Accepted Manuscript Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation S. Fogel, C. Vien, A. Karni, H. Benali, J. Carrier, J. Doyon PII:
S0197-4580(16)30249-4
DOI:
10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.009
Reference:
NBA 9748
To appear in:
Neurobiology of Aging
Received Date: 16 March 2016 Revised Date:
8 September 2016
Accepted Date: 3 October 2016
Please cite this article as: Fogel, S, Vien, C., Karni, A., Benali, H., Carrier, J, Doyon, J, Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation, Neurobiology of Aging (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.009. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1
Sleep spindles: A physiological marker of age-related changes in grey matter in brain
2
regions supporting motor skill memory consolidation
3
RI PT
Fogel S,a,b,c Vien, C.,a,b Karni, A.,d Benali, H.,a,e Carrier Ja,b,f & Doyon Ja,b a
SC
Functional Neuroimaging Unit, Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Montreal, Canada; bDepartment of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada; cSchool of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada; dLaboratory for Human Brain & Learning, Sagol Department of Neurobiology & the E.J. Safra Brain Research Center, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel; eFunctional Neuroimaging Laboratory, INSERM, France; fCentre d’études Avancées en Médecine du Sommeil, Hôpital du Sacré-Cœur de Montréal, Montreal, Canada.
Email:
[email protected] Phone: 1-514-340-2800 x 3284 Fax: 1-514-340-3530
M AN U
Corresponding Author: Julien Doyon, Ph.D. Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Montreal Director, Functional Neuroimaging Unit Centre de Recherche, Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal 4545 Chemin Queen Mary Montreal, Quebec, Canada, H3W 1W5
TE D
4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26
EP
27
AC C
28
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ABSTRACT
30
Sleep is necessary for the optimal consolidation of procedural learning, and in particular, for
31
motor sequential skills. Motor sequence learning (MSL) remains intact with age, but sleep-
32
dependent consolidation is impaired, suggesting that memory deficits for procedural skills are
33
specifically impacted by age-related changes in sleep. Age-related changes in spindles may
34
be responsible for impaired MSL consolidation, but the morphological basis for this deficit is
35
unknown. Here we found that grey matter in the hippocampus and cerebellum was positively
36
correlated with both sleep spindles and offline improvements in performance in young
37
participants, but not in older participants. These results suggest that age-related changes in
38
grey matter in the hippocampus relate to spindles, and may underlie age-related deficits in
39
sleep-related motor sequence memory consolidation. In this way, spindles can serve as a
40
biological marker for structural brain changes and the related memory deficits in older adults.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
29
41 KEYWORDS
43
Sleep, spindle, age, memory, learning, consolidation, motor sequence learning, motor skills,
44
procedural memory, hippocampus, cerebellum
EP
45
TE D
42
AC C
46
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 47
1. INTRODUCTION Alongside a healthy diet and regular exercise, sleep is one of the pillars of good
49
physical and mental health, and has important links to memory and cognition (e.g., see
50
(Rasch and Born, 2013), for a recent comprehensive review). However, as we age, the
51
quantity and quality of sleep are markedly reduced (Bixler et al., 1984; Buysse et al., 1992;
52
Carrier et al., 2011, 2001; Crowley et al., 2002; Darchia et al., 2003; Feinberg et al., 1967;
53
Floyd et al., 2007; Fogel et al., 2012; Gaudreau et al., 2001; Landolt and Borbely, 2001;
54
Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Peters et al., 2014; Redline et al., 2004; Zepelin and
55
McDonald, 1987), and recent studies suggest that the age-related changes in sleep may have
56
an impact on sleep-dependent memory processes (Fogel et al., 2014; Mander et al., 2015,
57
2013a, 2013b; Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011; Spencer et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2012). In
58
healthy young adults, non-rapid eye movement stage 2 (N2) sleep typically occupies about
59
45% to 55% of total sleep time (Carskadon and Dement, 2011). However, with age, the total
60
amount of N2 is reduced (Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Ramanand et al., 2010).
61
One of the main characteristic features of N2 sleep is the presence of sleep spindle (Iber et
62
al., 2007). Sleep spindles are brief bursts (<1 sec to ~3 sec) of oscillatory activity within the
63
sigma band, with a frequency of ~12-16 Hz and with a waxing and waning amplitude that is
64
discrete from the ongoing EEG (Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2007; Landolt et al., 1996;
65
Silber et al., 2007; Zeitlhofer et al., 1997). One of the most pronounced age-related changes
66
in N2 sleep is the reduction of sleep spindles marked by reduced duration, amplitude and
67
density (Bowersox et al., 1985; Cajochen et al., 2006; Carrier et al., 2001; Crowley et al.,
68
2002; Feinberg, 1974; Guazzelli et al., 1986; Huupponen et al., 2002; Knoblauch et al., 2005;
69
Landolt et al., 1996; Mander et al., 2013a; Martin et al., 2012; Nicolas et al., 2001; Peters et
70
al., 2014; Principe and Smith, 1982). However, the age-related changes in brain structure that
71
underlie age-related reduction in spindles, and their functional consequences, remain to be
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
48
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 72
fully elucidated. Animal studies of the neural circuitry that supports the generation of sleep spindles
74
have shown that spindles originate from rhythmic depolarization of thalamocortical neurons
75
(Steriade, 2006), modulated by GABAergic pathways in the reticular nucleus of the thalamus
76
(Bazhenov et al., 2000, 1999). However, the brain structures involved in the generation and
77
regulation of spindles remains a topic of debate (Timofeev and Chauvette, 2013), as recent
78
evidence suggests that spindles are initiated cortically and that the neocortex contributes to
79
the termination of spindle events (Bonjean et al., 2011; Timofeev, 2001). In addition to the
80
thalamus, human functional neuroimaging studies employing simultaneous EEG-fMRI, have
81
revealed that brain regions involved in sensory processing are activated time-locked to sleep
82
spindles, including the anterior cingulate, insula and superior temporal gyri (Schabus et al.,
83
2007). Moreover, fast spindles have been associated with increased activity in sensorimotor
84
areas, the mesial frontal cortex and the hippocampus (Schabus et al., 2007) – the latter
85
findings being consistent with the putative role of fast spindles in declarative memory
86
processing. To further investigate the structural correlates of sleep spindles, Saletin et al
87
(Saletin et al., 2013) found that inter-individual differences in grey matter volume of the insula
88
and auditory cortex were related to slow spindles, whereas, grey matter volume in the
89
hippocampus was associated with fast spindles. Thus, the latter findings suggest that slow
90
and fast spindles serve a dissociable role for sleep maintenance and possibly memory
91
processing, respectively.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
73
92
Indeed, spindles are thought to serve a protective function by preventing potentially
93
arousing external stimuli such as loud noises that disrupt sleep (Cote et al., 2000; Dang-Vu et
94
al., 2010; Pivik et al., 1999; Schabus et al., 2012), although see (Crowley et al., 2004) for data
95
to suggest otherwise. In addition to their function for sleep maintenance via the gating of
96
intrusive sensory information, however, a rich literature now exists supporting the role of 4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT spindles for procedural memory including motor skills, reasoning and rule-learning (Albouy et
98
al., 2013a; Barakat et al., 2012, 2011; Fogel and Smith, 2011, 2006; Fogel et al., 2015, 2014;
99
Lafortune et al., 2014; Milner et al., 2006; Nielsen et al., 2014; Nishida and Walker, 2007;
100
Peters et al., 2007; Ramanathan et al., 2015; Smith and Smith, 2003; Tamaki et al., 2009,
101
2008; Wamsley et al., 2013, 2012; Wilhelm et al., 2012). Sleep is thought to enhance the
102
memory consolidation process via the interplay between the hippocampus and striatum
103
(Albouy et al., 2015, 2013a, 2013b, 2013c, 2008; Schabus et al., 2007) by strengthening
104
newly formed, labile memory traces for lasting long-term neocortical storage. Animal studies
105
have shown that during the spindle event, there is a large influx of intracellular calcium, which
106
is necessary to set in motion long-term potentiation (LTP) of synapses. In vivo (Bergmann et
107
al., 2008; Rosanova and Ulrich, 2005; Steriade, 2005; Werk et al., 2005) and in vitro
108
(Rosanova and Ulrich, 2005) studies have also shown that spaced and repeated stimulation
109
in the spindle oscillatory frequency range is necessary for cortical LTP to take place in the
110
neocortex. Thus, spindles are an ideal physiological candidate for neocortical memory
111
consolidation via memory trace reactivation.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
97
Recent human neuroimaging studies have shown that sleep spindle amplitude was
113
correlated with overnight consolidation in motor sequence skill (Barakat et al., 2012, 2011)
114
and that overnight changes in activation of brain regions including the striatum and motor
115
cortical regions, recruited during motor sequence learning (MSL) (Debas et al., 2010) were
116
correlated with post-training spindle amplitude (Barakat et al., 2012). EEG-fMRI studies have
117
also identified activations in the striatum (Caporro et al., 2011; Tyvaert et al., 2008) and
118
hippocampus (Schabus et al., 2007) associated with spindle events, hence suggesting that
119
spindles are involved in the activation of structures important for sleep-dependent motor skill
120
memory consolidation. Taken together, this literature suggests that spindles play an active
121
role in sleep-dependent memory consolidation via related interaction between striatal,
AC C
EP
112
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 122
hippocampal and cortical memory systems, and may ultimately result in strengthening of
123
memory traces. The functional consequences of age-related changes in sleep on memory
125
consolidation are beginning to be better appreciated. Age-related changes in sleep have been
126
shown to have a negative impact on overnight memory consolidation of some procedural
127
skills (Brown et al., 2009; Spencer et al., 2007), and spindles, in particular, have been related
128
to this deficit (Fogel et al., 2014; Peters et al., 2008). Age-related characteristics of spindles,
129
including duration, amplitude, and density, decrease with age (Carrier et al., 2001; Feinberg,
130
1974; Martin et al., 2012; Peters et al., 2014). Taken together, these studies suggest that an
131
age-related reduction in spindle activity may help explain age-related, sleep-dependent
132
deficits in memory consolidation. In support of this notion, a recent functional neuroimaging
133
study by our group has shown that reduced spindles in the elderly are associated with a
134
reduction of the spindle-related, overnight changes in cerebral activity in structures important
135
for skill learning (e.g., putamen and cerebellum in young, but only the cerebellum in older
136
individuals) (Fogel et al., 2014). Thus, a spindle-related, sleep-dependent increase of activity
137
in structures such as the putamen and cerebellum appears to be crucial for off-line
138
improvements, for which older individuals do not derive the same benefit due to an age-
139
related reduction of sleep spindles. However, it is not known if changes in brain structure that
140
accompany normal, healthy aging may underlie this deficit, and whether this is related to an
141
age-related reduction of sleep spindles.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
124
142
In terms of brain structures, adults who learn to play a musical instrument from a young
143
age have been found to have increased grey matter in brain regions that support motor skill
144
learning (e.g., hippocampus, putamen motor cortex, somatosensory cortex, superior parietal
145
cortex and the cerebellum)(Gaser and Schlaug, 2003; Groussard et al., 2014; Vaquero et al.,
146
2016). However, changes in grey matter have been observed over much shorter time periods. 6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Motor sequence learning is associated with grey matter changes in the cerebellum, motor
148
cortex, premotor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Gryga et al., 2012), and the earliest
149
detectable signs of grey matter changes with motor skill training (over 3 days) have been
150
observed in the striatum (Hamzei et al., 2012). These structural changes in the striatum were
151
related to grey matter changes in motor cortical regions at later stages of motor skill
152
performance, with an increase in functional coupling between motor and striatal regions over
153
the same time period. However, it is not known if age-related changes in brain structures
154
relate to age-related changes in sleep and the resulting memory deficits. Here, we
155
investigated the age-related differences in grey matter (GM) assessed by voxel-based
156
morphometry that were correlated with both sleep spindles and sleep-dependent changes in
157
motor skills performance.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
147
We hypothesized that GM in structures that support MSL consolidation (e.g.,
159
cerebellum, striatum, hippocampus, motor cortical regions), would be correlated with both
160
offline changes in MSL performance and spindles in young participants. Yet in older
161
participants, the results of a recent functional neuroimaging study from our group suggests
162
that the correlation between brain activation during practice of a motor sequence task and
163
sleep spindles deteriorates with age (Fogel et al., 2014). Accordingly, if structural deficits
164
underlie (or are linked to) the functional deficits observed with age, then a similar reduction in
165
the magnitude of the relationship between spindles and grey matter should be expected.
166
Thus this study aimed to ascertain if structural brain changes in grey matter were associated
167
with sleep spindles and memory deficits in older adults. The latter findings are important as
168
they may lead to novel insights into the neurological basis for age-related memory deficits,
169
and highlight the importance of good sleep quality later in life, and perhaps even novel
170
treatments or interventions to improve sleep in elderly populations in order to derive a benefit
171
to memory and related mitigate cognitive impairment.
AC C
EP
TE D
158
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 172 173
2. METHODS
174
2.1. Participants All participants provided informed written consent before entering the study. Ethical
176
and scientific approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee at the “Institut de Geriatrie de
177
Montreal”, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. In total, 22 young participants and 34 older
178
participants were recruited to participate in the study that met the inclusion and exclusion
179
criteria (see section 2.1.1). The results presented here were part of a larger series of studies
180
investigating both the functional and structural basis of age-related changes of sleep on
181
memory consolidation (Fogel et al., 2014; Vien et al., n.d.).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
175
2.1.1. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria.
183
Eligibility was determined at the start of the experiment through both a telephone pre-
184
screening interview and a battery of questionnaires. To be included in the study, participants
185
had to be right-handed (scored > 40 on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield,
186
1971)), non-smokers, prescription medication-free, and with a normal body mass index (≤ 25).
187
Also, they must not have been diagnosed with a neurological, psychological or psychiatric
188
condition, nor with a sleep disorder. "Extreme morning" or "extreme evening" types were also
189
excluded (defined as scoring > 70 "moderate morning" or < 30 "moderate evening",
190
respectively on the Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Scale (Horne and Ostberg,
191
1976)). By contrast, participants who were not formally trained on a musical instrument, did
192
not work night-shifts and who had not taken a trans-meridian trip ≤ 3 months before the study
193
were included in the study. Participants scoring ≤ 8 on both the Beck Depression Scale (Beck
194
et al., 1974) and the Beck Anxiety Scale (Beck et al., 1988) were also included, as did older
195
participants who scored ≥ 24 on the Mini Mental State Examination (Cockrell and Folstein,
196
1988; Tombaugh and McIntyre, 1992). Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol,
AC C
EP
TE D
182
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT nicotine, caffeine, and daytime naps during the study. A regular sleep-wake cycle, defined by
198
a bed-time between 10:00 PM and 1:00 AM and wake-time between 07:00 AM and 10:00 AM,
199
was required to be included. Compliance with sleep-wake instructions was verified with
200
actigraphy measured from the left wrist, and with a sleep journal (using a modified version of
201
the National Sleep Foundation "Sleep Diary" http://sleepfoundation.org).
RI PT
197
2.1.2. Polysomnographic and Additional Screening.
203
If participants met the above criteria, a 90-min acclimatization nap was conducted at
204
1:00 PM, three days before the experimental sessions. To ensure that participants were able
205
to sleep in the laboratory environment during the daytime, participants had to achieve at least
206
5 minutes of consolidated NREM sleep during this nap opportunity to be included in the study.
207
An overnight polysomnographic (PSG) screening night was used in older participants to
208
exclude individuals who exhibited signs of sleep apnea (e.g., apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) >
209
5) and periodic limb movements (PLM; e.g., PLM index > 10). This acclimatization and
210
screening procedure provided participants extensive opportunity to become accustomed to
211
sleeping in the laboratory environment, and thus to minimize the impact on sleep quality
212
during the experimental testing sessions. As a result, 19 older participants were excluded in
213
total; 10 due to an elevated AHI or PLM index, 2 for sleeping < 5 min during the
214
acclimatization nap, and 2 for MRI safety concerns. Five voluntarily withdrew before
215
completing the study. In addition, 7 young participants were excluded; 6 voluntarily withdrew
216
and 1 subject was excluded due to a structural abnormality that was uncovered upon
217
examination of their structural T1-weighted scan. The final participant pool that completed the
218
study was comprised of 15 young (Y) participants (8 female) between 19 and 30 years of age
219
(M = 23.9, SD = 3.5) and 15 older (O) participants (9 female) between 55 and 69 years (M =
220
62.2, SD = 3.8). However, an additional 2 participants were excluded from data analyses in
221
the group of young participants. One participant reportedly suffered from severe sleepiness in
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
202
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT the last five blocks of training and succumbed to extreme tiredness during practice, thus
223
producing lapses in performance during the training session. A second subject performed
224
poorly on the MSL task (as this participant was outside the 99% confidence interval for all 14
225
blocks) and did not comply well with the instructions given throughout the experiment. The
226
remaining 13 young participants (6 female, M = 24.1, SD = 3.5 years old) were included in the
227
data analyses.
228
2.2. Overall Experimental Design
SC
RI PT
222
Participants arrived at the laboratory for 10:30 AM. The Stanford Sleepiness Scale
230
(SSS) (MacLean et al., 1992) and Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) (Dinges and Powell,
231
1985) were then administered to assess both subjective and objective sleepiness,
232
respectively, before MSL practice. At 11:00 AM, the MSL task was performed with alternating
233
periods of rest, in a block design, at the same time functional blood oxygen level dependent
234
(BOLD) imaging data were acquired (see (Fogel et al., 2014) for corresponding behavioural
235
and functional neuroimaging results in these participants). After being provided a light lunch,
236
participants were given a 90-min nap interval commencing at 1:00 PM. The MSL retest
237
session was administered at 4:00 PM, that is 1.5 h after the end of the 90-min experimental
238
interval to reduce possible effects of sleep inertia.
239
2.3. Finger Sequence Task
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
229
240
2.3.1. Task Instructions.
241
An adapted version of the finger sequence task developed by Karni (Karni et al., 1995)
242
was used to assess MSL in young and older participants across an interval of sleep (i.e., a
243
90-minute daytime nap opportunity). An MR-compatible response box (model HH-1x4-L;
244
Current Designs, Inc., Philadelphia PA) with a row of four equidistant push buttons was used.
245
To familiarize each subject with the task, the experimenter first demonstrated the use of the
246
keypad and explained the on-screen appearance of the task and instructions. Participants 10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT were then positioned supine in the MR scanner. Before the start of the training session, all
248
participants were asked to “slowly and accurately practice the 5-item sequence "4-1-3-2-4",
249
where 1 stands for the index finger and 4 for the little finger of the left hand” until they were
250
capable of performing the sequence three times in a row, without error. This ensured
251
comprehension of instructions, memorization of the sequence, and that the sequence could
252
be performed reliably prior to the start of the scanning session.
253
2.3.2. Training and Retest Practice Sessions.
254
During the training and retest sessions, the exact same sequence of finger movements
255
was to be performed, but participants were explicitly instructed "to perform the sequence by
256
pressing the buttons as quickly as possible, while making as few errors as possible". The
257
beginning of each practice block was indicated by the appearance of a green cross in the
258
center of the screen. After 60 key presses, the green cross turned red for 15 seconds,
259
indicating that the subject should stop practicing and rest. There were 14 practice blocks in
260
both training and retest sessions. If participants realized that they had made an error, they
261
were instructed to continue practicing at the start of the 5-item sequence. This ensured that
262
errors were isolated events within the train of 60 key presses and encouraged participants to
263
continue practicing the task despite making an error. To minimize age-related differences in
264
reaction time, the task was un-cued, (i.e., self-initiated). Performance was measured by key
265
press speed (defined as the average correct inter-key-press interval). All participants included
266
in the data analysis achieved an accuracy > 83.3%, equivalent to 10/12 correct sequences
267
per block. Offline changes in performance across the nap interval were calculated as the
268
mean performance of the first 4 blocks of retest minus the mean of the last 4 blocks of
269
training. This measure of consolidation was then used in the behavioral analyses, and as a
270
covariate of interest in the MRI analyses.
271
2.4. PSG Recording and Analysis
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
247
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT PSG data was recorded from 10 scalp derivations (F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4,
273
and Oz) using a Brain Products GmbH (Gilching, Germany) 16-channel, V-Amp16 system
274
(high pass filter = 0.3 Hz, low pass filter = 70 Hz). Signals were digitalized at 500 samples/s.
275
During the overnight polysomnographic screening in older participants, a nasal/oral thermistor
276
and respiratory effort belt recordings were used to screen for sleep apnea. Leg
277
electromyographic (EMG) recordings were used to screen participants for periodic leg
278
movements.
SC
RI PT
272
Sleep scoring (Table 1) was done visually in 20-s epochs of EEG (high pass filter = 0.5
280
Hz, low pass filter = 35 Hz) from central and occipital derivations (C3, C4, and Oz) re-
281
referenced to average mastoids (A1 and A2). Electrooculographic (EOG; high pass filter = 0.5
282
Hz, low pass filter = 35 Hz) activity was recorded from the lateral outer canthus of the eyes.
283
Bipolar submental EMG (high pass filter = 10 Hz) was also recorded. Sleep stages were
284
scored by an expert PSG technologist, following standards of the American Academy of
285
Sleep Medicine [Iber, 2007] using the "fMRI Artifact rejection and Sleep Scoring Toolbox
286
(FASST)" (ver. 0.302; http://www.montefiore.ulg.ac.be/~phillips/FASST.html) for Matlab
287
(Mathworks, Natick, MA; (Leclercq et al., 2011)). Movement continuously exceeding 100 µV
288
for > 100 ms during sleep was automatically detected and identified as bad data. This was
289
later confirmed visually and this data was excluded from further analyses.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
279
290
Sleep spindles were detected from Fz, Cz, and Pz in NREM sleep using a an
291
established (Albouy et al, 2014; Fogel et al, 2014) and validated method (Ray et al., 2015)
292
implemented in Brain Products (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) Analyzer software
293
(Version 2.1). For details of this method more recently implemented in Matlab, see Ray et al
294
(Ray et al., 2015). Given that previous studies have suggested that age-related differences
295
vary systematically along the midline (Martin et al., 2012), recordings from Fz, Cz, and Pz
296
were used to derive the number of spindles during NREM sleep amplitude (µV2), duration (s), 12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT and peak frequency (Hz). Slow and fast spindles have a known topography (De Gennaro and
298
Ferrara, 2003; De Gennaro et al., 2005, 2000; Dehghani et al., 2011; Doran, 2003;
299
Huupponen et al., 2002; Jobert et al., 1992; Werth et al., 1997a, 1997b; Zeitlhofer et al.,
300
1997) whereby slow spindles predominate anterior regions and fast spindles predominate
301
posterior regions. A frequency range for total bandwidth spindles of 11-17Hz was employed
302
so as to encompass spindles defined as 12-16Hz (Ferrarelli et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2014,
303
2007; Landolt et al., 1996; Zeitlhofer et al., 1997). Slow (11-14 Hz) spindles, and fast (14-17
304
Hz) spindles were categorized depending on their peak frequency in the same way as a
305
previously published study on the relationship between age-related changes in sleep spindles
306
and functional brain activation (Fogel et al., 2014) and also based on a thorough validation of
307
this detection procedure (Ray et al., 2015). This procedure ensures that slow and fast
308
spindles are orthogonal to one another at any given derivation. Thus, spindles from the
309
derivations for which they predominate (e.g., slow spindles from Fz and fast spindles from Pz)
310
were included in the structural imaging regression analyses (Table 2).
311
2.5. Brain Imaging Data Acquisition and Analysis
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
297
2.5.1. MRI Sequence Parameters.
313
A 3.0T TIM TRIO MRI system (Siemens, Germany) with a 12-channel head coil was
314
used. A 3D MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, TI = 900 ms, FA = 9°, 176
315
slices, FoV = 256 x 256 mm2, matrix size = 256 x 256 x 176, voxel size = 1 x 1 x 1 mm3) was
316
used to obtain high resolution T1-weighted anatomical images for all participants used for
317
subsequent VBM analyses.
AC C
EP
312
318
2.5.2. Preprocessing.
319
Structural
data was
analyzed
with
FSL
(Douaud
et
al.,
2007)
(but
see
320
http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLVBM for a detailed description of analysis procedures
321
employed here) using an optimized VBM protocol (Good et al., 2001a; Smith et al., 2004) 13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT described below. First, structural images were brain-extracted and grey matter-segmented
323
before being registered to the MNI 152 standard space using non-linear registration
324
(Andersson et al., 2007). Second, a study-specific grey matter (GM) template was created
325
with the FSL-VBM protocol. All brain-extracted images were segmented into GM, WM and
326
CSF. Then, GM images were affine-registered to the GM ICBM-152 template, concatenated
327
and averaged. Next, the GM images were then re-registered to this first-pass "affine" GM
328
template using non-linear registration, concatenated into a 4D image, averaged and flipped
329
along the x-axis. Both mirror images were averaged to create the final symmetric, study-
330
specific "non-linear" GM template based on an equal number of participants per group at
331
2x2x2mm3 resolution in standard space. Third, all native grey matter images were non-
332
linearly registered to the study-specific template generated in the previous step, and
333
modulated to correct for local expansion (or contraction) due to the non-linear component of
334
the spatial transformation (see Good et al., 2001). The modulated grey matter images were
335
then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 3 mm.
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
322
2.5.3. Statistical Analysis.
337
To investigate age-related differences in GM, voxelwise general linear modeling (GLM)
338
was applied using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) permutation-based non-
339
parametric testing using 10,000 permutations (Nichols and Holmes, 2002; Winkler et al.,
340
2014), for: 1) young, 2) older, and 3) young – older participants. This procedure generated
341
maps of t-statistics and maps of corresponding uncorrected p-values and Family-Wise Error
342
(FWE) corrected p-value maps.
AC C
EP
336
343
Total intracranial volume (TIV) was means-centered and included as a variable of non-
344
interest in the models. TIV was obtained using FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012) by linearly
345
aligning each subject brain extracted image to the MNI152 space, computing the inverse of
346
the determinant of the affine matrix ((Sargolzaei et al., 2015); see ENIGMA protocol: 14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT http://enigma.ini.usc.edu) and taking the sum of the gray matter and white matter partitions
348
extracted using an automated brain extraction tool (BET) (Smith, 2002) and segmentation
349
using FAST (Zhang et al., 2001). Offline changes in MSL performance and each spindle
350
parameter (density, amplitude, duration) were entered as mean-centered covariates of
351
interest into separate GLMs to investigate brain regions where GM correlated to a different
352
extent in young and older subjects with offline changes in MSL performance and sleep
353
spindles.
SC
RI PT
347
In addition, to co-localize differences in grey matter in young and older participants
355
which correlated with both offline changes in performance and sleep spindle characteristics,
356
the conjunction of performance-related GM t-maps and spindle-related GM t-maps was taken
357
as the minimum t-statistic using the global null hypothesis (Friston et al., 2005). The resulting
358
conjunction maps identify brain regions where GM was correlated to a different extent in
359
young and older (and young vs. older) participants with both offline changes in performance
360
and spindles, over: 1) the performance-correlated statistical parametric maps (SPM) in young
361
and older participants with 2) each spindle-correlated map (e.g., density, amplitude, duration)
362
in young and older participants (e.g., young, older and young vs. older). The conjunction
363
maps were thresholded so that results were significant at a combined p-level of p < 0.001
364
(uncorrected). Clusters that were significant at p < 0.05 (FWE corrected) were also indicated
365
in the results. Note that a significant conjunction does not necessarily mean all the contrasts
366
were individually significant (i.e., a conjunction of significance). Instead it indicates that the
367
effects were consistently high across all conditions included in the conjunction, such that they
368
were jointly statistically significant (Friston et al., 2005). Note that the minimum t-values do not
369
have the usual Student’s t-distribution and small minimum t-values can be highly significant.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
354
370 371
3. RESULTS 15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 372
3.1. MSL Behavioral Results Holm-Bonferonni corrected independent t-tests revealed that young participants had
374
significantly higher offline improvements in performance than older participants (t(26) = 3.87,
375
p = 0.001; Figure 1). Similarly, one sample t-tests showed that young participants offline
376
performance improvements were significantly different from zero (t(12) = 4.32, p = 0.004),
377
while the older participants performance did not significantly change (t(14) = -1.47, p = 0.163).
378
A previous report by our group indicated that offline performance improvements were
379
observed in young participants only, and only after a period of sleep, but not after a wake
380
period (Fogel et al., 2014), hence suggesting that sleep supported offline consolidation of
381
MSL in young, but not older participants.
SC
M AN U
******************************** INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE ********************************
AC C
EP
TE D
382
RI PT
373
383 384 385
Figure 1. Time between key presses (A) decreased over the course of the 14 blocks in the training session at the same rate for young and older participants. Young participants, but not 16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 386 387 388 389 390 391 392
older participants performance was then enhanced following a nap during the retest session, as indicated by (B) significant offline improvements in performance from the last 4 blocks of the training session to the first four blocks of the retest session. Note: Young participants performance in panel A scaled along the right-hand y-axis to illustrate similarity between young and older participants in MSL.
393
Both subjective and objective levels of sleepiness were measured before the MSL
394
training and retest sessions. A main effect of session revealed that subjective (F(1,26) = 8.47,
395
p = 0.007) and objective (F(1,26) = 8.63, p = 0.007) sleepiness was reduced following the
396
nap. There was no main effect of age group, nor a session by age group interaction for the
397
objective or subjective sleepiness scores (all p > 0.05). The latter findings suggest that sleep
398
during a daytime nap reduced sleepiness consistently across young and older participants,
399
but that sleepiness or vigilance alone did not affect MSL performance differentially across
400
groups, or explain group differences in MSL performance.
401
3.3. Sleep Architecture
M AN U
SC
RI PT
3.2. Subjective and Objective Sleepiness
As expected, young and older participants differed with respect to the sleep
403
characteristics measured during the 90-min daytime nap (Table 1). Older participants spent
404
significantly more time awake (t(26) = 3.31, p = 0.004). This was reflected by an increase in
405
wake time after sleep onset (t(26) = 2.77, p = 0.012), a decrease in total sleep time (t(26) =
406
3.23, p = 0.004) and a reduced sleep efficiency (t(26) = 3.06, p = 0.006). Older participants
407
had significantly less NREM sleep (t(26) = 2.76, p = 0.012) than young participants, the latter
408
being most likely driven by the significant reduction of slow wave sleep (SWS) as a percent of
409
total sleep time (t(26) = 2.09, p = 0.047). By contrast, N1 and REM sleep did not significantly
410
differ between young and older participants, hence indicating that sleep deficits in older
411
individuals were predominantly a result of changes in NREM sleep. The difference between
412
young and older participants for REM sleep did approach statistical significance (p < 0.10).
413
However, it should be noted that this was largely due to a floor effect, whereby 73% of older
AC C
EP
TE D
402
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT participants had no REM sleep at all, as compared to only 23% of young participants. Given
415
this unusual distribution, we also conducted non-parametric tests for REM sleep. However,
416
this revealed the same pattern of results, which did only approach statistical significance (U =
417
55.5, p > 0.05). Although this non-significant age difference is somewhat difficult to interpret, it
418
likely represents age-related difficulty in the initiation of REM sleep in a daytime nap, and not
419
necessarily REM duration per se. Due to the highly variable duration of N2 and an insufficient
420
quantity of SWS across participants in a daytime nap, N2 and SWS were collapsed into a
421
total measure of NREM sleep for spindle detection and analyses.
422
3.4. Sleep Spindles
M AN U
SC
RI PT
414
In addition to age-related differences in sleep architecture, older participants had
424
significantly shorter sleep spindle duration measured during NREM sleep at Fz (t(26) = 3.70,
425
p = 0.003), Cz (t(26) = 3.55, p = 0.002) and Pz(t(26) = 2.29, p = 0.030). In addition, their
426
spindles were significantly slower at Fz (t(26) = 3.33, p = 0.009; see Table 2). However, sleep
427
spindle density and amplitude were not significantly different between young and older
428
participants.
429
3.5. Spindle and MSL Behavioral Correlations
TE D
423
We conducted Pearson bivariate correlation tests to ascertain if sleep spindles were
431
linked to MSL performance improvements in young and older participants. These analyses
432
revealed that there were no significant correlations between spindles (density, amplitude and
433
duration) and offline performance improvements for the total spindles bandwidth at Cz, or
434
slow spindles at Fz for young and older participants (all p > 0.05). However, for fast spindles
435
at Pz, spindle density (r(11) = 0.60, p = 0.030), amplitude (r(11) = 0.614, p = 0.026) and
436
duration (r(11) = 0.69, p = 0.009) were significantly correlated with offline changes in MSL
437
performance in young participants. By contrast, spindle duration was negatively correlated
AC C
EP
430
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 438
with offline performance changes in older participants (r(13) = -0.67, p = 0.006) , whereby
439
larger spindle duration was associated with reduced offline changes in performance.
440
3.6. Structural MRI Results 3.6.1. GM differences in young vs. older participants:
442
As expected, greater GM was observed in young compared to older participants in a
443
widespread (n.b., the size of the significant striatal-cortical cluster in Table 3A) set of brain
444
regions bilaterally including the somatosensory, sensorimotor, premotor, frontal, dorsolateral,
445
medial prefrontal, cingulate, supplementary motor area, parietal, stratum and hippocampus
446
(Figure 2C).
M AN U
3.6.2. GM differences correlate with offline changes in performance and spindles
447 448
SC
RI PT
441
in young and older participants:
GM in young and older subjects correlated to a different extent with offline changes in
450
performance in the cerebellum (lobules Crus1, IV, IX; Table 3B). In addition, GM in young
451
and older subjects correlated to a different extent with total spindle duration in the cerebellum,
452
the hippocampus as well as the cingulate and parietal cortices (Table 3B). Slow spindle
453
amplitude and duration also correlated to a different extent in young and older adults with GM
454
in the supplementary motor area, and for fast spindle duration in the cerebellum (Table 3B).
EP
TE D
449
A conjunction analysis was used to identify regions where GM was correlated to a
456
different extent in young vs. older adults with both offline changes in performance and spindle
457
parameters (density, duration and amplitude) for slow, fast and total bandwidth spindles. In
458
young, but not older participants, GM in the hippocampus (bilaterally) and cerebellum were
459
positively correlated with both offline changes in performance and total spindle duration
460
(Table 3C; Figure 2). Furthermore, GM correlated with spindle duration to a different extent
461
when directly comparing young vs. older participants in these same regions (Table 3B).
AC C
455
462 19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT ******************************** INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE *********************************
M AN U
SC
RI PT
463
464
Figure 2. Upper: Significant clusters of grey matter (GM) positively correlated with offline changes in performance in conjunction with Cz spindle duration in the hippocampus and cerebellum for young (red) participants (see Table 3C): GM in the hippocampus was (A) positively correlated with offline changes in performance and (B) spindle duration in young (filled circles) but not older (open circles) participants, which was (C) also higher on average for young as compared to older participants (see Table 3A). This difference in GM between young and older subjects correlated with both spindle duration and offline changes in MSL performance in the hippocampus and cerebellum (Table 3B). Results significant at p<0.001, uncorrected. Note: Only significant clusters and scatterplots shown for Cz spindles duration, as these clusters overlapped completely with significant regions for other spindle parameters (amplitude, density) for either fast or slow spindles (see Table 3 for details). X, Y Z coordinates given in voxel coordinates, displayed in radiological orientation.
479
Over the course of normal, healthy aging, sleep quantity and quality is reduced (Bixler
480
et al., 1984; Buysse et al., 1992; Carrier et al., 2011, 2001; Crowley et al., 2002; Darchia et
481
al., 2003; Feinberg et al., 1967; Floyd et al., 2007; Fogel et al., 2012; Gaudreau et al., 2001;
482
Landolt and Borbely, 2001; Martin et al., 2012; Ohayon et al., 2004; Peters et al., 2014;
483
Redline et al., 2004; Zepelin and McDonald, 1987). The functional consequences of this for
AC C
EP
TE D
465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478
4. DISCUSSION
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT learning and memory are just starting to become clear. Specifically, recent studies have
485
shown that for simple, explicitly known motor sequences, older adults are able to learn at a
486
normal rate. Yet their ability to consolidate the memory trace compared to younger individuals
487
is impaired (Fogel et al., 2014; Mander et al., 2015, 2013a, 2013b; Pace-Schott and Spencer,
488
2011; Spencer et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2012). Interestingly, the preservation of MSL during
489
training in older adults appears to be specific to explicitly known sequences. By contrast,
490
implicit probabilistic motor sequence learning is impaired with age (Howard and Howard,
491
2013; Simon et al., 2011), and a growing body of literature suggests that sleep may not be
492
involved in the enhancement of implicit motor skills, even in younger adults (Nemeth et al.,
493
2010). These results suggest that while explicit MSL is unaffected by age, age has an impact
494
on the offline consolidation/stabilization of the newly learned explicit motor skill. In other
495
words, older individuals do not derive the same benefit as younger individuals from sleep for
496
the consolidation of newly learned explicit motor skills.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
484
In terms of brain functional activation related to MSL and sleep-dependent
498
consolidation, recent work from our group, in the same set of participants reported here, has
499
revealed that a spindle-related reduction of activity in the putamen, cerebellum, parietal and
500
temporal cortex may underlie this age-related, sleep-dependent memory deficit (Fogel et al.,
501
2014). Consistent with this finding, others have demonstrated that grey matter is also reduced
502
with normal aging, with the most pronounced changes in the thalamus, hippocampus,
503
striatum, frontal cortex, motor cortical regions and cingulate cortex (Fjell and Walhovd, 2010;
504
Good et al., 2001b; Gunning-Dixon et al., 1998; Raz et al., 2015, 2005). Here we investigated
505
if the changes in brain structure related to spindles and offline motor sequence memory
506
consolidation may help to explain why older adults do not derive the same benefit from sleep
507
as younger adults for the consolidation of motor skills. More specifically, we explored whether
508
age-related reductions of grey matter integrity relate to spindle characteristics (e.g., density,
AC C
EP
TE D
497
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT amplitude, duration) and overnight, sleep-dependent offline improvements in motor skill
510
performance. We found that a widespread network of brain regions had reduced GM in older
511
compared to young participants, including motor cortical regions, the hippocampus, striatum,
512
cingulate, dorsolateral, medial prefrontal, temporal, parietal, frontal regions of the neocortex.
513
Interestingly, only age-related differences in GM in the cerebellum were correlated with offline
514
changes in MSL performance following a daytime nap. By contrast, sleep spindles correlated
515
with age-related changes in GM in a well-known set of brain regions which support motor
516
sequence memory consolidation (Doyon and Benali, 2005; Doyon and Ungerleider, 2002;
517
Doyon et al., 2003) including the hippocampus, parietal, cerebellum and supplementary motor
518
area. However, when both offline changes in MSL performance and sleep spindles were
519
considered together, grey matter in the hippocampus and cerebellum was correlated to a
520
greater extent in young vs. older subjects with both sleep spindles and sleep-related changes
521
in performance. Thus, suggesting that sleep spindles may serve as markers for age-related
522
changes in GM in the hippocampus and cerebellum, regions which support sleep-related
523
memory consolidation for motor skills. These results are in line with previous work showing
524
that sleep-dependent consolidation of skills are related to increased functional activation of
525
the cortico-striatal-hippocampal and cortico-cerebellar network in young healthy adults
526
(Albouy et al., 2015; Debas et al., 2010; Fogel et al., 2014; Walker et al., 2005). This is also
527
consistent with our recent work in the same set of participants as the current study, showing
528
that sleep had a differential effect on functional brain activity from the MSL training session to
529
the retest session (retest-training) in the hippocampus and cerebellum between young and
530
older adults (Fogel et al., 2014). More specifically, young adults that slept and older adults
531
that were awake had increased activations, whereas young adults that were awake and older
532
adults that slept had decreased BOLD activity in the hippocampus, cerebellum and cortical
533
regions recruited during MSL (e.g., parietal and frontal cortex). This pattern of activity
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
509
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT paralleled the offline consolidation of MSL performance, thus suggesting that increased
535
hippocampal activity supports memory consolidation in young but not older adults. The
536
current study extends these results, providing the first evidence that structural grey matter
537
differences in regions correlated with sleep spindles may underlie age-related motor skill
538
memory consolidation deficits. Moreover, our results suggest that age-related GM atrophy in
539
regions such as the cerebellum and hippocampus, in particular (Kennedy and Raz, 2005; Raz
540
et al., 2015) may underlie spindle-related motor sequence memory consolidation deficits.
SC
RI PT
534
Although still controversial, age-related changes in grey matter may affect spindle
542
generating mechanisms that depend on synchronization of the neocortex (Bonjean et al.,
543
2012; Contreras and Steriade, 1996; Timofeev and Chauvette, 2013). Decreased spindle size
544
and frequency may be the result of cortical cell loss associated with the aging process. It has
545
been suggested that age-related loss of neurons may lead to a reduction of large-scale
546
synchronization, thereby affecting the size and frequency of sleep spindles (Crowley et al.,
547
2002; Nicolas et al., 2001). Consistent with this suggestion and a previous study by our group
548
(Martin et al., 2012), it was observed that sleep spindle duration was reduced in older
549
participants for spindles compared to younger participants, although unlike the current study,
550
no age-difference was previously observed for spindle frequency. However, similar to Landolt
551
et al. (Landolt and Borbely, 2001; Landolt et al., 1996) slow spindles were slower at frontal
552
regions in older participants with no change in faster frequencies. It should be noted that the
553
method of spindle detection and characterization used in the present study (Ray et al., 2015)
554
does not employ a minimum duration criteria, as we have demonstrated that the majority of
555
spindles are below the minimum criteria conventionally used to visually identify spindles.
556
Despite this, no spindles below 0.2 sec were identified. However, this is an important
557
methodological consideration, since not including spindles <0.5sec, may bias or
558
underestimate age-related changes in spindles. Thus the results of the current study, suggest
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
541
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 559
that with age, the relationship between grey matter and spindles is negatively impacted, and
560
related to a reduction in the sleep-related benefit to memory consolidation. Consistent with previous reports (Fogel et al., 2014; Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011;
562
Spencer et al., 2007), and despite the fact that performance in older participants was slower
563
overall as compared to younger participants, older participants exhibited the same pattern of
564
increasing performance over the course of the training session as the young participants. This
565
suggests that despite slower motor execution, explicit motor sequence learning remains
566
largely intact with age except under greater task complexity and implicit MSL (Howard and
567
Howard, 2013; King et al., 2013; Rieckmann et al., 2010; Simon et al., 2011). However,
568
following a retention interval filled with sleep, older individuals do not derive the same benefit
569
as younger participants who experienced offline consolidation/stabilization (Fogel et al., 2014;
570
Pace-Schott and Spencer, 2011; Spencer et al., 2007).
M AN U
SC
RI PT
561
In conclusion, our results suggest that age-related changes in sleep spindles are
572
differentially related to grey matter in young and older individuals in brain regions that support
573
sleep-dependent memory consolidation for motor skills. In this way, sleep spindles can serve
574
as a biological marker for structural brain changes in grey matter and their associated
575
memory deficits in older adults.
EP
TE D
571
AC C
576 577
5. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
578
The authors would like to acknowledge the technical support of Vo An Nguyen, Laura Ray,
579
Andre Cyr, Carollyn Hurst, Frederic Jeay, and Amel Bouyoucef. This research was funded by
580
a Canadian Institutes of Health research (CIHR) grant to author JD and fellowship support to
581
author SF from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC),
582
the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec (FRSQ).
583 24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 584 585
6. REFERENCES
586
Albouy, G., Fogel, S., King, B.R., Laventure, S., Benali, H., Karni, A., Carrier, J., Robertson, E.M., Doyon, J., 2015. Maintaining vs. enhancing motor sequence memories: respective
588
roles of striatal and hippocampal systems. Neuroimage 108, 423–34.
589
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.049
Albouy, G., Fogel, S., Pottiez, H., Nguyen, V.A., Ray, L., Lungu, O., Carrier, J., Robertson, E.,
SC
590
RI PT
587
Doyon, J., 2013a. Daytime sleep enhances consolidation of the spatial but not motoric
592
representation of motor sequence memory. PLoS One 8, e52805.
593
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052805
594
M AN U
591
Albouy, G., King, B.R., Maquet, P., Doyon, J., 2013b. Hippocampus and striatum: Dynamics
595
and interaction during acquisition and sleep-related motor sequence memory
596
consolidation. Hippocampus 23, 985–1004. doi:10.1002/hipo.22183 Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle, G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T.T.,
TE D
597
Darsaud, A., Ruby, P., Luppi, P.-H., Degueldre, C., Peigneux, P., Luxen, A., Maquet, P.,
599
2008. Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor
600
sequence memory. Neuron 58, 261–272. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008 Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Darsaud, A., Gais, S., Rauchs, G., Desseilles, M.,
AC C
601
EP
598
602
Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Phillips, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P.,
603
2013c. Interaction between hippocampal and striatal systems predicts subsequent
604
consolidation of motor sequence memory. PLoS One 8, e59490.
605
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059490
606
Andersson, J., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., 2007. Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA2.
607 608
Barakat, M., Carrier, J., Debas, K., Lungu, O., Fogel, S., Vandewalle, G., Hoge, R.D., Bellec, 25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 609
P., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Doyon, J., 2012. Sleep spindles predict neural
610
and behavioral changes in motor sequence consolidation. Hum Brain Mapp 34, 2918–
611
2928. doi:10.1002/hbm.22116 Barakat, M., Doyon, J., Debas, K., Vandewalle, G., Morin, A., Poirier, G., Martin, N.,
RI PT
612
Lafortune, M., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Carrier, J., 2011. Fast and slow
614
spindle involvement in the consolidation of a new motor sequence. Behav. Brain Res.
615
217, 117–121. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.019
616
SC
613
Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M., Sejnowski, T., 2000. Spiking-bursting activity in the thalamic reticular nucleus initiates sequences of spindle oscillations in thalamic networks.
618
J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1076–1087.
M AN U
617
619
Bazhenov, M., Timofeev, I., Steriade, M., Sejnowski, T., 1999. Self-sustained rhythmic activity
620
in the thalamic reticular nucleus mediated by depolarizing GABAA receptor potentials.
621
Nat. Neurosci. 2, 168–174. doi:10.1038/5729
Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., Steer, R.A., 1988. An inventory for measuring clinical
TE D
622
anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56, 893–897.
623 624
Beck, A.T., Rial, W.Y., Rickels, K., 1974. Short form of depression inventory: cross-validation. Psychol Rep 34, 1184–1186.
Bergmann, T.O., Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Kaya-Yildiz, L., Born, J., Roman Siebner, H., 2008. A
AC C
626
EP
625
627
local signature of LTP- and LTD-like plasticity in human NREM sleep. Eur J Neurosci 27,
628
2241–2249. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06178.x
629
Bixler, E.O., Kales, A., Jacoby, J.A., Soldatos, C.R., Vela-Bueno, A., 1984. Nocturnal sleep
630
and wakefulness: effects of age and sex in normal sleepers. Int J Neurosci 23, 33–42.
631
Bonjean, M., Baker, T., Bazhenov, M., Cash, S., Halgren, E., Sejnowski, T., 2012.
632
Interactions between Core and Matrix Thalamocortical Projections in Human Sleep
633
Spindle Synchronization. J. Neurosci. 32, 5250–5263. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.614126
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 11.2012
634 635
Bonjean, M., Baker, T., Lemieux, M., Timofeev, I., Sejnowski, T., Bazhenov, M., 2011. Corticothalamic feedback controls sleep spindle duration in vivo. J Neurosci 31, 9124–
637
9134. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0077-11.2011
638
RI PT
636
Bowersox, S.S., Kaitin, K.I., Dement, W.C., 1985. EEG spindle activity as a function of age: relationship to sleep continuity. Brain Res. 334, 303–8.
639
Brown, R.M., Robertson, E., Press, D.Z., 2009. Sequence skill acquisition and off-line
641
learning in normal aging. PLoS One 4, e6683. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006683
SC
640
Buysse, D.J., Browman, K.E., Monk, T.H., Reynolds, C.F., Fasiczka, A.L., Kupfer, D.J., 1992.
643
Napping and 24-hour sleep/wake patterns in healthy elderly and young adults. J. Am.
644
Geriatr. Soc. 40, 779–786.
645
M AN U
642
Cajochen, C., Münch, M., Knoblauch, V., Blatter, K., Wirz-Justice, A., 2006. Age-related changes in the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep. Chronobiol Int 23,
647
461–474. doi:10.1080/07420520500545813
648
TE D
646
Caporro, M., Haneef, Z., Yeh, H.J., Lenartowicz, A., Buttinelli, C., Parvizi, J., Stern, J.M., 2011. Functional MRI of sleep spindles and K-complexes. Clin Neurophysiol 123, 303–
650
309. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.06.018 Carrier, J., Land, S., Buysse, D.J., Kupfer, D.J., Monk, T.H., 2001. The effects of age and
AC C
651
EP
649
652
gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20-60 years
653
old). Psychophysiology 38, 232–242.
654
Carrier, J., Viens, I., Poirier, G., Robillard, R., Lafortune, M., Vandewalle, G., Martin, N.,
655
Barakat, M., Paquet, J., Filipini, D., 2011. Sleep slow wave changes during the middle
656
years of life. Eur J Neurosci 33, 758–66. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07543.x
657
Carskadon, M.A., Dement, W.C., 2011. Normal Human Sleep: An Overview, in: Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. pp. 16–26. doi:10.1016/B978-1-4160-6645-3.00002-5
658 27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 659
Cockrell, J.R., Folstein, M.F., 1988. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Psychopharmacol Bull 24, 689–692.
660 661
Contreras, D., Steriade, M., 1996. Spindle oscillation in cats: the role of corticothalamic feedback in a thalamically generated rhythm. J. Physiol. 490 ( Pt 1, 159–79.
663
RI PT
662
Cote, K.A., Epps, T.M., Campbell, K., 2000. The role of the spindle in human information processing of high-intensity stimuli during sleep. J Sleep Res 9, 19–26.
664
Crowley, K., Trinder, J., Colrain, I.M., 2004. Evoked K-complex generation: the impact of
SC
665
sleep spindles and age. Clin Neurophysiol 115, 471–476.
666
Crowley, K., Trinder, J., Kim, Y., Carrington, M., Colrain, I.M., 2002. The effects of normal
M AN U
667 668
aging on sleep spindle and K-complex production. Clin Neurophysiol 113, 1615–1622.
669
doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00237-7
670
Dang-Vu, T.T., McKinney, S.M., Buxton, O.M., Solet, J.M., Ellenbogen, J.M., 2010. Spontaneous brain rhythms predict sleep stability in the face of noise. Curr Biol 20,
672
R626–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.032
Darchia, N., Campbell, I.G., Feinberg, I., 2003. Rapid eye movement density is reduced in the normal elderly. Sleep 26, 973–977.
674 675
De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., 2003. Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Med. Rev. 7, 423– 440.
AC C
676
EP
673
TE D
671
677
De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Bertini, M., 2000. Topographical distribution of spindles:
678
variations between and within nrem sleep cycles. Sleep Res Online 3, 155–160.
679
De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M., Vecchio, F., Curcio, G., Bertini, M., 2005. An
680
electroencephalographic fingerprint of human sleep. Neuroimage 26, 114–122.
681
doi:S1053-8119(05)00049-2 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.020
682
Debas, K., Carrier, J., Orban, P., Barakat, M., Lungu, O., Vandewalle, G., Hadj Tahar, A., Bellec, P., Karni, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Benali, H., Doyon, J., 2010. Brain plasticity
683 28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 684
related to the consolidation of motor sequence learning and motor adaptation. Proc. Natl.
685
Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 17839–17844. doi:10.1073/pnas.1013176107
686
Dehghani, N., Cash, S., Halgren, E., 2011. Topographical frequency dynamics within EEG and MEG sleep spindles. Clin Neurophysiol 122, 229–235.
688
doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2010.06.018
689
RI PT
687
Dinges, D.F., Powell, J.W., 1985. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav. Res. Methods 17, 652–655.
691
Doran, S.M., 2003. The dynamic topography of individual sleep spindles. Sleep Res. Online
M AN U
5, 133–139.
692 693
SC
690
Douaud, G., Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Behrens, T., Johansen-Berg, H., Vickers, J., James, S.,
694
Voets, N., Watkins, K., Matthews, P.M., James, A., 2007. Anatomically related grey and
695
white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130, 2375–86.
696
doi:10.1093/brain/awm184
Doyon, J., Benali, H., 2005. Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of
TE D
697
motor skills. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 161–167. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004
698 699
Doyon, J., Penhune, V.B., Ungerleider, L.G., 2003. Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia 41, 252–262.
701
doi:S0028393202001586 [pii]
AC C
702
EP
700
Doyon, J., Ungerleider, L.G., 2002. Functional anatomy of motor skill learning. Neuropsychol. Mem. 3, 225–238.
703 704
Feinberg, I., 1974. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age. J. Psychiatr. Res. 10, 283–306. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(74)90011-9
705 706
Feinberg, I., Koresko, R.L., Heller, N., 1967. EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and
707
pathological aging in man, Journal of Psychiatric Research. doi:10.1016/0022-
708
3956(67)90027-1 29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 709
Ferrarelli, F., Peterson, M.J., Sarasso, S., Riedner, B.A., Murphy, M.J., Benca, R.M., Bria, P., Kalin, N.H., Tononi, G., 2010. Thalamic dysfunction in schizophrenia suggested by
711
whole-night deficits in slow and fast spindles. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 1339–48.
712
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121731
713
Fjell, A.M., Walhovd, K.B., 2010. Structural brain changes in aging: courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 187–221.
714
Floyd, J. a, Janisse, J.J., Jenuwine, E.S., Ager, J.W., 2007. Changes in REM-sleep
SC
715
RI PT
710
percentage over the adult lifespan. Sleep 30, 829–836.
716
Fogel, S., Albouy, G., Vien, C., Popovicci, R., King, B.R., Hoge, R.D., Jbabdi, S., Benali, H.,
718
Karni, A., Maquet, P., Carrier, J., Doyon, J., 2014. fMRI and sleep correlates of the age-
719
related impairment in motor memory consolidation. Hum Brain Mapp 35, 3625–3645.
720
doi:10.1002/hbm.22426
721
M AN U
717
Fogel, S., Martin, N., Lafortune, M., Barakat, M., Debas, K., Laventure, S., Latreille, V., Gagnon, J.F., Doyon, J., Carrier, J., 2012. NREM Sleep Oscillations and Brain Plasticity
723
in Aging. Front Neurol 3, 176. doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00176
724
TE D
722
Fogel, S., Ray, L.B., Binnie, L., Owen, A.M., 2015. How to become an expert: A new perspective on the role of sleep in the mastery of procedural skills. Neurobiol. Learn.
726
Mem. 125, 236–248. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2015.10.004
AC C
727
EP
725
Fogel, S., Smith, C., 2011. The function of the sleep spindle: a physiological index of
728
intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci
729
Biobehav Rev. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.003
730
Fogel, S., Smith, C., Cote, K.A., 2007. Dissociable learning-dependent changes in REM and
731
non-REM sleep in declarative and procedural memory systems. Behav. Brain Res. 180,
732
48–61. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.037
733
Fogel, S., Smith, C.T., 2006. Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sleep. J. Sleep Res. 15, 250–255. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00522.x
734
Friston, K., Penny, W., Glaser, D.E., 2005. Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage 25, 661–667. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013
736 737
Gaser, C., Schlaug, G., 2003. Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 514–7.
738 739
Gaudreau, H., Carrier, J., Montplaisir, J., 2001. Age-related modifications of NREM sleep EEG: from childhood to middle age. J. Sleep Res. 10, 165–172. doi:252 [pii]
SC
740 741
RI PT
735
Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I.S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R.S., 2001a. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains.
743
Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786
744
M AN U
742
Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I.S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S.,
745
2001b. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains.
746
Neuroimage 14, 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786
Groussard, M., Viader, F., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., Platel, H., 2014. The
748
effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: the dynamics of grey matter changes.
749
Brain Cogn. 90, 174–80. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013
TE D
747
Gryga, M., Taubert, M., Dukart, J., Vollmann, H., Conde, V., Sehm, B., Villringer, A., Ragert,
751
P., 2012. Bidirectional gray matter changes after complex motor skill learning. Front.
752
Syst. Neurosci. 6, 37. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2012.00037
AC C
EP
750
753
Guazzelli, M., Feinberg, I., Aminoff, M., Fein, G., Floyd, T.C., Maggini, C., Guazzelli, N.,
754
Feinberg, I., Aminoff, M., Fein, G., Floyd, T.C., Maggini, C., 1986. Sleep spindles in
755
normal elderly: comparison with young adult patterns and relation to nocturnal
756
awakening, cognitive function and brain atrophy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.
757
63, 526–539.
758
Gunning-Dixon, F.M., Head, D., McQuain, J., Acker, J.D., Raz, N., 1998. Differential aging of 31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 759
the human striatum: a prospective MR imaging study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 19, 1501–
760
1507. Hamzei, F., Glauche, V., Schwarzwald, R., May, A., 2012. Dynamic gray matter changes
762
within cortex and striatum after short motor skill training are associated with their
763
increased functional interaction. Neuroimage 59, 3364–72.
764
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.089
SC
Horne, J.A., Ostberg, O., 1976. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningnesseveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4, 97–110.
766 767
Howard, J.H., Howard, D. V., 2013. Aging mind and brain: Is implicit learning spared in healthy aging? Front. Psychol. 4, 1–6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00817
768 769
M AN U
765
RI PT
761
Huupponen, E., Himanen, S.L., Varri, A., Hasan, J., Lehtokangas, M., Saarinen, J., 2002. A
770
study on gender and age differences in sleep spindles. Neuropsychobiology 45, 99–105.
771
doi:nps45099 [pii]
Iber, C., Ancoli-Israel, S., Chesson, A.L., Quan, S.F., 2007. The AASM manual for the scoring
773
of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. American
774
Academy of Sleep Medicine Westchester, IL. Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., Woolrich, M.W., Smith, S.M., 2012. Fsl.
EP
775
TE D
772
Neuroimage 62, 782–790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015
777
AC C
776
Jobert, M., Poiseau, E., Jahnig, P., Schulz, H., Kubicki, S., 1992. Topographical analysis of sleep spindle activity. Neuropsychobiology 26, 210–217. doi:118923
778 779
Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M.M., Turner, R., Ungerleider, L.G., 1995.
780
Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning.
781
Nature 377, 155–158. doi:10.1038/377155a0
782
Kennedy, K.M., Raz, N., 2005. Age, sex and regional brain volumes predict perceptual-motor skill acquisition. Cortex 41, 560–569.
783 32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 784
King, B.R., Fogel, S., Albouy, G., Doyon, J., 2013. Neural correlates of the age-related
785
changes in motor sequence learning and motor adaptation in older adults. Front Hum
786
Neurosci 7, 142. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00142 Knoblauch, V., Münch, M., Blatter, K., Martens, W.L.J., Schröder, C., Schnitzler, C., Wirz-
RI PT
787 788
Justice, A., Cajochen, C., 2005. Age-related changes in the circadian modulation of
789
sleep-spindle frequency during nap sleep. Sleep 28, 1093–1101.
Lafortune, M., Gagnon, J.F., Martin, N., Latreille, V., Dubé, J., Bouchard, M., Bastien, C.,
791
Carrier, J., 2014. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next
792
morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep
793
Res 23, 159–167. doi:10.1111/jsr.12108
M AN U
794
SC
790
Landolt, H., Borbely, A.A., 2001. Age-dependent changes in sleep EEG topography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 369–377. doi:S1388-2457(00)00542-3 [pii]
795
Landolt, H., Dijk, D.J., Achermann, P., Borbely, A.A., 1996. Effect of age on the sleep EEG:
797
slow-wave activity and spindle frequency activity in young and middle-aged men. Brain
798
Res. 738, 205–212.
799
TE D
796
Leclercq, Y., Schrouff, J., Noirhomme, Q., Maquet, P., Phillips, C., 2011. fMRI artefact rejection and sleep scoring toolbox. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011, 598206.
801
doi:10.1155/2011/598206
AC C
802
EP
800
MacLean, A.W., Fekken, G.C., Saskin, P., Knowles, J.B., 1992. Psychometric evaluation of the Stanford Sleepiness Scale. J Sleep Res 1, 35–39.
803 804
Mander, B.A., Marks, S.M., Vogel, J.W., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Ancoli-Israel, S.,
805
Jagust, W.J., Walker, M.P., 2015. β-amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and
806
related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1051–7.
807
doi:10.1038/nn.4035
808
Mander, B.A., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Ancoli-Israel, S., Jagust, W., Walker, M., 2013a. 33
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 809
Impaired Prefrontal Sleep Spindle Regulation of Hippocampal-Dependent Learning in
810
Older Adults. Cereb Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht188
811
Mander, B.A., Rao, V., Lu, B., Saletin, J.M., Lindquist, J.R., Ancoli-Israel, S., Jagust, W., Walker, M., 2013b. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired
813
hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci 16, 357–364.
814
doi:10.1038/nn.3324
Martin, N., Lafortune, M., Godbout, J., Barakat, M., Robillard, R., Poirier, G., Bastien, C.,
SC
815
RI PT
812
Carrier, J., 2012. Topography of age-related changes in sleep spindles. Neurobiol Aging
817
34, 468–476. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.05.020
818
M AN U
816
Milner, C.E., Fogel, S., Cote, K.A., 2006. Habitual napping moderates motor performance
819
improvements following a short daytime nap. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 141–156.
820
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.015
Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K., Londe, Z., Ullman, M.T., Howard, D. V, Howard, J.H., 2010. Sleep
822
has no critical role in implicit motor sequence learning in young and old adults. Exp Brain
823
Res 201, 351–358. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2024-x
824
TE D
821
Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25. Nicolas, A., Petit, D., Rompré, S., Montplaisir, J., 2001. Sleep spindle characteristics in
AC C
826
EP
825
healthy subjects of different age groups. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 521–527.
827 828
Nielsen, T., O’Reilly, C., Carr, M., Dumel, G., Godin, I., Solomonova, E., Lara-Carrasco, J.,
829
Blanchette-Carrière, C., Paquette, T., 2014. Overnight improvements in two REM sleep-
830
sensitive tasks are associated with both REM and NREM sleep changes, sleep spindle
831
features, and awakenings for dream recall. Neurobiol Learn Mem.
832
doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2014.09.007
833
Nishida, M., Walker, M., 2007. Daytime naps, motor memory consolidation and regionally 34
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 834
specific sleep spindles. PLoS One 2, e341. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000341
835
Ohayon, M.M., Carskadon, M.A., Guilleminault, C., Vitiello, M. V, 2004. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing
837
normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27, 1255–1273.
838
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.
839
Pace-Schott, E.F., Spencer, R.M.C., 2011. Age-related changes in the cognitive function of
SC
840
sleep. Prog Brain Res 191, 75–89. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00012-6
841
Peters, K.R., Ray, L., Fogel, S., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2014. Age differences in the variability
M AN U
842
RI PT
836
843
and distribution of sleep spindle and rapid eye movement densities. PLoS One 9,
844
e91047. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091047
Peters, K.R., Ray, L., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2008. Changes in the density of stage 2 sleep
846
spindles following motor learning in young and older adults. J. Sleep Res. 17, 23–33.
847
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00634.x
848
TE D
845
Peters, K.R., Smith, V., Smith, C., 2007. Changes in sleep architecture following motor learning depend on initial skill level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 817–829.
850
doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.817 Pivik, R.T., Joncas, S., Busby, K.A., 1999. Sleep spindles and arousal: the effects of age and sensory stimulation. Sleep Res. Online 2, 89–100.
852 853
AC C
851
EP
849
Principe, J.C., Smith, J.R., 1982. Sleep spindle characteristics as a function of age. Sleep 5, 73.
854 855
Ramanand, P., Bruce, M.C., Bruce, E.N., 2010. Mutual information analysis of EEG signals
856
indicates age-related changes in cortical interdependence during sleep in middle-aged
857
versus elderly women. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 27, 274–284.
858
doi:10.1097/WNP.0b013e3181eaa9f5 35
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 859
Ramanathan, D.S., Gulati, T., Ganguly, K., 2015. Sleep-Dependent Reactivation of
860
Ensembles in Motor Cortex Promotes Skill Consolidation. PLoS Biol. 13, e1002263.
861
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002263 Rasch, B., Born, J., 2013. About Sleep’s Role in Memory. Physiol Rev 93, 681–766.
RI PT
862
doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012
863
Ray, L.B., Sockeel, S., Soon, M., Bore, A., Myhr, A., Stojanoski, B., Cusack, R., Owen, A.M.,
865
Doyon, J., Fogel, S., 2015. Expert and crowd-sourced validation of an individualized
866
sleep spindle detection method employing complex demodulation and individualized
867
normalization. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00507
M AN U
868
SC
864
Raz, N., Daugherty, A.M., Bender, A.R., Dahle, C.L., Land, S., 2015. Volume of the
869
hippocampal subfields in healthy adults: differential associations with age and a pro-
870
inflammatory genetic variant. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 2663–74. doi:10.1007/s00429-
871
014-0817-6
Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle,
873
C., Gerstorf, D., Acker, J.D., 2005. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults:
874
general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1676–89.
875
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi044
EP
Redline, S., Gottlieb, D.J., Kapur, V., Kirchner, H.L., Quan, S.F., Newman, A., 2004. The
AC C
876
TE D
872
877
Effects of Age, Sex, Ethnicity, and Sleep-Disordered Breathing on Sleep Architecture.
878
Arch. Intern. Med. 164, 406–418. doi:10.1001/archinte.164.4.406
879
Rieckmann, A., Fischer, H., Bäckman, L., 2010. Activation in striatum and medial temporal
880
lobe during sequence learning in younger and older adults: relations to performance.
881
Neuroimage 50, 1303–1312. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.015
882
Rosanova, M., Ulrich, D., 2005. Pattern-specific associative long-term potentiation induced by
883
a sleep spindle-related spike train. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 25, 9398–9405. 36
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2149-05.2005
884 885
Saletin, J.M., van der Helm, E., Walker, M.P., 2013. Structural brain correlates of human
886
sleep oscillations. Neuroimage 83, 658–668. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.021 Sargolzaei, S., Sargolzaei, A., Cabrerizo, M., Chen, G., Goryawala, M., Noei, S., Zhou, Q.,
888
Duara, R., Barker, W., Adjouadi, M., 2015. A practical guideline for intracranial volume
889
estimation in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. BMC Bioinformatics 16, S8.
890
doi:10.1186/1471-2105-16-S7-S8
SC
891
RI PT
887
Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Albouy, G., Balteau, E., Boly, M., Carrier, J., Darsaud, A., Degueldre, C., Desseilles, M., Gais, S., Phillips, C., Rauchs, G., Schnakers, C.,
893
Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., 2007. Hemodynamic cerebral
894
correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc. Natl.
895
Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 13164–13169. doi:0703084104 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.0703084104
896
M AN U
892
Schabus, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Heib, D.P.J., Boly, M., Desseilles, M., Vandewalle, G., Schmidt, C., Albouy, G., Darsaud, A., Gais, S., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Phillips, C., Luxen, A.,
898
Maquet, P., 2012. The Fate of Incoming Stimuli during NREM Sleep is Determined by
899
Spindles and the Phase of the Slow Oscillation. Front Neurol 3, 40.
900
doi:10.3389/fneur.2012.00040
EP
Silber, M.H., Ancoli-Israel, S., Bonnet, M.H., Chokroverty, S., Grigg-Damberger, M.M.,
AC C
901
TE D
897
902
Hirshkowitz, M., Kapen, S., Keenan, S., Kryger, M.H., Penzel, T., 2007. The visual
903
scoring of sleep in adults. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 3, 121–131.
904
Simon, J.R., Howard, J.H., Howard, D. V, 2011. Age differences in implicit learning of
905
probabilistic unstructured sequences. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 32–8.
906
doi:10.1093/geronb/gbq066
907
Smith, C., Smith, D., 2003. Ingestion of ethanol just prior to sleep onset impairs memory for procedural but not declarative tasks. Sleep 26, 185–191.
908 37
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 909
Smith, S.M., 2002. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–55. doi:10.1002/hbm.10062
910 911
Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M.W., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., JohansenBerg, H., Bannister, P.R., De Luca, M., Drobnjak, I., Flitney, D.E., Niazy, R.K., Saunders,
913
J., Vickers, J., Zhang, Y., De Stefano, N., Brady, J.M., Matthews, P.M., 2004. Advances
914
in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage
915
23 Suppl 1, S208–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051
SC
916
RI PT
912
Spencer, R.M.C., Gouw, A.M., Ivry, R.B., 2007. Age-related decline of sleep-dependent consolidation. Learn Mem 14, 480–484. doi:10.1101/lm.569407
918
M AN U
917
Steriade, M., 2006. Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137, 1087–1106. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.029
919 920
Steriade, M., 2005. Sleep, epilepsy and thalamic reticular inhibitory neurons. Trends Neurosci 28, 317–324. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.03.007
921
Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H., Hori, T., 2009. Activation of fast sleep spindles at the
TE D
922 923
premotor cortex and parietal areas contributes to motor learning: a study using
924
sLORETA. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 878–886. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2009.03.006 Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H., Hori, T., 2008. Fast sleep spindle (13-15 hz) activity
EP
925
correlates with sleep-dependent improvement in visuomotor performance. Sleep 31,
927
204–211.
AC C
926
928
Timofeev, I., 2001. Contribution of intrinsic and synaptic factors in the desynchronization of
929
thalamic oscillatory activity. Thalamus Relat. Syst. 1, 53–69. doi:10.1016/S1472-
930
9288(01)00004-8
931
Timofeev, I., Chauvette, S., 2013. The spindles: are they still thalamic? Sleep 36, 825–6. doi:10.5665/sleep.2702
932 933
Tombaugh, T.N., McIntyre, N.J., 1992. The mini-mental state examination: a comprehensive 38
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT review. J Am Geriatr Soc 40, 922–935.
934 935
Tyvaert, L., Levan, P., Grova, C., Dubeau, F., Gotman, J., 2008. Effects of fluctuating physiological rhythms during prolonged EEG-fMRI studies. Clin Neurophysiol 119, 2762–
937
2774. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.284
938
RI PT
936
Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripollés, P., Rojo, N., Sierpowska, J., François, C., Càmara, E., van Vugt, F.T., Mohammadi, B., Samii, A., Münte, T.F., Rodríguez-Fornells, A.,
940
Altenmüller, E., 2016. Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of
941
musical training onset. Neuroimage 126, 106–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008 Vien, C., Boré, A., Lungu, O., Benali, H., Carrier, J., Fogel, S., Doyon, J., n.d. Age-Related
M AN U
942
SC
939
943
White Matter Correlates of Motor Sequence Learning and Consolidation. Neurobiol.
944
Aging. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.08.006
945
Walker, M., Stickgold, R., Alsop, D., Gaab, N., Schlaug, G., 2005. Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain. Neuroscience 133, 911–917. doi:S0306-
947
4522(05)00396-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.007
948
TE D
946
Wamsley, E., Shinn, A.K., Tucker, M.A., Ono, K.E., McKinley, S., Ely, A. V, Goff, D.C., Stickgold, R., Manoach, D.S., 2013. The effects of eszopiclone on sleep spindles and
950
memory consolidation in schizophrenia: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Sleep 36,
951
1369–1376. doi:10.5665/sleep.2968
AC C
952
EP
949
Wamsley, E., Tucker, M.A., Shinn, A.K., Ono, K.E., McKinley, S., Ely, A. V, Goff, D.C.,
953
Stickgold, R., Manoach, D.S., 2012. Reduced sleep spindles and spindle coherence in
954
schizophrenia: mechanisms of impaired memory consolidation? Biol Psychiatry 71, 154–
955
161. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.008
956
Werk, C.M., Harbour, V.L., Chapman, C.A., 2005. Induction of long-term potentiation leads to
957
increased reliability of evoked neocortical spindles in vivo. Neuroscience 131, 793–800.
958
doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.020 39
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 959
Werth, E., Achermann, P., Borbely, A.A., 1997a. Fronto-occipital EEG power gradients in human sleep. J. Sleep Res. 6, 102–112.
960 961
Werth, E., Achermann, P., Dijk, D.J., Borbely, A.A., 1997b. Spindle frequency activity in the sleep EEG: individual differences and topographic distribution. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
963
Neurophysiol. 103, 535–542. doi:S0013469497000709 [pii]
964
RI PT
962
Wilhelm, I., Metzkow-Mészàros, M., Knapp, S., Born, J., 2012. Sleep-dependent consolidation of procedural motor memories in children and adults: the pre-sleep level of performance
966
matters. Dev Sci 15, 506–515. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01146.x
SC
965
Wilson, K., Baran, B., Pace-Schott, E.F., Ivry, R.B., Spencer, R.M.C., 2012. Sleep modulates
968
word-pair learning but not motor sequence learning in healthy older adults. Neurobiol
969
Aging 33, 991–1000. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.029
970
M AN U
967
Winkler, A.M., Ridgway, G.R., Webster, M.A., Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2014. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381–97.
972
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060
973
TE D
971
Zeitlhofer, J., Gruber, G., Anderer, P., Asenbaum, S., Schimicek, P., Saletu, B., 1997. Topographic distribution of sleep spindles in young healthy subjects. J. Sleep Res. 6,
975
149–155.
Zepelin, H., McDonald, C.S., 1987. Age differences in autonomic variables during sleep. J.
AC C
976
Gerontol. 42, 142–146.
977 978
EP
974
Zhang, Y., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2001. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden
979
Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans.
980
Med. Imaging 20, 45–57. doi:10.1109/42.906424
981
40
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
11.51 60.98 15.41 76.39 12.10
2.36 2.86 3.10 2.74 2.61
20.14 67.40 7.01 74.41 5.45
4.15 3.33 2.61 3.96 2.73
-1.08 -1.44 2.09 0.41 1.75
SC
Stage 1 % TST Stage 2 % TST SWS % TST NREM % TST REM % TST
RI PT
Table 1. Mean (and standard error; SE) sleep parameters recorded during the 90-minute daytime nap retention interval in both young and older participants. Young Older M SE M SE t p Wake (min) 19.85 3.44 47.09 7.47 -3.31 0.004 * Stage 1 (min) 7.87 1.36 6.18 0.76 1.28 0.270 Stage 2 (min) 43.31 2.28 31.02 5.22 2.16 0.044 * SWS (min) 11.45 2.41 4.60 1.75 2.35 0.027 * NREM (min) 54.77 3.13 35.62 6.20 2.76 0.012 * REM (min) 9.44 2.10 4.20 2.18 1.72 0.098 Total sleep time (min) 72.08 3.56 46.00 7.26 3.23 0.004 * 0.084 0.162 0.047 0.684 0.093
*
M AN U
Sleep onset latency (min) 11.92 1.91 18.40 5.26 -1.16 0.263 Wake after sleep onset (min) 10.87 2.69 31.13 6.79 -2.77 0.012 * Total recording time (min) 94.87 0.74 93.76 0.73 1.06 0.297 Sleep efficiency (%) 76.05 3.85 49.31 7.83 3.06 0.006 * Note: * indicates p < 0.05, df = 26, corrected for heterogeneity of variance where appropriate. Abbreviations: Minutes of sleep (min), Total sleep time (TST), rapid eye movement sleep (REM).
982
AC C
EP
TE D
983
41
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 984 2
M AN U
SC
RI PT
Table 2. Number, density (#/min NREM), duration (sec) and amplitude (uV ) and frequency (Hz) of NREM sleep spindles at Fz (slow: 11-14Hz), Cz (total: 11-17Hz) and Pz (fast: 14-17Hz) during the 90min daytime nap retention interval in young and older participants. Young Older M SE M SE t p Number Fz (slow) 193.38 17.27 149.73 26.61 1.99 0.394 Cz (total) 251.54 21.31 186.13 31.73 1.65 0.336 Pz (fast) 99.85 15.86 70.73 15.59 1.85 0.219 Density (#/min) Fz (slow) 3.45 0.22 4.07 0.14 -3.44 0.099 Cz (total) 4.51 0.25 5.21 0.15 -2.39 0.144 Pz (fast) 1.78 0.26 1.94 0.21 -0.68 0.651 Duration (sec) Fz (slow) * 0.50 0.02 0.38 0.02 5.19 0.005 Cz (total) * 0.48 0.03 0.34 0.02 3.55 0.004 Pz (fast) * 0.47 0.02 0.40 0.02 3.09 0.045 Amplitude (uV2) Fz (slow) 62.88 8.06 41.68 5.02 3.24 0.132 Cz (total) 90.18 12.32 67.24 8.12 1.50 0.298 Pz (fast) 67.78 11.07 74.39 11.70 -0.58 0.696 Frequency (Hz) Fz (slow) * 12.50 0.04 12.23 0.04 6.57 0.000 Cz (total) 13.36 0.07 13.29 0.10 0.55 0.585 Pz (fast) 14.69 0.04 14.83 0.07 -2.72 0.177 Note: * indicates p < 0.05, two-tailed. df = 26, corrected for homogeneity of variance, where appropriate and Holm-Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons for the number of sites.
985
AC C
EP
TE D
986
42
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 987
B) YOUNG vs. OLDER: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES MSL Right Left
MSL MSL
Cerebellum (IV) Cerebellum (IX,X)
26 3
Spindles Left Left Left Left Right Right Left
Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Total spindle duration Slow spindle amplitude Slow spindle duration Fast spindle duration
Cerebellum (IV) Hippocampus Cingulate cortex Parietal Supplementary motor area Supplementary motor area Cerebellum (IV)
Cerebellum (IX) Cerebellum (IIX) Hippocampus
RI PT
Table 3. Brain regions with (A) significantly greater GM in young vs. older participants, (B) GM correlated with offline changes (MSL) in performance in conjunction with spindles in young vs. older, (C) young and (D) older participants. X, Y Z coordinates given in voxel coordinates, in radiological orientation. L/R Covariates of interest Brain Region Voxels t p x y z A) YOUNG vs. OLDER Bilateral None Striatal-Cortical-Hippocampal 43200 6.98 0.0002* 46 82 47
0.002 0.002
35 53
39 47
25 15
439 439 245 11 22 23 225
6.13 5.40 5.22 4.63 4.30 4.7 4.69
0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001
61 65 52 66 33 33 59
47 52 43 35 66 66 29
21 27 55 64 66 67 26
42 9 4
2.16 2.1 2.17
0.018 0.029 0.030
53 28 61
44 40 50
12 24 28
918 10 90 16
2.68 2.16 2.17 1.94
0.006 0.009 0.002 0.017
62 53 61 62
34 44 50 52
20 12 28 28
MSL & spindle conjunction: Left MSL & total spindle duration Right MSL & total spindle duration Left MSL & total spindle duration
M AN U
SC
3.89 3.01
C) YOUNG: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES
TE D
MSL & spindle conjunction: Bilateral MSL & total spindle duration Bilateral MSL & total spindle duration Left MSL & total spindle duration Bilateral MSL & fast spindle density
Cerebellum (crus I, IV) Cerebellum (IX) Hippocampus Hippocampus
No significant voxels
EP
D) OLDER: GM CORRELATED WITH MSL & SPINDLES
AC C
Results from total spindles from Cz, slow spindles from Fz and fast spindles from Pz presented. Note: * indicates FWE-corrected p-value significant at p < 0.05. Coordinates for side where effect was maximal reported when bilateral.
988 989
43
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT HIGHLIGHTS Older individuals had intact motor sequence learning but impaired offline gains
•
Grey matter related to spindles and offline gains in young vs. older individuals
•
Spindles may serve as a biological marker for age-related memory deficits
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
•
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
RI PT
1. All authors disclose that: (a) There are no actual or potential conflicts of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other people or organizations within three years of beginning the work submitted that could inappropriately influence (bias) their work. (b)The author's institutions have no contracts relating to this research through which it or any other organization may stand to gain financially now or in the future. (c) No other agreements of authors or their institutions that could be seen as involving a financial interest in this work. 2. This research was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health research (CIHR) grant to author JD and fellowship support to author SF from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec (FRSQ).
SC
3. The results reported in the manuscript being submitted have not been previously published, have not been submitted elsewhere and will not be submitted elsewhere while under consideration at Neurobiology of Aging.
M AN U
4. Ethical and scientific approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee at the “Institut de Geriatrie de Montreal”, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
AC C
EP
TE D
5. All authors have had the opportunity to review the contents of the manuscript being submitted, approve of its contents and validate the accuracy of the data.