Sleep spindles: a physiological marker of age-related changes in gray matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation

Sleep spindles: a physiological marker of age-related changes in gray matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation

Accepted Manuscript Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consol...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 15 Views

Accepted Manuscript Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation S. Fogel, C. Vien, A. Karni, H. Benali, J. Carrier, J. Doyon PII:

S0197-4580(16)30249-4

DOI:

10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.009

Reference:

NBA 9748

To appear in:

Neurobiology of Aging

Received Date: 16 March 2016 Revised Date:

8 September 2016

Accepted Date: 3 October 2016

Please cite this article as: Fogel, S, Vien, C., Karni, A., Benali, H., Carrier, J, Doyon, J, Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation, Neurobiology of Aging (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.009. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1

Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain

2

regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation

3

RI PT

Fogel S,a,b,c Vien, C.,a,b Karni, A.,d Benali, H.,a,e Carrier Ja,b,f & Doyon Ja,b a

SC

Functional Neuroimaging Unit, Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Montreal, Canada; bDepartment of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada; cSchool of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada; dLaboratory for Human Brain & Learning, Sagol Department of Neurobiology & the E.J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel; eFunctional Neuroimaging Laboratory, INSERM, France; fCentre d’études Avancées en Médecine du Sommeil, Hôpital du Sacré-Cœur de Montréal, Montreal, Canada.

Email: [email protected] Phone: 1-514-340-2800 x 3284 Fax: 1-514-340-3530

M AN U

Corresponding Author: Julien Doyon, Ph.D. Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Montreal Director, Functional Neuroimaging Unit Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal 4545 Chemin Queen Mary Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H3W 1W5

TE D

4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26

EP

27

AC C

28

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ABSTRACT

30

Sleep is necessary for the optimal consolidation of procedural learning, and in particular, for

31

motor sequential skills. Motor sequence learning (MSL) remains intact with age, but sleep-

32

dependent consolidation is impaired, suggesting that memory deficits for procedural skills are

33

specifically impacted by age-related changes in sleep. Age-related changes in spindles may

34

be responsible for impaired MSL consolidation, but the morphological basis for this deficit is

35

unknown. Here we found that grey matter in the hippocampus and cerebellum was positively

36

correlated with both sleep spindles and offline improvements in performance in young

37

participants, but not in older participants. These results suggest that age-related changes in

38

grey matter in the hippocampus relate to spindles, and may underlie age-related deficits in

39

sleep-related motor sequence memory consolidation. In this way, spindles can serve as a

40

biological marker for structural brain changes and the related memory deficits in older adults.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

29

41 KEYWORDS

43

Sleep, spindle, age, memory, learning, consolidation, motor sequence learning, motor skills,

44

procedural memory, hippocampus, cerebellum

EP

45

TE D

42

AC C

46

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 47

1. INTRODUCTION Alongside a healthy diet and regular exercise, sleep is one of the pillars of good

49

physical and mental health, and has important links to memory and cognition (e.g., see

50

(Rasch and Born, 2013), for a recent comprehensive review). However, as we age, the

51

quantity and quality of sleep are markedly reduced (Bixler et al., 1984; Buysse et al., 1992;

52

Carrier et al., 2011, 2001; Crowley et al., 2002; Darchia et al., 2003; Feinberg et al., 1967;

53

Floyd et al., 2007; Fogel et al., 2012; Gaudreau et al., 2001; Landolt and Borbely, 2001;

54

Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Peters et al., 2014; Redline et al., 2004; Zepelin and

55

McDonald, 1987), and recent studies suggest that the age-related changes in sleep may have

56

an impact on sleep-dependent memory processes (Fogel et al., 2014; Mander et al., 2015,

57

2013a, 2013b; Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011; Spencer et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2012). In

58

healthy young adults, non-rapid eye movement stage 2 (N2) sleep typically occupies about

59

45% to 55% of total sleep time (Carskadon and Dement, 2011). However, with age, the total

60

amount of N2 is reduced (Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Ramanand et al., 2010).

61

One of the main characteristic features of N2 sleep is the presence of sleep spindle (Iber et

62

al., 2007). Sleep spindles are brief bursts (<1 sec to ~3 sec) of oscillatory activity within the

63

sigma band, with a frequency of ~12-16 Hz and with a waxing and waning amplitude that is

64

discrete from the ongoing EEG (Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2007; Landolt et al., 1996;

65

Silber et al., 2007; Zeitlhofer et al., 1997). One of the most pronounced age-related changes

66

in N2 sleep is the reduction of sleep spindles marked by reduced duration, amplitude and

67

density (Bowersox et al., 1985; Cajochen et al., 2006; Carrier et al., 2001; Crowley et al.,

68

2002; Feinberg, 1974; Guazzelli et al., 1986; Huupponen et al., 2002; Knoblauch et al., 2005;

69

Landolt et al., 1996; Mander et al., 2013a; Martin et al., 2012; Nicolas et al., 2001; Peters et

70

al., 2014; Principe and Smith, 1982). However, the age-related changes in brain structure that

71

underlie age-related reduction in spindles, and their functional consequences, remain to be

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

48

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 72

fully elucidated. Animal studies of the neural circuitry that supports the generation of sleep spindles

74

have shown that spindles originate from rhythmic depolarization of thalamocortical neurons

75

(Steriade, 2006), modulated by GABAergic pathways in the reticular nucleus of the thalamus

76

(Bazhenov et al., 2000, 1999). However, the brain structures involved in the generation and

77

regulation of spindles remains a topic of debate (Timofeev and Chauvette, 2013), as recent

78

evidence suggests that spindles are initiated cortically and that the neocortex contributes to

79

the termination of spindle events (Bonjean et al., 2011; Timofeev, 2001). In addition to the

80

thalamus, human functional neuroimaging studies employing simultaneous EEG-fMRI, have

81

revealed that brain regions involved in sensory processing are activated time-locked to sleep

82

spindles, including the anterior cingulate, insula and superior temporal gyri (Schabus et al.,

83

2007). Moreover, fast spindles have been associated with increased activity in sensorimotor

84

areas, the mesial frontal cortex and the hippocampus (Schabus et al., 2007) – the latter

85

findings being consistent with the putative role of fast spindles in declarative memory

86

processing. To further investigate the structural correlates of sleep spindles, Saletin et al

87

(Saletin et al., 2013) found that inter-individual differences in grey matter volume of the insula

88

and auditory cortex were related to slow spindles, whereas, grey matter volume in the

89

hippocampus was associated with fast spindles. Thus, the latter findings suggest that slow

90

and fast spindles serve a dissociable role for sleep maintenance and possibly memory

91

processing, respectively.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

73

92

Indeed, spindles are thought to serve a protective function by preventing potentially

93

arousing external stimuli such as loud noises that disrupt sleep (Cote et al., 2000; Dang-Vu et

94

al., 2010; Pivik et al., 1999; Schabus et al., 2012), although see (Crowley et al., 2004) for data

95

to suggest otherwise. In addition to their function for sleep maintenance via the gating of

96

intrusive sensory information, however, a rich literature now exists supporting the role of 4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT spindles for procedural memory including motor skills, reasoning and rule-learning (Albouy et

98

al., 2013a; Barakat et al., 2012, 2011; Fogel and Smith, 2011, 2006; Fogel et al., 2015, 2014;

99

Lafortune et al., 2014; Milner et al., 2006; Nielsen et al., 2014; Nishida and Walker, 2007;

100

Peters et al., 2007; Ramanathan et al., 2015; Smith and Smith, 2003; Tamaki et al., 2009,

101

2008; Wamsley et al., 2013, 2012; Wilhelm et al., 2012). Sleep is thought to enhance the

102

memory consolidation process via the interplay between the hippocampus and striatum

103

(Albouy et al., 2015, 2013a, 2013b, 2013c, 2008; Schabus et al., 2007) by strengthening

104

newly formed, labile memory traces for lasting long-term neocortical storage. Animal studies

105

have shown that during the spindle event, there is a large influx of intracellular calcium, which

106

is necessary to set in motion long-term potentiation (LTP) of synapses. In vivo (Bergmann et

107

al., 2008; Rosanova and Ulrich, 2005; Steriade, 2005; Werk et al., 2005) and in vitro

108

(Rosanova and Ulrich, 2005) studies have also shown that spaced and repeated stimulation

109

in the spindle oscillatory frequency range is necessary for cortical LTP to take place in the

110

neocortex. Thus, spindles are an ideal physiological candidate for neocortical memory

111

consolidation via memory trace reactivation.

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

97

Recent human neuroimaging studies have shown that sleep spindle amplitude was

113

correlated with overnight consolidation in motor sequence skill (Barakat et al., 2012, 2011)

114

and that overnight changes in activation of brain regions including the striatum and motor

115

cortical regions, recruited during motor sequence learning (MSL) (Debas et al., 2010) were

116

correlated with post-training spindle amplitude (Barakat et al., 2012). EEG-fMRI studies have

117

also identified activations in the striatum (Caporro et al., 2011; Tyvaert et al., 2008) and

118

hippocampus (Schabus et al., 2007) associated with spindle events, hence suggesting that

119

spindles are involved in the activation of structures important for sleep-dependent motor skill

120

memory consolidation. Taken together, this literature suggests that spindles play an active

121

role in sleep-dependent memory consolidation via related interaction between striatal,

AC C

EP

112

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 122

hippocampal and cortical memory systems, and may ultimately result in strengthening of

123

memory traces. The functional consequences of age-related changes in sleep on memory

125

consolidation are beginning to be better appreciated. Age-related changes in sleep have been

126

shown to have a negative impact on overnight memory consolidation of some procedural

127

skills (Brown et al., 2009; Spencer et al., 2007), and spindles, in particular, have been related

128

to this deficit (Fogel et al., 2014; Peters et al., 2008). Age-related characteristics of spindles,

129

including duration, amplitude, and density, decrease with age (Carrier et al., 2001; Feinberg,

130

1974; Martin et al., 2012; Peters et al., 2014). Taken together, these studies suggest that an

131

age-related reduction in spindle activity may help explain age-related, sleep-dependent

132

deficits in memory consolidation. In support of this notion, a recent functional neuroimaging

133

study by our group has shown that reduced spindles in the elderly are associated with a

134

reduction of the spindle-related, overnight changes in cerebral activity in structures important

135

for skill learning (e.g., putamen and cerebellum in young, but only the cerebellum in older

136

individuals) (Fogel et al., 2014). Thus, a spindle-related, sleep-dependent increase of activity

137

in structures such as the putamen and cerebellum appears to be crucial for off-line

138

improvements, for which older individuals do not derive the same benefit due to an age-

139

related reduction of sleep spindles. However, it is not known if changes in brain structure that

140

accompany normal, healthy aging may underlie this deficit, and whether this is related to an

141

age-related reduction of sleep spindles.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

124

142

In terms of brain structures, adults who learn to play a musical instrument from a young

143

age have been found to have increased grey matter in brain regions that support motor skill

144

learning (e.g., hippocampus, putamen motor cortex, somatosensory cortex, superior parietal

145

cortex and the cerebellum)(Gaser and Schlaug, 2003; Groussard et al., 2014; Vaquero et al.,

146

2016). However, changes in grey matter have been observed over much shorter time periods. 6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Motor sequence learning is associated with grey matter changes in the cerebellum, motor

148

cortex, premotor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Gryga et al., 2012), and the earliest

149

detectable signs of grey matter changes with motor skill training (over 3 days) have been

150

observed in the striatum (Hamzei et al., 2012). These structural changes in the striatum were

151

related to grey matter changes in motor cortical regions at later stages of motor skill

152

performance, with an increase in functional coupling between motor and striatal regions over

153

the same time period. However, it is not known if age-related changes in brain structures

154

relate to age-related changes in sleep and the resulting memory deficits. Here, we

155

investigated the age-related differences in grey matter (GM) assessed by voxel-based

156

morphometry that were correlated with both sleep spindles and sleep-dependent changes in

157

motor skills performance.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

147

We hypothesized that GM in structures that support MSL consolidation (e.g.,

159

cerebellum, striatum, hippocampus, motor cortical regions), would be correlated with both

160

offline changes in MSL performance and spindles in young participants. Yet in older

161

participants, the results of a recent functional neuroimaging study from our group suggests

162

that the correlation between brain activation during practice of a motor sequence task and

163

sleep spindles deteriorates with age (Fogel et al., 2014). Accordingly, if structural deficits

164

underlie (or are linked to) the functional deficits observed with age, then a similar reduction in

165

the magnitude of the relationship between spindles and grey matter should be expected.

166

Thus this study aimed to ascertain if structural brain changes in grey matter were associated

167

with sleep spindles and memory deficits in older adults. The latter findings are important as

168

they may lead to novel insights into the neurological basis for age-related memory deficits,

169

and highlight the importance of good sleep quality later in life, and perhaps even novel

170

treatments or interventions to improve sleep in elderly populations in order to derive a benefit

171

to memory and related mitigate cognitive impairment.

AC C

EP

TE D

158

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 172 173

2. METHODS

174

2.1. Participants All participants provided informed written consent before entering the study. Ethical

176

and scientific approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee at the “Institut de Geriatrie de

177

Montreal”, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. In total, 22 young participants and 34 older

178

participants were recruited to participate in the study that met the inclusion and exclusion

179

criteria (see section 2.1.1). The results presented here were part of a larger series of studies

180

investigating both the functional and structural basis of age-related changes of sleep on

181

memory consolidation (Fogel et al., 2014; Vien et al., n.d.).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

175

2.1.1. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria.

183

Eligibility was determined at the start of the experiment through both a telephone pre-

184

screening interview and a battery of questionnaires. To be included in the study, participants

185

had to be right-handed (scored > 40 on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield,

186

1971)), non-smokers, prescription medication-free, and with a normal body mass index (≤ 25).

187

Also, they must not have been diagnosed with a neurological, psychological or psychiatric

188

condition, nor with a sleep disorder. "Extreme morning" or "extreme evening" types were also

189

excluded (defined as scoring > 70 "moderate morning" or < 30 "moderate evening",

190

respectively on the Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Scale (Horne and Ostberg,

191

1976)). By contrast, participants who were not formally trained on a musical instrument, did

192

not work night-shifts and who had not taken a trans-meridian trip ≤ 3 months before the study

193

were included in the study. Participants scoring ≤ 8 on both the Beck Depression Scale (Beck

194

et al., 1974) and the Beck Anxiety Scale (Beck et al., 1988) were also included, as did older

195

participants who scored ≥ 24 on the Mini Mental State Examination (Cockrell and Folstein,

196

1988; Tombaugh and McIntyre, 1992). Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol,

AC C

EP

TE D

182

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT nicotine, caffeine, and daytime naps during the study. A regular sleep-wake cycle, defined by

198

a bed-time between 10:00 PM and 1:00 AM and wake-time between 07:00 AM and 10:00 AM,

199

was required to be included. Compliance with sleep-wake instructions was verified with

200

actigraphy measured from the left wrist, and with a sleep journal (using a modified version of

201

the National Sleep Foundation "Sleep Diary" http://sleepfoundation.org).

RI PT

197

2.1.2. Polysomnographic and Additional Screening.

203

If participants met the above criteria, a 90-min acclimatization nap was conducted at

204

1:00 PM, three days before the experimental sessions. To ensure that participants were able

205

to sleep in the laboratory environment during the daytime, participants had to achieve at least

206

5 minutes of consolidated NREM sleep during this nap opportunity to be included in the study.

207

An overnight polysomnographic (PSG) screening night was used in older participants to

208

exclude individuals who exhibited signs of sleep apnea (e.g., apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) >

209

5) and periodic limb movements (PLM; e.g., PLM index > 10). This acclimatization and

210

screening procedure provided participants extensive opportunity to become accustomed to

211

sleeping in the laboratory environment, and thus to minimize the impact on sleep quality

212

during the experimental testing sessions. As a result, 19 older participants were excluded in

213

total; 10 due to an elevated AHI or PLM index, 2 for sleeping < 5 min during the

214

acclimatization nap, and 2 for MRI safety concerns. Five voluntarily withdrew before

215

completing the study. In addition, 7 young participants were excluded; 6 voluntarily withdrew

216

and 1 subject was excluded due to a structural abnormality that was uncovered upon

217

examination of their structural T1-weighted scan. The final participant pool that completed the

218

study was comprised of 15 young (Y) participants (8 female) between 19 and 30 years of age

219

(M = 23.9, SD = 3.5) and 15 older (O) participants (9 female) between 55 and 69 years (M =

220

62.2, SD = 3.8). However, an additional 2 participants were excluded from data analyses in

221

the group of young participants. One participant reportedly suffered from severe sleepiness in

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

202

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT the last five blocks of training and succumbed to extreme tiredness during practice, thus

223

producing lapses in performance during the training session. A second subject performed

224

poorly on the MSL task (as this participant was outside the 99% confidence interval for all 14

225

blocks) and did not comply well with the instructions given throughout the experiment. The

226

remaining 13 young participants (6 female, M = 24.1, SD = 3.5 years old) were included in the

227

data analyses.

228

2.2. Overall Experimental Design

SC

RI PT

222

Participants arrived at the laboratory for 10:30 AM. The Stanford Sleepiness Scale

230

(SSS) (MacLean et al., 1992) and Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) (Dinges and Powell,

231

1985) were then administered to assess both subjective and objective sleepiness,

232

respectively, before MSL practice. At 11:00 AM, the MSL task was performed with alternating

233

periods of rest, in a block design, at the same time functional blood oxygen level dependent

234

(BOLD) imaging data were acquired (see (Fogel et al., 2014) for corresponding behavioural

235

and functional neuroimaging results in these participants). After being provided a light lunch,

236

participants were given a 90-min nap interval commencing at 1:00 PM. The MSL retest

237

session was administered at 4:00 PM, that is 1.5 h after the end of the 90-min experimental

238

interval to reduce possible effects of sleep inertia.

239

2.3. Finger Sequence Task

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

229

240

2.3.1. Task Instructions.

241

An adapted version of the finger sequence task developed by Karni (Karni et al., 1995)

242

was used to assess MSL in young and older participants across an interval of sleep (i.e., a

243

90-minute daytime nap opportunity). An MR-compatible response box (model HH-1x4-L;

244

Current Designs, Inc., Philadelphia PA) with a row of four equidistant push buttons was used.

245

To familiarize each subject with the task, the experimenter first demonstrated the use of the

246

keypad and explained the on-screen appearance of the task and instructions. Participants 10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT were then positioned supine in the MR scanner. Before the start of the training session, all

248

participants were asked to “slowly and accurately practice the 5-item sequence "4-1-3-2-4",

249

where 1 stands for the index finger and 4 for the little finger of the left hand” until they were

250

capable of performing the sequence three times in a row, without error. This ensured

251

comprehension of instructions, memorization of the sequence, and that the sequence could

252

be performed reliably prior to the start of the scanning session.

253

2.3.2. Training and Retest Practice Sessions.

254

During the training and retest sessions, the exact same sequence of finger movements

255

was to be performed, but participants were explicitly instructed "to perform the sequence by

256

pressing the buttons as quickly as possible, while making as few errors as possible". The

257

beginning of each practice block was indicated by the appearance of a green cross in the

258

center of the screen. After 60 key presses, the green cross turned red for 15 seconds,

259

indicating that the subject should stop practicing and rest. There were 14 practice blocks in

260

both training and retest sessions. If participants realized that they had made an error, they

261

were instructed to continue practicing at the start of the 5-item sequence. This ensured that

262

errors were isolated events within the train of 60 key presses and encouraged participants to

263

continue practicing the task despite making an error. To minimize age-related differences in

264

reaction time, the task was un-cued, (i.e., self-initiated). Performance was measured by key

265

press speed (defined as the average correct inter-key-press interval). All participants included

266

in the data analysis achieved an accuracy > 83.3%, equivalent to 10/12 correct sequences

267

per block. Offline changes in performance across the nap interval were calculated as the

268

mean performance of the first 4 blocks of retest minus the mean of the last 4 blocks of

269

training. This measure of consolidation was then used in the behavioral analyses, and as a

270

covariate of interest in the MRI analyses.

271

2.4. PSG Recording and Analysis

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

247

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT PSG data was recorded from 10 scalp derivations (F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4,

273

and Oz) using a Brain Products GmbH (Gilching, Germany) 16-channel, V-Amp16 system

274

(high pass filter = 0.3 Hz, low pass filter = 70 Hz). Signals were digitalized at 500 samples/s.

275

During the overnight polysomnographic screening in older participants, a nasal/oral thermistor

276

and respiratory effort belt recordings were used to screen for sleep apnea. Leg

277

electromyographic (EMG) recordings were used to screen participants for periodic leg

278

movements.

SC

RI PT

272

Sleep scoring (Table 1) was done visually in 20-s epochs of EEG (high pass filter = 0.5

280

Hz, low pass filter = 35 Hz) from central and occipital derivations (C3, C4, and Oz) re-

281

referenced to average mastoids (A1 and A2). Electrooculographic (EOG; high pass filter = 0.5

282

Hz, low pass filter = 35 Hz) activity was recorded from the lateral outer canthus of the eyes.

283

Bipolar submental EMG (high pass filter = 10 Hz) was also recorded. Sleep stages were

284

scored by an expert PSG technologist, following standards of the American Academy of

285

Sleep Medicine [Iber, 2007] using the "fMRI Artifact rejection and Sleep Scoring Toolbox

286

(FASST)" (ver. 0.302; http://www.montefiore.ulg.ac.be/~phillips/FASST.html) for Matlab

287

(Mathworks, Natick, MA; (Leclercq et al., 2011)). Movement continuously exceeding 100 µV

288

for > 100 ms during sleep was automatically detected and identified as bad data. This was

289

later confirmed visually and this data was excluded from further analyses.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

279

290

Sleep spindles were detected from Fz, Cz, and Pz in NREM sleep using a an

291

established (Albouy et al, 2014; Fogel et al, 2014) and validated method (Ray et al., 2015)

292

implemented in Brain Products (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) Analyzer software

293

(Version 2.1). For details of this method more recently implemented in Matlab, see Ray et al

294

(Ray et al., 2015). Given that previous studies have suggested that age-related differences

295

vary systematically along the midline (Martin et al., 2012), recordings from Fz, Cz, and Pz

296

were used to derive the number of spindles during NREM sleep amplitude (µV2), duration (s), 12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT and peak frequency (Hz). Slow and fast spindles have a known topography (De Gennaro and

298

Ferrara, 2003; De Gennaro et al., 2005, 2000; Dehghani et al., 2011; Doran, 2003;

299

Huupponen et al., 2002; Jobert et al., 1992; Werth et al., 1997a, 1997b; Zeitlhofer et al.,

300

1997) whereby slow spindles predominate anterior regions and fast spindles predominate

301

posterior regions. A frequency range for total bandwidth spindles of 11-17Hz was employed

302

so as to encompass spindles defined as 12-16Hz (Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2014,

303

2007; Landolt et al., 1996; Zeitlhofer et al., 1997). Slow (11-14 Hz) spindles, and fast (14-17

304

Hz) spindles were categorized depending on their peak frequency in the same way as a

305

previously published study on the relationship between age-related changes in sleep spindles

306

and functional brain activation (Fogel et al., 2014) and also based on a thorough validation of

307

this detection procedure (Ray et al., 2015). This procedure ensures that slow and fast

308

spindles are orthogonal to one another at any given derivation. Thus, spindles from the

309

derivations for which they predominate (e.g., slow spindles from Fz and fast spindles from Pz)

310

were included in the structural imaging regression analyses (Table 2).

311

2.5. Brain Imaging Data Acquisition and Analysis

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

297

2.5.1. MRI Sequence Parameters.

313

A 3.0T TIM TRIO MRI system (Siemens, Germany) with a 12-channel head coil was

314

used. A 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, TI = 900 ms, FA = 9°, 176

315

slices, FoV = 256 x 256 mm2, matrix size = 256 x 256 x 176, voxel size = 1 x 1 x 1 mm3) was

316

used to obtain high resolution T1-weighted anatomical images for all participants used for

317

subsequent VBM analyses.

AC C

EP

312

318

2.5.2. Preprocessing.

319

Structural

data was

analyzed

with

FSL

(Douaud

et

al.,

2007)

(but

see

320

http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLVBM for a detailed description of analysis procedures

321

employed here) using an optimized VBM protocol (Good et al., 2001a; Smith et al., 2004) 13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT described below. First, structural images were brain-extracted and grey matter-segmented

323

before being registered to the MNI 152 standard space using non-linear registration

324

(Andersson et al., 2007). Second, a study-specific grey matter (GM) template was created

325

with the FSL-VBM protocol. All brain-extracted images were segmented into GM, WM and

326

CSF. Then, GM images were affine-registered to the GM ICBM-152 template, concatenated

327

and averaged. Next, the GM images were then re-registered to this first-pass "affine" GM

328

template using non-linear registration, concatenated into a 4D image, averaged and flipped

329

along the x-axis. Both mirror images were averaged to create the final symmetric, study-

330

specific "non-linear" GM template based on an equal number of participants per group at

331

2x2x2mm3 resolution in standard space. Third, all native grey matter images were non-

332

linearly registered to the study-specific template generated in the previous step, and

333

modulated to correct for local expansion (or contraction) due to the non-linear component of

334

the spatial transformation (see Good et al., 2001). The modulated grey matter images were

335

then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 3 mm.

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

322

2.5.3. Statistical Analysis.

337

To investigate age-related differences in GM, voxelwise general linear modeling (GLM)

338

was applied using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) permutation-based non-

339

parametric testing using 10,000 permutations (Nichols and Holmes, 2002; Winkler et al.,

340

2014), for: 1) young, 2) older, and 3) young – older participants. This procedure generated

341

maps of t-statistics and maps of corresponding uncorrected p-values and Family-Wise Error

342

(FWE) corrected p-value maps.

AC C

EP

336

343

Total intracranial volume (TIV) was means-centered and included as a variable of non-

344

interest in the models. TIV was obtained using FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012) by linearly

345

aligning each subject brain extracted image to the MNI152 space, computing the inverse of

346

the determinant of the affine matrix ((Sargolzaei et al., 2015); see ENIGMA protocol: 14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT http://enigma.ini.usc.edu) and taking the sum of the gray matter and white matter partitions

348

extracted using an automated brain extraction tool (BET) (Smith, 2002) and segmentation

349

using FAST (Zhang et al., 2001). Offline changes in MSL performance and each spindle

350

parameter (density, amplitude, duration) were entered as mean-centered covariates of

351

interest into separate GLMs to investigate brain regions where GM correlated to a different

352

extent in young and older subjects with offline changes in MSL performance and sleep

353

spindles.

SC

RI PT

347

In addition, to co-localize differences in grey matter in young and older participants

355

which correlated with both offline changes in performance and sleep spindle characteristics,

356

the conjunction of performance-related GM t-maps and spindle-related GM t-maps was taken

357

as the minimum t-statistic using the global null hypothesis (Friston et al., 2005). The resulting

358

conjunction maps identify brain regions where GM was correlated to a different extent in

359

young and older (and young vs. older) participants with both offline changes in performance

360

and spindles, over: 1) the performance-correlated statistical parametric maps (SPM) in young

361

and older participants with 2) each spindle-correlated map (e.g., density, amplitude, duration)

362

in young and older participants (e.g., young, older and young vs. older). The conjunction

363

maps were thresholded so that results were significant at a combined p-level of p < 0.001

364

(uncorrected). Clusters that were significant at p < 0.05 (FWE corrected) were also indicated

365

in the results. Note that a significant conjunction does not necessarily mean all the contrasts

366

were individually significant (i.e., a conjunction of significance). Instead it indicates that the

367

effects were consistently high across all conditions included in the conjunction, such that they

368

were jointly statistically significant (Friston et al., 2005). Note that the minimum t-values do not

369

have the usual Student’s t-distribution and small minimum t-values can be highly significant.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

354

370 371

3. RESULTS 15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 372

3.1. MSL Behavioral Results Holm-Bonferonni corrected independent t-tests revealed that young participants had

374

significantly higher offline improvements in performance than older participants (t(26) = 3.87,

375

p = 0.001; Figure 1). Similarly, one sample t-tests showed that young participants offline

376

performance improvements were significantly different from zero (t(12) = 4.32, p = 0.004),

377

while the older participants performance did not significantly change (t(14) = -1.47, p = 0.163).

378

A previous report by our group indicated that offline performance improvements were

379

observed in young participants only, and only after a period of sleep, but not after a wake

380

period (Fogel et al., 2014), hence suggesting that sleep supported offline consolidation of

381

MSL in young, but not older participants.

SC

M AN U

******************************** INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE ********************************

AC C

EP

TE D

382

RI PT

373

383 384 385

Figure 1. Time between key presses (A) decreased over the course of the 14 blocks in the training session at the same rate for young and older participants. Young participants, but not 16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 386 387 388 389 390 391 392

older participants performance was then enhanced following a nap during the retest session, as indicated by (B) significant offline improvements in performance from the last 4 blocks of the training session to the first four blocks of the retest session. Note: Young participants performance in panel A scaled along the right-hand y-axis to illustrate similarity between young and older participants in MSL.

393

Both subjective and objective levels of sleepiness were measured before the MSL

394

training and retest sessions. A main effect of session revealed that subjective (F(1,26) = 8.47,

395

p = 0.007) and objective (F(1,26) = 8.63, p = 0.007) sleepiness was reduced following the

396

nap. There was no main effect of age group, nor a session by age group interaction for the

397

objective or subjective sleepiness scores (all p > 0.05). The latter findings suggest that sleep

398

during a daytime nap reduced sleepiness consistently across young and older participants,

399

but that sleepiness or vigilance alone did not affect MSL performance differentially across

400

groups, or explain group differences in MSL performance.

401

3.3. Sleep Architecture

M AN U

SC

RI PT

3.2. Subjective and Objective Sleepiness

As expected, young and older participants differed with respect to the sleep

403

characteristics measured during the 90-min daytime nap (Table 1). Older participants spent

404

significantly more time awake (t(26) = 3.31, p = 0.004). This was reflected by an increase in

405

wake time after sleep onset (t(26) = 2.77, p = 0.012), a decrease in total sleep time (t(26) =

406

3.23, p = 0.004) and a reduced sleep efficiency (t(26) = 3.06, p = 0.006). Older participants

407

had significantly less NREM sleep (t(26) = 2.76, p = 0.012) than young participants, the latter

408

being most likely driven by the significant reduction of slow wave sleep (SWS) as a percent of

409

total sleep time (t(26) = 2.09, p = 0.047). By contrast, N1 and REM sleep did not significantly

410

differ between young and older participants, hence indicating that sleep deficits in older

411

individuals were predominantly a result of changes in NREM sleep. The difference between

412

young and older participants for REM sleep did approach statistical significance (p < 0.10).

413

However, it should be noted that this was largely due to a floor effect, whereby 73% of older

AC C

EP

TE D

402

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT participants had no REM sleep at all, as compared to only 23% of young participants. Given

415

this unusual distribution, we also conducted non-parametric tests for REM sleep. However,

416

this revealed the same pattern of results, which did only approach statistical significance (U =

417

55.5, p > 0.05). Although this non-significant age difference is somewhat difficult to interpret, it

418

likely represents age-related difficulty in the initiation of REM sleep in a daytime nap, and not

419

necessarily REM duration per se. Due to the highly variable duration of N2 and an insufficient

420

quantity of SWS across participants in a daytime nap, N2 and SWS were collapsed into a

421

total measure of NREM sleep for spindle detection and analyses.

422

3.4. Sleep Spindles

M AN U

SC

RI PT

414

In addition to age-related differences in sleep architecture, older participants had

424

significantly shorter sleep spindle duration measured during NREM sleep at Fz (t(26) = 3.70,

425

p = 0.003), Cz (t(26) = 3.55, p = 0.002) and Pz(t(26) = 2.29, p = 0.030). In addition, their

426

spindles were significantly slower at Fz (t(26) = 3.33, p = 0.009; see Table 2). However, sleep

427

spindle density and amplitude were not significantly different between young and older

428

participants.

429

3.5. Spindle and MSL Behavioral Correlations

TE D

423

We conducted Pearson bivariate correlation tests to ascertain if sleep spindles were

431

linked to MSL performance improvements in young and older participants. These analyses

432

revealed that there were no significant correlations between spindles (density, amplitude and

433

duration) and offline performance improvements for the total spindles bandwidth at Cz, or

434

slow spindles at Fz for young and older participants (all p > 0.05). However, for fast spindles

435

at Pz, spindle density (r(11) = 0.60, p = 0.030), amplitude (r(11) = 0.614, p = 0.026) and

436

duration (r(11) = 0.69, p = 0.009) were significantly correlated with offline changes in MSL

437

performance in young participants. By contrast, spindle duration was negatively correlated

AC C

EP

430

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 438

with offline performance changes in older participants (r(13) = -0.67, p = 0.006) , whereby

439

larger spindle duration was associated with reduced offline changes in performance.

440

3.6. Structural MRI Results 3.6.1. GM differences in young vs. older participants:

442

As expected, greater GM was observed in young compared to older participants in a

443

widespread (n.b., the size of the significant striatal-cortical cluster in Table 3A) set of brain

444

regions bilaterally including the somatosensory, sensorimotor, premotor, frontal, dorsolateral,

445

medial prefrontal, cingulate, supplementary motor area, parietal, stratum and hippocampus

446

(Figure 2C).

M AN U

3.6.2. GM differences correlate with offline changes in performance and spindles

447 448

SC

RI PT

441

in young and older participants:

GM in young and older subjects correlated to a different extent with offline changes in

450

performance in the cerebellum (lobules Crus1, IV, IX; Table 3B). In addition, GM in young

451

and older subjects correlated to a different extent with total spindle duration in the cerebellum,

452

the hippocampus as well as the cingulate and parietal cortices (Table 3B). Slow spindle

453

amplitude and duration also correlated to a different extent in young and older adults with GM

454

in the supplementary motor area, and for fast spindle duration in the cerebellum (Table 3B).

EP

TE D

449

A conjunction analysis was used to identify regions where GM was correlated to a

456

different extent in young vs. older adults with both offline changes in performance and spindle

457

parameters (density, duration and amplitude) for slow, fast and total bandwidth spindles. In

458

young, but not older participants, GM in the hippocampus (bilaterally) and cerebellum were

459

positively correlated with both offline changes in performance and total spindle duration

460

(Table 3C; Figure 2). Furthermore, GM correlated with spindle duration to a different extent

461

when directly comparing young vs. older participants in these same regions (Table 3B).

AC C

455

462 19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ******************************** INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE *********************************

M AN U

SC

RI PT

463

464

Figure 2. Upper: Significant clusters of grey matter (GM) positively correlated with offline changes in performance in conjunction with Cz spindle duration in the hippocampus and cerebellum for young (red) participants (see Table 3C): GM in the hippocampus was (A) positively correlated with offline changes in performance and (B) spindle duration in young (filled circles) but not older (open circles) participants, which was (C) also higher on average for young as compared to older participants (see Table 3A). This difference in GM between young and older subjects correlated with both spindle duration and offline changes in MSL performance in the hippocampus and cerebellum (Table 3B). Results significant at p<0.001, uncorrected. Note: Only significant clusters and scatterplots shown for Cz spindles duration, as these clusters overlapped completely with significant regions for other spindle parameters (amplitude, density) for either fast or slow spindles (see Table 3 for details). X, Y Z coordinates given in voxel coordinates, displayed in radiological orientation.

479

Over the course of normal, healthy aging, sleep quantity and quality is reduced (Bixler

480

et al., 1984; Buysse et al., 1992; Carrier et al., 2011, 2001; Crowley et al., 2002; Darchia et

481

al., 2003; Feinberg et al., 1967; Floyd et al., 2007; Fogel et al., 2012; Gaudreau et al., 2001;

482

Landolt and Borbely, 2001; Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Peters et al., 2014;

483

Redline et al., 2004; Zepelin and McDonald, 1987). The functional consequences of this for

AC C

EP

TE D

465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478

4. DISCUSSION

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT learning and memory are just starting to become clear. Specifically, recent studies have

485

shown that for simple, explicitly known motor sequences, older adults are able to learn at a

486

normal rate. Yet their ability to consolidate the memory trace compared to younger individuals

487

is impaired (Fogel et al., 2014; Mander et al., 2015, 2013a, 2013b; Pace-Schott and Spencer,

488

2011; Spencer et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2012). Interestingly, the preservation of MSL during

489

training in older adults appears to be specific to explicitly known sequences. By contrast,

490

implicit probabilistic motor sequence learning is impaired with age (Howard and Howard,

491

2013; Simon et al., 2011), and a growing body of literature suggests that sleep may not be

492

involved in the enhancement of implicit motor skills, even in younger adults (Nemeth et al.,

493

2010). These results suggest that while explicit MSL is unaffected by age, age has an impact

494

on the offline consolidation/stabilization of the newly learned explicit motor skill. In other

495

words, older individuals do not derive the same benefit as younger individuals from sleep for

496

the consolidation of newly learned explicit motor skills.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

484

In terms of brain functional activation related to MSL and sleep-dependent

498

consolidation, recent work from our group, in the same set of participants reported here, has

499

revealed that a spindle-related reduction of activity in the putamen, cerebellum, parietal and

500

temporal cortex may underlie this age-related, sleep-dependent memory deficit (Fogel et al.,

501

2014). Consistent with this finding, others have demonstrated that grey matter is also reduced

502

with normal aging, with the most pronounced changes in the thalamus, hippocampus,

503

striatum, frontal cortex, motor cortical regions and cingulate cortex (Fjell and Walhovd, 2010;

504

Good et al., 2001b; Gunning-Dixon et al., 1998; Raz et al., 2015, 2005). Here we investigated

505

if the changes in brain structure related to spindles and offline motor sequence memory

506

consolidation may help to explain why older adults do not derive the same benefit from sleep

507

as younger adults for the consolidation of motor skills. More specifically, we explored whether

508

age-related reductions of grey matter integrity relate to spindle characteristics (e.g., density,

AC C

EP

TE D

497

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT amplitude, duration) and overnight, sleep-dependent offline improvements in motor skill

510

performance. We found that a widespread network of brain regions had reduced GM in older

511

compared to young participants, including motor cortical regions, the hippocampus, striatum,

512

cingulate, dorsolateral, medial prefrontal, temporal, parietal, frontal regions of the neocortex.

513

Interestingly, only age-related differences in GM in the cerebellum were correlated with offline

514

changes in MSL performance following a daytime nap. By contrast, sleep spindles correlated

515

with age-related changes in GM in a well-known set of brain regions which support motor

516

sequence memory consolidation (Doyon and Benali, 2005; Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002;

517

Doyon et al., 2003) including the hippocampus, parietal, cerebellum and supplementary motor

518

area. However, when both offline changes in MSL performance and sleep spindles were

519

considered together, grey matter in the hippocampus and cerebellum was correlated to a

520

greater extent in young vs. older subjects with both sleep spindles and sleep-related changes

521

in performance. Thus, suggesting that sleep spindles may serve as markers for age-related

522

changes in GM in the hippocampus and cerebellum, regions which support sleep-related

523

memory consolidation for motor skills. These results are in line with previous work showing

524

that sleep-dependent consolidation of skills are related to increased functional activation of

525

the cortico-striatal-hippocampal and cortico-cerebellar network in young healthy adults

526

(Albouy et al., 2015; Debas et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2005). This is also

527

consistent with our recent work in the same set of participants as the current study, showing

528

that sleep had a differential effect on functional brain activity from the MSL training session to

529

the retest session (retest-training) in the hippocampus and cerebellum between young and

530

older adults (Fogel et al., 2014). More specifically, young adults that slept and older adults

531

that were awake had increased activations, whereas young adults that were awake and older

532

adults that slept had decreased BOLD activity in the hippocampus, cerebellum and cortical

533

regions recruited during MSL (e.g., parietal and frontal cortex). This pattern of activity

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

509

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT paralleled the offline consolidation of MSL performance, thus suggesting that increased

535

hippocampal activity supports memory consolidation in young but not older adults. The

536

current study extends these results, providing the first evidence that structural grey matter

537

differences in regions correlated with sleep spindles may underlie age-related motor skill

538

memory consolidation deficits. Moreover, our results suggest that age-related GM atrophy in

539

regions such as the cerebellum and hippocampus, in particular (Kennedy and Raz, 2005; Raz

540

et al., 2015) may underlie spindle-related motor sequence memory consolidation deficits.

SC

RI PT

534

Although still controversial, age-related changes in grey matter may affect spindle

542

generating mechanisms that depend on synchronization of the neocortex (Bonjean et al.,

543

2012; Contreras and Steriade, 1996; Timofeev and Chauvette, 2013). Decreased spindle size

544

and frequency may be the result of cortical cell loss associated with the aging process. It has

545

been suggested that age-related loss of neurons may lead to a reduction of large-scale

546

synchronization, thereby affecting the size and frequency of sleep spindles (Crowley et al.,

547

2002; Nicolas et al., 2001). Consistent with this suggestion and a previous study by our group

548

(Martin et al., 2012), it was observed that sleep spindle duration was reduced in older

549

participants for spindles compared to younger participants, although unlike the current study,

550

no age-difference was previously observed for spindle frequency. However, similar to Landolt

551

et al. (Landolt and Borbely, 2001; Landolt et al., 1996) slow spindles were slower at frontal

552

regions in older participants with no change in faster frequencies. It should be noted that the

553

method of spindle detection and characterization used in the present study (Ray et al., 2015)

554

does not employ a minimum duration criteria, as we have demonstrated that the majority of

555

spindles are below the minimum criteria conventionally used to visually identify spindles.

556

Despite this, no spindles below 0.2 sec were identified. However, this is an important

557

methodological consideration, since not including spindles <0.5sec, may bias or

558

underestimate age-related changes in spindles. Thus the results of the current study, suggest

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

541

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 559

that with age, the relationship between grey matter and spindles is negatively impacted, and

560

related to a reduction in the sleep-related benefit to memory consolidation. Consistent with previous reports (Fogel et al., 2014; Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011;

562

Spencer et al., 2007), and despite the fact that performance in older participants was slower

563

overall as compared to younger participants, older participants exhibited the same pattern of

564

increasing performance over the course of the training session as the young participants. This

565

suggests that despite slower motor execution, explicit motor sequence learning remains

566

largely intact with age except under greater task complexity and implicit MSL (Howard and

567

Howard, 2013; King et al., 2013; Rieckmann et al., 2010; Simon et al., 2011). However,

568

following a retention interval filled with sleep, older individuals do not derive the same benefit

569

as younger participants who experienced offline consolidation/stabilization (Fogel et al., 2014;

570

Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011; Spencer et al., 2007).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

561

In conclusion, our results suggest that age-related changes in sleep spindles are

572

differentially related to grey matter in young and older individuals in brain regions that support

573

sleep-dependent memory consolidation for motor skills. In this way, sleep spindles can serve

574

as a biological marker for structural brain changes in grey matter and their associated

575

memory deficits in older adults.

EP

TE D

571

AC C

576 577

5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

578

The authors would like to acknowledge the technical support of Vo An Nguyen, Laura Ray,

579

Andre Cyr, Carollyn Hurst, Frederic Jeay, and Amel Bouyoucef. This research was funded by

580

a Canadian Institutes of Health research (CIHR) grant to author JD and fellowship support to

581

author SF from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC),

582

the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec (FRSQ).

583 24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 584 585

6. REFERENCES

586

Albouy, G., Fogel, S., King, B.R., Laventure, S., Benali, H., Karni, A., Carrier, J., Robertson, E.M., Doyon, J., 2015. Maintaining vs. enhancing motor sequence memories: respective

588

roles of striatal and hippocampal systems. Neuroimage 108, 423–34.

589

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.049

Albouy, G., Fogel, S., Pottiez, H., Nguyen, V.A., Ray, L., Lungu, O., Carrier, J., Robertson, E.,

SC

590

RI PT

587

Doyon, J., 2013a. Daytime sleep enhances consolidation of the spatial but not motoric

592

representation of motor sequence memory. PLoS One 8, e52805.

593

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052805

594

M AN U

591

Albouy, G., King, B.R., Maquet, P., Doyon, J., 2013b. Hippocampus and striatum: Dynamics

595

and interaction during acquisition and sleep-related motor sequence memory

596

consolidation. Hippocampus 23, 985–1004. doi:10.1002/hipo.22183 Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle, G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T.T.,

TE D

597

Darsaud, A., Ruby, P., Luppi, P.-H., Degueldre, C., Peigneux, P., Luxen, A., Maquet, P.,

599

2008. Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor

600

sequence memory. Neuron 58, 261–272. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008 Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Darsaud, A., Gais, S., Rauchs, G., Desseilles, M.,

AC C

601

EP

598

602

Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Phillips, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P.,

603

2013c. Interaction between hippocampal and striatal systems predicts subsequent

604

consolidation of motor sequence memory. PLoS One 8, e59490.

605

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059490

606

Andersson, J., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., 2007. Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA2.

607 608

Barakat, M., Carrier, J., Debas, K., Lungu, O., Fogel, S., Vandewalle, G., Hoge, R.D., Bellec, 25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 609

P., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Doyon, J., 2012. Sleep spindles predict neural

610

and behavioral changes in motor sequence consolidation. Hum Brain Mapp 34, 2918–

611

2928. doi:10.1002/hbm.22116 Barakat, M., Doyon, J., Debas, K., Vandewalle, G., Morin, A., Poirier, G., Martin, N.,

RI PT

612

Lafortune, M., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Carrier, J., 2011. Fast and slow

614

spindle involvement in the consolidation of a new motor sequence. Behav. Brain Res.

615

217, 117–121. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.019

616

SC

613

Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M., Sejnowski, T., 2000. Spiking-bursting activity in the thalamic reticular nucleus initiates sequences of spindle oscillations in thalamic networks.

618

J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1076–1087.

M AN U

617

619

Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M., Sejnowski, T., 1999. Self-sustained rhythmic activity

620

in the thalamic reticular nucleus mediated by depolarizing GABAA receptor potentials.

621

Nat. Neurosci. 2, 168–174. doi:10.1038/5729

Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., Steer, R.A., 1988. An inventory for measuring clinical

TE D

622

anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56, 893–897.

623 624

Beck, A.T., Rial, W.Y., Rickels, K., 1974. Short form of depression inventory: cross-validation. Psychol Rep 34, 1184–1186.

Bergmann, T.O., Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Kaya-Yildiz, L., Born, J., Roman Siebner, H., 2008. A

AC C

626

EP

625

627

local signature of LTP- and LTD-like plasticity in human NREM sleep. Eur J Neurosci 27,

628

2241–2249. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06178.x

629

Bixler, E.O., Kales, A., Jacoby, J.A., Soldatos, C.R., Vela-Bueno, A., 1984. Nocturnal sleep

630

and wakefulness: effects of age and sex in normal sleepers. Int J Neurosci 23, 33–42.

631

Bonjean, M., Baker, T., Bazhenov, M., Cash, S., Halgren, E., Sejnowski, T., 2012.

632

Interactions between Core and Matrix Thalamocortical Projections in Human Sleep

633

Spindle Synchronization. J. Neurosci. 32, 5250–5263. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.614126

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11.2012

634 635

Bonjean, M., Baker, T., Lemieux, M., Timofeev, I., Sejnowski, T., Bazhenov, M., 2011. Corticothalamic feedback controls sleep spindle duration in vivo. J Neurosci 31, 9124–

637

9134. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0077-11.2011

638

RI PT

636

Bowersox, S.S., Kaitin, K.I., Dement, W.C., 1985. EEG spindle activity as a function of age: relationship to sleep continuity. Brain Res. 334, 303–8.

639

Brown, R.M., Robertson, E., Press, D.Z., 2009. Sequence skill acquisition and off-line

641

learning in normal aging. PLoS One 4, e6683. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006683

SC

640

Buysse, D.J., Browman, K.E., Monk, T.H., Reynolds, C.F., Fasiczka, A.L., Kupfer, D.J., 1992.

643

Napping and 24-hour sleep/wake patterns in healthy elderly and young adults. J. Am.

644

Geriatr. Soc. 40, 779–786.

645

M AN U

642

Cajochen, C., Münch, M., Knoblauch, V., Blatter, K., Wirz-Justice, A., 2006. Age-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol Int 23,

647

461–474. doi:10.1080/07420520500545813

648

TE D

646

Caporro, M., Haneef, Z., Yeh, H.J., Lenartowicz, A., Buttinelli, C., Parvizi, J., Stern, J.M., 2011. Functional MRI of sleep spindles and K-complexes. Clin Neurophysiol 123, 303–

650

309. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.06.018 Carrier, J., Land, S., Buysse, D.J., Kupfer, D.J., Monk, T.H., 2001. The effects of age and

AC C

651

EP

649

652

gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20-60 years

653

old). Psychophysiology 38, 232–242.

654

Carrier, J., Viens, I., Poirier, G., Robillard, R., Lafortune, M., Vandewalle, G., Martin, N.,

655

Barakat, M., Paquet, J., Filipini, D., 2011. Sleep slow wave changes during the middle

656

years of life. Eur J Neurosci 33, 758–66. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07543.x

657

Carskadon, M.A., Dement, W.C., 2011. Normal Human Sleep: An Overview, in: Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. pp. 16–26. doi:10.1016/B978-1-4160-6645-3.00002-5

658 27

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 659

Cockrell, J.R., Folstein, M.F., 1988. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Psychopharmacol Bull 24, 689–692.

660 661

Contreras, D., Steriade, M., 1996. Spindle oscillation in cats: the role of corticothalamic feedback in a thalamically generated rhythm. J. Physiol. 490 ( Pt 1, 159–79.

663

RI PT

662

Cote, K.A., Epps, T.M., Campbell, K., 2000. The role of the spindle in human information processing of high-intensity stimuli during sleep. J Sleep Res 9, 19–26.

664

Crowley, K., Trinder, J., Colrain, I.M., 2004. Evoked K-complex generation: the impact of

SC

665

sleep spindles and age. Clin Neurophysiol 115, 471–476.

666

Crowley, K., Trinder, J., Kim, Y., Carrington, M., Colrain, I.M., 2002. The effects of normal

M AN U

667 668

aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin Neurophysiol 113, 1615–1622.

669

doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00237-7

670

Dang-Vu, T.T., McKinney, S.M., Buxton, O.M., Solet, J.M., Ellenbogen, J.M., 2010. Spontaneous brain rhythms predict sleep stability in the face of noise. Curr Biol 20,

672

R626–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.032

Darchia, N., Campbell, I.G., Feinberg, I., 2003. Rapid eye movement density is reduced in the normal elderly. Sleep 26, 973–977.

674 675

De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., 2003. Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Med. Rev. 7, 423– 440.

AC C

676

EP

673

TE D

671

677

De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Bertini, M., 2000. Topographical distribution of spindles:

678

variations between and within nrem sleep cycles. Sleep Res Online 3, 155–160.

679

De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Vecchio, F., Curcio, G., Bertini, M., 2005. An

680

electroencephalographic fingerprint of human sleep. Neuroimage 26, 114–122.

681

doi:S1053-8119(05)00049-2 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.020

682

Debas, K., Carrier, J., Orban, P., Barakat, M., Lungu, O., Vandewalle, G., Hadj Tahar, A., Bellec, P., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Doyon, J., 2010. Brain plasticity

683 28

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 684

related to the consolidation of motor sequence learning and motor adaptation. Proc. Natl.

685

Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 17839–17844. doi:10.1073/pnas.1013176107

686

Dehghani, N., Cash, S., Halgren, E., 2011. Topographical frequency dynamics within EEG and MEG sleep spindles. Clin Neurophysiol 122, 229–235.

688

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.06.018

689

RI PT

687

Dinges, D.F., Powell, J.W., 1985. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav. Res. Methods 17, 652–655.

691

Doran, S.M., 2003. The dynamic topography of individual sleep spindles. Sleep Res. Online

M AN U

5, 133–139.

692 693

SC

690

Douaud, G., Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Behrens, T., Johansen-Berg, H., Vickers, J., James, S.,

694

Voets, N., Watkins, K., Matthews, P.M., James, A., 2007. Anatomically related grey and

695

white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130, 2375–86.

696

doi:10.1093/brain/awm184

Doyon, J., Benali, H., 2005. Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of

TE D

697

motor skills. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 161–167. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004

698 699

Doyon, J., Penhune, V.B., Ungerleider, L.G., 2003. Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia 41, 252–262.

701

doi:S0028393202001586 [pii]

AC C

702

EP

700

Doyon, J., Ungerleider, L.G., 2002. Functional anatomy of motor skill learning. Neuropsychol. Mem. 3, 225–238.

703 704

Feinberg, I., 1974. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age. J. Psychiatr. Res. 10, 283–306. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(74)90011-9

705 706

Feinberg, I., Koresko, R.L., Heller, N., 1967. EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and

707

pathological aging in man, Journal of Psychiatric Research. doi:10.1016/0022-

708

3956(67)90027-1 29

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 709

Ferrarelli, F., Peterson, M.J., Sarasso, S., Riedner, B.A., Murphy, M.J., Benca, R.M., Bria, P., Kalin, N.H., Tononi, G., 2010. Thalamic dysfunction in schizophrenia suggested by

711

whole-night deficits in slow and fast spindles. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 1339–48.

712

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121731

713

Fjell, A.M., Walhovd, K.B., 2010. Structural brain changes in aging: courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 187–221.

714

Floyd, J. a, Janisse, J.J., Jenuwine, E.S., Ager, J.W., 2007. Changes in REM-sleep

SC

715

RI PT

710

percentage over the adult lifespan. Sleep 30, 829–836.

716

Fogel, S., Albouy, G., Vien, C., Popovicci, R., King, B.R., Hoge, R.D., Jbabdi, S., Benali, H.,

718

Karni, A., Maquet, P., Carrier, J., Doyon, J., 2014. fMRI and sleep correlates of the age-

719

related impairment in motor memory consolidation. Hum Brain Mapp 35, 3625–3645.

720

doi:10.1002/hbm.22426

721

M AN U

717

Fogel, S., Martin, N., Lafortune, M., Barakat, M., Debas, K., Laventure, S., Latreille, V., Gagnon, J.F., Doyon, J., Carrier, J., 2012. NREM Sleep Oscillations and Brain Plasticity

723

in Aging. Front Neurol 3, 176. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00176

724

TE D

722

Fogel, S., Ray, L.B., Binnie, L., Owen, A.M., 2015. How to become an expert: A new perspective on the role of sleep in the mastery of procedural skills. Neurobiol. Learn.

726

Mem. 125, 236–248. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2015.10.004

AC C

727

EP

725

Fogel, S., Smith, C., 2011. The function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of

728

intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci

729

Biobehav Rev. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.003

730

Fogel, S., Smith, C., Cote, K.A., 2007. Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and

731

non-REM sleep in declarative and procedural memory systems. Behav. Brain Res. 180,

732

48–61. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.037

733

Fogel, S., Smith, C.T., 2006. Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 30

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sleep. J. Sleep Res. 15, 250–255. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00522.x

734

Friston, K., Penny, W., Glaser, D.E., 2005. Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage 25, 661–667. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013

736 737

Gaser, C., Schlaug, G., 2003. Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 514–7.

738 739

Gaudreau, H., Carrier, J., Montplaisir, J., 2001. Age-related modifications of NREM sleep EEG: from childhood to middle age. J. Sleep Res. 10, 165–172. doi:252 [pii]

SC

740 741

RI PT

735

Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I.S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R.S., 2001a. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains.

743

Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

744

M AN U

742

Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I.S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S.,

745

2001b. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains.

746

Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

Groussard, M., Viader, F., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., Platel, H., 2014. The

748

effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: the dynamics of grey matter changes.

749

Brain Cogn. 90, 174–80. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013

TE D

747

Gryga, M., Taubert, M., Dukart, J., Vollmann, H., Conde, V., Sehm, B., Villringer, A., Ragert,

751

P., 2012. Bidirectional gray matter changes after complex motor skill learning. Front.

752

Syst. Neurosci. 6, 37. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2012.00037

AC C

EP

750

753

Guazzelli, M., Feinberg, I., Aminoff, M., Fein, G., Floyd, T.C., Maggini, C., Guazzelli, N.,

754

Feinberg, I., Aminoff, M., Fein, G., Floyd, T.C., Maggini, C., 1986. Sleep spindles in

755

normal elderly: comparison with young adult patterns and relation to nocturnal

756

awakening, cognitive function and brain atrophy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

757

63, 526–539.

758

Gunning-Dixon, F.M., Head, D., McQuain, J., Acker, J.D., Raz, N., 1998. Differential aging of 31

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 759

the human striatum: a prospective MR imaging study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 19, 1501–

760

1507. Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Schwarzwald, R., May, A., 2012. Dynamic gray matter changes

762

within cortex and striatum after short motor skill training are associated with their

763

increased functional interaction. Neuroimage 59, 3364–72.

764

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.089

SC

Horne, J.A., Ostberg, O., 1976. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningnesseveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4, 97–110.

766 767

Howard, J.H., Howard, D. V., 2013. Aging mind and brain: Is implicit learning spared in healthy aging? Front. Psychol. 4, 1–6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00817

768 769

M AN U

765

RI PT

761

Huupponen, E., Himanen, S.L., Varri, A., Hasan, J., Lehtokangas, M., Saarinen, J., 2002. A

770

study on gender and age differences in sleep spindles. Neuropsychobiology 45, 99–105.

771

doi:nps45099 [pii]

Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A.L., Quan, S.F., 2007. The AASM manual for the scoring

773

of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. American

774

Academy of Sleep Medicine Westchester, IL. Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., Woolrich, M.W., Smith, S.M., 2012. Fsl.

EP

775

TE D

772

Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

777

AC C

776

Jobert, M., Poiseau, E., Jahnig, P., Schulz, H., Kubicki, S., 1992. Topographical analysis of sleep spindle activity. Neuropsychobiology 26, 210–217. doi:118923

778 779

Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M.M., Turner, R., Ungerleider, L.G., 1995.

780

Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning.

781

Nature 377, 155–158. doi:10.1038/377155a0

782

Kennedy, K.M., Raz, N., 2005. Age, sex and regional brain volumes predict perceptual-motor skill acquisition. Cortex 41, 560–569.

783 32

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 784

King, B.R., Fogel, S., Albouy, G., Doyon, J., 2013. Neural correlates of the age-related

785

changes in motor sequence learning and motor adaptation in older adults. Front Hum

786

Neurosci 7, 142. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00142 Knoblauch, V., Münch, M., Blatter, K., Martens, W.L.J., Schröder, C., Schnitzler, C., Wirz-

RI PT

787 788

Justice, A., Cajochen, C., 2005. Age-related changes in the circadian modulation of

789

sleep-spindle frequency during nap sleep. Sleep 28, 1093–1101.

Lafortune, M., Gagnon, J.F., Martin, N., Latreille, V., Dubé, J., Bouchard, M., Bastien, C.,

791

Carrier, J., 2014. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next

792

morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep

793

Res 23, 159–167. doi:10.1111/jsr.12108

M AN U

794

SC

790

Landolt, H., Borbely, A.A., 2001. Age-dependent changes in sleep EEG topography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 369–377. doi:S1388-2457(00)00542-3 [pii]

795

Landolt, H., Dijk, D.J., Achermann, P., Borbely, A.A., 1996. Effect of age on the sleep EEG:

797

slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle-aged men. Brain

798

Res. 738, 205–212.

799

TE D

796

Leclercq, Y., Schrouff, J., Noirhomme, Q., Maquet, P., Phillips, C., 2011. fMRI artefact rejection and sleep scoring toolbox. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011, 598206.

801

doi:10.1155/2011/598206

AC C

802

EP

800

MacLean, A.W., Fekken, G.C., Saskin, P., Knowles, J.B., 1992. Psychometric evaluation of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale. J Sleep Res 1, 35–39.

803 804

Mander, B.A., Marks, S.M., Vogel, J.W., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Ancoli-Israel, S.,

805

Jagust, W.J., Walker, M.P., 2015. β-amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and

806

related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1051–7.

807

doi:10.1038/nn.4035

808

Mander, B.A., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Ancoli-Israel, S., Jagust, W., Walker, M., 2013a. 33

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 809

Impaired Prefrontal Sleep Spindle Regulation of Hippocampal-Dependent Learning in

810

Older Adults. Cereb Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht188

811

Mander, B.A., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Lindquist, J.R., Ancoli-Israel, S., Jagust, W., Walker, M., 2013b. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired

813

hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci 16, 357–364.

814

doi:10.1038/nn.3324

Martin, N., Lafortune, M., Godbout, J., Barakat, M., Robillard, R., Poirier, G., Bastien, C.,

SC

815

RI PT

812

Carrier, J., 2012. Topography of age-related changes in sleep spindles. Neurobiol Aging

817

34, 468–476. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.020

818

M AN U

816

Milner, C.E., Fogel, S., Cote, K.A., 2006. Habitual napping moderates motor performance

819

improvements following a short daytime nap. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 141–156.

820

doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.015

Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Londe, Z., Ullman, M.T., Howard, D. V, Howard, J.H., 2010. Sleep

822

has no critical role in implicit motor sequence learning in young and old adults. Exp Brain

823

Res 201, 351–358. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2024-x

824

TE D

821

Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25. Nicolas, A., Petit, D., Rompré, S., Montplaisir, J., 2001. Sleep spindle characteristics in

AC C

826

EP

825

healthy subjects of different age groups. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 521–527.

827 828

Nielsen, T., O’Reilly, C., Carr, M., Dumel, G., Godin, I., Solomonova, E., Lara-Carrasco, J.,

829

Blanchette-Carrière, C., Paquette, T., 2014. Overnight improvements in two REM sleep-

830

sensitive tasks are associated with both REM and NREM sleep changes, sleep spindle

831

features, and awakenings for dream recall. Neurobiol Learn Mem.

832

doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2014.09.007

833

Nishida, M., Walker, M., 2007. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally 34

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 834

specific sleep spindles. PLoS One 2, e341. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000341

835

Ohayon, M.M., Carskadon, M.A., Guilleminault, C., Vitiello, M. V, 2004. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing

837

normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27, 1255–1273.

838

Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.

839

Pace-Schott, E.F., Spencer, R.M.C., 2011. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of

SC

840

sleep. Prog Brain Res 191, 75–89. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00012-6

841

Peters, K.R., Ray, L., Fogel, S., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2014. Age differences in the variability

M AN U

842

RI PT

836

843

and distribution of sleep spindle and rapid eye movement densities. PLoS One 9,

844

e91047. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091047

Peters, K.R., Ray, L., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2008. Changes in the density of stage 2 sleep

846

spindles following motor learning in young and older adults. J. Sleep Res. 17, 23–33.

847

doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00634.x

848

TE D

845

Peters, K.R., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2007. Changes in sleep architecture following motor learning depend on initial skill level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 817–829.

850

doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.817 Pivik, R.T., Joncas, S., Busby, K.A., 1999. Sleep spindles and arousal: the effects of age and sensory stimulation. Sleep Res. Online 2, 89–100.

852 853

AC C

851

EP

849

Principe, J.C., Smith, J.R., 1982. Sleep spindle characteristics as a function of age. Sleep 5, 73.

854 855

Ramanand, P., Bruce, M.C., Bruce, E.N., 2010. Mutual information analysis of EEG signals

856

indicates age-related changes in cortical interdependence during sleep in middle-aged

857

versus elderly women. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 27, 274–284.

858

doi:10.1097/WNP.0b013e3181eaa9f5 35

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 859

Ramanathan, D.S., Gulati, T., Ganguly, K., 2015. Sleep-Dependent Reactivation of

860

Ensembles in Motor Cortex Promotes Skill Consolidation. PLoS Biol. 13, e1002263.

861

doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002263 Rasch, B., Born, J., 2013. About Sleep’s Role in Memory. Physiol Rev 93, 681–766.

RI PT

862

doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

863

Ray, L.B., Sockeel, S., Soon, M., Bore, A., Myhr, A., Stojanoski, B., Cusack, R., Owen, A.M.,

865

Doyon, J., Fogel, S., 2015. Expert and crowd-sourced validation of an individualized

866

sleep spindle detection method employing complex demodulation and individualized

867

normalization. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00507

M AN U

868

SC

864

Raz, N., Daugherty, A.M., Bender, A.R., Dahle, C.L., Land, S., 2015. Volume of the

869

hippocampal subfields in healthy adults: differential associations with age and a pro-

870

inflammatory genetic variant. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 2663–74. doi:10.1007/s00429-

871

014-0817-6

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle,

873

C., Gerstorf, D., Acker, J.D., 2005. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults:

874

general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–89.

875

doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi044

EP

Redline, S., Gottlieb, D.J., Kapur, V., Kirchner, H.L., Quan, S.F., Newman, A., 2004. The

AC C

876

TE D

872

877

Effects of Age, Sex, Ethnicity, and Sleep-Disordered Breathing on Sleep Architecture.

878

Arch. Intern. Med. 164, 406–418. doi:10.1001/archinte.164.4.406

879

Rieckmann, A., Fischer, H., Bäckman, L., 2010. Activation in striatum and medial temporal

880

lobe during sequence learning in younger and older adults: relations to performance.

881

Neuroimage 50, 1303–1312. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.015

882

Rosanova, M., Ulrich, D., 2005. Pattern-specific associative long-term potentiation induced by

883

a sleep spindle-related spike train. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 25, 9398–9405. 36

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2149-05.2005

884 885

Saletin, J.M., van der Helm, E., Walker, M.P., 2013. Structural brain correlates of human

886

sleep oscillations. Neuroimage 83, 658–668. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.021 Sargolzaei, S., Sargolzaei, A., Cabrerizo, M., Chen, G., Goryawala, M., Noei, S., Zhou, Q.,

888

Duara, R., Barker, W., Adjouadi, M., 2015. A practical guideline for intracranial volume

889

estimation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. BMC Bioinformatics 16, S8.

890

doi:10.1186/1471-2105-16-S7-S8

SC

891

RI PT

887

Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Albouy, G., Balteau, E., Boly, M., Carrier, J., Darsaud, A., Degueldre, C., Desseilles, M., Gais, S., Phillips, C., Rauchs, G., Schnakers, C.,

893

Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., 2007. Hemodynamic cerebral

894

correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc. Natl.

895

Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 13164–13169. doi:0703084104 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.0703084104

896

M AN U

892

Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Heib, D.P.J., Boly, M., Desseilles, M., Vandewalle, G., Schmidt, C., Albouy, G., Darsaud, A., Gais, S., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Phillips, C., Luxen, A.,

898

Maquet, P., 2012. The Fate of Incoming Stimuli during NREM Sleep is Determined by

899

Spindles and the Phase of the Slow Oscillation. Front Neurol 3, 40.

900

doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00040

EP

Silber, M.H., Ancoli-Israel, S., Bonnet, M.H., Chokroverty, S., Grigg-Damberger, M.M.,

AC C

901

TE D

897

902

Hirshkowitz, M., Kapen, S., Keenan, S., Kryger, M.H., Penzel, T., 2007. The visual

903

scoring of sleep in adults. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 3, 121–131.

904

Simon, J.R., Howard, J.H., Howard, D. V, 2011. Age differences in implicit learning of

905

probabilistic unstructured sequences. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 32–8.

906

doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq066

907

Smith, C., Smith, D., 2003. Ingestion of ethanol just prior to sleep onset impairs memory for procedural but not declarative tasks. Sleep 26, 185–191.

908 37

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 909

Smith, S.M., 2002. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–55. doi:10.1002/hbm.10062

910 911

Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.W., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., JohansenBerg, H., Bannister, P.R., De Luca, M., Drobnjak, I., Flitney, D.E., Niazy, R.K., Saunders,

913

J., Vickers, J., Zhang, Y., De Stefano, N., Brady, J.M., Matthews, P.M., 2004. Advances

914

in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage

915

23 Suppl 1, S208–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

SC

916

RI PT

912

Spencer, R.M.C., Gouw, A.M., Ivry, R.B., 2007. Age-related decline of sleep-dependent consolidation. Learn Mem 14, 480–484. doi:10.1101/lm.569407

918

M AN U

917

Steriade, M., 2006. Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137, 1087–1106. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.029

919 920

Steriade, M., 2005. Sleep, epilepsy and thalamic reticular inhibitory neurons. Trends Neurosci 28, 317–324. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.03.007

921

Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H., Hori, T., 2009. Activation of fast sleep spindles at the

TE D

922 923

premotor cortex and parietal areas contributes to motor learning: a study using

924

sLORETA. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 878–886. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2009.03.006 Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H., Hori, T., 2008. Fast sleep spindle (13-15 hz) activity

EP

925

correlates with sleep-dependent improvement in visuomotor performance. Sleep 31,

927

204–211.

AC C

926

928

Timofeev, I., 2001. Contribution of intrinsic and synaptic factors in the desynchronization of

929

thalamic oscillatory activity. Thalamus Relat. Syst. 1, 53–69. doi:10.1016/S1472-

930

9288(01)00004-8

931

Timofeev, I., Chauvette, S., 2013. The spindles: are they still thalamic? Sleep 36, 825–6. doi:10.5665/sleep.2702

932 933

Tombaugh, T.N., McIntyre, N.J., 1992. The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive 38

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT review. J Am Geriatr Soc 40, 922–935.

934 935

Tyvaert, L., Levan, P., Grova, C., Dubeau, F., Gotman, J., 2008. Effects of fluctuating physiological rhythms during prolonged EEG-fMRI studies. Clin Neurophysiol 119, 2762–

937

2774. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.284

938

RI PT

936

Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripollés, P., Rojo, N., Sierpowska, J., François, C., Càmara, E., van Vugt, F.T., Mohammadi, B., Samii, A., Münte, T.F., Rodríguez-Fornells, A.,

940

Altenmüller, E., 2016. Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of

941

musical training onset. Neuroimage 126, 106–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008 Vien, C., Boré, A., Lungu, O., Benali, H., Carrier, J., Fogel, S., Doyon, J., n.d. Age-Related

M AN U

942

SC

939

943

White Matter Correlates of Motor Sequence Learning and Consolidation. Neurobiol.

944

Aging. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.08.006

945

Walker, M., Stickgold, R., Alsop, D., Gaab, N., Schlaug, G., 2005. Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain. Neuroscience 133, 911–917. doi:S0306-

947

4522(05)00396-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.007

948

TE D

946

Wamsley, E., Shinn, A.K., Tucker, M.A., Ono, K.E., McKinley, S., Ely, A. V, Goff, D.C., Stickgold, R., Manoach, D.S., 2013. The effects of eszopiclone on sleep spindles and

950

memory consolidation in schizophrenia: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Sleep 36,

951

1369–1376. doi:10.5665/sleep.2968

AC C

952

EP

949

Wamsley, E., Tucker, M.A., Shinn, A.K., Ono, K.E., McKinley, S., Ely, A. V, Goff, D.C.,

953

Stickgold, R., Manoach, D.S., 2012. Reduced sleep spindles and spindle coherence in

954

schizophrenia: mechanisms of impaired memory consolidation? Biol Psychiatry 71, 154–

955

161. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.008

956

Werk, C.M., Harbour, V.L., Chapman, C.A., 2005. Induction of long-term potentiation leads to

957

increased reliability of evoked neocortical spindles in vivo. Neuroscience 131, 793–800.

958

doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.020 39

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 959

Werth, E., Achermann, P., Borbely, A.A., 1997a. Fronto-occipital EEG power gradients in human sleep. J. Sleep Res. 6, 102–112.

960 961

Werth, E., Achermann, P., Dijk, D.J., Borbely, A.A., 1997b. Spindle frequency activity in the sleep EEG: individual differences and topographic distribution. Electroencephalogr. Clin.

963

Neurophysiol. 103, 535–542. doi:S0013469497000709 [pii]

964

RI PT

962

Wilhelm, I., Metzkow-Mészàros, M., Knapp, S., Born, J., 2012. Sleep-dependent consolidation of procedural motor memories in children and adults: the pre-sleep level of performance

966

matters. Dev Sci 15, 506–515. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01146.x

SC

965

Wilson, K., Baran, B., Pace-Schott, E.F., Ivry, R.B., Spencer, R.M.C., 2012. Sleep modulates

968

word-pair learning but not motor sequence learning in healthy older adults. Neurobiol

969

Aging 33, 991–1000. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.029

970

M AN U

967

Winkler, A.M., Ridgway, G.R., Webster, M.A., Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2014. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381–97.

972

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060

973

TE D

971

Zeitlhofer, J., Gruber, G., Anderer, P., Asenbaum, S., Schimicek, P., Saletu, B., 1997. Topographic distribution of sleep spindles in young healthy subjects. J. Sleep Res. 6,

975

149–155.

Zepelin, H., McDonald, C.S., 1987. Age differences in autonomic variables during sleep. J.

AC C

976

Gerontol. 42, 142–146.

977 978

EP

974

Zhang, Y., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2001. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden

979

Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans.

980

Med. Imaging 20, 45–57. doi:10.1109/42.906424

981

40

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

11.51 60.98 15.41 76.39 12.10

2.36 2.86 3.10 2.74 2.61

20.14 67.40 7.01 74.41 5.45

4.15 3.33 2.61 3.96 2.73

-1.08 -1.44 2.09 0.41 1.75

SC

Stage 1 % TST Stage 2 % TST SWS % TST NREM % TST REM % TST

RI PT

Table 1. Mean (and standard error; SE) sleep parameters recorded during the 90-minute daytime nap retention interval in both young and older participants. Young Older M SE M SE t p Wake (min) 19.85 3.44 47.09 7.47 -3.31 0.004 * Stage 1 (min) 7.87 1.36 6.18 0.76 1.28 0.270 Stage 2 (min) 43.31 2.28 31.02 5.22 2.16 0.044 * SWS (min) 11.45 2.41 4.60 1.75 2.35 0.027 * NREM (min) 54.77 3.13 35.62 6.20 2.76 0.012 * REM (min) 9.44 2.10 4.20 2.18 1.72 0.098 Total sleep time (min) 72.08 3.56 46.00 7.26 3.23 0.004 * 0.084 0.162 0.047 0.684 0.093

*

M AN U

Sleep onset latency (min) 11.92 1.91 18.40 5.26 -1.16 0.263 Wake after sleep onset (min) 10.87 2.69 31.13 6.79 -2.77 0.012 * Total recording time (min) 94.87 0.74 93.76 0.73 1.06 0.297 Sleep efficiency (%) 76.05 3.85 49.31 7.83 3.06 0.006 * Note: * indicates p < 0.05, df = 26, corrected for heterogeneity of variance where appropriate. Abbreviations: Minutes of sleep (min), Total sleep time (TST), rapid eye movement sleep (REM).

982

AC C

EP

TE D

983

41

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 984 2

M AN U

SC

RI PT

Table 2. Number, density (#/min NREM), duration (sec) and amplitude (uV ) and frequency (Hz) of NREM sleep spindles at Fz (slow: 11-14Hz), Cz (total: 11-17Hz) and Pz (fast: 14-17Hz) during the 90min daytime nap retention interval in young and older participants. Young Older M SE M SE t p Number Fz (slow) 193.38 17.27 149.73 26.61 1.99 0.394 Cz (total) 251.54 21.31 186.13 31.73 1.65 0.336 Pz (fast) 99.85 15.86 70.73 15.59 1.85 0.219 Density (#/min) Fz (slow) 3.45 0.22 4.07 0.14 -3.44 0.099 Cz (total) 4.51 0.25 5.21 0.15 -2.39 0.144 Pz (fast) 1.78 0.26 1.94 0.21 -0.68 0.651 Duration (sec) Fz (slow) * 0.50 0.02 0.38 0.02 5.19 0.005 Cz (total) * 0.48 0.03 0.34 0.02 3.55 0.004 Pz (fast) * 0.47 0.02 0.40 0.02 3.09 0.045 Amplitude (uV2) Fz (slow) 62.88 8.06 41.68 5.02 3.24 0.132 Cz (total) 90.18 12.32 67.24 8.12 1.50 0.298 Pz (fast) 67.78 11.07 74.39 11.70 -0.58 0.696 Frequency (Hz) Fz (slow) * 12.50 0.04 12.23 0.04 6.57 0.000 Cz (total) 13.36 0.07 13.29 0.10 0.55 0.585 Pz (fast) 14.69 0.04 14.83 0.07 -2.72 0.177 Note: * indicates p < 0.05, two-tailed. df = 26, corrected for homogeneity of variance, where appropriate and Holm-Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons for the number of sites.

985

AC C

EP

TE D

986

42

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 987

B) YOUNG vs. OLDER: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES MSL Right Left

MSL MSL

Cerebellum (IV) Cerebellum (IX,X)

26 3

Spindles Left Left Left Left Right Right Left

Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Slow spindle amplitude Slow spindle duration Fast spindle duration

Cerebellum (IV) Hippocampus Cingulate cortex Parietal Supplementary motor area Supplementary motor area Cerebellum (IV)

Cerebellum (IX) Cerebellum (IIX) Hippocampus

RI PT

Table 3. Brain regions with (A) significantly greater GM in young vs. older participants, (B) GM correlated with offline changes (MSL) in performance in conjunction with spindles in young vs. older, (C) young and (D) older participants. X, Y Z coordinates given in voxel coordinates, in radiological orientation. L/R Covariates of interest Brain Region Voxels t p x y z A) YOUNG vs. OLDER Bilateral None Striatal-Cortical-Hippocampal 43200 6.98 0.0002* 46 82 47

0.002 0.002

35 53

39 47

25 15

439 439 245 11 22 23 225

6.13 5.40 5.22 4.63 4.30 4.7 4.69

0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001

61 65 52 66 33 33 59

47 52 43 35 66 66 29

21 27 55 64 66 67 26

42 9 4

2.16 2.1 2.17

0.018 0.029 0.030

53 28 61

44 40 50

12 24 28

918 10 90 16

2.68 2.16 2.17 1.94

0.006 0.009 0.002 0.017

62 53 61 62

34 44 50 52

20 12 28 28

MSL & spindle conjunction: Left MSL & total spindle duration Right MSL & total spindle duration Left MSL & total spindle duration

M AN U

SC

3.89 3.01

C) YOUNG: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES

TE D

MSL & spindle conjunction: Bilateral MSL & total spindle duration Bilateral MSL & total spindle duration Left MSL & total spindle duration Bilateral MSL & fast spindle density

Cerebellum (crus I, IV) Cerebellum (IX) Hippocampus Hippocampus

No significant voxels

EP

D) OLDER: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES

AC C

Results from total spindles from Cz, slow spindles from Fz and fast spindles from Pz presented. Note: * indicates FWE-corrected p-value significant at p < 0.05. Coordinates for side where effect was maximal reported when bilateral.

988 989

43

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT HIGHLIGHTS Older individuals had intact motor sequence learning but impaired offline gains



Grey matter related to spindles and offline gains in young vs. older individuals



Spindles may serve as a biological marker for age-related memory deficits

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT



ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

RI PT

1. All authors disclose that: (a) There are no actual or potential conflicts of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other people or organizations within three years of beginning the work submitted that could inappropriately influence (bias) their work. (b)The author's institutions have no contracts relating to this research through which it or any other organization may stand to gain financially now or in the future. (c) No other agreements of authors or their institutions that could be seen as involving a financial interest in this work. 2. This research was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health research (CIHR) grant to author JD and fellowship support to author SF from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec (FRSQ).

SC

3. The results reported in the manuscript being submitted have not been previously published, have not been submitted elsewhere and will not be submitted elsewhere while under consideration at Neurobiology of Aging.

M AN U

4. Ethical and scientific approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee at the “Institut de Geriatrie de Montreal”, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

AC C

EP

TE D

5. All authors have had the opportunity to review the contents of the manuscript being submitted, approve of its contents and validate the accuracy of the data.