Sensory attributes of craved a n d aversive foods in healthy women Diedre M Blank ABSTRACT:- This article is an edited version of a paper presented at the Sigma Theta Tau International Nursing Research Congress in Madrid, Spain on June 22, 1993. It explores the sensory characteristics of craved and aversive foods in healthy women. Typically, cravings are characterised as intermittents, sweet-tasting and pleasant-smelling. Aversions are continuous, bad-tasting and unpleasant-smelling. Women reporting aversions tend to have less body mass than women without aversions: p<0.06. The data suggest that craved and aversive foods may exert unequal effects and that sensory characteristics may provide cues for the formation of cravings and aversions. These early findings guide health-care practitioners in helping individuals to improve their nutritional intake.
Diet has long been known to play a critical role in health,
al 1981; De Silva & Rachman 1987) shows that food
but only recently has a systematic review of research
preferences are influenced by factors other than nutrition
been used to document the role of diet in health
and energy content; eg sensory, cultural, environmental,
promotion and disease prevention (US Department of
economic, physiologic, psychologic and social factors.
Health and Human Services [USDHHS] 1988). We can now assume that the daily dietary choices we make not
There is strong support (Mattes 1987) for the important
only
role of the chemical senses in dietary intake and nutrient
contribute
to
selected
diseases
but,
more
importantly, affect our current state of health and help
utilisation. Some studies (Hook 1978; Tierson et al 1985)
prevent certain diseases.
suggest that changes in gustatory (taste) and olfactory
Health-care practitioners are often in a unique position to
cravings and aversions. In other research (Mattes et al
(smell) sensitivity may account for changes in food
assess a patient's diet, not only during disease but also in
1990), abnormalities of taste and smell have been
periods of health (Houldin et al 1987). In assessing the
associated with food-related complaints and changes in
role of diet in promoting health and preventing disease,
body weight.
it is essential that we recognise that health and disease
smell distortions tended to report weight loss, while
are determined by a host of factors. It is uncertain to
individuals with sensory loss tended to report weight
what extent diet plays a direct role.
Specifically, individuals with taste and
Therefore, it is
gain. Additional information is needed to document the
impossible to decide the proportion of disease that could
extent to which sensory properties serve as the basis for
be reduced by dietary changes (USDIII IS 1988). Thus, it
craved a n d / o r aversive foods, and what impact this
is essential that researchers explore - from various
might have on body weight.
perspectives - the factors affecting our selection and rejection of foods, particularly given the magnitude of
Finally, previous reports on craved foods have largely
health and economic consequences resulting from poor
been anecdotal.
dietary practices.
documentation of craving incidence during pregnancy
Research has been confined to either
(Sugiura et al 1983; Worthington-Roberts 1989), the
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
specific
foods
usually craved
or rejected
during
pregnancy (Little et al 1976; Hook 1978; Darwish et al Foods are accepted or rejected for a variety of reasons.
1982; Hdanza & Fidanza 1986) or both (Dickens &
Although
to
Trcthowan 1971; Finley et al 1985; Osman 1985; Tierson et
physiological control mechanisms (Worsley 1980), others
al 1985). Other studies (Baylis et al 1983, Walker et al
suggest more behavioural mechanisms (Fantino 1984;
1985) have concentrated on the relationship of specific
Rozin & Vollmecke 1986). Research (Khan 1981; Togue et
food
. AUSTRALIA
some
attribute
these
preferences
21
practices
to pregnancy-related
symptoms
COLLEGIAN VOLUME 3 (1) JANUARY 1996
-
eg nausea and vomiting. Few studies (Little et al 1976;
the
Brown & Toma 1986; Blank & Mattes 1990a) have
Specifically, women were asked three questions in
addressed the sensory properties attributed to craved
relation to sensory aspects:
and aversivc foods.
sensory
characteristics
of
each
listed
item.
This study explored the sensory
attributes of craved and aversive foods in healthy
•
women.
"Why was this particular item craved (or aversive}?" They were to circle all the listed answers that applied; eg smell, taste, texture, cooling sensation.
METHOD •
"I low would you describe the predominant taste?" Mere they were to circle one response from the listed
Sample
responses of bitter, sour, sweet, salty and other. A convenience sample of 58 healthy, non-pregnant female adults (including 29 paras and 29 nulliparas)
•
"Mow would you describe the smell?" Women were
between the ages of 18 and 50 years (X -- 33.7; SD - 8.4)
to circle all the listed responses that applied; eg
participated in the study.
pleasant, unpleasant, strong, weak.
Women with a history of
pregnancy were included only if they reported a history of full-term pregnancies. Individuals with prior medical
In addition, one question asked, "How would you best
conditions known to affect the chemical senses - eg sinus
describe the frequency of this craving?" Women were to
or taste disorders (Schiffman 1983) - were excluded from
circle one of the following responses:
the study. •
intermittent (coming and going, but not at set times);
•
continuous;
•
only one occurrence;
•
present during part or all of pregnancy, indicating
Instrument and procedure
Women were recruited from two universities by public advertisement. A brief screening questionnaire was used to decide eligibility and collect demographic data. The women completed a two-part dietary questionnaire, which elicited retrospective information based upon each
what, if any, trimesters were involved;
subject's overall past life experiences, without any prompts being given in regard to specific periods in their
•
related to a specific phase of the menstrual cycle,
life. In the first part, subjects responded to the following
indicating whether it was in a 10-day period prior to
questions:
or after the onset of menstruation, or
•
"Have you ever had a craving or strong desire to eat or drink a food, beverage or other
•
other.
substance
(including spices, herbs, seasonings or items not
In the second part, subjects recorded their preferences for
usually thought of as edible), such that you simply
specific tastes - ic sweet, salty, sour and bitter — and their
could not resist consuming the item?" and
use of selected spices: anise, bay, cinnamon, cloves, ginger, nutmeg, salt, spearmint, sugar and vanilla.
•
"I lave you ever developed an aversion or strong
Sweets have been highly targeted for cravings (Smith &
dislike for any previously acceptable food, beverage
Sander 1969; Dickens & Trethowan 1971; Hook 1978) and
or
aversions (Mattes et al 1987).
other
substance
(including
spices,
herbs,
Also, spices have
seasonings or items not usually thought of as edible),
previously been targeted as both craved and aversive
such that you felt it would make you physically ill if
items in the general population (Logue & Smith 1986),
consumed?"
and in response to pregnancy and menstruation (Hook 1978; Snow & Johnson 1978; Darwish et al 1982; Osman
If they answered 'yes' to either question they were asked
1985; Culick et al 1986).
to list all craved a n d / o r aversive items and to prioritize
attributed to the study spices has recently been reported
The degree of sweet taste
their lists. For the three most craved a n d / o r aversive
(Blank & Mattes 1990b).
items, subjects were asked to respond to questions about
collection was approximately 20 minutes.
C^Ui^ftHy^tU^:i- ; (i)j^UAHY1996
.22
The average time of data
ROYAL COLtest'OF NURSING. AUSTRALIA
Statistical analyses
16 (55 per cent) of the nulliparous women reported one or more aversions. A trend was noted for women reporting
Descriptive statistics were used to describe and synthesise
food aversions to have a lower BMI than women without
the data.
aversions: (X2 (1)=3.38, p<0.07).
Pie charts were used to illustrate ungrouped
frequency distributions for food craving and aversion incidence.
Bar charts were used to display grouped
Figure 1 presents a breakdown of the data in terms of
frequency distributions for specific sensory modalities and
relative frequency of food cravings and aversions.
descriptions of food cravings and aversions.
Subjects
comparisons for body
Group
mass index (BMI) (16.2 to
typically
intermittent
characterised
food
in nature, whereas they
cravings found
as food
22.8kg/(M) 2 versus 22.9 to 36.7kg/(M) 2 were made using
aversions to be mainly continuous in nature. Of those
chi square tests. T-tcsts for independent samples were
women reporting cravings and aversions associated with
used to compare spice use.
their menstrual cycle, eight of the nine cravings and all
Tbe level of statistical
three aversions were reported to occur during the 10-day
significance for group comparisons was set at 0.05.
period before menstruation (luteal phase).
RESULTS Figure 2 illustrates the sensory modalities credited to Forty-two {72 per cent) of the 58 subjects experienced at
craved and aversive foods.
While taste was over-
least one food craving, with a total of 100 craved items
whelmingly attributed to craved foods, a combination of
identified. Twenty (69 per cent) of the 29 parous women
taste, odour and chemesthesis (temperature, irritation,
reported one or more cravings, whereas 22 (76 per cent)
texture etc) was attributed to aversive foods.
of the 29 nulliparous women reported one or more cravings. Twenty-eight (48 per cent) of the 58 subjects
Figure 3 lists the specific sensory descriptions assigned to
experienced at least one food aversion, with a total of 48
both
avcrsive items identified. Twelve (41 per cent) of the 29
predominated for craved foods, followed closely by salty
parous women reported one or more aversions, whereas
taste, whereas 'other' tastes mainly characterised aversive
Cravings (per cent) n-100
food
cravings
and
aversions.
Sweet
taste
Aversions (per cent) n=48
2.08
•
Intermittent
Menstrual cycle
Pregnancy
One occurence
Continuous
•
Other (cravings only)
Figure 1. Incidence of food cravings and aversion.
ROYAL COLLEGE OF NURSING. AUSTRALIA
23
COLLEGIAN VOLUME 3 (1) JANUARY 1996
Figure 2. Sensory modalities attributed to food.
foods. 'Other' tastes reported for food aversions included descriptions such as 'rotten', 'fishy', 'oily', 'nauseating' and 'difficult to describe'. Examples of 'other' tastes reported for food cravings included descriptions such as 'spicy', 'spicy-hot', 'peanut-like', 'refreshing',
'crunchy'
and
'bland'. Odours for craved foods were reported mainly as pleasant and familiar, while odours for aversive foods were reported as unpleasant and strong. The major chemesthetic descriptors attributed to craved foods included texture and characteristics related to temperature - ie cooling and burning, whereas texture and irritation were attributed more so to aversive foods.
Finally, there was a trend for parous women to eat more spices than nuiliparous women: t (56) = 1.81, p<0.08. In particular, parous women reported a tendency to consume more salt than nuiliparous women: t (56) = 1.91, p<0.06.
DISCUSSION The incidence of food cravings in this study generally agrees with previous reports of cravings experienced by pregnant women (Dickens & Trcthowan 1971; Tierson et ill 1985; Walker et al 1985) and by non-pregnant women (Blank & Mattes 1990a; Rodin et al 1991). Most healthy women in the study reported at least one craving, despite reproductive history. This finding supports Rodin et al's (1991) view that cravings may represent something other than a physiological need. As expected from previous research (Hook 1978; Blank & Mattes 1990a), food is craved predominantly for its sweet and salty taste.
COLLEGIAN VOLUME 3 0) JANUARY 1996
24
Figure 3. Incidence of food cravings and aversions.
ROYAL COLLEGE OF NURSING, AUSTRALIA
The findings that food cravings are mainly intermittent
The
or transient also agrees with previous literature (Dalvit
continuous and that there is a trend for aversions to be
1981; Tierson et al 1985; Cohen et al 1987; Bancroft et al
connected to a lower BM1 suggests that aversions may
1988; Blank & Mattes 1990a). This finding suggests that
have more immediate implications for individrials.
cravings may not have long-term consequences.
findings
that
food
aversions
are
generally
In
Thus, health-care practitioners must be able to assess an
contrast, the study trend of a lower BM1 with a history of
individual's ability to meet the minimum level of
aversions is consistent with the previous findings of Garb
nutrients needed to achieve or maintain good health.
and Stunkard (1974).
These authors found that both
overweight and underweight subjects, but not subjects of
CONCLUSION
normal weight, tended to report more aversions. I he study findings on craving and aversion incidence The trend for women with a previous history of
present some interesting similarities and differences. Of
pregnancy to eat more spices, particularly salt, supports
particular note, the findings suggest that craved and
in part the previous work of Brown and Toma (1986).
aversive foods may exert unequal effects and that
These authors found
sensory characteristics may provide cues for
that pregnant women
were
the
significantly less able to correctly identify concentration
formation of cravings and aversions.
differences for salt solutions and preferred
using a prospective approach and a larger sample is
stronger
solutions, but that these differences decreased after
Future research
needed to validate these early observations.
pregnancy. The current study is limited in that it does not address when salt intake occurred, nor the period of
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
past pregnancy. Additional study is needed to sort out conflicting literature on taste changes during pregnancy.
This study was supported in part by a Henry 1: Rutgers
That is, some literature (Schmidt 1925) suggests that taste
fellowship and a grant from the Rutgers University
changes during this period are due to decreased taste sensitivity, while other literature (Brown & Toma 1986) promotes a physiologic mechanism for increasing salt intake during pregnancy. In addition, Rodin et al (1991)
Research Council, #202113.
Linda Flynn MS RN,
doctoral student, College of Nursing, Rutgers University, the State University of New Jersey, is acknowledged for her assistance in the data collection.
found that overweight women reported more consistent preferences for foods high in salt and perceived flavour intensity than did underweight women. These findings, when considered together, suggest that sensory factors as they relate to salt taste and preference may play an important role in times of increased caloric intake.
REFERENCES Bancroft |, Cook A anil Williamson L 1988 Fond craving, mood and the menstrual cycle. Psychological Medicine 18:855-60 Baylis JM, Leeds AR and Challacumbe DN 1983 nausea and food aversions in pregnancy.
Persistent
Clinical Allergy
13:263-69
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
Blank DM and Mattes RD 1990a
Lxploration of the sensorv
characteristics of craved and aversive foods. Journal of Sensory
Given that food cravings and aversions are quite common in healthy women, health-care practitioners
should
address early in their nutritional assessment of individuals the effects of sensory factors on the perception of satiety Alternate strategies that individuals might use to satisfy their sense of taste and smell, without having to compromise their caloric intake, need to be examined.
Studies 5:193-202 Blank DM and Mattes RD 1990b Sugar and spice: similarities and sensory attributes. Nursing Research 39(5):290-93 Brown JL and Toma RB 1986 Taste changes during pregnancy. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 43:414-18 Cohen IT, Sherwin bb and Fleming AS 1987 mood, and the menstrual cycle.
Food cravings,
Hormones and Behaviour
21:457-70 Dalvit SP 1981 The effect of the menstrual cycle on patterns of
Because of such a strong desire for sweet and salty tastes in craved food, health-care workers must be particularly vigilant to healthy foods containing these properties. They also need to be aware of specific disorders and medications (Schiffman 1983) that can affect taste and smell, so they can advise their clients accordingly.
ROYAL COLLEGE OF NURSING. AUSTRALIA
25
food intake.
the American Journal of Clinical Nutrition
34:1811-15 Darwish OA, Amine EK and Abdalla SM 1982
Food habits
during pregnancy and lactation in Iraq. Food and Nutrition Bulletin 4(3):14-16 Dc Silva P and Rachman S 1987 Human food aversions: nature and acquisition. Behaviour Research and Therapy 25(6):457-68
COLLEGIAN VOLUME 3 (1) JANUARY 1996
Dickens G and Trethowan VVli 1971
Cravings and aversions
during pregnancy. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 15:259-68 Fantino M 1984 Role of sensory input in Ihe control of food intake. Journal of the Autonomic Nervous System 10:347-58 Fidanza AA and Fidanza K 1986 A nutrition study involving a group of pregnant women in Assisi, Italv. International Journal
Finley DA, Dewey KG, Lonnerdal B and Grivetti LL 1985 Food choices of vegetarians and nonvegetarians during pregnancy Journal of the American Dietetic Association
Tropical Pediatrics 31:16-20 Rodin J,MancusoJ,GrangerJ et al 1991 Food cravings in relation to body mass index, restraint and estradiol levels: a repeated measures study in healthy women. Appetite 17:177-85 Review of Nutrition 6:433-56 Schmidt H 1925 Transient loss of the sense of smell and taste during pregnancy. Klinische Wochensehrift 4:1967-1968 Schiffman SS 1983 Taste and smell in disease. The New England
85(6):678-85 GarbJLandStunkard AJ 1974 Taste aversions in man. American
Journal of Medicine 308:1275-79 Smith SL and Sauder C 1969 Food cravings, depression, and
Journal of Psychiatry 131:1204-07 Gulick F.F, Franklin CM and Elinson M 1986 Food beliefs and food behaviours among minority pregnant women. Journal of
premenstrual problems. Psychosomatic Medicine 31(4):281-87 Snow LF and Johnson SM 1978
Folklore, food,
female
reproductive cvclc. Fcologv of Food and Nutrition 7:41-49
Perinatology 6(3):197-202 Hook F.B 1978 Dietary cra\ ings and aversions during pregnancy. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 31:1355-62 1 louldin AD, Sal stein SW and Canley KM 1987 Nursing diagnosis for wellness: supporting strengths. JB I.ippincott, Philadelphia Khan MA 1981
Dietarv practices and aversions during
pregnancy and lactation among Sudanese women. Journal of
Rnzin P and Vollmecke TA 1985 Food likes and dislikes. Annual
of Vitamin and Nutrition Research 56:373-80
and lactation.
Osman AK 1985
Evaluation of food selection patterns and
preferences. CRC Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition
Sugiura K, Tanuma J, Inomata Y et al 1983 Study on dietary habits of prenatal class attendants at health centers of Yokohama City. Japanese Journal of Nutrition 41(l):ll-20 Tierson FD, Olsen CT. and Hook FB 1985 Influence of cravings and aversions on diet in pregnancy.
Ecology of Food and
Nutrition 17:117-29 US Department of Health and Human Services 1988 Ihe Surgeon
15:129-53 Little RF, Sennit/ FA and Mandell W 1976
Drinking during
pregnancy, journal of Studies on Alcohol 37(3)375-79
General's report on nutrition and health (Dl ISS publication No. 88-50210). Government Printing Office, Washington DC
Logue AW, Ophir 1 and Strauss KF 1981 The acquisition of taste
Walker ARP, Walker BF, Jones J et al 1985 Nausea and vomiting
aversions in humans. Behaviour Research and Therapy 19:319-33
and dietary cravings and aversions during pregnancy in South
Logue AW and Smith ML 1986 Predictors of fond preferences in
African women. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology 92:484-89
adult humans. Appetite 7:109-25 Mattes RD 1987 Sensory influences on food intake and utilisation in humans. Human Nutrition: Applied Nutrition 41A:77-95 Mattes RD, Arnold C and Boraas M 1987 Learned food aversions among cancer chemotherapy patients. Cancer 60:2576-80 Mattes RD, Cowart BJ, Schiano MA et al 1990 Dietarv evaluation of patients with smell a n d / o r taste disorders.
Worsley A 1980 Thought for food: investigations of cognitive aspects of food. Fcology of Food and Nutrition 9:65-80 Worthington-Roberts B, Little KL, Lambert MD et al 1989 Dietary cravings and aversions in the postpartum period. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 89(5):647-51
American
Journal of Clinical Nutrition 51:233-40
Royal College of Nursing
Australia
(Queensland
Chapter)
Distinguished Nursing Service Award Nominations are invited for the 1996 presentation of the Distinguished Nursing Service Award. The award may be given by the Queensland Chapter to a person in recognition of outstanding performance or achievement in nursing and/or outstanding contribution to nursing in Queensland. Nominations close on Friday February 16, 1996. Nomination forms and information regarding the selection procedure and criteria can be obtained from: Ms Jenny Bichel, Honorary Secretary Royal College of Nursing, AListralia Queensland Chapter PO Box 311 Spring Hill, Qld 4004
COLLEGIAN VOLUME 3 (1) JANUARY 1996
26
ROYAL COLLEGE OF NURSING. AUSTRALIA