Sot. .Sci. #Med. Vol. 31. No. 2. pp. 115-128, 1990 Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved
Copyright
c
0277-9536.‘90 53.00 + 0.00 1990 Pergamon Press plc
SELF-CONCEPT AND CANCER IN ADULTS: THEORETICAL AND METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES BARBARA
CURBOW,* MARK SOMERFIELD, MARCIA LEGRO and JOHN SONNEGA Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21205, U.S.A.
Abstract-Research and theory development on the self-concept have increased dramatically over the last decade. Investigators of the psychosocial aspects of cancer have utilized the self-concept as both an independent and dependent variable. This article discusses quantitative adult studies from the self-concept and cancer literature in terms of their correspondence with current conceptualizations of the self-concept in psychology. The wide gap between recent empirical work and self-concept theorizing is discussed and strategies for future investigations of the self-concept/cancer relationship are outlined. Kqv words-self-concept,
neoplasms, adult
INTRODUmON Not only does cancer cause physical
suffering and the loss of productive years across all segments of the lifespan [l, 21, it also is often associated with decrements in psychosocial functioning [3]. Researchers of the psychosocial aspects of cancer have responded to the compelling problems generated by the disease and there are growing bodies of work on the many facets of cancer’s consequences for psychological functioning. One emerging area of research explores the relationship between cancer and the self-concept. While self-concept has been viewed as a relevant variable in the study of chronic illness in general [4, 51. aspects of cancer (e.g. uncertainty regarding its etiology and course) and cancer treatment (e.g. the aggressiveness of many therapies) serve to make patients and survivors particularly vulnerable to changes in their self views. Mages and Mendelsohn [6] have argued, more specifically, that the physical damage resulting from the treatment of cancer may force changes in self-perception. Evidence of the growing recognition of the importance of self-related variables can be found in a recent review of self-concept and quality of life in cancer patients [7’J,discussions of the importance of self-esteem and body image for cancer patients [S], and notation of the role of disfigurement in the psychosocial functioning of patients with hematological malignancies [9]. Because the investigation of self-concept and other closely related variables is on the rise in cancer research, it is appropriate to characterize the extent to which the empirical research is linked to current theory. The purposes of this article are twofold: (a) to describe the methods and theoretical frameworks of recent work on the self-concept and cancer, and (b) to ascertain how well this research fits within current conceptualizations of the self-concept as found in psychological literature. It is not the goal of this paper to review the findings of studies on self-concept *Address correspondence to: Barbara Curbow. Health Psychology Program, Department of Environmental Health Sciences. Johns Hopkins University. 615 North Wolfe St, Baltimore. MD 21205, U.S.A.
and cancer. Such a task would be beyond the scope of this discussion. We begin our discussion with an overview of how the cancer and self-concept studies were selected. We then turn to a discussion of the following six issues: (a) the definition of self-concept; (b) self-concept as a dependent and independent variable; (c) unique and shared components of the self-concept; (d) multidimensional aspects of the self-concept; (e) the bidirectional influence of the self-concept; and (f) changes in the self-concept over time. SELECTION AND OVERVIEW THE STUDIES DESCRIBED
OF
Computer searches of Index Medicus and Ps_vchowere conducted for this paper. Both and Psychological Abstracts were searched using the keyword ‘neoplasms’ in combination with each of the following: ‘self-concept,’ ‘self-esteem, ’ ‘identity, ’ ‘self-image,’ and ‘self-perception.’ To reflect the rapid increase in theory and research on the self-concept during the past decade, each search covered the time period from 1978 to 1988. After reviewing the preliminary set of articles produced by the computer searches, it was evident that four distinct subgroups of studies existed: (a) quantitative adult, (b) qualitative adult, (c) quantitative pediatric, and (d) qualitative pediatric. Because the studies in these subgroups differed in their approaches, methods and target audiences, we limit this discussion to quantitative adult research. Additional articles were excluded because they did not contain self-related variables, or because they focused on siblings or caregivers of cancer patients, or on persons at high risk for getting cancer. In addition to the computer searches, a manual review of 22 major health psychology journals [IO] was completed. and a direct search was conducted for articles based on dissertation abstracts located by the computer searches. Forty-four quantitative studies on cancer and selfconcept emerged from the literature review. Five studies were described in two separate articles each; therefore, one report of each was excluded from the
logical Abstracts Index Medicus
115
BARBARA CLXBOW et al
116
review. An additional three studies were discarded because they did not contain self-related variables. The resultant 36 studies [I I-461 are described in Tables I and 2. Table 1 includes information on the sample (including number, gender. age, and type of cancer) and the time frame of the study. Table 2 presents information on whether the self variable is used as a dependent or independent variable, the predictor or outcome variables associated with self, and the self variable that was investigated. A DEFINITION OF THE SELF-COXEPT
In her recent review of the influence of cancer on self-concept, Foltz [7] adopted a definition of the term employed by Schain [47]. According to this definition, the self-concept is defined as the “sum total of all that a person feels about himself’herself.” Foltz then noted and summarized research on four subcomponents of the self-concept: body self, interpersonal self, achievement self, and identification self. While this definition appears plausible, and indeed it may be workable under some circumstances. there are two major issues that must be raised. First, the definition of self-concept is more closely akin to what the psychological literature would identify as a self-esteem. Self-esteem is the atfective component of the self-concept; it describes an attitude. feeling, or evaluation concerning the self [48]. Selfesteem may be used as a global term. as measured by standard instruments such as the Rosenberg Selfesteem Scale [49]. or it can be based upon feelings about components of the self (e.g. feelings about body-image following a mastectomy). In the Foltz review article, and elsewhere in the oncology self-concept and self-esteem are used literature. interchangeably. The second issue that must be raised concerns the implementation of Foltz’s definition vvhcn applied to the literature. In the Foltz review. the body self is broken into two areas: functional and body image. Keeping with her definition of the self-concept, the functional body self should include ferlings about how the self is functioning. Instead. the review cites studies that focus on actual levels of functioning such as activity levels. arm weakness. shoulder mobility, and ability to carry out household tasks. This inconsistency of definitions is apparent u hen she examines other self domains such as the interpersonal self, the sexual self. the achievement self. and the identification self. This apparent confusion regarding the definition and usage of terms such as self-concept and self-esteem is found in other studies in the oncology literature. For example, although we specified narrowly defined terms in our literature search. there was great variability in the aspects of self uncovered (see Table 2). The current climate of intense theorv generation about. and research activity on. the self-concept in the psychological literature allows for a more precise definition of the term. The self-concept is best vievved as a collection of self representattons. Self-esteem is a component of this group of representations. These representations vary in their structure and function and have been given a variety of labels (images, goals.
OYK2)(?6]
Hallal
cf crl. (1979) 1301
Lewis
rA
tThe
weighted
number
mean
from
indicatrs
was calcula1ed
in parentheses
illnesses other
(19X4) 14.5)
(441
1421
(19X5) (411
(I 987)
includes
Youssef
sample
and Weisman
(19114) (461
Worden
‘Total
Cl Cd. (1986)
wolcolt
et d
(1983) 143)
den Borne
Walson
Van
and Jan&Bulman
Timko
[40]
YI ol. (1984)
ef ul. (1985)
St&&erg
Taylor
et RI. (1981) [3X]
Spiegel
1391
CI rd. (1985) (371
Schain
rr ol. (19113) 1361
Revenson
(19X5) [W]
PI rd. (19X5) 1351
PI d.
Ramhoer
Pruyn
YI NI. (19X7) 1331
Penman
(19X4) I3 I]
(19X8) 1321
and Winefield
Neuling
and Anderson
(19X2) [2Y]
Lewis
MacDonald
L’r cd. (1983) [2X]
(19X4) [27)
IIan
I lohhll andWallisch
Burgess (19X7) [2SJ
and
Greer
(26%) (33%) (25%)
5&5Y 6&69
29-78
various
(3)
stir
fix
cancer
is withm
groups
included.
of persons. were
in the 1ex1.
lypes of cancer
&ala provided
many
11 = 39.5t or other
Sample
parcnthcscs.
El = both
males and
females;
IO days of diagnosis; hospitalization
within (7)
during
27 60
breast
various
IK F
IX
over
II7
B
M =42
3-6.
points,
P = females;
M = males.
al 2. 4, 6. I2 months
postdischarge
at 2 time
intrrvals
postdiagnosls
apart
entrance
1X months
after
(b)
into
postsurgery
postsurgery, months
I2- I5 months 6 9, and Y-12
Y- 12, and
II +
years (23%)
p&biopsy
a~ 4 months
interviews
sahgroups
month post-lransplant,
I
all had BMTs
months
postsurgery.
17-51
19-91
day
M = 27.9
(4)
4-7th
= 25.5 M = 8.9
M&I
M = 15.X
stage of treatment
postsurgery,
postsurgery,
pslsurgery.
16F
various
all had ostomics
months
months
months
indelerminant
l-20
l-60
543
IO M
21 M
M = 66
IOF
Hodgkins
breast
369 B
42 F
breast
breas1
23-8 I
Mcln = 53
breas1
M = 53.4 -
78 F
31-6X M = 52.Xt
diagnosis,
al O-3.
6-9.
poslsurgery
years (24%).
collected
67 F
I year after study
data
for
breast
M = 54.4
postreconslruclion
5X F
postdiagnosis
(51%)
years
(49%)
breast
2 months-X.5
5 years of diagnosis
< I year postreconslruclion
+
wi1hin
bet’ore diagnosis
majority
women
I + year
40-M)
63 F
(3)
Hodgkins
breast
various
43% -
57%
no surgery
6-10
studies at O-3. 3-6,
study contacted
(a) cross-sectional groups)
longitudinal
comparison
I and
3 months
poslsurgery
8 weeks postsurgery
3 months
2, 5, and
years (53%).
presurgery;
l-5
breast
days
postsurgery:
surgery;
audit
all terminal
wi1h chart
I5 weeks
postbiopsy
1I, and
3 months
+ years postdiagnosis
I day before
2-7
IO biopsy;
chemotherapy
prior
(cholecystechxny,
breast
reclal
not specified
-=zl-7
before
I day
3, 7.
postdiagnosis
surgery,
I2 months
before
postmasleclomy
no1 applicable
3 and
night
3, I year
M =4x
5&X3 M =6l
a-61+ M z49.11 -
(16%)
3&3Y
E 54.0
40-49
Mrln
I5 M
than cancer
how
(34%)
75+ 3&X2
(38%)
< 65 (28%) 65-74
(22) 17 F
105-
l23M
375 F
1715 F
58 F
210 M
27-65 M = 49.6
(23) 2lOF
M = 54.0
46 B
hreost
(16)
various
various
21 M
23.-80 M = 53.2
36 M
suspected
heallhy
(3)
21-79
M = 3X.2
(13) 30 F
35 F
2X~ 58
hU F
207 F 21X
I R-70
(31) 107 F various
breasl
(13) 61 F
34 M
breast
34-57 M = 47.5: -
24 F
z
R ?
w
8
WJ ‘p ;?
Cella
(1986)[16]
(361
Youssef
survival practice
IV
IV
tThc
marital
medical
adoption
lift
cvcnts stress, and
lack
in this study included
of supports,
the ti~llowing:
research
treatment
teams.
as a cimfcr
medical
conditions,
controls
patients
vs patient
fellow
patients
controls
transplant
(BSE)
(MRM)
age, social status, chronic
hy the respective
diagnosis
diet
state depression
of unproven
state anricty. compliimcc
controls
with
counseling
contact
biopsy,
and demographic
in the future)
mastectomy
reconstruction
psychosocial
only,
breast
locus of control,
of illness patients)
mastectomy
patient
vs kidney
postoperative therapy,
length
surgery
OC cancer
self-examination
patient
ot’ hrcast
time
functioning
lo have hccn developed
t&tors
status,
and demographic
and employment
psychosocial
various
of control,
a citation
[35]
IV
without
et cd. (1985)
Ramboer
lSctf-variahlcs
IV
IV
were assumed
social
IV
IV
depression
IV
BMT
preventive
therapy,
short-term group
DV
(free
control
radical
breast
in or out of treatment, in short-term
DV
cr rrl. (1985) (341
1271
BMT
DV
I’ruyn
(1984)
patients
invtdnerdbility
about
modified
support
or delayed
beliefs
vs donors
diagnosis,
DV
perceived
attributions.
DV
DV
DV
type of surgery:
DV
early
psychological
immediate, weekly
DV
controls
depression,
years since surgery
various
therapy,
age, rx.
(general
no surgery;
and adjuvant
cholccystectomy. faclorst
vs noncrisis
DV
,‘I II/. (I’~X3)/?X]
and Waltisch
support
disability
anxiety,
and healthy
controls
over life and health,
symptoms,
of a colostomy,
mastectomy
social
presence
surgery
healthy
waiting
list controls
variables
in the quantitative
since treatment
program,
status
program,
since diagnosis.
control
growth,
crisis (cancer)
personal
tumor
age. sex, time
level of crisis cancer,
patients,
time -
styles, demographic
reconstruction
of tumor
breast
training
knowledge
in a social
styles
maseclomy
persons
coping
or evacuation
vs late stage cancer;
vs delayed
early
unconscious coping
negative
scar characteristics
support,
immediate
cancer,
gender,
social
employed
Predictor/outcome
of the self, variables
types: chemotherapy
2. Description
treatment
Table
social support
or
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
DV
(IV)
(DV)
variable
independent
llano
Ilohl’oll
[ 141
(1987) (I21
CI N/. (1985)[19]
(261
[42]
1321
(1984) [a]
[44]
(1984)
Richardson
and Spiegel
Jcdtlal (1982)
Edwards
Bloom
Bisno and
and Weisman
(1984)
Worden
(461
r/ ul. (1986)
Wolcolt
CI (I/. (1987)
(1983) 1431
Watson
den Borne
(1984)
Van
and JanotT-Bulman
141)
Timko
1401
134
PI cl/. (1984)
YI ol. (1985)
Taylor
Steinberg
ef ol. (1981)
Spiegel
(1988)
[33]
[3X]
CI el. (1985)(37]
Schain
CI ol. (1983)
cr cl. (1987)
Revenson
and Winetield
Penman
and Anderson
Neuling
(l984)(3l]
MacDonald
(1979)
[29]
cf d.
Lewis
[JO]
(1982)
Lewis
(1982) 1241
and Lewis
Burgess (1987) [25]
and
[23]
Greer
(1982)
I]
Gottesman
Gerard
(1981) 122)
CI cl. (1984)(21]
Felton
Fiegenbaum
[ZO]
(1984)
and Craddick
PI ul. (1983)[18]
er a/. (1984) [ 17)
and Tress
Engelman
Dean
Clifford
et cl/. (1983)
[IS]
CI crl. (1980)[1
( 1982) [I 21
Cassileth
Bloom
Berkowitz
Study
Dependent
self-esteem
self-esteem
estimated
information
xll’rsttwn
self-erlccm
self-esteem
self-concept
self-regard
self-concept
self-concept
selfesteem
1965) 1491
(Rosenberg.
(Rosenberg,
(Rosenberg,
items.
group
weight
from
participation.
loss
physician
197X) 1901
Inventory.
weight,
k~ween
reported
locus
1966) [X9]
1958) 179))
1965) (491
(Schain.
loss of parent(s),
of diet
early
and adoption
Shostrom.
Scale (Janis.
Concerns)
195l)(Rl]
and
concerns
(Rosenberg,
ol’ the relationship
197X) ]‘JlI
and Schooler. (*I ol..
as a moderator
(I.um
(Pearlin
(Fitts.
from
Orientation 1965) 187)
(adapted (Personal
Janis-Field
of Current
1983) [SN] 1965)[87]
(Inventory (Fitts,
1965) [49]
1967) (861
(791)
dissatisfaction
global
(Jourdrd
(Kahn,
of mastectomy
1965) [87], self-esteem (Simmons,
(Fitts.
(Rosenberg,
Scale (Eagly,
results
1965) [49] of viewing JanissField
distress
(Rosenberg,
self-image
image
only
cathexis
1965) (491. body
1965) [49]
1965) [49]
1965) [49]
and individual
(Rosenberg,
(global
(Rosenberg,
1977) 1821, body
1965) 1491
(1958)
and aspects of
Janis
femininity,
Scale,
self and cancer
‘Me’,
CI rrl., 1972) [X4], feminine
self appraisal
self-esteem
self-concept
self-esteem
sclf~estcem
self-esteem
self-image
self-esteem
emotional
self-esteem
1977) INS]
(Berscheid
self-esteem
self-esteem
srigma
self-esteem
self-esteem
here)
(Polivy,
1955) 1831
self-concept Secord.
1957) [NO]
self and cure,
(Rosenberg.
self-discontent
self-esteem
between
PI al.,
1965) [49]
scar
Janis-Field
Self-variable*
to evaluate
(Rosenberg, ditTerential
image
from
of surgical
{adapted
impact
(Osgood
distance
body
body
semantic
self-esteem
cosmetic
self-concept
self-image
studies
social contacts.
adult
P
a m
c
?
5
= 00
Self-concept and cancer tasks, etc.). Their organization has been described as spaces [SO],a confederation [S!], or a type of system. According to Markus and Wurf [52], many recent models of the self-concept focus on the nature of the cognitive representations of the self. Not a!! of the self representations that create the self-concept are alike. Markus and Wurf cite several dimensions that characterize and differentiate self representations: importance, valence (i.e. positive or negative), time orientation (past, present, or future), actual or idea!, feared, and likely. For example, a cancer patient may have a self-concept that includes a representation of the self on a physical strength dimension. This physical strength component may be characterized for that person as important, negative, current, and feared. Perhaps the most apparent difference among self representations is their importance. Some representations are ‘core’ conceptions while others are ‘periphera!‘. Two representations of the self-concept relevant to many cancer patients, physical appearance and spirituality, were found in one study in the genera! population [53] to have the greatest variability among individuals in terms of importance. Additionally, current research views the self-concept as dynamic-as active, forceful, and capable of change [52]. The self-concept is viewed not only as having content and structure, but also as having a role in intrapersonal processes (information processing, affect, and motivation) and a wide variety of interpersonal processes (social perception; choice of situation, partner, and interaction strategy; and reaction to feedback). According to this view, it is appropriate to speak not only of the content and structure of the self-concept, but also of the power of the self-concept to define and determine interactions with the social environment. The confusion in the oncology literature over the use of self terminology might be clarified by adopting a more uniform theoretical stance. In keeping with current psychological theory, the self-concept should be operationalized as a dynamic collection of cognitive representations. SELF-CONCEPT AS DEPENDENT OR INDEPENDENT VARIABLE
Work in this area has generally taken one of two positions: self-concept as either an outcome or as a causal agent. The first position maintains that the experience of cancer has an effect on the self-concept. This belief appears to stem from the vivid persona! accounts of survivors, as told by individuals (e.g. [54]) and as discussed in the collective by researchers (e.g. [55]). The essential question from this perspective is: What happens to the self-concept during and after the diagnosis of, and the treatment for, cancer? This question treats the self-concept as a dependent variable-cancer changes some aspects of the self-concept-and leads to further inquiries: What about the self-concept is susceptible to change? When do the changes occur? How long do the changes last? The second position, which is compatible with the new field of psycho-oncology, as defined by Dreher [56]. holds that something about the self-concept is a causal agent in the self-cancer relationship. The
119
essential questions of this position are: HOW do aspects of the self-concept influence the development and course of cancer, the outcome of treatment, and adjustment? In this line of questioning, the selfconcept is treated as either an independent or moderator variable; something about the self-concept can either bring about the illness. affect the duration or severity of its course, influence the response to treatment, or facilitate or inhibit adjustment. AS depicted in Table 2, over three-quarters (77.8%) of the studies used some aspect of the self as a dependent variable. The most frequently investigated variable was self-esteem, often measured by the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale [49]. Other self-related variables included self-concept, body image, selfdiscontent, self-appraisal, self-image, and sti_ma. Changes in the self-concept and other related aspects of the self are important outcomes for cancer researchers to consider. However, a strong case also could be made for using self-related variables as predictors of outcomes such as adherence to treatment regimen, adjustment to role loss or change, and perceived quality of life. As we will argue in later sections, it is likely that self-variables are both predictors and outcomes. The investigation of such concepts in well-planned longitudinal designs may explicate the conditions under which self-variables take on each role. UNIQUE AND SHARED COMPONENTS OF THE SELF-CONCEPT
Although the exact structural configuration of the self-concept is not yet determined, it is generally accepted that its cognitive content varies across persons [51]. This proposition corresponds with the belief that the self-concept is influenced by experience [57]. Thus, to the extent that experiences and their interpretations are shared, the content of the selfconcept may be similar across persons. This premise has implications for the study of self-concept and cancer. First, the effects of cancer on the self-concept are likely to be at least partially individualized. That is, individuals facing cancer and its treatment will come to the situation with a unique set of resources. For example, some persons may see themselves as being able to endure easily the most arduous of treatments, whereas others may see themselves as weak and likely to ‘fall apart.’ Second, the extent to which there are commonalities across self-concepts may be tied to how similar the individuals and the cancer experiences are. Similarities in individuals may be based on personality, lifestyle, or demographic features. Similarities in the experience may be based on the diagnosis, stage of the illness, prognosis, treatment protocol, care givers, or patient cohort. Our conceptualization of this issue is depicted in Fig. 1. In this figure, we illustrate two situations. In situation one, persons ‘A’ and ‘B’ have both been treated with bone marrow transplantation, an aggressive treatment used for several forms of cancer. Although these people have the same treatment, they are depicted both as having different self domains that are important to them and as having different levels of impairment in those domains. A self-concept
BARBARA CURBOW et al.
_ _--__ Q .z” a
5
5.
2 2
mg 5 E
Self-concept
inventory that did not include the self as a religious participant would identify person B as more impaired overall than he or she is. In situation two, we illustrate what might happen when persons who value the same self domains experience different treatments. For example, a mastectomy may not impair person C’s ability to have children, whereas a bone marrow transplant would almost certainly preclude it for person D [9]. An adequate research program on self-concept should untangle how responses to cancer are both unique and shared. However, as Cella and Tross [ 161 observe, most studies on survivors of cancer have used heterogeneous subject pools. To distinguish between shared and unique responses, researchers could employ these strategies: (a) narrow their samples to include specific groups of persons (e.g. Which self domains are affected in young females who are having lumpectomies?), (b) build individual difference variables into their designs (e.g. How do the affected self domains differ for young adult and middle-aged females who are having lumpectomies?), or (c) compare responses across different groups (e.g. How do the self domains that are affected differ for lumpectomy and mastectomy patients?). An examination of the subjects and designs employed in the self-concept and cancer studies reveals the extent to which these strategies have been considered. The subject samples from the 36 studies are described in Table I. Self-concept and cancer was most often studied for women; 18 of the studies (50.0%) investigated women only, while just two (5.5%) were restricted to men. Paralleling the use of women as subjects, a sizeable proportion of the studies (44.4%) focused exclusively on breast cancer. One-third of the studies either used patients from more than one cancer group or did not specify the type of cancer. The sizeable literature on breast cancer presented here is just a fraction of that found on other psychosocial aspects of this disease (e.g. [58-61]). This is perhaps the most specialized of the cancer literatures, and gains are being made in ascertaining which aspects of self-concept are most vulnerable. Aspects of self-concept that may be of importance in studies of breast cancer include sexuality, body image, and femininity. However, this type of detail is not available for most other forms of cancer. Although most of the studies reported a wide range in the ages of participants, an inspection of the means and medians reveals that approximately half of the studies investigated persons in their forties or fifties. Less information is available for other age groups (excluding pediatric patients) and few studies look for differences across age groups. A notable exception was the study reported by Penman et aI. [33] in which the sample was stratified by age decade. Finally, Table 1 presents information on whether the stage of the treatment process was controlled for by each study. A majority of the studies either did not narrow the time frame of the study or did not provide information on the time frame. For example, Bloom [ 131 reports that the participants were one week to two and one half years post-surgery; Clifford, Clifford, and Georgiade [17] report that the patients were in “some phase of reconstruction.” The remain-
and cancer
I21
ing studies did narrow the time frames so as to isolate the effects of specific events in the course of treatment. For example, Engelman and Craddick [20] investigated the short time span between biopsy for breast cancer and diagnosis; Hobfoll and Walfisch [27] collected their data one day before the biopsy and again three months post-biopsy. Penman et al. [33] collected both cross-sectional and longitudinal data. In both portions of their study, the researchers collected data at five clearly defined time periods post-surgery. As we will discuss in a later section, this is a critical piece of information to consider in study designs. Overah, many of these studies used broadlydefined samples: investigators often did not narrow the age groups, the phase in the treatment cycle, the treatment, or the type of cancer. However, research opportunities to control on all of the above variables are probably rare and there is evidence that investigators did consider this issue (e.g. [l8, 191). With the notable exception of Penman et al. [33], there were few examples where individual difference variables were built into study designs, although some studies did consider the effects of age or gender (e.g. [ 15,25,31]). A sizeable proportion of the studies made comparisons across groups; most frequently they examined the effects of different medical or counseling treatments. MULTIDIMENSIONAL ASPECTS OF THE SELF-CONCEPT
One significant achievement of the past decade has been to recognize the limitations of using generalized. global measures of self-concept [52]. Global measures may not be sensitive to subtle changes in individuals or tap the particular aspects of self that are most vulnerable to change in a set of circumstances. For example, a cancer patient whose treatment causes extensive mutilation of the body may have changes in self-concept that are vastly different from those experienced by a patient whose treatment is less invasive. While a global measure (such as self-esteem) may show disturbances as compared to healthy populations, it will not show which aspects of the self-concept have been altered. Both the conditions of the illness and treatment and individual differences in what is important to the patients will affect which aspects of the self-concept are influenced. For example, some treatments such as the high levels of radiation in bone marrow transplants, interfere with patients’ reproductive functions [62]. This obviously has different meanings for young adults than for middle-aged persons. Within demographic subgroups, people will vary on what they most value about themselves, for one person it may be athletic abilities, for another it may be cognitive abilities. A global measure cannot describe individualized losses. A useful research strategy to deal with this issue would be to conduct formative studies on the particular aspects of the self-concept that are vulnerable to change for a defined target group (cf. [63]). Once these aspects of self are identified, they can be tracked over time for members of the group. These individualized measures can be consolidated with global
BARBARA CURBOW et al.
122
indices for psychometric (e.g. establishing validity) and comparison purposes. Such an approach has been reported for bone marrow transplant patients 1641. The multidimensional aspects of the self-concept were not widely addressed. Most of the studies employed global measures of self-esteem or selfconcept. One area of research that is starting to make inroads into the development of more specialized measures is that of breast cancer. Because breast cancer often requires disfiguring surgery, some researchers have focused on variables such as body image [IS], emotional distress at viewing the results of a mastectomy [37], and various aspects of self-image who have [39]. Similarly, some researchers investigated the psychosocial impact of other cancer surgeries have examined variables such as selfdiscontent [22], the cosmetic impact of the surgical scar 1151, and stigma [31]. BrDtt~Ec-rtoN~L INFLCENCE SELF-CONCEPT
0~ THE
The self is no longer viewed as a passive recipient of information and influence from the social environment. Rather, the self-concept is now viewed as an active agent that “. . . mediates most significant intrapersonal processes (including information processing, affect, and motivation) and a wide variety of interpersonal processes (including social perception; choice of situation. partner, and interaction strategy; and reactions to feedback)” [52, p. 3001. As an active agent, the self-concept of the cancer patient can have an influence on other members of the social environment. This may happen by routes such as the biased processing of information, the employment of behavioral tactics that will moderate negative emotions, the withdrawal from threatening social interactions, and the misreading of interactional cues. For example, there are distinct biases in the processing of self-related information; people are inclined to focus judgment and memory on the self, perceive the self as effective in achieving desired ends, and resist cognitive change [51, 571. Outcomes of these biases may include an inability to process accurately information regarding treatment options, technological aspects of procedures, or even the actions of significant others. This faulty processing could lead not only to poor decision-making but to strained interpersonal relations as well. The implication of this theorizing for research on the self-concept and cancer is that. in addition to looking at the cognitive content and the affective regard of the individual patients, investigators need to focus on both the intrapersonal and interpersonal outcomes of changes in the self-concept. Researchers could: (a) explore the longitudinal relationships between self-concept and variables such as decisionmaking skills, realistic knowledge of the disease, treatment, and prognosis, and cognitive functioning (keeping in mind that the treatments may cause impairments in these areas [65]), and (b) trace the impact of the variables on other _ patient’s . self-concept . members of the soctal envtronment.
We did not find any instances of this bidirectional approach. One study that approximated this goal was conducted by Bloom and Spiegel [14]. Using a crosssectional design, these authors found that selfconcept was positively related to social functioning. CHANGES IN THE SELF-CONCEPT OVER TIME
A long-standing issue of debate is whether the self-concept is stable or malleable. Gergen [66] has summarized evidence for the existence of ‘momentary fluctuations’ in the self-concept and reviewed solutions that allow for ‘stability within change.’ One solution is to distinguish between what is central (stable) to an individual and what is peripheral (unstable). Markus and Wurf [52] discuss the “working self-concept” as the “. . . continually active, shifting array of accessible self-knowledge” (p. 306). The working self-concept may be the portion that is most amenable to change; core areas may remain relatively stable. While the dominant view among theorists is that some aspects of the self-concept are susceptible to change, a second position (e.g. [67]) points to the tendency of the self to resist discrepant information. Even this position, however, allows for the possibility of changes to the self-concept by outside agents under conditions of ‘potent feedback’-feedback that comes from a credible source. that is not farfetched. that relates directly to the self-concept. and that is delivered by multiple sources [67]. Given the extreme nature of the feedback that engulfs many cancer patients. it is likely that changes to the self-concept will occur; however. these changes may not occur at the same rate for all persons. If people actively resist discrepant information regarding the self-concept [67], or engage in cognitive conservatism [51, 571, it is plausible that they will vary in their ability to resist cancer-related changes. The implication of this reasoning for research is that people may continue to experience changes to the self-concept over time, but that the patterns and rates of change may vary among individuals. Individuals will bring a ‘precancer self-concept’ to the situation that is unique in terms of the exact configuration of content and structure. Once individuals crossover into the ‘cancer self,’ they may begin to share more content and structure with others; however, the selfconcept will shift over time as patients become immersed in their treatments and recoveries. For example, the self-concept at diagnosis may be quite different from the self-concept during treatment or the self-concept during relapse. While theoretically it is possible for a self to pass through and become ‘postcancer,’ this may not occur for a sizable proportion of persons. Some persons will die before a postcancer self is attained and some will never be free of its influence. (See Cella and Tross [l6], for a thoughtful discussion of the psychological late effects of cancer.) An illustration of this point is found in Fig. 2. For this person, four domains that are important and unthreatened are present before the diagnosis. However, as the person proceeds through stages of treatment and recovery, these self domains change in both importance and level of threat.
Prediagnosis self
1
0
l
Minor threat to self domain
Moderate threat to wlf domain
Major threat to self domain
Short-term Post-Treatment
Fig. 2. Depiction of changes to self domains over time. Size of domain reflects relative level of importance.
Legend
Postdiagnosis, pretreatment self
Long-term Post-Treatmen
E
BARBARA CURBOW et al.
124
Since cross-sectional studies examine only one point in time, they cannot capture the process or change that may occur in an individual. In addition, a cross-sectional study that examines some aspect of the self-concept in a loosely defined sample of ‘post-treatment’ patients may inadvertently combine persons who are in recovery, relapse, or postcancer. Approximately one-third of the studies employed two or more measurement cycles. One group of researchers [24,30] employed four to five measurement periods at carefully spaced intervals. Penman et al. [33] collected data at five points post-surgery. DISCUSSION
The studies reviewed here do not, in general, reflect current conceptualizations of the self-concept or employ design features that could assist in untangling the self-concept/cancer relationship. These problems are not unique to this area of research in psychosocial oncology; indeed, as the field has grown there have been other calls for improvements in methods and measures (e.g. [68]) and other sets of guidelines for research (e.g. [69]). At the risk of repeating points that have been made by others, we offer specific research strategies that might be employed to improve the quality of work in self-concept and cancer. Our suggestions are summarized in Table 3 and discussed in detail below. Definition
of self-concept
The most basic task in the field is to work towards clarifying the definition of self-concept that is being investigated. As we have noted previously, there is widespread interchange of the terms self-concept and self-esteem in the literature, and the latter is generally used to refer to global self-esteem. The definition of self-concept (or self-esteem) employed in a particular study should be linked to current definitions in the field of psychology or other behavioral or social sciences. We believe the goal of psychosocial oncology should be to meld existing theory with applied research and to offer feedback when theory and practice do not fit. Advances in gaining consensus on the most appropriate definitions of self-concept and self-esteem in Table Aspect
of self-concept
Definition
3. Summarv
of research
cancer research will depend upon more intensive interactions between those who generate theories and those who test them in applied settings. While this debate can be taken up by the literature, perhaps other forums might be more appropriate. Interdisciplinary bridges might be built by holding special conferences on the topic. by sponsoring symposia at professional meetings, and by adding persons with strong theoretical backgrounds to research teams. Our final suggestion concerning the definition of self-concept is to focus on aspects of the self that are most central to the population of interest. Although concepts such as global self-esteem may be of relevance to all cancer patients, other more specific aspects of selfhood may be important to subgroups of patients. For example, sexuality might be central to breast cancer patients, disfigurement to head and neck cancer patients, and infertility to bone marrow transplant patients. Both the literature and pilot studies should be used to inform researchers on the aspects of self that need detailed examination in a study. Self-concept
strateev
Unique
and shared
Multidimensional
or independent
components
influences
variable
definition
definition
influences
formalize
(a)
build
(b)
specify
Cc)
compare
(a)
conduct
model
individual
time
of hypothesized difference
subgroups responses
track
(c)
consolidate
(a)
explore
across studies
relationships
variables
into
to the population
(I priori
the design
different
groups
on which
aspects
of the self-concept
measures
individualized
the
longitudinal
over time
and standard
measures
relationships
between
self-concept
variables
trace the impact
of the patient’s
longitudinal
select time
are
to change
individualized
employ
literature
that are central
a priori
formative
tb)
strategies
psychosocial
(a)
(b) over
to current
focus on aspects of the self-concept
cognitive
Changes
research
employed
cc)
susceptible
Bidirectional
cariable
recommendations Possible
clarify link
as a dependent
or independent
Most of the literature reviewed here treated selfconcept as a dependent variable. We would argue that the role of self-concept should not always be assumed to be that of an outcome variable. Research on self-esteem and social support illustrates this point. Recent discussions (e.g. [70]) and research [32,71] have emphasized the role of social support in determining self-esteem in cancer patients. However, there also exists a competing interpretation: persons low in self-esteem are unable to garner social support. Wortman [72] observed that certain characteristics of the support recipient (e.g. personality traits, coping style) may influence whether or how much social support is provided. Regarding self-esteem, it is reasonable, on the basis of previous research ([73] cited in [72]), to speculate the cancer patients high in self-esteem are likely to receive greater social support than those who are low in self-esteem. In yet a third interpretation, Hobfoll and Walfisch [27] found that self-esteem acted as a resource variable that assisted cancer patients in coping with stressors. These researchers tested social support against self-esteem and found that social support did not contribute to
research
of self-concept
Self-concept
as a dependent
points
self-concept
on other
designs that
are meaningful
to the population
persons
and
Self-concept
adjustment after the effects of self-esteem and mastery were taken into account. What is clear is that self variables may play a variety of roles in cancer research: dependent variable, independent variable, moderator, or mediator. An important step in the design of studies is to formalize a model of the hypothesized relationships apriori. Designs should then be chosen that allow for the direct testing of the model. Unique and shared components
We have argued that all cancer patients are individuals; they have unique combinations of concerns, expectations, strengths, values, and resources. While we may look for generalities across the lives of patients, this search may never be complete-at the end there will always be a ‘proportion of variance’ in responses that is unique to the individual. Our goal must be to segment subgroups in the most precise manner so that we can identify the aspects of self that are likely to be involved in cancer diagnosis, treatment and recovery. Our suggestions are pragmatic: build individual difference variables into the design, specify subgroups a priori. and compare responses across different groups. The implementation of these suggestions is not so straightforward as they imply a body of knowledge that is, as yet, incomplete. There are several avenues for informing decisions concerning group segmentation and the selection of comparison groups. First, and most basic, groups should be selected based upon knowledge of life span development. As Rowland has discussed [74, 751, life tasks vary across developmental stages; the aspects of the self that are important are also likely to vary. Evidence for this perspective comes from McCrae and Costa [76] who found differences in the salience of certain aspects of the self across age groups. Specifically, they found that older persons were more likely to describe themselves in terms of their life circumstances, interests, beliefs, and hobbies, while younger persons described themselves in terms of routine tasks, personal relationships, nuclear family roles, and personality traits. Second, other individual difference variables that may be important should be considered. These may include gender, level of education, type and nature of employment, income, race, personality traits, interpersonal resources, illness history, and treatment protocol. While no study could reasonably account for all of these variables, it is realistic to expect that several of the most important indicators be included. These variables should be selected based upon the nature of the study’s hypotheses. For example, a study on return to work by cancer patients may be most concerned with selecting patients that come from a variety of pretreatment occupational categories (e.g. is return to work more salient to the self-concept of professionals than for blue collar workers?) Finally, it is important to draw comparisons across groups whenever possible. This may be done by using comparison groups within a single study (e.g. mastectomy versus lumpectomy patients) or by using instruments that have been employed with other groups of cancer patients.
and cancer
125
Multidimensional aspects
Implied in our discussion thus far is the premise that there are multiple aspects of the self that may be implicated in cancer and cancer treatment. For this reason, it will be most productive for the field to move away from designs that depend solely upon global indices of self-esteem or self-concept. Our suggestions here are to conduct formative studies on which aspects of the self-concept are susceptible to change, track individualized measures over time. and consolidate individualized and standard measures. Researchers might employ a strategy such as the following. First, through pilot studies (focus groups, interviews, surveys) researchers may identify aspects of the self that are most susceptible to change for a particular group. Second, standard instruments that focus on these aspects of the self may be incorporated into the study design. With these measures, particular domains of self could be tracked over time to see if they remain salient. Third, the relationship between the domains of self and global indices of self-concept or self-esteem could be examined and the relative predictive power compared. For example, using an instrument that measures personal changes resulting from bone marrow transplantation, Curbow et al. [64] found in a crosssectional survey that a frequent negative change among long-term survivors was a decrease in physical strength. Based upon this knowledge, future studies of this group might incorporate scales that focus on the physical self-concept. Scores on this domain might also be related to global self-esteem or other salient self domains. Bidirectional injuence
Research to date has generally failed to consider how the self-concepts of cancer patients may influence other outcomes. We have posited two avenues of this influence: the biased processing of information and the structuring of interpersonal encounters. One particularly rich area for study is to investigate the role of self-conceptions in the processing of medical and treatment information. For example, do persons who highly value a self domain ignore, discount, or transform information that concerns that domain? Questions such as this might be investigated by examining the influence of a particular self-conception (e.g. the sexual self) on the ability to process accurately and recall information that involves that domain (e.g. impotency or sterility). The ability of persons to change their self-concepts is also an area for study. Taylor and Brown [77] have suggested that, in response to recurring negative feedback in a particular life domain, individuals protect their self-esteem by downgrading the importance of the threatened domain. In this way, individuals isolate the persisting negative feedback from the rest of the self-concept. McCrae and Costa [76] have similarly argued that individuals will attempt to devalue the salience of a role they cannot perform well, focusing their attention instead on life domains that are more in line with high self-esteem. Cancer patients’ ability to adapt to self changes by devaluing threatened aspects of the self may be an important predictor of long-term adjustment.
BARBARA CIXEIOW er al.
126
We also suggested that the role of the patients’ self-concept in determining social interactions be explored. As discussed previously, it is possible that self-esteem may influence the nature and extent of social support a patient receives. It is also possible that self-variables may inff uence the way patients are treated by physicians and other medical personnel. For example, information may be withheld or ‘softened’ for patients with low self-esteem or other vulnerabilities. This implies using the patients’ selfconcept or self-esteem as a predictor of outcomes that involve other persons.
4
Changes
8.
ocer
time
Finally, we have argued that individuals change over time and that these changes may occur at different rates. Our first suggestion, which has often been voiced in the cancer literature, is to conduct longitudinal studies. Our second suggestion is to attempt to select time points that are most germane to the population. For example, Lesko [78] has mapped recently the treatment courses for a number of hematological malignancies. Depending upon the diagnosis and the treatment, patients will have varying medical and psychosocial milestones. Data points should be tied to these milestones rather than to arbitrary collection times. Although there is a burgeoning research literature on self-concept and cancer, the area is still in its infancy. We have outlined some strategies that might help to move the area along with the realization that many of these tasks will not be easy to implement. We believe that continued study on this topic has the potential for creating meaningful contributions to both theory and treatment. Most important, research on self-concept and cancer may lead to positive changes in patients’ lives. To cite one example, Markus and Nurius [50] have proposed that the ability to construct positive possible selves may be one factor that facilitates recovery from a life crisis. If research proved this to be true for cancer patients, this psychological skill could be taught to recovering patients through participation in counseling or self-help groups. Progress in this area, however, will be made only if future research tests hypotheses generated from current conceptualizations of the self-concept. Ackno~~,(edgenlenrs-Preparation of this paper was funded by a Biomedical Research Support grant from the Johns Hopkins University, Grant PBR-40 from the American Cancer Society and Grant CA49218 from the National Cancer Institute.
5
6
7
9.
10 II.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
REFERENCES I. Rice
D. P.. Hodgson T. A. and Kopstein A. N. The economic costs of illness: a replication and update. Hlth Care Financ. Rec. 7, 61-80. 1985. 2. U.S. Bureau of the Census. Statistical abstracts of the Unired States: 198% 107th edn. U.S. Government Printing Office. Washington, DC, 1986. 3. Holland J. C.. Silverfarb P., Tross S. and Cella D. Psychosocial research in cancer: the cancer and leukemia _ group . B (CALGB) experience. In Assessment of Quality of Life and Cancer- Treatment (Edited by Ventafridda V.. Dam F. S. A. M. van. Yancik R. and Tamburini M.). Excerpta Medica, Amsterdam, 1986.
22.
23. 24.
2s.
Moos R. H. and Tsu V. D. The crisis of physical illness: an overview. In Coping with Physical Illness (Edited by Moos R.). Plenum Press, New York. 1977. Turk D. C. Factors influencing the adaptive process with chronic illness. In Srress and .-lnxiery (Edited by Sarason 1. G. and Spielberger C. D.). Vol. 6. Hemisphere, Washington. -DC. 1979. Maees N. L. and Mendelsohn G. A. Effects of can;er on patients’ lives: a personological approach. In Health Psychology (Edited by Stone G. C.. Cohen F.. Adler N. E. et al.). Jossey-Bass. San Francisco, CA. 19’9. Fohz A. T. The influence of cancer on self-concept and life aualitv. Seminars Ocol. ‘Vnrs. 3, 303-312. 1987. Quigiey K. M. The adult cancer survivor: psychosocial consequences of cure. Seminars Oncol. Nurs. 5, 6349, 1989. Lesko L. M. and Holland J. C. Psychological issues in patients with hematological malignancies. Rec. Results Cancer Res. 108, 243-270, 1988. Swencionis C. Journals relevant to health psvchologv. __ __ Hlth Psychol. 1, 307-313, 1982. Berkowitz R. S.. Marean A. R.. Hamilton N.. Bernstein M.. Engle R. P. Jr, Sandman G. and Goldstein D. P. Psychological and social impact of gestational trophoblastic neoplasia. J. Reproduct. .Wed. 25, 14-16, 1980. Bisno B. and Richardson J. L. The relationship between depression and reinforcing events in cancer patients. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 2, 63-71. 1987. Bloom J. R. Social support. accommodation to stress and adjustment to breast cancer. Sot. Sci. Med. 16, 1329-1338, 1982. Bloom J. R. and Spiegel D. The relationship of two dimensions of social support to the psychological wellbeing and social functioning of women with-advanced breast cancer. Sot. Sci. Med. 19. 831-837. 1984. Cassileth B. R., Lusk E. J. and Tenaglia A. N. Patients’ perceptions of the cosmetic impact of melanoma resection. Plastic Reconstr. Surg. 71. 73-75. 1983. Celia D. F. and Tress S Psvchological adjustment to survival from Hodgkin’s djsease. J. consult. clin. Pswhol. 54, 616622, 1986. Clifford E., Clifford M. and Georgiade N. G. The meaning of concepts related to breast reconstruction. Ann. Plast. Surg. 13, 34-37, 1984. Dean C., Chetty U. and Forrest A. P. M. Effects of immediate breast reconstruction on psychosocial morbidity after mastectomy. Lancer 8322, 459-462, 1983. Edwards J., DiClemente C. and Samuels M. L. Psychological characteristics: a pretreatment survival marker of patients with testicular cancer. J. Psychosoc. Oncol. 3, 79-94, 1985. Engelman S. R. and Craddick R. The symbolic relationship of breast cancer patients to their cancer, cure. physician, and themselves. Psychorher. Psychosom. 41, 68-76, 1984. Felton B. J., Revenson T. A. and Hinrichsen G. A. Stress and coping in the explanation of psychological adjustment among chronically-ill adults. Sot. Sci. Med. 18, 889-898, 1984. Fiegenbaum W. A social training program for clients with facial disfigurations: a contribution to the rehabilitation of cancer patients. Inr. J. Rehabilit. Res. 4, 501-509, 1981. Gerard D. Sexual functioning after mastectomy: life vs. lab. J. Sex Marital Ther. 8. 305-315. 1982. Gottesman D. and Lewis M. S. Differences in crisis reactions among cancer and surgery patients. J. consult. clin. Psychol. 50, 381-388. 1982. Greer S. and Burgess C. A self-esteem measure for patients with cancer. Psychol. & Hlth 1, 327-340. 1987.
Self-concept 26. Hallal J. C. The relationship of health beliefs, health locus of control. and self-concept to the practice of breast self-examination in adult women. Nurs. Res. 31, 137-142. 1982. 27. Hobfoll S. E. and Walfisch S. Coping with a threat to life: a longitudinal study of self-concept, social support, and psychological distress. Am. J. Commun. Ps.rchoi. 12, 87-100, 1984. 28. Itano J., Tanabe P., Lum J. L. J., Lamkin L., Rizzo E., Wieland M. and Sato P. Compliance of cancer patients to therapy. West. J. Nurs. Res. 5, S-16, 1983. 29. Lewis F. M. Experienced personal control and quality of life in late-stage cancer patients. Nurs. Rex 31, 113-l 19, 1982. 30. Lewis M. S., Gottesman D. and Gutstein S. The course and duration of crisis. J. consulr. clin. Psychol. 47, 128-134, 1979. 31. MacDonald L. D. and Anderson H. R. Stigma in patients with rectal cancer: a community-study. J. Epidem. Commun. 38. 284290. 1984. 32. Nettling S. J. and Wineheld H. R. Social support and recovery after surgery for breast cancer: frequency and correlates of supportive behaviors by family, friends, and surgeon. Sot. Sci. Med. 27, 385-392, 1988. 33. Penman- D. T., Bloom J. R., Fotopoulos S.. Cook M. R.. Holland J. C.. Gates C.. Flamer D.. Murawski B.. Ross R. Brandt U., Muenz L. R. and Pee D. The impact of mastectomy on self-concept and social function: a combined cross-sectional and longitudinal study with comparison groups. Women & Hlrh 11, 101-130. 1987. 34. Pruyn J. F. A.. Rijckman R. M., Brunschot C. J. M. van and Born H. W. van den. Cancer patients’ personality characteristics, physician-patient communication and adoption of the Moerman Diet. Sot. Sci. Med. 20, 841-847. 1985. 35. Ramboer C.. Verhamme M. and Vermeire L. Patients’ perception of involuntary weight loss: implications of underestimation and overestimation. Br. med. J. 291, 1091, 1985. 36. Revenson T. A.. Wollman C. A. and Felton B. J. Social supports as stress buffers for adult cancer patients. P&chosom. Med. 45, 321-331, 1983. 37. Schain W. S.. Wellisch D. K.. Pasnau R. 0. and Landsverk J. The sooner the better: a study of psychological factors in women undergoing immediate versus delayed breast reconstruction. Am. J. Psychiat. 142, 4@46, 1985. 38. Spiegel D.. Bloom J. R. and Yalom I. Group support for patients with metastic cancer: a randomized prospective outcome study. Archs gen. Psychial. 38, 527-533. 198 I. 39. Steinberg M. D.. Juliano M. A. and Wise L. Psychological outcome of lumpectomy versus mastectomy in the treatment of breast cancer. Am. J. Psychiar. 142, 34-39, 1985. 40. Taylor S. E., Lichtman R. R. and Wood J. V. Attributions, beliefs about control, and adjustment to breast cancer. J. Person. sot. Psychol. 46, 489-502. 1984. 41. Timko C. and Janoff-Bulman R. Attributions, vulnerability, and psychological adjustment: the case of breast cancer. Hlrh Ps_vchol. 4, 521-544, 1985. 42. Van den Borne H. W., Pruyn J. F. A. and Van den Heuvel W. J. A. Effects of contacts between cancer patients on their psychosocial problems. Parienr Educ. Counsel. 9. 33-51. 1987. 43. Watson P. G. The effects of short-term postoperative counseling on cancer/ostomy patients. Cancer Nurs. 6, 21-29, 1983. 44. Wolcott D. L., Wellisch D. K., Fawzy F. I. and Landsverk J. Adaptation of adult bone marrow transplant recipient long-term survivors. Transplantation 41, 478-484. 1986.
and cancer
I27
45. Worden J. W. and Weisman A. D. Preventive psychosocial intervention with newly diagnosed cancer patients. Gen. Hosp. P.ywhiat. 6, 243-249. 1984. 46. Youssef F. A. Crisis intervention: a group-therapy approach for hospitalized breast cancer patients. J. Adc. Nurs. 9, 307-3 13. 1984. 47. Schain W. Sexual functioning. self-esteem and cancer care. Front. Radial. Ther. & Oncol. 14, 12-29. 1980. 48. Greenwald A. G.. Bellezza F. S. and Banaji M. R. Is self-esteem a central ingredient of the self-concept? Person. Sot. P.rychol. Bull. 14, 3445. 1988. 49. Rosenberg M. Socier! and the Adolescenr Self Image. Princeton University Press. Princeton. NJ. 1965. 50. Markus H.. and Nurius P. Possible selves. .dnt. PsychoI. 41, 954-969, 1986. 51. Greenwald A. G. and Pratkanis A. R. The self. In Handbook of Social Coanirion (Edited bv. Wver _ R. S. and Srull T. K.). Vol. 3. pp. 129-178. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. 1984. 52. Markus H. and Wurf E. The dynamic self-concept: a social pscyhological perspective. A. Rec.. Ps.vchol. 38, 299-337, 1987. 53. Marsh H. W. Global self-esteem: its relation to specific facets of self-concept and their importance. J. Person. sot. Psycho/. 51, 1224-1236. 1986: 54. Mullan F. Vital Signs: A Young Docror’s Sfruggle with Cancer. Farrar. Straus & Giroux. New York. 1982. 55. Koocher G. P. and O’Malley E. 0. The Damocles Svndrome. McGraw-Hill. New York. 198 I. 56. Dreher H. Cancer and the mind: current concepts in psycho-oncology. Advances 4, 2743. 1987. 57. Schlenker B. R. Introduction: foundations of the self in social life. In The Self and Social Lifti (Edited by Schlenker B. R.). pp. l-28. McGraw-Hill. Sew York, 1985. 58. Meyerowitz B. E. Psychological correlates of breast cancer and its treatments. Psycho/. Bull. 87, 108-131, 1980. 59. Andersen B. L. Sexual functioning morbidity among cancer survivors: current status and future research directions. Cancer 55, 1835-1842, 1985. 60. Haes J. C. J. M. de, Oostrom M. A. van and Welvaart K. The effect of radical and conserving surgery on the quality of life of early breast cancer patients. Eur. J. Surg. Oncol. 12, 337-342, 1986. 61. Aaronson N. K., Bartelink H.. Dongen J. A. van and Dam F. S. A. M. van. Evaluation of breast conserving therapy: clinical, methodological, and psychosocial perspectives. Eur. J. Surg. Oncol. 14, 133-140, 1988. 62. Thompson C. B. and Thomas E. D. Bone marrow transplantation. Surg. Clin. N. Am. 66, 589~01, 1986. 63. Hoelter J. W. The structure of self conception: conceptualization and measurement. J. Person. sot. Psychol. 49, 1392-1407, 1985. 64. Curbow B., Baker F., Wingard J.. Legro M. and Somerfield M. The challenged self: personal changes and psychosocial adaptation to an aggressive cancer treatment. Unpublished manuscript. 65. Davis B. D., Fernandez F., Adams F., Holmes V., Levy J. K.. Lewis D. and Niedhart J. Diagnosis of dementia in cancer patients. Psychosomatics 28, 175-179, 1987. 66. Gergen K: J. From self to science: what is there to know? In Psrcholonical Perwecrires on the Self(Edited by Suls J.),.Vol. i. pp. 129-149. Laurence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1982. 67. Swann W. B. Jr. The self as architect of social reality. In The Selfand Social Li/e (Edited by Schlenker B. R:). McGraw-Hill. New York. 1985. 68. Singer J. E. Some issues in the study of coping. Cancer 53, 2303-23 13, 1984. 69. Cella D. F., Jacobsen P. B. and Lesko L. M. Research methods in psychooncology. In Handbook of Psycho-
BARBARA CURBOWet al.
128
oncology: Psychological Care of rhe Patient with Cancer
(Edited by Holland J. C. and Rowland J. H.). pp. 737-749. Oxford University Press, New York, 1989. 70. Rowland J. H. Interpersonal resources: social support. In Handbook of‘ Psychooncology: Psychological Care o/ rhe Parienr Wirh Cancer (Edited by Holland J. C. and Rowland J. H.), pp. 58-71. Oxford University Press, New York. 1989. 71. Zemore R. and Shepel L. F. Etfects of breast cancer and mastectomy on emotional support and adjustment. Sot. Sci. Med. 28, 19-27. 1989. 72. Wortman C. B. Social support and the cancer patient: conceptual and methodologic issues. Cancer Suppl. 53, 2339-2360, 1984. 73. Coates D., Wortman C. B. and Abbey A. Reactions to victims. In Nenz Approaches IO Social Problems (Edited by Frieze I. H., Bar-tal D. and Carroll J. S.). Jossey-Bass. San Francisco, CA, 1979. 74. Rowland J. H. Developmental stage and adaptation: adult model. In Handbook of Psvchooncologv: Psvchological Care 01 the Patien; With Cancer -(Edited by Holland J. C. and Rowland J. H.). DD. 2543. Oxford University Press, New York, 1989. 75. Rowland J. H. Developmental stage and adaptation: child and adolescent model. In Handbook of PsychoI.
.
.
oncology: Psychological Care of the Parient With Cancer
J. H.), pp. 218-231. Oxford University Press. New York, 1989. 79. Janis I. L. Psychological Stress. Wiley, New York, 1958. 80. Osgood C. E., Suci G. J. and Tannenbaum P. H. The Measurement of Meaning. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, IL, 1957. 81. Kahn T. An original test of symbol arrangement validated on organic psychotics. J. cons&. Psychol. 15, 439444. 1951. 82. Polivy J. Psychological
effects of mastectomy on a woman’s feminine self-concept. J. nerr. menu. Disorders
16, 77-87, 1977. 83. Jourard S. M. and Secord P. F. Body cathexis and the ideal female figure. J. abnorm. sot. Psychol. 50.243-246, 1955. 84. Berscheid E., Walster E. and Bohrnstedt G. Body image: A Psychology Today questionnaire. Psychol. Today 6, 58-66, 1972. 85. Schain W. Personal problem checklist. Unpublished
manuscript. 86. Eagly A. H. Involvement as a determinant of response to favorable and unfavorable information. J. Person. sot. Psychol. 9, I-15, 1967. 87. Fitts W. Tennessee Self Concear Scale Manual. Coun-
(Edited by Holland J. C. and Rowland J. H.), pp. 519-543. Oxford University Press, New York, 1989. 76. McCrae R. R. and Costa P. T. Jr. Age, personality, and the spontaneous self-concept. J. Geronr.: Sot. Sri. 43,
selor Recordings and Tests, Nashville. TN, 1965. 88. Simmons R. G. Long-term reactions of renal recipients and donors. In Psvchonephrology (Edited by Levy N. B.), Vol. 2, p. 275. Plenum, New York, 1983. 89. Shostrom E. L. Manual of the Personal Orienrarion
S177-185, 1988. 71. Taylor S. E. and Brown J. D. Illusion and well-being:
Incenrory and Innemory for the Measurement of Self Acfuali:ation. Educational & Industrial Teaching Ser-
a social psychological
perspective on mental health.
Psycho/. Bull. 103, 193-210,
1988.
78. Lesko L. M. Hematological malignancies. In Handbook of Psychooncology: Psychological Care of the Patienr Wirh Cancer (Edited by Holland J. C. and Rowland
vice, San Diego, CA, 1966. 90. Pearlin H. J. and Schooler C. The structure of coping. J. Hllh sot. Behav. 19, 2-22, 1978. 91. Lum J. Nursing care of oncology patients receiving chemotherapy. Nurs. Res. 27, 34&347. 1978.