Accepted Manuscript Selection for behavior and hemopoiesis in American mink (Neovison vison) A.G. Kizhina, L.B. Uzenbaeva, V.A. Ilyukha, L.I. Trapezova, N.N. Tyutyunnik, O.V. Trapezov PII:
S1558-7878(16)30149-6
DOI:
10.1016/j.jveb.2016.09.004
Reference:
JVEB 998
To appear in:
Journal of Veterinary Behavior
Received Date: 5 May 2016 Revised Date:
7 September 2016
Accepted Date: 7 September 2016
Please cite this article as: Kizhina, A.G., Uzenbaeva, L.B., Ilyukha, V.A., Trapezova, L.I., Tyutyunnik, N.N., Trapezov, O.V., Selection for behavior and hemopoiesis in American mink (Neovison vison), Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.09.004. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
Selection for behavior and hemopoiesis in American mink (Neovison vison)
2
Kizhina A.G. 1*, Uzenbaeva L.B. 1, Ilyukha V.A. 1, Trapezova L. I. 2, Tyutyunnik N.N. 1,
3
Trapezov O.V. 2
4
1
5
(11 Pushkinskaya St., 185910 Petrozavodsk, Karelia, Russia; tel. +7 (8142) 573107, Fax: (8142)
6
573107)
7
2
8
Russia (10 Prospekt Lavrentyeva, 630090, Novosibirsk, Russia; tel. +7 (383) 363-49-63*1230, Fax:
9
(3833) 33 12 78)
RI PT
*Corresponding author, E-mail:
[email protected]
SC
Institute of Cytology and Genetics, Siberian Department, Russian Academy of Sciences, Novosibirsk,
M AN U
10
Institute of Biology, Karelian Research Centre, Russian Academy of Sciences, Petrozavodsk, Russia
Abstract
12
It has been known that selective breeding of animals for behavior associated with
13
domestication changes a variety of physiological traits including response to stress and hypothalamo–
14
pituitary–adrenocortical system (HPAS) activity. The aim of our research is to find association
15
between behavioral type and differential leukocyte counts as an indicator of homeostasis activity. We
16
used farm-raised 5-mo-old minks (Neovison vison) that had been selective bred for 17 generations on
17
the basis of behavior towards human. After selection, minks were divided into the following groups:
18
aggressive (from -4 to -1), ‘domestic’ (from +1 to +5). Blood leukocytes were analyzed in 5-mo-old
19
minks (n=105) of different behavior lines: aggressive (-1, -2), ‘domestic’ (+1 and from +3 to +5),
20
fearful, and minks never subjected to selective breeding (control). We found strong effects of selection
21
for behavior of minks on differential blood leukocyte count. There were differences between behavior
22
types in differential leukocyte counts. Neutrophils and eosinophils were the leukocyte types the most
23
sensitive to selection for behavior. The counts of eosinophils in peripheral blood in ‘domestic’ (+3,
24
+5) minks were higher than in control and aggressive (–2) minks. The percentage of neutrophils was
25
higher in the most aggressive minks (–2) in comparison to ‘domestic’ minks. We have concluded that
26
differences found in leukocytes profile are the result of neuroendocrine system modification.
27 28
AC C
EP
TE D
11
Key words: leukocytes, selection, domestication, aggressiveness, hypothalamo–pituitary– adrenocortical system, American mink
1
29
1. Introduction
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
The American mink (Neovison vison), a North American native semi-aquatic generalist
31
predator, has expanded its range globally as a result of their breeding in fur farms (Dunstone, 1993;
32
Bonesi and Palazon, 2007). First attempts to keep wild individuals under human care, thus starting the
33
domestication process, have reportedly taken place around 1866 (Shackelford, 1949, 1984), when
34
trappers in Canada began to keep and breed wild mink in enclosures. Farm mink after domestication
35
differ from their wild counterparts in fur color, skull dimensions, brain, body size and temperament
36
(Belliveau et al., 1999; Kruska, 1996; Kruska and Sidorovich, 2003). Domesticated mink are bred to
37
be larger, with higher reproductive success and less aggressive than wild ones. Fearfulness and fear-
38
induced aggression in mink may be beneficial in a natural context, but may be detrimental to the
39
animal’s welfare in the captive environment. Therefore one of the first selected traits in the history of
40
mink domestication was tame behavior to humans (Trapezov, 2000).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
30
The investigation of the effects of domestication provides insight into evolution problems and
42
is of practical interest. One of the first selection experiments for domestic behavior was conducted
43
with silver fox (Vulpes vulpes) at the Institute of Cytology and Genetics, Novosibirsk, Russia
44
(Belyaev, 1979). Belyaev (1979) supposed that selection of silver fox for behavior in response to
45
human interference can be a pivotal factor of domestication. Long-term selection of farm-bred mink
46
(Neovison vison) for tame and aggressive defensive reaction towards man has been carried out at the
47
Institute’s experimental farm since the 1980s. Tame and aggressive lines of standard (+/+) minks
48
differing in their defensive response towards humans were developed using a breeding protocol
49
designed to study the effects of destabilizing selection (Klotchkov et al., 1998). As revealed in studies
50
using silver foxes and minks, behavior-controlling genes have a broad pleiotropic effect. Selection of
51
silver foxes and minks for low aggression and tameness modifies numerous regulatory processes and
52
leads to a variety of morphological, behavioral and physiological traits. Trapezov (2013) and Wilkins
53
et al. (2014) note certain traits of domestication in mammals: depigmentation, floppy ears, shorter
54
muzzles, smaller teeth, docility, smaller brain or cranial capacity, more frequent estrous cycles,
55
neotenous (juvenile) behavior, curly tails.
AC C
EP
TE D
41
56
The influence of selection on the activity of regulatory systems controlling homeostasis has
57
been demonstrated for numerous animal models. Selection for low aggressiveness to humans leads to
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
a change in behavioral manner and is accompanied by genetically–determined changes of the
59
neuroendocrine system (Klotchkov et al., 1998). A result of selection for domestic behavior in minks
60
is alteration of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical system (HPAS) activity (Trapezov, 2000)
61
and levels of serotonin and other monoamines in the mink brain (Nikulina et al., 1985). Animals
62
selected over several generations for tameness showed lower basal and stress-induced hypothalamo–
63
pituitary–adrenocortical activity, reached sexual maturity earlier and were easier to mate than more
64
fearful animals (Jeppesen and Pedersen, 1998; Nikula et al., 2000; Malmkvist, 2001).
RI PT
58
There is a need to study the association of behavioral type and differential leukocyte counts
66
as the indicator of homeostasis activity. According to some authors, the effects of HPAS under stress
67
include elevated plasma corticosteroids, reduced T and B lymphocyte proliferation and neutrophil
68
phagocytosis, increased neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio, as well as leukocyte adhesiveness/aggregation
69
(Irwin et al., 1990; Song et al., 1994). Since domestication changes neuroendocrine system activity we
70
can expect animals within distinct behavior categories to show discrepancy in differential leukocyte
71
count. As revealed in previous works by Davis et al. (2008), the counting of white blood cells from
72
blood smears (leukocyte profiles) can be used to assess stress and welfare in animals. They concluded
73
that this given method can provide a reliable assessment for stress in all vertebrate taxa.
76 77 78
M AN U
TE D
with different behavior types under prolonged experimental domestication. 2. Materials and methods
EP
75
The aim of the present study is to research differential leukocyte counts in American mink
2.1. Experimental animals
AC C
74
SC
65
Experiments were performed with standard dark brown (+/+) minks at the experimental
79
farm facility of the Institute of Cytology and Genetics (Novosibirsk, Russia). The animals were tested
80
for defensive reaction to man only once a year at 5 months age (September) by one person (Trapezov)
81
throughout the selection period (17 generations). The “hand catch test” was applied for this purpose.
82
Both males and females were tested for defensive reaction towards man during an entire day, with
83
breaks for the morning and evening feeding. The experimenter opened the cage where the tested
84
animal was kept, slowly reached for the animal, and tried to catch it with a hand protected with a
85
special mitten.
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
86
The technique allowed discrimination among three types of behavior towards man:
87
aggressive, fearful, and ‘domestic’ (tame). To study minks of different behavioral categories we
88
followed the classification of Trapezov (1987). Mink with fearful behavior tried to avoid contact with
89
man as much as possible and when the experimenter tried to catch them, they ran about the cage in
90
panic, shrieking. This behavior was assigned score “0”.
91
RI PT
Aggressive behavior varied qualitatively, which allowed scoring it on a four-point scale as follows.
93
Score – 1. Fearful response towards humans. When attempts were made to catch the mink, it rapidly
94
retreated, hid in its nest box, gaped and bared its teeth, cried shrilly or hissed; its posture was tense,
95
showing severe emotional stress.
96
Score – 2. Attack from the nest box. When attempts were made to catch the animal, it jumped to the
97
entrance of the nest box and hid there to attack the experimenter’s gloved hand from the box and bit it
98
fiercely.
99
Score – 3. Active attack outside the shelter. When attempts were made to catch the mink, it
M AN U
SC
92
immediately attacked the experimenter’s hand instead of hiding.
101
Score – 4. Onset of attack in response to a human approaching. Even before the test began, i.e. before
102
the experimenter opened the cage, the mink loudly shrieked, ran about the cage, and gnawed the bars.
TE D
100
Animals with domesticated behavior did not show any signs of fear or aggression on contact with
104
man, which allowed the experimenters to work barehanded. On the whole, the domestic behavior was
105
defined as absence of fear or defense reaction towards man. The expression of domesticated behavior also varied qualitatively, making it possible to score
AC C
106
EP
103
107
it using a five-point scale as follows.
108
Score +1. Exploratory responses. The mink calmly responded to the experimenter’s stretched hand
109
and showed an exploratory response, sniffing the hand and quivering the vibrissae.
110
Score +2. Calm response to contact with human hand. The mink accepted the forced contact with the
111
experimenter’s hand and did not retreat, allowing the hand to touch its face, chest, and paws.
112
Score +3. Active contact on the part of the animal. When the experimenter opened the cage, the mink
113
got up leaning against the open door and reached out with its snout to the human hand. Within the
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
cage, the mink actively sniffed about the hand, examined it, and often leant against the hand with its
115
paws.
116
Score +4. The mink allowed any part of its body to be touched. The animal displayed an active
117
exploratory response, examined the experimenter’s hands, played with them, but resisted all attempts
118
to be handled.
119
Score +5. The mink allowed humans to handle it. These individuals displayed the most pronounced
120
domesticated behavior, allowing the experimenters to handle them without showing any signs of fear
121
or aggression.
RI PT
114
Selective breeding specific patterns of handler-directed behavior has been ongoing for 17
123
generations based on the results of annual tests. The study reported here was performed on 5-mo-old
124
male and female minks (n=105) selected for ‘domestic’, aggressive and fearful behavior. The control
125
group included minks of the same age that had never been subjected to selective breeding. Animals
126
were chosen at random from the population of minks scoring +1,+3, +4, +5 (‘domestic’ group), and
127
from the population scoring -1, -2 (aggressive group). The sample did not include +2 and -3, -4 scored
128
minks, since animals within these groups were too few. Тable 1 shows the numbers of animals in each
129
group and their sex distribution.
M AN U
TE D
Table 1. Behavioral categories of minks used in the experiment. Behavioral categories N ‘Domestic’ Females Males +5 9 10 +4 8 9 +3 1 5 +1 2 Avoidant fear (0) 10 9 Aggressive -1 4 -2 9 8 Control (Non selected) 8 13
131
AC C
EP
130
SC
122
132
All animals were homed in regulation cages on the same farm and given diet and water. All
133
the experiments were conducted according to EU guidelines on the use of animals for biochemical
134
research (86/609/EU).
135
2.2. Blood sampling
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
136
Blood samples were obtained from claw capillaries in the morning after overnight fasting.
137
Blood smears were stained with May-Grünwald and Romanowski stains (MiniMed, Russia).
138
Differential leukocyte counts were performed by identifying 200 leukocytes in each blood smear using
139
light microscope Axioscop 40 (Zeiss, Germany) and computer software for image analysis
140
“VideoTest” (VideoTest Ltd, Russia). All investigations were performed in accordance with the “Guide for the Care and Use of
142
Laboratory Animals” published by the US National Institute of Health (NIH publication No. 85–23,
143
1996). The study conforms to the principles outlined in the Helsinki Declaration (2000).
145
The research was performed using the facilities of the Equipment Sharing Center of the
SC
144
RI PT
141
Institute of Biology, Karelian Research Centre, Russian Academy of Sciences. 2.3. Statistical analysis
147
All the collected and calculated numerical data were transformed into SI units and
148
processed statistically as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Differential leukocyte counts were
149
evaluated by nonparametric Mann-Whitney U-test for comparison between behavioral categories. We
150
conducted a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with differential leukocyte counts as
151
response variables, and sex and behavior type as factors. Linear regression analyses were employed to
152
evaluate the relationships between percentage values of eosinophil, neutrophil counts, neutrophil
153
versus lymphocyte (N:L) ratio and expression of domestication behavior.
156
157
TE D
EP
155
The statistical analyses were performed using MS-Excel® (Microsoft Corp., Inc., USA), Sigma-Stat 2.03 (Systat Software Inc., Point Richmond, California, USA). 3. Results
AC C
154
M AN U
146
Figure 1 demonstrates two lines of selection for ‘domestic’ and aggressive behavior in
158
standard (+/+) minks covering 17 generations. Selection for domestication showed a more complex
159
pattern. The selection effect declined in the 5th and 6th generations: a substantial part of the progeny
160
demonstrated aggressive reaction towards humans.
6
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Fig 1. Changes in the average behavior scores in 17 generations of minks selected for
163
domestic and aggressive behavior. The patterns of selection for ‘domestic’ and aggressive behavior
164
types differed from one another. The series of aggressive mink generations followed a uniform and
165
smooth trend for increase of aggressiveness. The progeny of the minks selected for ‘domestic’
166
behavior however included many individuals with aggressive reaction to humans in generations F5
167
and F6.
TE D
161 162
There were clear-cut distinctions in differential leukocyte counts between behavior types
169
(see Table 2). Neutrophil and eosinophil counts significantly varied between groups. The eosinophil
170
level in the blood of the minks that had not been specially selected was the lowest as compared to
171
other behavioral types. The eosinophil counts in the peripheral blood of ‘domestic’ (+3, +5) minks
172
were higher than in control and aggressive (–2) minks. In addition to ‘domestic’ minks, animals from
173
group –1 also demonstrated elevated eosinophil level compared to control and aggressive (–2) minks.
174
Animals showing cowardice or avoidance reactions (0) yielded increased eosinophil counts compared
175
to the control. The neutrophil level was lower in the blood of ‘domestic’ minks and minks from group
176
–1 as compared to aggressive (–2) minks. Animals from group +4 delivered the highest monocyte
177
counts, whereas individuals from group +5 had the lowest monocyte counts. The highest basophil
AC C
EP
168
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
178
level was detected in the blood of the most aggressive minks (–2). No significant differences in the
179
lymphocyte and band neutrophil counts were found among minks of different behavior categories. Therefore, we performed multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with differential
181
leukocyte counts as response variables, and sex and expression of domesticated behavior as factors
182
(Table 3). MANOVA revealed influence of sex on percentage values of monocytes and basophils. The
183
percentage values of neutrophils, basophils and values of neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio were influenced
184
by the expression of domesticated behavior, while there was no sex-related effect on lymphocytes and
185
eosinophils.
RI PT
180
Linear regression analysis revealed significant positive correlation between expression of
187
domesticated behavior and percentage values of eosinophils (Fig. 2C and 2D). Negative correlation
188
between expression of domesticated behavior and neutrophils number was observed (Fig. 2A and 2B).
189
The expression of domesticated behavior and values of N:L ratio were also negatively correlated with
190
each other (Fig. 2E and 2F). No significant correlations were observed between expression of
191
‘domestic’ behavior and percentage values of eosinophils in females and expression of domesticated
192
behavior scores and values of N:L ratio in males (Fig. 2D and 2F).
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
186
193 194 195
Fig. 2. Correlations between expression of ‘domesticated’ behavior scores and percentage of neutrophils (A, B), eosinophils (C, D) and neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio (E, F).
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
196
The Graphs A, C, E show average data for males and females (♦
197
B shows linear regression for females (●
198
excluded females (no significant correlation, p>0.05), Graph F excluded males (no significant
199
correlation, p>0.05).
200
4. Discussion
201
Our study has demonstrated that leukocyte profiles vary in minks from different behavioral
202
categories. Selection for domestication significantly increased eosinophil content in ‘domestic’ minks.
203
The lowest eosinophil counts were revealed in control and aggressive “-2” animals, whereas elevated
204
eosinophil level was noted in ‘domestic’ and slightly aggressive “-1” minks. Experiments of Hansen
205
and Damgaard (1993) showed that initial eosinophil level in beech martens before an acute stress
206
event was higher in aggressive animals than in slightly aggressive ones. However, after an acute stress
207
event, eosinophil levels were significantly lower in aggressive animals compared to slightly aggressive
208
ones. Hansen and Damgaard (1993) explained this difference by the fact that in slightly aggressive
209
beech martens eosinophils returned to the initial normal level sooner than in aggressive animals. There
210
are contradictory findings concerning the effect of chronic stress on eosinophil counts. Denial of
211
access to a nest box in silver foxes (Jeppesen and Pedersen, 1991) and repeated immobility stress in
212
mink (Heller and Jeppesen, 1985) are reported to be accompanied by increased eosinophil counts, after
213
initial decreases. Eosinophil counts were however reduced when minks were deprived of the use of
214
nest box and immobilized in a mink trap for 30 min daily over 10 days (Hansen and Damgaard, 1991).
215
The different leukocyte counts in ‘domestic’ and aggressive minks are promoted via HPAS. It
216
has been shown in numerous experiments that HPAS controls physiological reaction to acute stress.
217
The blood eosinophil level is a hormone-dependent parameter, and taken together with
218
neutrophil/lymphocyte ratios it may help distinguish between leukocyte responses to stress. Both
219
emotional (Kerr, 1956) and physiological stress, such as long lasting cold (Denison and Zarrow, 1954)
220
or immobilization (Heller and Jeppesen, 1985), have been proved to reduce the eosinophil level.
221
Dhabhar et al. (1995, 1996) and López-Olvera et al. (2007) demonstrated in studies on humans and
222
mammals that glucocorticoid-induced stress leads to a reduction in eosinophil numbers. Sabag et al.
), p<0.05. Graph D
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
) and males (■
both sexes), the Graph
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
223
(1978) reported cortisol-induced migration of eosinophils from the blood to spleen, lymph nodes, and
224
thymus, and experimental steroid eosinopenia. As revealed in our study neutrophil counts were also dependent on animal behavioral types.
226
The most aggressive minks were characterized by elevated neutrophil levels contrary to ‘domestic’
227
ones. Both stress and glucocorticoid treatment caused the number of neutrophils to increase and the
228
number of lymphocytes to decrease (Davis et al., 2008). In studies devoted to the effect of treatment
229
with cortisol or synthetic glucocorticoids, such as hydrocortisone, dexamethasone or prednisolone,
230
these agents produced neutrophilia or lymphopenia, or both, in rats (Cox and Ford 1982), mice (Van
231
Dijk et al., 1979), guinea pigs (Fauci, 1975), humans (Fauci and Dale, 1974; Dale et al., 1975), etc.
232
Similar research has been conducted using adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH), which stimulates
233
glucocorticoids release from adrenal glands. ACTH treatment raised neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio in
234
boars (Bilandzic et al., 2006) and horses (Rossdale et al., 1982).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
225
Some researchers demonstrated that leukocyte response to stress may be distinct between wild
236
and domestic mammals. Burguez et al. (1983) showed that injections of cortisol did not significantly
237
elevate neutrophil/lymphocyte ratios until 2 h after injection in foals and 4 h later in adult horses. In
238
contrast to these studies on domestic mammals, more recent work shows that the leukocyte response
239
may occur on a more rapid time scale in wild mammals. In a study with wild ungulates, neutrophil
240
numbers were found to double, and lymphocytes reduced by half, within 1 h of capture (López-Olvera
241
et al., 2007).
EP
TE D
235
Studies concerning different leukocyte counts, response of leukocytes to stress, HPAS activity
243
in wild and domestic mammals are not so numerous. It is likely that leukocyte profiles should be
244
different in wild and domestic mammals; however information regarding this is limited and difficult to
245
analyze. Schulte-Hostedde et al. (2012) found that differences in spleen mass between wild and
246
domestic mink, and between feral domestic mink and domestic mink on farms are the result of
247
discrepancy in the prevalence and abundance of parasites and pathogens. Wild animals are widely
248
exposed to many parasites and pathogens that are less prevalent in the domestic environment because
249
of veterinary care.
AC C
242
250
We found no differences in lymphocyte levels in minks of different behavioral categories.
251
Similarly, Delehantry and Boonstra (2009) found no statistically significant change in lymphocytes in
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
squirrels under capture-induced stress. Song et al. (1994) showed that in contrast to neutrophil
253
phagocytosis and WBC count, lymphocyte proliferation did not appear to be as sensitive to acute
254
behavioral stress. They indicate that there is no close relationship between corticosterone and
255
lymphocyte proliferation.
256
Others (Miller, 1998) have reported a close relationship between neuroendocrinal and immune
257
systems. Selection for domestication and aggressiveness affects differential blood leukocytes content
258
and appears to be related to modification of HPAS activity, as reported by others (Veenema et al.,
259
2005). Handled animals had lower levels of cortisol than the unhandled control group in experiments
260
with silver fox (Pedersen, 1994). Aggressive rat males were much more sensitive even to a soft
261
stressor, since the concentration of corticosterone increased more significantly and changes of
262
differential leukocyte count appeared earlier and were more expressed than in tame animals. Low-
263
aggression mice responded to stress with a higher and longer increase in the plasma corticosterone
264
level in comparison to aggressive mice (Kolesnikova and Oskina, 2003).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
252
Kudryavtseva (2006) o showed that in male mice with long experience with aggression, the
266
dopaminergic system was activated and the serotoninergic system inhibited. These alterations in the
267
monoaminergic system activity in the brain are accompanied by changes in the activity of certain
268
genes in brain regions. Selection for low aggressiveness to humans is known to promote the activity of
269
the serotonin system and perhaps of other mediator systems of the mink brain, which decreases the
270
activity of the animals. Modification and subsequent hereditary reorganization of this and other
271
mediator systems of the brain lead to various functional changes (Naumenko et al., 1987; Nikulina et
272
al., 1985).
EP
AC C
273
TE D
265
We found selection for behavior in minks to strongly influence differential blood leukocyte
274
counts. Among the leukocyte types most sensitive to selection for behavior are neutrophils and
275
eosinophils. ‘Domestic’ and aggressive minks had significant differences in differential leukocyte
276
count: the greatest changes were observed in ‘domestic’ mink. The results of this study suggest that
277
differential leukocyte counts differ in aggressive and ‘domestic’ minks, and might be related to HPAS
278
activity. We have concluded that differences in differential leukocyte counts in mink selected for
279
behavioral traits reflect alterations of the neuroendocrinal regulation system.
280
Conflict of interest
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
None.
282
Acknowledgments
283
The study was carried out under state orders (project no. 0221-2014-001 and project no. 0324-
284
2015-0004) and was partially funded by Russian Presidential grant for Leading Scientific Schools #
285
1410.2014.4. This work was carried out in the Genetic Pools of Fur Animals and Livestock Center for
286
Collective Use of the Institute of Cytology and Genetics of the Siberian Branch of the Russian
287
Academy of Sciences and was supported by state contract VI.53.2.1 and the Russian Foundation for
288
Basic Research (grant no. 13-04-00968-a).
RI PT
281
References
290
Bonesi, L., Palazon, S., 2007. The American mink in Europe: status, impacts, and control. Biol. Conserv. 134, 470-483.
M AN U
291
SC
289
292
Bilandžič, N., Žurič, M., Lojkiě, M., Šimiě, B., Miliě, D., Baraě, I., 2006. Cortisol and
293
immune measures in boars exposed to 3-day administration of exogenous adrenocorticotropic
294
hormone. Vet. Res. Commun. 30, 433-444.
Belliveau, A.M., Farid, A., O’Connell, M., Wright, J.M., 1999. Assessment of genetic
296
variability in captive and wild American mink (Mustela vison) using microsatellite markers. Can. J.
297
Anim. Sci. 79, 7-16.
298
301 302
308.
EP
300
Belyaev, D.K., 1979. Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. J Hered. 70, 301-
Burguez, P.N., Ousey, J., Cash, R.S.G., Rossdale, P.D., 1983. Changes in blood neutrophil and lymphocyte counts following administration of cortisol to horses and foals. Equine Vet. J. 15, 58-60.
AC C
299
TE D
295
Cox, J.H., Ford, W.L., 1982. The migration of lymphocytes across specialized vascular
303
endothelium 4: prednisolone acts at several points on the re-circulation pathways of lymphocytes.
304
Cell. Immunol. 66, 407-422.
305
Dale, D.C., Fauci, A.S., Guerry, D., Wolff, S.M., 1975. Comparison of agents producing a
306
neutrophilic leukocytosis in man – hydrocortisone, prednisone, endotoxin, and etiocholanolone. J.
307
Clin. Invest. 56, 808-813.
308 309
Davis, A.K., Maney, D.L., Maerz, J.C., 2008. The use of leukocyte profiles to measure stress in vertebrates: a review for ecologists. Function. Ecol. 22, 760-772.
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Declaration of Helsinki. (2000) 18th World Medical Association General Assembly, Helsinki,
311
Finland, June 1964; 29th World Medical Association General Assembly, Tokyo, Japan, October,
312
1975; 35th World Medical Association General Assembly, Venice, Italy, October, 1983; 41st World
313
Medical Association General Assembly, Hong Kong, September 1989; 48th World Medical
314
Association General Assembly, Somerset West, Republic of South Africa, October, 1996; 52nd World
315
Medical Association General Assembly, Edinburgh, Scotland, October 2000.
318 319 320 321 322 323
ground squirrel (Spermophilus richardsonii). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 160, 176-182.
Denison, M.E., Zarrow M.X., 1954. Eosinophils of blood during prolonged exposure to cold
SC
317
Delehanty, B., Boonstra, R., 2009. Impact of live trapping n stress profiles of Richardson’s
and chronic administration of cortisone acetate. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 85, 433-437. Dhabhar, F.S., Miller, A.H., McEwen, B.S., Spencer, R.L., 1995. Effects of stress on immune
M AN U
316
RI PT
310
cell distribution – dynamics and hormonal mechanisms. J. Immun. 154, 5511-5527. Dhabhar, F.S., Miller, A.H., McEwen, B.S., Spencer, R.L., 1996. Stress induced changes in blood leukocyte distribution – role of adrenal steroid hormones. J. Immun. 157, 1638-1644. Dunstone, N., 1993. The Mink. Poyser, London.
325
Fauci, A.S., 1975. Mechanisms of corticosteroid action on lymphocyte subpopulations 1.
326
Redistribution of circulating T lymphocytes and B lymphocytes to bone marrow. Immunology 28,
327
669-680.
330 331 332
EP
329
Fauci, A.S., Dale, D.C., 1974. Effect of in-vivo hydrocortisone on subpopulations of human lymphocytes. J. Clin. Invest. 53, 240-246. Hansen, S. W., Damgaard, B. M., 1991. Effect of environmental stress and immobilization on
AC C
328
TE D
324
stress physiological variables in farmed mink. Behav. Proc. 25, 191-201. Hansen, S. W., Damgaard, B. M., 1993. Behavioral and adrenocortical coping strategies and
333
the effect on eosinophil leukocyte level and heterophil/lymphocyte-ratio in beech marten (Martes
334
foina). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 35, 369-388.
335 336 337 338
Heller, K. E., Jeppesen, L. L., 1985. Behavioral and eosinophil leukocyte responses to single and repeated immobility stress in mink. Scientifur. 9, 174-178. Jeppesen, L. L., Pedersen, V., 1991. Effect of whole year nest-boxes on cortisol, circulating leukocytes, exploration and agonistic behavior in silver foxes. Behav. Processes 25, 171-177.
13
340 341 342 343 344
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Jeppesen, L.L., Pedersen, V., 1998. Selektionsforsøg for tillidsfuld sølvræv i Danmark. In: NJF Seminar., N. 295, Bergen, Norge, 7–9 September. Irwin, M., Patterson, T., Smith, T. L., Caldwell, C., Brown, S. A., Gillin, J. C., Grant, I., 1990. Reduction of immune function in life stress and depression. Biol. Psychiat. 27, 22-30. Kerr, A.C., 1956. The effect of mental stress on the eosinophil leukocyte count in man.Q. J. Exp. Physiol. Cogn. Med. Sci. 41, 18-24
RI PT
339
Klotchkov, D.V., Trapezov, O.V., Kharlamova, A.V., 1998. Folliculogenesis, onset of puberty
346
and fecundity of mink (Mustela vison Schreb.) selectively bred for docility or aggressiveness.
347
Theriogenology 49, 1545-1553.
SC
345
Kolesnikova, L.A., Oskina, I.N., 2003. Blood cortisone concentration and differential blood-
349
count in stressed Norway rats with different modes of behaviour. Biology Bulletin 30, 75-78. (in
350
Russian)
M AN U
348
351
Kudryavtseva, N.N., 2006. The psychopathology of repeated aggression: a neurobiological
352
aspect, in Morgan, J.P. (Ed.), Perspectives on the Psychology of Aggression. Nova Science Publ. Inc.,
353
New York, pp. 35-64.
355
Kruska, D., 1996. The effect of domestication on brain size and composition in the mink
TE D
354
(Mustela vison). J. Zool., Lond. 239, 645-661.
Kruska, D.C.T., Sidorovich V.E., 2003. Comparative allometric skull morphometrics in mink
357
(Mustela vison Schreber, 1777) of Canadian and Belarus origin; taxonomic status. Mam. Biol. 68,
358
257-276.
López-Olvera, J.R., Marco, I., Montané, J., Casas-Díaz, E., Lavín, S., 2007. Effects of
AC C
359
EP
356
360
acepromazine on the stress response in Southern chamois (Rupicapra pyrenaica) captured by means of
361
drive-nets. Can. J. Vet. Res. 71, 41-51.
362
Malmkvist, J., 2001. Behavioural and physiological effects of buspirone in genetic lines of
363
mink selected for against fearfulness. In: Fear in Farm Mink (Mustela vison). Consequences of
364
Behavioural Selection. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Copenhagen.
365 366
Miller, A.H., 1997. Neuroendocrine and immune system interactions in stress and depressions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21, 443-463.
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
367 368
Naumenko, E.V., Popova, N.K., Ivanova, L.N., 1987. Neuroendocrine and neurochemical mechanisms of domestication. Genetica 23, 1011-1025. (in Russian)
369
Nikula, S., Smeds, K., Hietanen, H., Kenttämies, H., Ojala, M., 2000. Confident behaviour and
370
production traits results from a field study of foxes, in: Proceedings of Seventh International Scientific
371
Congress in Fur Animal Production. 24, 4 (3). Scientifur, Kastoria, Greece, pp. 99–103. Nikulina, E. M., Trapezov, O.V., Popova, N. K., 1985. Content of monoamines in the brain of
373
minks differing by reaction to humans. Zhurn. Vysshey Nervn. Deyatelnosti. 35, 1142-1146. (in
374
Russian)
RI PT
372
Pedersen, V., 1994. Long-term effects of different handling procedures on behavioural
376
physiological, and production-related parameters in silver foxes. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 40, 285-296.
377
Rossdale, P.D., Burguez, P.N., Cash, R.S.G., 1982. Changes in blood neutrophil/lymphocyte
M AN U
ratio related to adrenocortical function in the horse. Equine Vet. J. 14, 293-298.
379 380
Sabag, N., Castrillon, M.A., Tchernitchin, A. 1978. Cortisol-induced migration of eosinophil leukocytes to lymphoid organs. Experientia 34, 666-667.
381 382
Schulte-Hostedde, A.I., Bowman, J., Nituch, L.A., 2012. Dynamic spleen mass in wild and domestic American mink. Biol. J. Linnean Soc. 107, 624-631.
383
386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394
14.
Shackelford, R. M., 1984. American mink. in: Mason, I. L. (Ed.), Evolution of domesticated
EP
385
Shackelford, R. M., 1949. Origin of the American ranch-bred mink. Am. Fur Breeder 22, 12-
animals. Longman, London, New York, pp. 229-233. Song, C., Kelly, J.P., Leonard, B.E., 1994. The effect of stressful behavioural exposure on
AC C
384
TE D
378
SC
375
endocrine and immune parameters in the rat. Stress Med. 10, 239-245 Trapezov O.V., 1987. Selection-induced modification of the defense reaction towards man in
the American mink (Mustela vison Schreb). Genetica 23, 1120-1127. (in Russian) Trapesov, O.V., 2000. Behavioural polymorphism in defensive behaviour towards man in farm raised mink (Mustela vison Schreber, 1777). Scientifur. 24, 103-109. Trapesov, O.V., 2013. Domestication as first intellectual achievement of humanity. Russian J. Genet.: Appl. Res. 17, 872-883. (in Russian)
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
395
Van Dijk, H., Bloksma, N., Rademaker, P.M., Schouten, W.J., Willers, J.M., 1979.
396
Differential potencies of corticosterone and hydro-cortisone in immune and immune-related processes
397
in the mouse. Int. J. Immunopharmacol. 1, 285-292. Veenema, A.H., Sijtsma, B., Koolhaas, J.M., de Kloet, E.R. 2005. The stress response to
399
sensory contact in mice: genotype effect of the stimulus animal. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 550-
400
557.
RI PT
398
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R.W., Fitch, W.T., 2014. The “Domestication syndrome” in
402
mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behaviour and genetics. Genetics 197, 795-
403
808.
SC
401
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
404
16
406
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 2. Differential leukocyte counts (Mean±SEM) in Standard mink of different behavioral categories. Leukocyte types, % Monocytes Lymphocytes Band neutrophils Neutrophils Eosinophils Basophils
407
+5 (n=19) 5.9±0.4**
‘Domestic’ line +4 (n=17) +3 (n=6) 7.9±0.6♦ 6.9±1.2
+1 (n=2) 6.3±0.7
0 7.6±0.6
Aggressive line -1 (n=4) -2 (n=17) 6.9±0.4 6.8±0.5
7.5±0.4
48.7±1.9
51.4±2.5
53.9±3.54
32.8±13.2
49.2±2.7
50.3±5.6
43.8±2.7
47.1±3.9
0.7±0.1 32.6±2.3◊ 12.1±1.9**◊ 0.2±0.1
2.1±0.9 29.7±1.7◊◊ 8.9±1.4* 0.1±0.5
0.4±0.3 24.6±4.5◊ 13.9±3.1*◊ 0.3±0.1
0.8±0.5 52.5±12.2 7.5±1.2 0.0±0.0
0.9±0.2 34.1±2.6 8.1±1.1* 0.2±0.1
0.8±0.2 28.5±2.3◊ 13.3±3.5*◊ 0.1±0.1
0.8±0.2 41.8±2.7 6.4±0.9 0.4±0.0*
1.1±0.2 38.6±3.9 5.5±0.4 0.1±0.0
(n=19)
409
from “+5” group: ♦ p < 0.05 (Mann–Whitney U-test)
p < 0.01, significant differences
M AN U
Table 3. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) of differential leukocyte counts and neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio (N/L ratio).
Monocytes Lymphocytes Band neutrophils Neutrophils Eosinophils Basophils N/L ratio
Factor of influence
Behavior Type F 1.25 ns
2
η ,% 9.63 0.80 0.50 28.29 14.91 16.29 29.93
0.72 0.42 4.38 1.90 2.19 4.68
η
p
ns
TE D
Dependent variable
ns 0.04 ns 0.04 0.002
Sex F
2
p
6.72
6.09
0.02
15.11
1.94
ns
8.81 2.11 2.9×10-5 4.88 0.51
1.05 2.28 0.00 4.58 0.58
ns ns ns 0.04 ns
EP
Notes: ns=not significant (p > 0.05)
AC C
412
◊◊
SC
0.01, significant differences from “-2” group: ◊ p < 0.05;
411
(n=21)
Notes: Significant differences from control group (non selected): *p < 0.05; **p <
408
410
Control
RI PT
405
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1. Behavioral categories of minks used in the experiment.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Behavioral categories N ‘Domestic’ Females Males +5 9 10 +4 8 9 +3 1 5 +1 2 Avoidant fear (0) 10 9 Aggressive -1 4 -2 9 8 Control (Non selected) 8 13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 2. Differential leukocyte counts (Mean±SEM) in Standard mink of different behavioral categories.
+5 (n=19) 5.9±0.4**
‘Domestic’ line +4 (n=17) +3 (n=6) 7.9±0.6♦ 6.9±1.2
+1 (n=2) 6.3±0.7
0 7.6±0.6
Aggressive line -1 (n=4) -2 (n=17) 6.9±0.4 6.8±0.5
7.5±0.4
48.7±1.9
51.4±2.5
53.9±3.54
32.8±13.2
49.2±2.7
50.3±5.6
43.8±2.7
47.1±3.9
0.7±0.1 32.6±2.3◊ 12.1±1.9**◊ 0.2±0.1
2.1±0.9 29.7±1.7◊◊ 8.9±1.4* 0.1±0.5
0.4±0.3 24.6±4.5◊ 13.9±3.1*◊ 0.3±0.1
0.8±0.5 52.5±12.2 7.5±1.2 0.0±0.0
0.9±0.2 34.1±2.6 8.1±1.1* 0.2±0.1
0.8±0.2 28.5±2.3◊ 13.3±3.5*◊ 0.1±0.1
0.8±0.2 41.8±2.7 6.4±0.9 0.4±0.0*
1.1±0.2 38.6±3.9 5.5±0.4 0.1±0.0
(n=19)
RI PT
Leukocyte types, % Monocytes Lymphocytes Band neutrophils Neutrophils Eosinophils Basophils
Control (n=21)
Notes: Significant differences from control group (non selected): *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01,
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
group: ♦ p < 0.05 (Mann–Whitney U-test)
SC
significant differences from “-2” group: ◊ p < 0.05; ◊◊p < 0.01, significant differences from “+5”
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 3. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) of differential leukocyte counts and neutrophil/lymphocyte ratio (N/L ratio).
Monocytes Lymphocytes Band neutrophils Neutrophils Eosinophils Basophils N/L ratio
Factor of influence Behavior Type F 1.25 ns
η2, % 9.63
p
6.72
6.09
0.02
1.94
ns
0.80
0.72
ns
15.11
0.50 28.29 14.91 16.29 29.93
0.42 4.38 1.90 2.19 4.68
ns 0.04 ns 0.04 0.002
8.81 2.11 2.9×10-5 4.88 0.51
1.05 2.28 0.00 4.58 0.58
ns ns ns 0.04 ns
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
Notes: ns=not significant (p > 0.05)
Sex F
η2
p
RI PT
Dependent variable
1
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Highlights •
Long-term selection for tame and aggressive behaviour changes differential blood leukocyte
counts. •
Both neutrophils and eosinophils among examined parameters are sensitive and dependent on
•
RI PT
behavior type. Differential blood leukocyte counts in mink selected for behavioural traits reflect alterations of
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
the neuroendocrinal regulation system.