Sleep-associated respiratory disorders and their psychobehavioral consequences in children

Sleep-associated respiratory disorders and their psychobehavioral consequences in children

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 98 (3rd series) Sleep Disorders, Part 1 P. Montagna and S. Chokroverty, Editors # 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights r...

900KB Sizes 1 Downloads 119 Views

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 98 (3rd series) Sleep Disorders, Part 1 P. Montagna and S. Chokroverty, Editors # 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 32

Sleep-associated respiratory disorders and their psychobehavioral consequences in children 1 2

HAWLEY E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS 1 AND DAVID GOZAL 2 * Departments of Psychology and Pediatrics, West Virginia University, Morgantown, WV, USA

Department of Pediatrics, Comer Children’s Hospital, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA

SLEEP DISTURBANCES IN CHILDREN Pediatric sleep continues to gain significant recognition due to both increasing evidence of a high prevalence of sleep disorders among children, and by virtue of the potential somatic and psychobehavioral effects of disrupted sleep during early development. Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is by far the most frequently diagnosed pediatric sleep disorder, affecting at least 1–3% of children, while symptoms consistent with risk for sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) have been reported in 6–27% of children (Brouillette et al., 1984; Ali et al., 1993; Gislason and Benediktsdottir, 1995; Hulcrantz et al., 1995; Archbold et al., 2002; Young et al., 2002; Montgomery-Downs et al., 2003). Children with SDB experience more frequent pulmonary hypertension (Shiomi et al., 1993), systemic hypertension, and other cardiovascular disturbances such as left ventricular hypertrophy (Marcus et al., 1998; Amin et al., 2002, 2005; O’Brien and Gozal, 2005), deficient somatic growth (Everett et al., 1987), and comorbid chronic illnesses (Rona et al., 1998; Archbold et al., 2002). In addition, pediatric OSA is associated with poor quality of life (Rosen et al., 2002; Crabtree et al., 2004; Mitchell and Kelly, 2005; Stewart et al., 2005), depressed mood (Crabtree et al., 2004; Mitchell and Kelly, 2006), and increased health care utilization (Reuveni et al., 2002; Tarasiuk et al., 2004). For the majority of children with mild to moderate SDB, psychobehavioral comorbidities are understood to be the most crucial consequence. Studies on these specific costs have focused on the daytime effects of

sleep restriction and sleep-related breathing disorders (e.g., snoring and OSA), which are also associated with both intermittent hypoxia and sleep fragmentation. The consequences of disrupted sleep specific to the effects of sleep-associated respiratory disorders will form the specific focus of this review.

Sleepiness The predominant manifestation of sleeplessness or sleep disturbance is “sleepiness” and thus it is important first to address what is meant by this sometimes ambiguous term. Originally described by Carskadon and Dement (1977), one objective technique used to quantify sleepiness is the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT). This simple method involves a series of 20–30-minute polysomnographically recorded daytime nap opportunities in a sleep-promoting environment (i.e., darkened and quiet room, comfortable bed and temperature). Under such standardized circumstances, the shorter the latency to sleep onset during these nap opportunities, the higher the degree of sleepiness. Such assessment is expensive, labor-intensive, and difficult with children so rare studies have objectively measured daytime sleepiness in children, though it has been found that compared to control children, those with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have greater daytime sleepiness (Golan et al., 2004). Like studies with adults, for sleepiness measured by MSLT with children there seems to be a limited relationship with subjective report (Chervin et al., 2006a). However, it is important to note that

*Correspondence to: David Gozal, M.D., Herbert T. Abelson Distinguished Professor and Chair, Department of Pediatrics, Physician-in-Chief, Comer Children’s Hospital, The University of Chicago, 5721 S. Maryland Avenue, MC 8000, Suite K-160, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. Tel: (773) 702-3360, Fax: (773) 702-4523, E-mail: [email protected]

490 H.E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS AND D. GOZAL behavioral sleepiness may display differently in chilbehavioral problems (Lavigne et al., 1999), and the dren than it does in adults. The manifestations of reciprocal of this observation holds true as well, since sleepiness in children will be discussed throughout this improvements in sleep are associated with improvereview. ment in daytime behavior (Minde et al., 1994; Ali et al., 1996; Chervin et al., 2006a). Thus, sleep and behavior exhibit dynamic interacBEHAVIORAL CONSEQUENCES tions that may either interfere with each other or synerOF SLEEP DISTURBANCE gistically enhance each other in children. In order to When sleep fragmentation is experimentally induced in understand further the potential impact of SDB on healthy adults using auditory stimuli to elicit arousals cognitive and behavioral functioning it is necessary to throughout the night, performance detriments are understand that hypoxia is unlikely to occur in isolaclearly apparent the following day (Stepansky et al., tion. Rather, SDB is typified by the co-occurrence of 1984, 1987; Chugh et al., 1996). In this context, cogniintermittent hypoxia and brief arousals that cause fragtive functions requiring concentration and motor mented architecture. Therefore, a short review of the dexterity are preferentially affected by sleep fragmeneffects of sleep restriction on higher-level functioning tation and often confusion and disorientation also in children is appropriate. occur, and have led to the term “sleep drunkenness.” Aggressive outbursts, irritability, anxiety, and depresSleep restriction sion are all known manifestations of excessive daytime Acute sleep restriction for one night in children has sleepiness in adults, and appear to be fully reversed been shown to increase inattentive behavior the followonce sleep is allowed and recovery occurs (for review, ing day, although without changes in hyperactive or see Roth and Roehrs, 1996). impulsive behaviors (Fallone et al., 2001). Extended Similar to adult findings, Sadeh and colleagues sleep restriction for 7 nights led to increased parent(2000) reported a high prevalence of sleep fragmentareported oppositional and inattentive behaviors tion in children but the effects of sleep fragmentation (Fallone et al., 2000) and teacher-reported academic on daytime functioning have yet to be examined in problems and attention problems (Fallone et al., detail among pediatric populations. It is known that 2005). Notwithstanding such observations, total sleep infants, toddlers, and school-age children who are time does not appear to be the major determinant of reported by their parents to be poor sleepers display daytime behavior problems in children. Rather, disrupincreased incidence and severity of behavioral issues tion of the sleep process instead of total amount of compared to children without reported sleep problems sleep may be the key factor underlying behavioral (Zuckerman et al., 1987; Ali et al., 1993, 1994; Minde alterations that are vulnerable to sleep disruption et al., 1993; Chervin et al., 1997; Stein et al., 2001). (Stores, 1996). Thus, an association between neuroThese observations have been confirmed by objective behavioral disturbances and the fragmentation by assessments, in which the degree of sleep disturbance multiple arousals observed in OSA or in periodic limb and the severity of behavioral changes are strongly movement disorder of sleep would be expected. To associated in most studies (Guilleminault et al., 1981; corroborate this supposition, a study from our laboraAli et al., 1996; Aronen et al., 2000; Chervin et al., tory found significant relationships between arousals 2000; Chervin and Archbold, 2001; O’Brien et al., associated with periodic limb movements during sleep 2003). However, work by Chervin and colleagues and ADHD (Crabtree et al., 2003). Thus, an associative (2006a) showed that, although adenotonsillectomy and possibly causal link appears to be present between improved neurobehavioral and parent report measures fragmented sleep and hyperactive behaviors. of hyperactivity to the extent that children no longer Developmental changes during adolescence also differed from controls (who did not change), neither afford an opportunity to examine the effects of sleep baseline SDB measures nor improvement predicted restriction. It has been clear for some time that both the degree of improvement on any outcome measure homeostatic influences (e.g., the time elapsed since the other than sleepiness. previous sleep period and circadian clock regulatory sysNevertheless, 36% of young children with global tems), and individual differences (e.g., motivation to fall reports of sleep problems presented with significant asleep and psychological tension or anxiety) affect daybehavioral problems (Smedje et al., 2001) and daytime time sleepiness in this age group. Extensive work by hyperactivity, and anxiety and depressive symptoms Carskadon and colleagues has shown that pubertal have been associated with prolonged sleep latency development is associated with increased daytime sleep(Smedje et al., 2001; Stein et al., 2001). Preschool chiliness, such that postpubertal adolescents require more dren with shorter total sleep time exhibited more

SLEEP-ASSOCIATED RESPIRATORY DISORDERS IN CHILDREN 491 sleep to retain prepubertal levels of alertness (Carskadon looking” and slow to respond to questions, yet it took et al., 1980; Carskadon and Dement, 1987). a century before Osler’s observations on neurocognitive The Carskadon lab has established that there is a decrements in pediatric OSA were investigated using biological shift in the homeostatic drive during adolesobjective methodology. Of particular emphasis is the fact cence, inhibiting later-stage pubertal adolescents’ ability that OSA in children is radically distinct from the OSA to obtain sleep early in the night (Jenni et al., 2005). that occurs in adults, and such differences are particuDespite the physiological evidence, school start times larly striking in relation to racial and gender distriin the USA and many countries around the world are bution, clinical manifestations, and treatment (Carroll organized so that secondary school students have to and McLoughlin, 1992; Rosen et al., 1992). wake up earlier than primary school students, an In children, OSA is frequently diagnosed in associaarrangement that appears in paradox with the biological tion with adenotonsillar hypertrophy, and is common in preferences for later bed and wake-up times during the children with craniofacial abnormalities and neurologimore advanced stages of puberty (Carskadon et al., cal disorders affecting upper-airway patency during 1993; Jenni et al., 2005; Taylor et al., 2005). The resultsleep. In early reports, Guilleminault et al. (1976) suging sleepiness has been repeatedly shown to have an gested that removal of the enlarged adenotonsillar impact on students’ learning and behavior and, in distissue would lead to complete resolution of clinical tricts where school start times are delayed for secondary symptoms and cure of OSA. However, although students, improvements can be dramatic (for review, see enlarged tonsils and adenoids are by far the most Carskadon et al., 2004; Millman, 2005). An awareness important contributor to the pathophysiology of OSA of increased adolescent risk-taking behaviors in associain children, children with OSA also demonstrate the tion with inadequate sleep has also emerged (O’Brien presence of increased upper-airway collapsibility and Mindell, 2005). (Isono et al., 1998; Gozal and Burnside, 2004). Thus, In addition to these experimental and naturalistic adenotonsillar hypertrophy alone is usually not suffiexamples of the effects of sleep restriction on daytime cient to cause OSA; in fact, some children with sleepiness in children and adolescents, sleep disorders “kissing tonsils” will not have OSA, while others with may contribute to sleep that is inadequate, fragmented, relatively small adenotonsillar tissue will manifest or both and have been shown repeatedly to have an severe OSA and may not be cured after adenotonsiladverse impact on daytime functioning, while early lectomy (Suen et al., 1995; Lipton and Gozal, 2003). treatment often reverses these effects. Among these, Emphasis has also been placed on the importance of OSA is the most frequently diagnosed disorder and a multispecialty approach to clinical assessment and has received the greatest attention from research treatment of pediatric OSA, including sleep medicine laboratories around the world. specialists, maxillofacial and otolaryngologist surgeons, and orthodontists, particularly for patients with craniofacial abnormalities (Guilleminault and Abad, Sleep-disordered breathing 2004). Clearly, OSA is a complicated disorder, but addOSA is a more severe form of SDB, and is a relatively ing to this is the emerging perspective that OSA is only common disorder among both adults and children, with one end of a spectrum disorder. up to 3% of young children affected (Brouillette et al., The primary symptom of OSA is habitual snoring, 1984; Ali et al., 1993; Gislason and Benediktsdottir, a symptom that may affect up to 27% of children, with a 1995; Hulcrantz et al., 1995; Young et al., 2002; median revolving around 10–12% (Teculescu et al., 1992; Montgomery-Downs et al., 2003). OSA is characterAli et al., 1993; Gislason and Benediktsdottir, 1995; ized by repeated events of partial or complete upperHulcrantz et al., 1995; Owen et al., 1996; Ferreira et al., airway obstruction during sleep. These upper-airway 2000; O’Brien et al., 2003; Montgomery-Downs et al., changes induce disruption of normal alveolar ventila2003). Prevalence rates of snoring similar to those of tion and sleep structure, and lead to blood gas abnormpreschool and early school-age children have also been alities and sleep fragmentation (American Thoracic reported among infants. Indeed, habitual snoring was Society, 1995) (Figures 32.1 and 32.2). found in 5% of 2–4-month-olds (Kelmanson, 2000) and Despite the fact that OSA and its associated mani6–12-month-olds (Gislason and Benediktsdottir, 1995), festations were first described over 130 years ago with higher rates in infants aged 1–8 months (16–26%) (McKenzie, 1880; Osler, 1892), it was not until 1976 (Mitchell and Thompson, 2003). We have found habitual that Guilleminault et al. reported OSA as a clinically snoring in 1–9% of infants and toddlers (2–24 months of relevant entity in children. Furthermore, Osler (1892) age) (Montgomery-Downs and Gozal, 2006a). This relareported that children with “loud and snorting” respiratively high frequency of habitual snoring usually decreases tions with “prolonged pauses” were often “stupid in 9–14-year-olds to around 3–5% (Corbo et al., 2001).

492

H.E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS AND D. GOZAL

Fig. 32.1. Illustrative 1-minute example of a partial upper-airway obstruction (hypopnea), with repeated reductions (OH) of oronasal airflow in the presence of respiratory efforts, and with associated oxyhemoglobin desaturation, and sleep arousal (circles). These events occurred during rapid eye movement sleep (R). EEG, electroencephalogram; ECG, electrocardiogram; Chin EMG, chin electromyogram; Snore, sound channel; Flow, airflow; Abd, abdominal respiratory effort; SaO2, oxyhemoglobin saturation.

It needs to be stressed that the presence of snoring should not be viewed as a normal feature of sleeping children, because it indicates that increased upperairway resistance is present. A substantial percentage of snoring children may have primary snoring (i.e., habitual snoring without obvious visually recognizable disruptions in sleep architecture, alveolar ventilation, and oxygenation) but despite the traditional view of primary snoring as a benign condition, our laboratory has reported that primary snoring may in fact be associated with a higher risk for neurobehavioral deficits, albeit less severe than the deficits found in children with OSA (O’Brien et al., 2003). Of note, daytime sleepiness, behavioral hyperactivity, learning problems, and restless sleep are all significantly more common in habitual snorers (Ali et al., 1993; Blunden et al., 2000; Ferreira et al., 2000; Blunden et al., 2001; Chervin et al., 2002; O’Brien et al., 2003). Another report from our lab indicates that infants with higher snore-related arousal indices had lower

scores on a standardized mental development assessment. Because neither apneas nor hypopneas were present in these otherwise normal, healthy children, these findings constitute further evidence that snoring is not just an innocent noise during sleep in infants, but may in fact represent the lower end of the disease spectrum associated with SDB (Montgomery-Downs and Gozal, 2006b). We still do not know enough about the natural history of snoring and snore-induced arousals in infancy or early childhood to disregard any snoring. Zucconi and colleagues (1993) found that, among 18–24-month-olds evaluated for nightly snoring and referred for surgical treatment, the parents of >50% reported that the habitual snoring had developed during the child’s first year of life, and 16% reported an onset in the first month of life. The consequences of snoring and OSA with their associated hypoxemia and sleep fragmentation in children reveal complex pathophysiological mechanisms (Bass et al., 2004). If left untreated or treated late,

SLEEP-ASSOCIATED RESPIRATORY DISORDERS IN CHILDREN

493

Fig. 32.2. Overnight hypnogram of an 8-year-old child with mild to moderate obstructive sleep apnea illustrating the clustering of respiratory events during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep periods (circles). PLM, periodic limb movement; A/H, apnea/ hypopnea; SaO2, oxyhemoglobin saturation.

pediatric OSA may lead to significant morbidity affecting multiple target organs and systems, and such injurious consequences may, under certain circumstances, be partially irreversible despite appropriate therapy.

PSYCHOBEHAVIORAL CONSEQUENCES OF SLEEP DISRUPTION IN SDB Behavior Both habitual snoring and OSA are associated with behavioral problems, particularly hyperactivity and ADHD (Ali et al., 1993, 1996; Chervin and Archbold, 2001). Hyperactive and inattentive behaviors occur frequently in children with OSA (Guilleminault et al., 1981; Carroll and McLoughlin, 1992; Gozal, 1998; Blunden et al., 2000) and with habitual snoring (Weissbluth et al., 1983; Ali et al., 1993; Chervin et al., 1997, 2002; Blunden et al., 2000; Chervin and Archbold, 2001; O’Brien et al., 2003). Furthermore, approximately 30% of all children with frequent, loud

snoring or OSA manifest parentally reported hyperactivity and inattentive symptoms (Ali et al., 1993), with improvements noted following surgical treatment of SDB (Ali et al., 1996; Dagan-Friedman et al., 2002; Chervin et al., 2006b; Mitchell and Kelly, 2006). Interestingly, although children with ADHD appear to exhibit more sleep disturbances than normal children (Berry et al., 1986; Trommer et al., 1988), we found that, despite parental reports of sleep disturbances in >70% of children with ADHD, only 20% of these children had sleep disturbances when assessed by objective polysomnographic criteria (O’Brien et al., 2003). In this study, SDB was not more likely to occur among children with true ADHD (i.e., diagnoses following the stringent criteria recommended by the Academy of Pediatrics and the Academy of Psychiatry), yet SDB was significantly more prevalent among children with parent-reported hyperactive behaviors that do not fulfill strict ADHD criteria, suggesting that the subtle disruptions of sleep elicited by the presence of SDB may be associated with significant behavioral effects.

494 H.E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS AND D. GOZAL In contrast, Chervin and colleagues (2006b) found that via disruption of PFC-dependent processes (Beebe and children diagnosed with ADHD and reported by their Gozal, 2002) and this is also the foundation for a proparents to be hyperactive improved following adenoposed heuristic model for interpreting the prolific and tonsillectomy, but that severity of sleep measures dynamic research on both animal models and humans did not predict either baseline or improvement post(Beebe, 2005). The individual processes that make up surgery. As mentioned earlier, sleep fragmentation neurocognitive processes will be described separately associated with periodic limb jerks is more frequent below. in children with ADHD, thus supporting the notion that restless sleep is indeed more common in ADHD Attention patients. The ability to remain focused on a task and respond appropriately to extraneous stimuli in the environment Neurocognition plays an important role in learning, and consequently In adult patients, substantial cumulative evidence indiin social and academic development. Using acute sleep cates that neurocognitive deficits emanate from sleep restriction and sleep deprivation, Carskadon and coldisruption induced by SDB. These may include deficits leagues (1981a, b) have shown that, despite increased in attention, concentration, memory, and verbal and sleep propensity, as measured by the MSLT, no impairnonverbal intelligence (Greenberg et al., 1987; Bedard ments could be found on auditory attention, visual suset al., 1991; Naegele et al., 1995; Kim et al., 1997; tained attention, or inhibition (Fallone et al., 2001). Engleman and Joffe, 1999; Lee et al., 1999). Sleep depThese laboratory findings contradict parental and rivation exerts profound effects on cognitive function self-reports of impaired attention following acute sleep in adults, with complex tasks being more susceptible restriction. For example, children with early school to such deprivation when compared to more simple start times have reported more difficulty than their or automatic tasks (Harrison and Horne, 1998). Arnedt later-starting peers when asked to rate their level of and colleagues (2005) found that postcall performance attention and concentration abilities during school impairment during a heavy call rotation is comparable hours (Epstein et al., 1998). Sadeh and colleagues to impairment from blood alcohol concentration (2002) found no correlations between sleep schedules 0.04–0.05 g% (per 100 ml of blood) during a light call or duration and neurobehavioral functioning, and rotation, as measured by sustained attention, vigilance, Meijer and colleagues (2000) found no significant relaand simulated driving tasks. Further, adult patients tionships between subjective sleep variables such as with OSA exhibit a wide range of neurocognitive defitime in bed, quality of sleep, feeling rested, and difficits, particularly those underlying executive functionculty getting up in the morning, and performance on ing (i.e., the brain processes mediating the planning, a task of selective attention. Thus, the cumulative eviinitiation, and self-regulation of goal-oriented behadence would suggest that sleep deprivation rather than viors) (Lezak, 1995). While the literature is not as restriction is associated with more important effects on extensive in children, similar deficits in neurocognitive attention. function emerge as a result of sleep disruption. However, inattentive behavior has been reported in In the context of SDB, the magnitude and probabilchildren with OSA (Guilleminault et al., 1981, 1982) ity of neurocognitive dysfunction in children with OSA and with habitual snoring (Ali et al., 1993; Chervin are more profound than those associated with primary et al., 1997, 2002). Furthermore, a dose–response in snoring (Shiomi et al., 1993; Blunden et al., 2000; the scores obtained using attention-impulsivity scales O’Brien et al., 2003). Furthermore, hypoxemia is in the presence of OSA in children has been suggested closely correlated with deficits in executive function, (Owens-Stively et al., 1997). Blunden and colleagues whereas sleepiness is preferentially associated with (2000) also reported that children with mild SDB attention loss (Bedard et al., 1991; Naegele et al., demonstrated diminished selective and sustained atten1995). The frequently reported deficits in executive tion compared with control children. Studies from our performance in adults with SDB may emanate from laboratory further buttress the concept that children hypoxemia-induced frontal lobe dysfunction (Beebe with primary snoring (O’Brien et al., 2003) as well as and Gozal, 2002). Several groups of investigators have those with OSA (O’Brien et al., 2004a, b) are at higher posited that sleep disturbances are associated with dysrisk for deficits in attention compared to control chilfunction of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in adults, and dren when measured on parental report scales, and that the same principles should be applicable to children such deficits are substantially improved following ade(Dahl, 1996). We have introduced a theoretical model, notonsillectomy (Guilleminault et al., 1982; Ali et al., whereby sleep apnea induces daytime cognitive deficits 1996; Owens et al., 2000; Chervin et al., 2006b).

SLEEP-ASSOCIATED RESPIRATORY DISORDERS IN CHILDREN

Memory Following acute sleep restriction, no deficits are usually apparent in word memory tasks (Carskadon et al., 1981a), yet such deficits emerge following 38 hours of sleep deprivation in a sample of similar-aged children (Carskadon et al., 1981b). Performance of a verbal memory task was unaffected by acute sleep restriction (Randazzo et al., 1998a) and 3 nights of restricted sleep in children aged 10–14 years did not suggest the presence of any deficits on a workingmemory task (Randazzo et al., 1998b). However, in children with OSA, memory performance on standardized psychometric tests is significantly affected compared with control children (Rhodes et al., 1995; Blunden et al., 2000), with children with higher respiratory disturbance indices showing greater memory deficits (Rhodes et al., 1995). These findings are not consistently reported; neither Owens-Stively and colleagues (1997) nor O’Brien and colleagues (2004a, b) found any differences in memory performance in children with varying degrees of OSA severity when compared to control children. The limited number of studies in this area and the contradictory results emphasize the need for further investigation.

Intelligence Children’s general cognitive ability appears to be unaffected by acute sleep restriction but is impaired on abstract problem-solving tests and verbal fluency (Randazzo et al., 1998a, b). Acute sleep restriction (Carskadon et al., 1981a), sleep restriction for 3 nights (Randazzo et al., 1998b), and total sleep deprivation for 38 hours (Carskadon et al., 1981b) did not lead to significant decrements on computational accuracy, although computational speed declines after total sleep deprivation. Thus, cognitive functions that require verbal creativity and abstract thinking may be more sensitive to sleep restriction in children than their visual/ imagery counterparts. In children with OSA, several studies have documented significantly reduced IQ scores (obtained from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – WISC-III) compared with control children (Rhodes et al., 1995; Blunden et al., 2000; Beebe et al., 2004). In these studies, the probability for lower normal or borderline range performance was much higher in children with SDB. We have documented significantly impaired General Conceptual Ability scores (a measure of IQ obtained from the Differential Ability Scales: DAS) in school-age (O’Brien et al., 2004a) and preschoolage (Montgomery-Downs et al., 2005) children with OSA when compared with control children. However, Ali and colleagues (1996) failed to detect any

495

differences in the short-form version of the WISC-III. Interestingly, Lewin and colleagues (2002) found an inverse relationship between the severity of OSA and verbal ability (obtained from DAS). These investigators suggested that only severe OSA is a risk factor for disruption of verbal abilities. However, this issue merits further investigation because in our extensive study the majority of children had an apnea–hypopnea index between 5 and 10, i.e., not severe OSA, and despite relatively modest level of SDB severity, the verbal abilities and overall language scores were adversely affected (O’Brien et al., 2004a). In a study of preschoolers with OSA who were prospectively screened through parental questionnaires, we were further able to show complete reversibility of cognitive deficits following timely treatment (Montgomery-Downs et al., 2005). Thus, these studies suggest that early SDB diagnosis and intervention may lead to overall favorable outcomes (Friedman et al., 2003).

Learning and school performance School problems have been reported in multiple case series of children with OSA, and such findings may underscore more extensive behavioral disturbances such as restlessness, aggressive behavior, excessive daytime sleepiness, and poor test performances (Guilleminault et al., 1981; Weissbluth et al., 1983; Ali et al., 1993, 1996; Rhodes et al., 1995; Chervin et al., 1997; Owens et al., 1998; Chervin and Archbold, 2001). Improvements in behavior emerge following treatment for OSA in children (Stradling et al., 1990; Ali et al., 1996; Gozal, 1998), suggesting that at least some of the deficits may be reversible. Epidemiological surveys in which the total amount of sleep is assessed with questionnaires have indicated that children with later, irregular bedtimes, short sleep duration, and increased daytime sleepiness have lower academic achievements than other children (Kahn et al., 1989; Wolfson and Carskadon, 1998), although such findings have not been consistent (Eliasson et al., 2002). Lower school performance has also been described in children with OSA (Guilleminault et al., 1981, 1982, 1996; Stradling et al., 1990; Carroll and McLoughlin, 1992; Richards and Ferdman, 2000) and the reciprocal has also been shown to be true, i.e., children with poor academic performance are more likely to have sleep disturbances such as snoring and breathing difficulties (Weissbluth et al., 1983; Gozal, 1998). Indeed, we found a six- to ninefold increase in the expected incidence of OSA among first-grade children who ranked in the lowest 10th percentile of their class (Gozal, 1998), and significant improvements emerged in school grades after those children with OSA were

496

H.E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS AND D. GOZAL

effectively treated. Since the optimal intellectual ability and academic performance for these children were unknown, we cannot exclude the possibility that longterm residual deficits may be present after treatment. To examine this possibility further, we investigated the history of snoring during early childhood in two groups of 13–14-year-old children who were matched for age, gender, race, school attended, and socioeconomic status, but whose performance was either in the upper or lower quartile of their class, and found that children who snored frequently and loudly during early childhood were at greater risk for lower academic performance in later years, well after snoring had resolved (Gozal and Pope, 2001). These findings suggest that, even if the major portion of OSA-induced learning deficits is reversible, there may be long-lasting residual deficits in learning capability. The latter could represent either a “learning debt,” i.e., the decreased learning capacity during OSA may have led to such a delay in learned skills that recuperation is only possible with additional teaching assistance, or may suggest that OSA may have irreversibly altered the performance characteristics of the neuronal circuitry responsible for learning particular skills. In summary, sleep disturbance in children, whether due to poor sleep habits, developmental changes, or SDB, is accompanied by marked and obvious behavioral and neurocognitive deficits. Both sleep fragmentation and intermittent hypoxia contribute to elicit neurobehavioral morbidity in pediatric SDB. While the long-term outcome for children with untreated SDB is currently unknown, reversibility of neurobehavioral morbidities following treatment has been reported. These findings have been extended to those children with snoring, now considered the end of the SDB spectrum disorder. Increased awareness by physicians and parents and early identification and treatment of conditions leading to altered sleep and nocturnal oxygenation should lead to improved psychobehavioral outcomes in pediatric sleep-related respiratory disorders.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (HL65270 and HL69932), The Children’s Foundation Endowment for Sleep Research, and The Commonwealth of Kentucky Research Challenge Trust Fund.

REFERENCES Ali NJ, Pitson DJ, Stradling JR (1993). Snoring, sleep disturbance, and behaviour in 4–5 year olds. Arch Dis Child 68: 360–366.

Ali NJ, Pitson DJ, Stradling JR (1994). Natural history of snoring and related behaviour problems between the ages of 4 and 7 years. Arch Dis Child 71: 74–76. Ali NJ, Pitson D, Stradling JR (1996). Sleep disordered breathing: effects of adenotonsillectomy on behaviour and psychological functioning. Eur J Pediatr 155: 56–62. American Thoracic Society (1995). Standards and indications for cardiopulmonary sleep studies in children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 153: 866–878. Amin RS, Kimball TR, Bean JA et al. (2002). Left ventricular hypertrophy and abnormal ventricular geometry in children and adolescents with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 165: 1395–1399. Amin RS, Kimball TR, Kalra M et al. (2005). Left ventricular function in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Am J Cardiol 95: 801–804. Archbold KH, Pituch KJ, Panahi P et al. (2002). Symptoms of sleep disturbances among children at two general pediatrics clinics. J Pediatr 140 (1): 97–102. Arnedt JT, Owens J, Crouch M et al. (2005). Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion. JAMA 294 (9): 1025–1033. Aronen ET, Paavonen EJ, Fjallberg M et al. (2000). Sleep and psychiatric symptoms in school-age children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 39: 502–508. Bass JL, Corwin M, Gozal D et al. (2004). The effect of chronic or intermittent hypoxia on cognition in childhood: a systematic review of the literature. Pediatrics 114: 805–816. Bedard MA, Montplasir J, Richer F et al. (1991). Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: pathogenesis of neuropsychological deficits. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13: 950–964. Beebe DW (2005). Neurobehavioral effects of obstructive sleep apnea: an overview and heuristic model. Curr Opin Pulm Med 11 (6): 494–500. Beebe DW, Gozal D (2002). Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res 11: 1–16. Beebe DW, Wells CT, Jeffries J et al. (2004). Neuropsychological effects of pediatric obstructive sleep apnea. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 10: 962–975. Berry DTR, Webb WB, Block AJ et al. (1986). Nocturnal hypoxia and neuropsychological variables. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 8: 229–238. Blunden S, Lushington K, Kennedy D et al. (2000). Behavior and neurocognitive performance in children aged 5–10 years who snore compared to controls. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 22: 554–568. Blunden S, Lushington K, Kennedy D (2001). Cognitive and behavioral performance in children with sleep-related obstructive breathing disorders. Sleep Med Rev 5: 447–461. Brouillette R, Hanson D, David R et al. (1984). A diagnostic approach to suspected obstructive sleep apnea in children. J Pediatr 105: 10–14. Carroll JL, McLoughlin GM (1992). Diagnostic criteria for obstructive sleep apnea in children. Pediatr Pulmonol 14: 71–74.

SLEEP-ASSOCIATED RESPIRATORY DISORDERS IN CHILDREN Carskadon MA, Dement WC (1977). Sleep tendency: an objective measure of sleep loss. Sleep Res 6: 200. Carskadon MA, Dement WC (1987). Sleepiness in the normal adolescent. In: C Guilleminault (Ed.), Sleep and Its Disorders in Children. Raven Press, New York, pp. 53–66. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Duke P et al. (1980). Pubertal changes in daytime sleepiness. Sleep 2: 453–460. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC (1981a). Sleep loss in young adolescents. Sleep 4: 299–312. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Dement WC (1981b). Acute restriction of nocturnal sleep in children. Percept Mot Skills 53: 103–112. Carskadon MA, Viera C, Acebo C (1993). Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep 16: 258–262. Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Jenni OG (2004). Regulation of adolescent sleep: implications for behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1021: 276–291. Chervin RD, Archbold KH (2001). Hyperactivity and polysomnographic findings in children evaluated for sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep 24: 313–320. Chervin R, Dillon J, Bassetti C et al. (1997). Symptoms of sleep disorders, inattention, and hyperactivity in children. Sleep 20: 1185–1192. Chervin RD, Hedger K, Dillon JE et al. (2000). Pediatric sleep questionnaire (PSQ): validity and reliability of scales for sleep-disordered breathing, snoring, sleepiness, and behavioral problems. Sleep Med 1: 21–32. Chervin RD, Archbold KH, Dillon JE et al. (2002). Inattention, hyperactivity, and symptoms of sleep disordered breathing. Pediatrics 109: 449–456. Chervin RD, Weatherly RA, Ruzicka DL et al. (2006a). Subjective sleepiness and polysomnographic correlates in children scheduled for adenotonsillectomy vs other surgical care. Sleep 29: 495–503. Chervin RD, Ruzicka DL, Giordani BJ et al. (2006b). Sleepdisordered breathing, behavior, and cognition in children before and after adenotonsillectomy. Pediatrics 117 (4): e769–e778. Chugh DK, Weaver TE, Dinges DF (1996). Neurobehavioral consequences of arousals. Sleep 19: S198–S201. Corbo GM, Forastiere F, Agabiti N et al. (2001). Snoring in 9- to 15-year-old children: risk factors and clinical relevance. Pediatrics 180: 1149–1154. Crabtree VM, Ivanenko A, O’Brien LM et al. (2003). Periodic limb movement disorder of sleep in children. J Sleep Res 12: 73–81. Crabtree V, Varni JW, Gozal D (2004). Quality of life and depressive symptoms in children with suspected sleepdisordered breathing. Sleep 27: 1131–1138. Dagan-Friedman B, Hendeles-Amitay A, Kozminzki E et al. (2002). Impaired cognitive performance in children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 165: A263. Dahl RE (1996). The impact of inadequate sleep on children’s daytime and cognitive function. Semin Pediatr Neurol 3: 44–50.

497

Eliasson A, Eliasson A, King J et al. (2002). Association of sleep and academic performance. Sleep Breath 6: 45–48. Engleman H, Joffe D (1999). Neuropsychological function in obstructive sleep apnoea. Sleep Med Rev 3: 59–78. Epstein R, Chillag N, Lavie P (1998). Starting times of school: effects on daytime functioning of fifth-grade children in Israel. Sleep 21: 250–256. Everett AD, Koch WC, Saulsbury FT (1987). Failure to thrive due to obstructive sleep apnea. Clin Pediatr 26: 90–92. Fallone G, Seifer R, Acebo C et al. (2000). Prolonged sleep restriction in 11- and 12-year-old children: effects on behavior, sleepiness, and mood. Sleep 23 (Suppl. 2): A28. Fallone G, Acebo C, Arnedt TA et al. (2001). Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Percept Mot Skills 93: 213–229. Fallone G, Acebo C, Seifer R et al. (2005). Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: effects on teacher ratings. Sleep 28 (12): 1561–1567. Ferreira AM, Clemente V, Gozal D et al. (2000). Snoring in Portuguese primary school children. Pediatrics 106 (5). Friedman BC, Hendeles-Amitai A, Kozminsky E et al. (2003). Adenotonsillectomy improves neurocognitive function in children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep 26: 999–1005. Gislason T, Benediktsdottir B (1995). Snoring, apneic episodes, and nocturnal hypoxemia among children 6 months to 6-years-old. Chest 107: 963–966. Golan N, Shahar E, Ravid S et al. (2004). Sleep disorders and daytime sleepiness in children with attention-deficit/ hyperactive disorder. Sleep 15; 27 (2): 261–266. Gozal D (1998). Sleep-disordered breathing and school performance in children. Pediatrics 102: 616–620. Gozal D, Burnside MM (2004). Increased upper airway collapsibility in awake children with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Resp Crit Care Med 169: 163–167. Gozal D, Pope DW (2001). Snoring during early childhood and academic performance at age thirteen to fourteen years. Pediatrics 107: 1394–1399. Greenberg GD, Watson RK, Deptula D (1987). Neuropsychological dysfunction in sleep apnea. Sleep 10: 254–262. Guilleminault C, Abad VC (2004). Obstructive sleep apnea. Curr Treat Options Neurol 6 (4): 309–317. Guilleminault C, Eldridge F, Simmons FB et al. (1976). Sleep apnea in eight children. Pediatrics 58: 28–31. Guilleminault C, Korobkin R, Winkle R (1981). A review of 50 children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Lung 159: 275–287. Guilleminault C, Winkle R, Korobkin R et al. (1982). Children and nocturnal snoring – evaluation of the effects of sleep related respiratory resistive load and daytime functioning. Eur J Pediatr 139: 165–171. Guilleminault C, Pelayo R, Ledger D et al. (1996). Recognition of sleep disordered breathing in children. Pediatrics 98: 871–882. Harrison Y, Horne JA (1998). Sleep loss impairs short and novel language tasks having a prefrontal focus. J Sleep Res 7: 95–100.

498

H.E. MONTGOMERY-DOWNS AND D. GOZAL

Hulcrantz E, Lofstarnd TB, Ahlquist RJ (1995). The epidemiology of sleep related breathing disorders in children. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 32: S63–S66. Isono S, Shimada A, Utsugi M et al. (1998). Comparison of static mechanical properties of the passive pharynx between normal children and children with sleepdisordered breathing. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 157: 1201–1212. Jenni OG, Achermann P, Carskadon MA (2005). Homeostatic sleep regulation in adolescents. Sleep 28: 1446–1454. Kahn A, Van de Merckt C, Rebuffat E et al. (1989). Sleep problems in healthy preadolescents. Pediatrics 84: 542–546. Kelmanson IA (2000). Snoring, noisy breathing in sleep and daytime behaviour in 2–4-month-old infants. Eur J Pediatr 159: 734–739. Kim HC, Young T, Matthews CG et al. (1997). Sleep disordered breathing and neuropsychological deficits. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 156: 1813–1819. Lavigne JV, Arend R, Rosenbaum D et al. (1999). Sleep and behaviour problems among preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr 20: 164–169. Lee MM, Strauss ME, Adams N et al. (1999). Executive functions in persons with sleep apnea. Sleep Breath 3: 13–16. Lewin DS, Rosen RC, England SJ et al. (2002). Preliminary evidence of behavioral and cognitive sequelae of obstructive sleep apnea in children. Sleep Med 3: 5–13. Lezak MD (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd edn. Oxford University Press, New York. Lipton AJ, Gozal D (2003). Treatment of obstructive sleep apnea in children: do we really know how? Sleep Med Rev 7: 61–80. McKenzie M (1880). A Manual of Diseases of the Throat and Nose, Including the Pharynx, Larynx, Trachea Oesophagus, Nasal Cavities, and Neck. Churchill, London. Marcus CL, Greene MG, Carroll JL (1998). Blood pressure in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 157: 1098–1103. Meijer AM, Habekothe HT, Van Den Wittenboer GL (2000). Time in bed, quality of sleep and school functioning of children. J Sleep Res 9: 145–153. Millman RP (2005). Working Group on Sleepiness in Adolescents/Young Adults; AAP Committee on Adolescence. Excessive sleepiness in adolescents and young adults: causes, consequences, and treatment strategies. Pediatrics 115: 1774–1786. Minde K, Popiel K, Leos N et al. (1993). The evaluation and treatment of sleep disorders in young children. J Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34: 521–533. Minde K, Faucon A, Falkner S (1994). Sleep problems in toddlers: effects of treatment on their daytime behavior. J Child Adolescent Psychiatry 33: 1114–1121. Mitchell RB, Kelly J (2005). Quality of life after adenotonsillectomy for SDB in children. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 133 (4): 569–572. Mitchell RB, Kelly J (2006). Long-term changes in behavior after adenotonsillectomy for obstructive sleep apnea

syndrome in children. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 134 (3): 374–378. Mitchell EA, Thompson JMD (2003). Snoring in the first year of life. Acta Paediatr 92: 425–429. Montgomery-Downs HE, Gozal D (2006a). Snore-associated sleep fragmentation in infancy: mental development effects and contribution of secondhand cigarette smoke exposure. Pediatrics 117 (3): e496–e502. Montgomery-Downs HE, Gozal D (2006b). Sleep habits and risk factors for sleep-disordered breathing in infants and young toddlers in Louisville, Kentucky. Sleep Med 7 (3): 211–219. Montgomery-Downs HE, Jones VF, Molfese V et al. (2003). Snoring in preschoolers: potential associations with sleepiness, ethnicity, and learning. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 42 (8): 719–726. Montgomery-Downs HE, Crabtree VM, Gozal D (2005). Cognition sleep and respiration in at-risk children treated for obstructive sleep apnea. Eur Resp J 25: 336–342. Naegele B, Thouvard V, Pepin JL et al. (1995). Deficits of cognitive executive functions in patients with sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep 18: 43–52. O’Brien LM, Gozal D (2005). Autonomic dysfunction in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep 28: 747–752. O’Brien EM, Mindell JA (2005). Sleep and risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Behav Sleep Med 3 (3): 113–133. O’Brien LM, Holbrook CR, Mervis CB et al. (2003). Sleep and neurobehavioral characteristics in 5–7 year old hyperactive children. Pediatrics 111: 554–563. O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR et al. (2004a). Neurobehavioral implications of habitual snoring in children. Pediatrics 114: 44–49. O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR et al. (2004b). Neurobehavioral correlates of sleep disordered breathing in children. J Sleep Res 13: 165–172. Osler W (1892). The Principles and Practice of Medicine. Appleton, New York, pp. 335–339. Owen GO, Canter RJ, Robinson A (1996). Snoring, apnea and ENT symptoms in the paediatric community. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 21: 130–134. Owens J, Opipari L, Nobile C et al. (1998). Sleep and daytime behavior in children with obstructive sleep apnea and behavioral sleep disorders. Pediatrics 102: 1178–1184. Owens JA, Spiritio A, Marcotte A et al. (2000). Neuropsychological and behavioral correlates of obstructive sleep apnea in children: a preliminary study. Sleep Breath 2: 67–78. Owens-Stively J, McGuinn M, Berkelhammer L et al. (1997). Neuropsychological and behavioral correlates of obstructive sleep apnea in children. Sleep Res 26 (Suppl): 452. Randazzo AC, Schweitzer PK, Walsh JK (1998a). Cognitive function following 3 nights of sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep 21: 249. Randazzo AC, Muehlbach MJ, Schweitzer PK et al. (1998b). Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10–14. Sleep 21: 861–868.

SLEEP-ASSOCIATED RESPIRATORY DISORDERS IN CHILDREN Reuveni H, Simon T, Tal A et al. (2002). Healthcare services utilization in children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Pediatrics 110: 68–72. Rhodes SK, Shimoda KC, Wald LR et al. (1995). Neurocognitive deficits in morbidly obese children with obstructive sleep apnea. J Pediatr 127: 741–744. Richards W, Ferdman RM (2000). Prolonged morbidity due to delays in the diagnosis and treatment of obstructive sleep apnea in children. Clin Pediatr 39: 103–108. Rona JR, Li L, Gulliford MC et al. (1998). Disturbed sleep: effects of sociocultural factors and illness. Arch Dis Child 78: 20–25. Rosen CL, D’Andrea L, Haddad GG (1992). Adult criteria for obstructive sleep apnea do not identify children with serious obstruction. Am Rev Resp Dis 146: 1231–1234. Rosen CL, Palermo TM, Larkin EK et al. (2002). Healthrelated quality of life and sleep-disordered breathing in children. Sleep 25: 648–657. Roth T, Roehrs TA (1996). Etiologies and sequelae of excessive daytime sleepiness. Clin Ther 18 (4): 562–576. Sadeh A, Raviv A, Gruber R (2000). Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in school age children. Dev Psychol 36: 291–301. Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A (2002). Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev 73: 405–417. Shiomi T, Guilleminault C, Stoohs R et al. (1993). Obstructed breathing in children during sleep monitored by echocardiography. Acta Paediatr 82: 863–871. Smedje H, Broman JE, Hetta J (2001). Associations between disturbed sleep and behavioural difficulties in 635 children aged six to eight years: a study based on parents’ perceptions. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 10: 1–9. Stein MA, Mendelsohn J, Obermeyer WH et al. (2001). Sleep and behavior problems in school-aged children. Pediatrics 107: e60. Stepansky EJ, Lamphere P, Badia P (1984). Sleep fragmentation and daytime sleepiness. Sleep 7: 18–26. Stepansky EJ, Lamphere J, Roehrs T (1987). Experimental sleep fragmentation in normal subjects. Int J Neurosci 33: 207–214.

499

Stewart MG, Glaze DG, Friedman EM et al. (2005). Quality of life and sleep study findings after adenotonsillectomy in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 131 (4): 308–314. Stores G (1996). Practitioner review: assessment and treatment of sleep disorders in children and adolescents. J Child Adolesc Psychol Psychiatry 37: 907–925. Stradling JR, Thomas G, Warley ARH et al. (1990). Effect of adenotonsillectomy on nocturnal hypoxaemia, sleep disturbance, and symptoms in snoring children. Lancet 335: 249–253. Suen JS, Arnold JE, Brooks LJ (1995). Adenotonsillectomy for treatment of obstructive sleep apnea in children. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 121: 525–530. Tarasiuk A, Simon T, Tal A et al. (2004). Adenotonsillectomy in children with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome reduces health care utilization. Pediatrics 113: 351–356. Taylor DJ, Jenni OG, Acebo C et al. (2005). Sleep tendency during extended wakefulness: insights into adolescent sleep regulation and behavior. J Sleep Res 14 (3): 239–244. Teculescu DB, Caillier I, Perrin P et al. (1992). Snoring in French preschool children. Pediatr Pulmonol 13: 239–244. Trommer BL, Hoeppner JB, Rosenberg RS et al. (1988). Sleep disturbance in children with attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol 24: 322. Weissbluth M, Davis A, Poncher J et al. (1983). Signs of airway obstruction during sleep and behavioral, developmental and academic problems. Dev Behav Pediatr 4: 119–121. Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA (1998). Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Dev 69: 875–887. Young T, Peppard PE, Gottlieb DJ (2002). Epidemiology of obstructive sleep apnea: a population health perspective. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 165: 1217–1239. Zucconi M, Ferini Strambi L, Pestalozza G et al. (1993). Habitual snoring and obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in children: effects of early tonsil survery. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 26: 235–243. Zuckerman B, Stevenson J, Bailey V (1987). Sleep problems in early childhood: continuities, predictive factors, and behavioral correlates. Pediatrics 80: 664–671.