease controls and
MA
ALS patients are shown in Figure 1. The cognitive test scores are summarized in Table 5. There are significant differences in total ACE-III, ACE-III attention and language
ED
scores and FAB total score between the groups. There were no significant differences in the other components of the ACE-III or in the executive section of the ECAS. The
EP T
percentages of each group with cognitive impairment are shown in Table 6. Between people with ALS and controls there is a significant difference in the percentage with
AC C
abnormal results of ACE-III and FAB. However, no significant differences were found between the two groups in the scores of ECAS executive function and social cognition. Of the patients with known causative genes, one with C9orf72 repeat expansion had normal cognitive function, and the other patients (2 with C9orf72 repeat expansions, one with TARDBP mutation and one with SOD1 mutation) had cognitive impairment, defined as abnormal result on ACE-III testing.
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 5 Results of cognitive tests Test ALS Controls ACE-III total score ( median 92 (60-100) 94 (79-100) range) Attention score ( mean SD) 16.4±1.9 17.2±1.0 Memory score ( mean SD) 23.1±2.9 23.6±2.0 Fluency score ( mean SD) 10.9±3.0 11.6±2.2 Language score ( mean SD) 24.5±2.1 25.2±1.0 Visuospatial score ( mean 15.3±1.1 15.5±0.9 SD) FAB total score ( median range) 16(6-18) 17(12-18) ECAS executive score ( median 40(17-45) 40(26-47) range) Social cognition subsection 12(0-12) 12(0-12) score(median range) *: P<0.05 **: P<0.01 # Mann-Whitney U test was used.
P value# P=0.023*
RI
PT
P=0.003** P=0.552 P=0.343 P=0.024* P=0.305 P=0.004** P=0.487
NU
SC
P=0.192
Table 6 Percentage of people with cognitive impairment Cut off value for abnormality
Controls percent abnormal 11.7% 3.3% 16.7%
P value#
P=0.013* P=0.033* P=0.802
EP T
ED
ACE-III ≤88 FAB ≤12 ECAS executive ≤33 function *: P<0.05, # χ2 test was used
ALS percent abnormal 30.0% 14.0% 22.0%
MA
Test
The behavioral test scores are shown in Table 7. The frequencies of behavioral
AC C
impairment measured by ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B are 32.1% and 39.4% respectively. The rates of disinhibition, apathy and stereotypical behaviour are 9.1%, 21.2% and 7.6%, respectively.
Table 7 Results of behavioral testing Test ALS-FTD-Q score MiND-B total score Disinhibition Apathy Stereotypical behaviour
Cut off value for abnormality ≥22 <34 <13 <9 <5 14
ALS (median, range) 15(0-57) 34(14-36) 16(8-16) 11(4-12) 8(2-8)
Percentage with abnormality 32.1% 39.4% 9.1% 21.2% 7.6%
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1.3 Correlation between cognitive and behavioral impairment and other clinical features Multiple logistic regression analysis was used to investigate whether age, gender, years of education, disease duration at the time of testing and site of onset influenced
PT
the results of the ACE-III, FAB and ECAS cognitive tests. The detailed results are given in Supplementary 1 file. For ACE-III, age and years of education influence the
RI
results. One year increase in age can increase hazard odds of cognitive impairment on
SC
ACE-III testing by 5.3%, while one year increase in the years of school education can reduce hazard odds of cognitive impairment by 27%. There was no effect of site of
NU
onset, disease duration and gender on the results of ACE-III testing.
MA
For ECAS executive function, one year increase in education can decrease 26% of ratio of cognitive impairment. The other clinical factors did not affect the test result.
ED
For FAB, the site of onset affected the results. Compared with lower limb onset,
EP T
people with bulbar onset of ALS had 61% less hazard odds ratio of cognitive impairment. There is no significant correlation between other variables and cognitive
AC C
impairment as measured by FAB. The effects of the clinical variables in ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B are also presented in Supplementary 1 file. Age, gender disease duration and the relationship of the carer to the person with ALS have no significant effect on behavioral impairment. The results of ACE-III and FAB in patients with and without positive family history of ALS and dementia are summarized in Supplementary 1 file. The presence of a positive family history of ALS or dementia had no significant effect on the cognitive impairment based on ACE-III and FAB results. 15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT We assessed the correlation between disease severity measured by ALSFRS-R and cognitive function measured by ACE-III, FAB and ECAS by using Spearman correlation tests (Supplementary 2 file). There was no significant correlation. This indicates that cognition does not worsen as the disease becomes more severe.
PT
We also tested the relationship between disease severity and behavioral testing. The Spearman correlation tests between ALS-FTD-Q or MiND- B and ALSFRS-R
RI
score are shown in Figure 2. There is a significant negative relationship between ALS-
SC
FTD-Q and ALSFRS-R score. There is a significant positive association between
worse as disease becomes more advanced.
NU
MiND-B and ALSFRS-R score. This indicates that behavioral impairment becomes
1.4 Correlation with the survival time in cognitive and behavioral tests
MA
We then investigated the effect on survival of cognitive and behavioral impairment measured by ACE-III, FAB, ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B. The results for
ED
ACE-III, FAB and ALS-FTD-Q are shown in Figure 3. We first plotted Kaplan Meier
EP T
survival curves. The survival curve of MiND-B is presented in Supplementary 3 file. There is a significant effect on survival of cognitive and behavioral impairment measured by ACE-III and ALS-FTD-Q. For cognitive impairment measured by FAB,
AC C
there was an apparent effect, but this was not statistically significant. For behavioral impairment measured by MiND-B, there was no apparent effect on survival. We then performed Cox regression analysis of the effects of cognitive and
behavioral testing and other clinical features on survival. The Cox regression models of ACE-III, FAB, ALS-FTD-Q and MiND-B which included age, gender and site of onset are shown in Supplementary 3 file. Patients with cognitive impairment defined by performance on ACE-III had significant shorter survival than the patients without cognitive impairment (P=0.0276), 16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT with no additional effect of age or gender. There was no significant effect on survival time based on FAB (P=0.1746). The site of onset affected the survival time of patients with bulbar onset leading to a significant increase in comparison with the patients with lower limb onset after adjusting the age, gender and cognitive impairment ( P=0.045).
PT
Patients with behavioral impairment based on ALS-FTD-Q had significantly shorter survival than the patients without (P=0.0054). There are no significant
RI
differences between survival time of patients with and without behavioral impairment
SC
assessed with MiND-B (P= 0.5685). The Cox regression model of MiND-B illustrates that other variables including gender, age, disinhibition, apathy and stereotypical
1.5 The longitudinal study of cognition
NU
behaviour do not impact the survival time of the patients.
MA
We performed a longitudinal study of 37 people with ALS. This was to determine whether cognitive impairment develops over time in people who are
ED
cognitively normal on first testing. The longitudinal curves for 37 patients with
EP T
different sites of onset based on ACE-III and FAB are presented in Figure 4 and Figure 5 respectively. The five domains of ACE-III serial tests results are shown in Supplementary 4 file. Some patients show a decline with serial testing, but most do not.
AC C
There were fewer tests for the third and fourth time points due to death or advanced progression of the patients. For statistical analysis, we used generalized estimating equations for the first
two tests of ACE-III and FAB (Supplementary 4 file). The GEE analysis for ACE-III found that there was no significant difference between the first two consecutive tests (P=0.080). However, patients with bulbar onset of ALS have 3.25 higher ACE-III scores than the patients with lower limb onset over the time course (P=0.033).
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT The GEE analysis for FAB found no significant difference between the first two serial tests (P=1.00). For FAB, the site of onset did not affect the results of FAB performance over time (P=0.190).
Discussion
PT
This study assessed cognition and behaviour in people with ALS by using
RI
simple clinic based testing. As comparison we used people with neuromuscular disease. in our cohort, the prevalence of cognitive impairment was approximately 30% and 14%
SC
based on ACE-III and FAB, respectively This is similar to the rates of cognitive
NU
impairment found in previous studies [13, 16] (Table 2). The prevalence of behavioral impairment in our cohort is also just over 30% and approximately 40% based on ALS-
MA
FTD-Q and MiND-B. This is slightly less than that found by others [16, 30] (Table 2). Previous studies compared people with ALS with normal healthy controls [12,
ED
24] or had no controls. Our controls came from the same population as the people with
EP T
ALS. None of the controls had evidence of disease of central nervous system, although two had mitochondrial myopathy, which could indicate a multi-system disorder. Both the average scores and the frequency of cognitive impairment were significantly
AC C
different between the groups. In the ACE-III subdomains, significant differences were found in attention and language. Our study did not find fluency difference between the two groups, in contrast to previous studies that compared ALS patients with healthy controls [47, 48]. We note that the tasks and scoring of fluency vary among the different tools, reducing the ability to compare results. We used multiple logistic regression analysis to investigate factors that influence the development of cognitive or behavioural impairment in ALS. We found that age and years of education affect the cognition measured by ACE-III. This is unsurprising 18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT as age and years of education are known to affect cognitive performance in the healthy population [43] and to reduce the risk of dementia [49]. Age and lower levels of education have been observed in several previous ALS studies as risk factors for cognitive impairment [13, 21, 50], although others found no significant effect of these
would be desirable in future studies of cognition in ALS.
PT
factors [35, 51, 52]. Using age corrected or education level corrected scoring guide
RI
Previously, some studies have found that people with bulbar onset of ALS are
SC
more likely to have cognitive impairment than those with spinal onset [39, 53, 54], but others do not [33, 35, 55]. In our study, the site of onset had no effect of the presence
NU
of cognitive impairment as determined by the ACE-III, but site of onset influenced cognitive impairment based on FAB logistic regression results. In our cohort, the
MA
bulbar onset patients had decreased ratio of cognitive impairment and longer survival
ED
in the Cox regression model. This needs to be studied further. In our cohort, the patients with cognitive or behavioral impairment based on
EP T
ACE-III and ALS-FTD-Q had worse prognosis than the ones without. This is consistent with some previous studies [56, 57] but contrary to other studies [35, 58]. In
AC C
our study, stereotypical behaviour, apathy, and disinhibition do not predict the survival of ALS patients, which is consistent with one previous study using the same scale [59]. However another study found that the apathy measured with the revised version of the Cambridge behavioral inventory (CBI-R) is good indicator of survival [60]. Classifying the patients into different level of cognitive and behavioral impairment could be better than using cutoff value in predicting the prognosis. A possible explanation for the worse prognosis of patients with cognitive impairment is that they have a disease that is more widespread and extensive than those with only motor symptoms. Another possibility is that those with cognitive impairment have different 19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT causative genes than those without. The most prominent gene that causes ALS with cognitive impairment is C9orf72 [3, 4], but there are others such as TARDBP and fused in sarcoma (FUS) [61, 62]. In our longitudinal study, only a limited number of people had more than two
PT
tests. Because of variability in attendance at the clinic, the interval between two consecutive time points ranges from 6 months to 33 months. Therefore we used a
RI
generalized estimating equation (GEE) to investigate whether there is difference
SC
between first two serial tests. The GEE statistical model is very robust to abnormal distributions, missing data and heterogeneity in timing of measurement. No significant
NU
difference was found between the first two visits in both ACE-III and FAB, which suggests that the cognitive test performance is relatively stable, and these patients who
MA
are cognitively normal at first testing will remain so over time. However, more people
ED
with ALS are needed at later time points to clarify this. Previous studies showed conflicting results. One study found that in bulbar onset
EP T
patients, cognitive impairment was progressive over time [39], another found that 37% of patients developed cognitive deficit over a six month follow-up period [40], and
AC C
another found that cognitive impairment was greater at the later stages of disease, when only 14% of patients had normal cognition [41], thus providing evidence that cognition is progressively affected in ALS. The previous serial studies of cognition are summarised in Table 8. If there were a progressive deterioration of cognition over time, this could be due to the spread of pathology from motor regions to other regions, through prion- like spread, as has recently been proposed [63]. However, the finding that some patients have cognitive impairment at presentation while others remain cognitively normal throughout disease would be more consistent with ALS being a heterogeneous disease [38, 64]. 20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT One weakness of this paper is that we have only limited genotyping. Another weakness is that we used simple screening tools that may not have detected subtle deficits. However, the tools that we have used are available for use in ALS clinics and provide evidence that some patients with ALS have cognitive impairment, which is a poor prognostic marker, that patients who are cognitively normal at onset show little
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
education are risk factors for cognitive impairment in ALS.
PT
evidence of deterioration over 6 months and possibly longer, and that age and
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 8 Summary of longitudinal studies of cognition in ALS Study
Number
Results
T P
M.J. Strong et al. [34]
8
K.M. Robinson et al. [65]
19
Cognitive impairment in the patients with bulbar onset but with limb-onset appeared to progress over the time course. 7 of 19 patients had a decline of cognition over time.
F.Trojs et al. [41]
74
Cognitive impairment was more serious in the advanced stage of ALS.
S.Abrahams et al. [66]
20
No evidence of deterioration of cognition over time.
E.Kasper et al. [42]
93
No significant decline in executive function over time.
M.Kilani et al. [67]
18
No decline in cognitive function over 12 months
M.Elamin et al. [68]
98 for the second visit 48 for the third visit 52
H.Schreiber et al. [69]
I R
C S
U N
T P
D E
E C
A M
Cognitive function declined faster in patients who were cognitively impaired at the first visit. Cognitive deficits did not substantially decline on follow-up.
C A
The list and the captions of Figures
1. Figure 1: Frequency distribution of ACE-III and FAB score in ALS and controls. A: Frequency distribution of ACE-III in ALS patients B: Frequency distribution of FAB in ALS patients. C: Frequency distribution of ACE-III in disease control D: Frequency distribution o f FAB in disease control.
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
2. Figure 2: Correlation of behavioural testing with disease severity. A: ALS-FTD-Q correlation with ALSFRS-R. B: MiND-B correlation with ALSFRS-R. Significant negative correlation was found between ALS-FTD-Q score and ALSFRS-R score (P=0.0068). Significant positive correlation was found between MiND-B score and ALSFRS-R score (P=0.02). The solid red line and red curve line are the simple linear regression and its 95% confidence interval, respectively.
T P
3. Figure 3: Survival curve of ACE-III, FAB and ALS-FTD-Q. A: Kaplan Meier survival curve of ACE-III B: Kaplan Meier survival curve of FAB C: Kaplan Meier survival curve of ALS-FTD-Q.
I R
C S
4. Figure 4: ACE-III longitudinal results in three different site of onset. The longitudinal curves were plotted in every patient according to different site of onset.
U N
5. Figure 5: FAB longitudinal results in three different site of onset. The longitudinal curves were plotted in every patient according to different site of onset.
D E
A M
T P
E C
C A
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Acknowledgements: We would like to acknowledge the help of the participants in the study, and the assistance of the Susan Heggie and Kathryn Thorpe. Zhouwei Xu was supported by China Scholarship Council (CSC) scholarship.
PT
Reference [1] B.R. Brooks, R.G. Miller, M. Swash, T.L. Munsat, D. World Federation of Neurology
RI
Research Group on Motor Neuron, El Escorial revisited: revised criteria for the
SC
diagnosis of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosi s and other motor neuron disorders : official publication of the World Federation of Neurology,
NU
Research Group on Motor Neuron Diseases 1(5) (2000) 293-9.
MA
[2] L. Chare, J.R. Hodges, C.E. Leyton, C. McGinley, R.H. Tan, J.J. Kril, G.M. Halliday, New criteria for frontotemporal dementia syndromes: clinical and pathological
ED
diagnostic implications, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 85(8) (2014) 865-70.
EP T
[3] M. DeJesus-Hernandez, I.R. Mackenzie, B.F. Boeve, A.L. Boxer, M. Baker, N.J. Rutherford, A.M. Nicholson, N.A. Finch, H. Flynn, J. Adamson, N. Kouri, A. Wojtas, P.
AC C
Sengdy, G.Y. Hsiung, A. Karydas, W.W. Seeley, K.A. Josephs, G. Coppola, D.H. Geschwind, Z.K. Wszolek, H. Feldman, D.S. Knopman, R.C. Petersen, B.L. Miller, D.W. Dickson, K.B. Boylan, N.R. Graff-Radford, R. Rademakers, Expanded GGGGCC hexanucleotide repeat in noncoding region of C9ORF72 causes chromosome 9p-linked FTD and ALS, Neuron 72(2) (2011) 245-56. [4] A.E. Renton, E. Majounie, A. Waite, J. Simon-Sanchez, S. Rollinson, J.R. Gibbs, J.C. Schymick, H. Laaksovirta, J.C. van Swieten, L. Myllykangas, H. Kalimo, A. Paetau, Y. Abramzon, A.M. Remes, A. Kaganovich, S.W. Scholz, J. Duckworth, J. Ding, D.W. 24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Harmer, D.G. Hernandez, J.O. Johnson, K. Mok, M. Ryten, D. Trabzuni, R.J. Guerreiro, R.W. Orrell, J. Neal, A. Murray, J. Pearson, I.E. Jansen, D. Sondervan, H. Seelaar, D. Blake, K. Young, N. Halliwell, J.B. Callister, G. Toulson, A. Richardson, A. Gerhard, J. Snowden, D. Mann, D. Neary, M.A. Nalls, T. Peuralinna, L. Jansson, V.M. Isoviita, A.L.
PT
Kaivorinne, M. Holtta-Vuori, E. Ikonen, R. Sulkava, M. Benatar, J. Wuu, A. Chio, G. Restagno, G. Borghero, M. Sabatelli, I. Consortium, D. Heckerman, E. Rogaeva, L.
RI
Zinman, J.D. Rothstein, M. Sendtner, C. Drepper, E.E. Eichler, C. Alkan, Z. Abdullaev,
SC
S.D. Pack, A. Dutra, E. Pak, J. Hardy, A. Singleton, N.M. Williams, P. Heutink, S. Pickering-Brown, H.R. Morris, P.J. Tienari, B.J. Traynor, A hexanucleotide repeat
NU
expansion in C9ORF72 is the cause of chromosome 9p21-linked ALS-FTD, Neuron 72(2)
MA
(2011) 257-68.
[5] E. Mioshi, K. Dawson, J. Mitchell, R. Arnold, J.R. Hodges, The Addenbrooke's
ED
Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening, International journal of geriatric psychiatry 21(11) (2006) 1078-85.
EP T
[6] S. Hsieh, P. Lillo, M.C. Kiernan, J.R. Hodges, E. Mioshi, When more is needed: the utility of the frontotemporal dementia scale in ALS, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis &
AC C
frontotemporal degeneration 14(3) (2013) 169-71. [7] P. Lillo, B. Garcin, M. Hornberger, T.H. Bak, J.R. Hodges, Neurobehavioral features in frontotemporal dementia with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Archives of neurology 67(7) (2010) 826-30. [8] J.A. Matias-Guiu, R. Fernandez de Bobadilla, G. Escudero, J. Perez-Perez, A. Cortes, E. Morenas-Rodriguez, M. Valles-Salgado, T. Moreno-Ramos, J. Kulisevsky, J. MatiasGuiu, Validation of the Spanish version of Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination III for diagnosing dementia, Neurologia 30(9) (2015) 545-51. 25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [9] B. Dubois, A. Slachevsky, I. Litvan, B. Pillon, The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside, Neurology 55(11) (2000) 1621-6. [10] A. Slachevsky, J.M. Villalpando, M. Sarazin, V. Hahn-Barma, B. Pillon, B. Dubois, Frontal assessment battery and differential diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia and
PT
Alzheimer disease, Archives of neurology 61(7) (2004) 1104-7. [11] S.W. Ahn, S.H. Kim, J.E. Kim, S.M. Kim, S.H. Kim, J.J. Sung, K.W. Lee, Y.H. Hong,
RI
Frontal assessment battery to evaluate frontal lobe dysfunction in ALS patients, The
SC
Canadian journal of neurological sciences. Le journal canadien des sciences neurologiques 38(2) (2011) 242-6.
NU
[12] S. Abrahams, J. Newton, E. Niven, J. Foley, T.H. Bak, Screening for cognition and
degeneration 15(1-2) (2014) 9-14.
MA
behaviour changes in ALS, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal
ED
[13] S. Ye, Y. Ji, C. Li, J. He, X. Liu, D. Fan, The Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioural ALS
e0155496.
EP T
Screen in a Chinese Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Population, PloS one 11(5) (2016)
[14] D. Lule, C. Burkhardt, S. Abdulla, S. Bohm, K. Kollewe, I. Uttner, S. Abrahams, T.H.
AC C
Bak, S. Petri, M. Weber, A.C. Ludolph, The Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioural Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Screen: a cross-sectional comparison of established screening tools in a German-Swiss population, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 16(1-2) (2015) 16-23. [15] E. Niven, J. Newton, J. Foley, S. Colville, R. Swingler, S. Chandran, T.H. Bak, S. Abrahams, Validation of the Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioural Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Screen (ECAS): A cognitive tool for motor disorders, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 16(3-4) (2015) 172-9. 26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [16] B. Poletti, F. Solca, L. Carelli, F. Madotto, A. Lafronza, A. Faini, A. Monti, S. Zago, D. Calini, C. Tiloca, A. Doretti, F. Verde, A. Ratti, N. Ticozzi, S. Abrahams, V. Silani, The validation of the Italian Edinburgh Cognitive and Behavioural ALS Screen (ECAS), Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration (2016) 1-10.
PT
[17] S.C. Woolley, M.K. York, D.H. Moore, A.M. Strutt, J. Murphy, P.E. Schulz, J.S. Katz, Detecting frontotemporal dysfunction in ALS: utility of the ALS Cognitive Behavioral
RI
Screen (ALS-CBS), Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis : official publication of the World
SC
Federation of Neurology Research Group on Motor Neuron Diseases 11(3) (2010) 303-11.
NU
[18] E. Mioshi, S. Hsieh, J. Caga, E. Ramsey, K. Chen, P. Lillo, N. Simon, S. Vucic, M.
MA
Hornberger, J.R. Hodges, M.C. Kiernan, A novel tool to detect behavioural symptoms in ALS, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 15(3-4) (2014)
ED
298-304.
[19] J. Raaphorst, E. Beeldman, B. Schmand, J. Berkhout, W.H. Linssen, L.H. van den
EP T
Berg, Y.A. Pijnenburg, H.F. Grupstra, J.G. Weikamp, H.J. Schelhaas, J.M. Papma, J.C. van Swieten, M. de Visser, R.J. de Haan, The ALS-FTD-Q: a new screening tool for
AC C
behavioral disturbances in ALS, Neurology 79(13) (2012) 1377-83. [20] K. Abe, H. Fujimura, K. Toyooka, S. Sakoda, S. Yorifuji, T. Yanagihara, Cognitive function in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Journal of the neurological sciences 148(1) (1997) 95-100. [21] P.J. Massman, J. Sims, N. Cooke, L.J. Haverkamp, V. Appel, S.H. Appel, Prevalence and correlates of neuropsychological deficits in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 61(5) (1996) 450-5.
27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [22] Q. Wei, X. Chen, Z. Zheng, R. Huang, X. Guo, B. Cao, T.H. Bak, H. Shang, Screening for cognitive impairment in a Chinese ALS population, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 16(1-2) (2015) 40-5. [23] J. Murphy, P. Factor-Litvak, R. Goetz, C. Lomen-Hoerth, P.L. Nagy, J. Hupf, J.
PT
Singleton, S. Woolley, H. Andrews, D. Heitzman, R.S. Bedlack, J.S. Katz, R.J. Barohn, E.J. Sorenson, B. Oskarsson, J.A. Fernandes Filho, E.J. Kasarskis, T. Mozaffar, Y.D. Rollins,
RI
S.P. Nations, A.J. Swenson, B.A. Koczon-Jaremko, H. Mitsumoto, C. Als, Cognitive-
SC
behavioral screening reveals prevalent impairment in a large multicenter ALS cohort, Neurology 86(9) (2016) 813-20.
NU
[24] J. Phukan, M. Elamin, P. Bede, N. Jordan, L. Gallagher, S. Byrne, C. Lynch, N.
MA
Pender, O. Hardiman, The syndrome of cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a population-based study, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and
ED
psychiatry 83(1) (2012) 102-8.
[25] M. Witgert, A.R. Salamone, A.M. Strutt, A. Jawaid, P.J. Massman, M. Bradshaw, D.
EP T
Mosnik, S.H. Appel, P.E. Schulz, Frontal-lobe mediated behavioral dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, European journal of neurology 17(1) (2010) 103-10.
AC C
[26] M. Bock, Y.N. Duong, A. Kim, I. Allen, J. Murphy, C. Lomen-Hoerth, Cognitivebehavioral changes in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: Screening prevalence and impact on patients and caregivers, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration (2016) 1-8. [27] A. Nidos, D.S. Kasselimis, P.G. Simos, M. Rentzos, T. Alexakis, I. Zalonis, V. Zouvelou, C. Potagas, I. Evdokimidis, S.C. Woolley, Frontotemporal Dysfunction in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis: A Discriminant Function Analysis, Neuro-degenerative diseases 16(3-4) (2016) 140-6. 28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [28] J. Turon-Sans, J. Gascon-Bayarri, R. Rene, I. Rico, C. Gamez, A. Paipa, M. Povedano, Cognitive impairment in ALS patients and validation of the Spanish version of the ALS-CBS test, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 17(3-4) (2016) 221-7.
PT
[29] R.A. Osborne, R. Sekhon, W. Johnston, S. Kalra, Screening for frontal lobe and general cognitive impairment in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Journal of
RI
the neurological sciences 336(1-2) (2014) 191-6.
SC
[30] Q. Wei, X. Chen, Z. Zheng, R. Huang, X. Guo, B. Cao, B. Zhao, H.F. Shang, Frontal lobe function and behavioral changes in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a study from
NU
Southwest China, Journal of neurology 261(12) (2014) 2393-400.
MA
[31] B. Oskarsson, D. Quan, Y.D. Rollins, H.E. Neville, S.P. Ringel, D.B. Arciniegas, Using the Frontal Assessment Battery to identify executive function impairments in
ED
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: A preliminary experience, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis : official publication of the World Federation of Neurology Research Group on Motor
EP T
Neuron Diseases 11(1-2) (2010) 244-7. [32] H. Schreiber, T. Gaigalat, U. Wiedemuth-Catrinescu, M. Graf, I. Uttner, R. Muche,
AC C
A.C. Ludolph, Cognitive function in bulbar– and spinal–onset amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: A longitudinal study in 52 patients, Journal of neurology 252(7) (2005) 772781.
[33] G.M. Ringholz, S.H. Appel, M. Bradshaw, N.A. Cooke, D.M. Mosnik, P.E. Schulz, Prevalence and patterns of cognitive impairment in sporadic ALS, Neurology 65(4) (2005) 586-90. [34] M.J. Strong, G.M. Grace, J.B. Orange, H.A. Leeper, R.S. Menon, C. Aere, A prospective study of cognitive impairment in ALS, Neurology 53(8) (1999) 1665-70. 29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [35] G.A. Rippon, N. Scarmeas, P.H. Gordon, P.L. Murphy, S.M. Albert, H. Mitsumoto, K. Marder, L.P. Rowland, Y. Stern, An observational study of cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Archives of neurology 63(3) (2006) 345-52. [36] M. Elamin, J. Phukan, P. Bede, N. Jordan, S. Byrne, N. Pender, O. Hardiman,
PT
Executive dysfunction is a negative prognostic indicator in patients with ALS without dementia, Neurology 76(14) (2011) 1263-9.
RI
[37] W.T. Hu, M. Shelnutt, A. Wilson, N. Yarab, C. Kelly, M. Grossman, D.J. Libon, J.
SC
Khan, J.J. Lah, A.I. Levey, J. Glass, Behavior Matters—Cognitive Predictors of Survival in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis, PloS one 8(2) (2013) e57584.
NU
[38] P.A. McCombe, N.R. Wray, R.D. Henderson, Extra-motor abnormalities in
neurotherapeutics (2017) 1-17.
MA
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: another layer of heterogeneity, Expert review of
ED
[39] M.J. Strong, G.M. Grace, J.B. Orange, H.A. Leeper, R.S. Menon, C. Aere, A prospective study of cognitive impairment in ALS, Neurology 53(8) (1999) 1665.
EP T
[40] K.M. Robinson, S.C. Lacey, P. Grugan, G. Glosser, M. Grossman, L.F. McCluskey, Cognitive functioning in sporadic amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a six month
AC C
longitudinal study, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 77(5) (2006) 668-670.
[41] F. Trojsi, G. Santangelo, G. Caiazzo, M. Siciliano, T. Ferrantino, G. Piccirillo, C. Femiano, V. Cristillo, M.R. Monsurro, F. Esposito, G. Tedeschi, Neuropsychological assessment in different King's clinical stages of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal degeneration 17(3-4) (2016) 228-35. [42] E. Kasper, K. Zydatiss, C. Schuster, J. Machts, D. Bittner, J. Kaufmann, R. Benecke, S. Vielhaber, S. Teipel, J. Prudlo, No Change in Executive Performance in ALS Patients: 30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT A Longitudinal Neuropsychological Study, Neuro-degenerative diseases 16(3-4) (2016) 184. [43] M. Ganguli, B.E. Snitz, C.W. Lee, J. Vanderbilt, J.A. Saxton, C.C. Chang, Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based
PT
cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team, Aging & mental health 14(1) (2010) 100-7.
RI
[44] J.M. Cedarbaum, N. Stambler, E. Malta, C. Fuller, D. Hilt, B. Thurmond, A.
SC
Nakanishi, The ALSFRS-R: a revised ALS functional rating scale that incorporates assessments of respiratory function. BDNF ALS Study Group (Phase III), Journal of the
NU
neurological sciences 169(1-2) (1999) 13-21.
MA
[45] S. Hsieh, S. Schubert, C. Hoon, E. Mioshi, J.R. Hodges, Validation of the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's
ED
disease, Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders 36(3-4) (2013) 242-50. [46] J. Raaphorst, E. Beeldman, B. Jaeger, B. Schmand, L.H. van den Berg, J.G.
EP T
Weikamp, H.J. Schelhaas, M. de Visser, R.J. de Haan, Is the Frontal Assessment Battery reliable in ALS patients?, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis & frontotemporal
AC C
degeneration 14(1) (2013) 73-4. [47] S. Abrahams, P.N. Leigh, A. Harvey, G.N. Vythelingum, D. Grise, L.H. Goldstein, Verbal fluency and executive dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), Neuropsychologia 38(6) (2000) 734-47. [48] E. Beeldman, J. Raaphorst, M. Klein Twennaar, M. de Visser, B.A. Schmand, R.J. de Haan, The cognitive profile of ALS: a systematic review and meta-analysis update, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 87(6) (2016) 611-9.
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [49] E.C.C. Members, C. Brayne, P.G. Ince, H.A. Keage, I.G. McKeith, F.E. Matthews, T. Polvikoski, R. Sulkava, Education, the brain and dementia: neuroprotection or compensation?, Brain : a journal of neurology 133(Pt 8) (2010) 2210-6. [50] C. Lomen-Hoerth, J. Murphy, S. Langmore, J.H. Kramer, R.K. Olney, B. Miller, Are
PT
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients cognitively normal?, Neurology 60(7) (2003) 1094-7.
RI
[51] F. Cui, W. Zhu, Z. Zhou, Y. Ren, Y. Li, M. Li, Y. Huo, X. Huang, Frequency and risk
SC
factor analysis of cognitive and anxiety-depressive disorders in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis/motor neuron disease, Neuropsychiatric disease and
NU
treatment 11 (2015) 2847-54.
MA
[52] L. Jelsone-Swain, C. Persad, K.L. Votruba, S.L. Weisenbach, T. Johnson, K.L. Gruis, R.C. Welsh, The Relationship between Depressive Symptoms, Disease State, and
ED
Cognition in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis, Frontiers in Psychology 3 (2012) 542. [53] Q. Yuan, J. Jia, Reduced cognitive function in Chinese patients with early
EP T
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and associated factors, The International journal of neuroscience 120(10) (2010) 641-6.
AC C
[54] A.M. Strutt, J. Palcic, J.G. Wager, C. Titus, C. Macadam, J. Brown, B.M. Scott, Y. Harati, P.E. Schulz, M.K. York, Cognition, behavior, and respiratory function in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, ISRN neurology 2012 (2012) 912123. [55] Y. Ji, L. Wei, D. Chui, K. Wang, D. Fan, Prospective memory tasks: a more sensitive method for screening cognitive impairment in ALS?, BMC neurology 12 (2012) 142. [56] R. Rusina, P. Ridzon, P. Kulist'ak, O. Keller, A. Bartos, M. Buncova, L. Fialova, F. Koukolik, R. Matej, Relationship between ALS and the degree of cognitive impairment,
32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT markers of neurodegeneration and predictors for poor outcome. A prospective study, European journal of neurology 17(1) (2010) 23-30. [57] W.T. Hu, M. Shelnutt, A. Wilson, N. Yarab, C. Kelly, M. Grossman, D.J. Libon, J. Khan, J.J. Lah, A.I. Levey, J. Glass, Behavior matters--cognitive predictors of survival in
PT
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, PloS one 8(2) (2013) e57584. [58] R.M. Ahmed, J. Caga, E. Devenney, S. Hsieh, L. Bartley, E. Highton-Williamson, E.
RI
Ramsey, M. Zoing, G.M. Halliday, O. Piguet, J.R. Hodges, M.C. Kiernan, Cognition and
SC
eating behavior in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: effect on survival, Journal of neurology 263(8) (2016) 1593-1603.
NU
[59] E. Mioshi, J. Caga, P. Lillo, S. Hsieh, E. Ramsey, E. Devenney, M. Hornberger, J.R.
MA
Hodges, M.C. Kiernan, Neuropsychiatric changes precede classic motor symptoms in ALS and do not affect survival, Neurology 82(2) (2014) 149-55.
ED
[60] H.J. Wear, C.J. Wedderburn, E. Mioshi, C.H. Williams-Gray, S.L. Mason, R.A. Barker, J.R. Hodges, The Cambridge Behavioural Inventory revised, Dementia &
EP T
Neuropsychologia 2 (2008) 102-107.
[61] J. Sreedharan, I.P. Blair, V.B. Tripathi, X. Hu, C. Vance, B. Rogelj, S. Ackerley, J.C.
AC C
Durnall, K.L. Williams, E. Buratti, F. Baralle, J. de Belleroche, J.D. Mitchell, P.N. Leigh, A. Al-Chalabi, C.C. Miller, G. Nicholson, C.E. Shaw, TDP-43 mutations in familial and sporadic amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Science 319(5870) (2008) 1668-72. [62] C. Vance, B. Rogelj, T. Hortobagyi, K.J. De Vos, A.L. Nishimura, J. Sreedharan, X. Hu, B. Smith, D. Ruddy, P. Wright, J. Ganesalingam, K.L. Williams, V. Tripathi, S. AlSaraj, A. Al-Chalabi, P.N. Leigh, I.P. Blair, G. Nicholson, J. de Belleroche, J.M. Gallo, C.C. Miller, C.E. Shaw, Mutations in FUS, an RNA processing protein, cause familial amyotrophic lateral sclerosis type 6, Science 323(5918) (2009) 1208-11. 33
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [63] J. Brettschneider, K. Del Tredici, J.B. Toledo, J.L. Robinson, D.J. Irwin, M. Grossman, E. Suh, V.M. Van Deerlin, E.M. Wood, Y. Baek, L. Kwong, E.B. Lee, L. Elman, L. McCluskey, L. Fang, S. Feldengut, A.C. Ludolph, V.M. Lee, H. Braak, J.Q. Trojanowski, Stages of pTDP-43 pathology in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Annals of neurology
PT
74(1) (2013) 20-38. [64] E. Beghi, A. Chio, P. Couratier, J. Esteban, O. Hardiman, G. Logroscino, A. Millul, D.
RI
Mitchell, P.M. Preux, E. Pupillo, Z. Stevic, R. Swingler, B.J. Traynor, L.H. Van den Berg,
SC
J.H. Veldink, S. Zoccolella, C. Eurals, The epidemiology and treatment of ALS: focus on the heterogeneity of the disease and critical appraisal of therapeutic trials,
NU
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis : official publication of the World Fede ration of
MA
Neurology Research Group on Motor Neuron Diseases 12(1) (2011) 1-10. [65] K.M. Robinson, S.C. Lacey, P. Grugan, G. Glosser, M. Grossman, L.F. McCluskey,
ED
Cognitive functioning in sporadic amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a six month
668-70.
EP T
longitudinal study, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry 77(5) (2006)
[66] S. Abrahams, P.N. Leigh, L.H. Goldstein, Cognitive change in ALS: a prospective
AC C
study, Neurology 64(7) (2005) 1222-6. [67] M. Kilani, J. Micallef, C. Soubrouillard, D. Rey-Lardiller, C. Demattei, M. Dib, P. Philippot, M. Ceccaldi, J. Pouget, O. Blin, A longitudinal study of the evolution of cognitive function and affective state in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and other motor neuron disorders : official publication of the World Federation of Neurology, Research Group on Motor Neuron Diseases 5(1) (2004) 46-54.
34
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT [68] M. Elamin, P. Bede, S. Byrne, N. Jordan, L. Gallagher, B. Wynne, C. O'Brien, J. Phukan, C. Lynch, N. Pender, O. Hardiman, Cognitive changes predict functional decline in ALS: a population-based longitudinal study, Neurology 80(17) (2013) 1590-7. [69] H. Schreiber, T. Gaigalat, U. Wiedemuth-Catrinescu, M. Graf, I. Uttner, R. Muche,
PT
A.C. Ludolph, Cognitive function in bulbar- and spinal-onset amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. A longitudinal study in 52 patients, Journal of neurology 252(7) (2005) 772-
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
81.
35
NU
SC
RI
PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
Fig. 1
36
RI
PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
Fig. 2
37
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Fig. 3
38
NU
SC
RI
PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
Fig. 4
39
NU
SC
RI
PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP T
ED
MA
Fig. 5
40
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Highlights
PT RI SC NU MA ED EP T
Comparison of cognitive and behavioral impairment between ALS patients and patients with neuromuscular disease. Multiple cognitive and behavioral tests were used in this study. Longitudinal study was done in the cognitive tests of ALS patients.
AC C
41