Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies

Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies

COIS 453 1–7 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect 1 2 3 4Q1 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 Socialit...

380KB Sizes 0 Downloads 68 Views

COIS 453 1–7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect 1 2 3 4Q1

5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21

22 23 24 25

Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies Volker Nehring1 and Sandra Steiger2 Recognition and communication are essential processes, when it comes to interaction of organisms with their biotic environment. As especially social interactions are coordinated by communication, it has been predicted that social evolution drives communicative complexity. However, studies comparing olfactory signals or receptor repertoires of solitary and eusocial insects found only mixed evidence for the social complexity hypothesis. We present some possible explanations and especially argue that our current knowledge of intermediate levels of sociality is insufficient to fully test the hypothesis, for which a more balanced comparative dataset would be required. We illustrate with chosen examples how complex communication within the other insect societies can be: Many messages are not unique to eusocial insects. Studying the other insect societies will provide us with a more detailed picture of the link between social and communicative complexity. Addresses 1 Department for Evolutionary Biology and Animal Ecology, University of Freiburg, 79104 Freiburg, Germany 2 Institute of Insect Biotechnology, University of Gießen, 35392 Gießen, Germany Corresponding authors: Nehring, Volker ([email protected]), Steiger, Sandra ([email protected])

26

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:xx–yy

27

This review comes from a themed issue on Social insects section

28

Edited by Joel Meunier and Sandra Steiger

29

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2018.04.002

30

2214-5745/ã 2018 Published by Elsevier Inc.

31

Introduction

32

The beauty and surprising complexity of animal communication has always fascinated both scientists and laypeople alike. From birdsong to bee dance, interactions between individuals are almost always coordinated by communication [1]. Cooperation in groups, such as mammalian societies or the large colonies of social insects, appears to require the most intricate coordination. Social evolution has therefore been predicted to drive the complexity of recognition and communicative systems and cognitive abilities (‘social complexity hypothesis’,

33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41

www.sciencedirect.com

Figure 1a) [2–4]. The hypothesis has found support in diverse vertebrate taxa [2,5]. For example in grounddwelling sciurids, species living in more complex social systems produce a higher number of distinct alarm call [6]. However, when it comes to insects, empirical evidence for such a pattern is rather mixed.

42

In our review, we shortly introduce insect studies that tested the ‘social complexity hypothesis’ and analyse potential reasons for the lack of a clear support. We highlight that studying communication in The Other Insect Societies can help us fill some of the gaps in order to better understand the relationship between communication and social evolution.

48

‘The social complexity hypothesis’: evidence from insect social evolution

55

There are currently a few studies available that analysed the relationship between insect social evolution and communicative complexity. In insects, the olfactory channel is the most dominant one and a variety of information is conveyed by pheromones and chemical cues. Consequently, it is not surprising that the majority of the studies focused on chemical communication. The currently best studied group of chemical compounds that play an important role in recognition and communication are cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs), chemicals that are omnipresent on the cuticular surface of insects [7]. They are known to contain information of, for example, sex, fertility, caste, and kin. Because coordination of groups requires various messages to be exchanged, CHC profiles have been predicted to increase in complexity with the emergence of eusociality (Figure 1a). However, a large comparative study analysing CHC profiles of 241 hymenopteran species found no difference in the number of substance classes and isomers between solitary and eusocial insects [8]. In fact, the polyphyletic group of solitary parasitoid wasps produced some of the most complex CHC profiles across the Hymenoptera, with ants having slightly less complex CHC profiles. Bees and social wasps, however, bear surprisingly simple CHC blends, in particular when considering their social complexity.

57

When looking at the receiver side of communication, there is no clear-cut picture either. A study by Zhou et al. [9] compared the chemoreceptor repertoire of 13 solitary and social Hymenoptera and found that the evolution of sociality does not necessarily increase the numbers of, or positive selection on, odorant receptor

83

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

43 44 45 46 47

49 50 51 52 53 54

56

58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82

84

85 86 87

COIS 453 1–7

2 Social insects section

Figure 1

Solitary

Eusocial

(a) Social

Complexity of the communication system

complexity

(b) Precursor

(c) Social

conflict

Time Current Opinion in Insect Science

Three major hypotheses that predict how communication systems evolve along a gradient of sociality. (a) According to the ‘social complexity hypothesis’, communicative complexity increases with social complexity. (b) The ‘precursor hypothesis’ predicts that the evolution of sociality is more likely to occur in species already equipped with a complex ‘communicative repertoire’. (c) The ‘conflict hypothesis’ predicts that communicative complexity peaks at intermediate level of sociality, where conflict between group members is more likely to occur and where group members are more likely to be recognised individually. The complexity of the communication system is plotted against evolutionary time and the social complexity coded for by colour. The dotted line depicts a solitary species evolving from the same ancestor.

88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99

(OR) genes ([9,see also 10]). In fact, phylogenetic comparisons of multiple solitary and social lineages suggest that a chemoreceptor repertoire expansion may have preceded the evolution of eusociality [9]. Interestingly, the pattern for OR gene and CHC complexity is very similar, with ants having twice as many OR genes than the social bees. Considering that the 9-exon ORs that bind to CHCs are narrowly tuned, with each OR mostly binding to only one or very few substances [11], a simple prediction is that OR and CHC complexity coevolve, with each additional CHC that is used in communication requiring an additional OR. Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

Another recent study compared solitary and eusocial halictid bees and used sensilla density on antennae as a proxy for communicative complexity [12]. They found that, while sensilla density is lower in secondary solitary halictid bees than in the eusocial ones, the ancestral state seems to be a solitary bee with high density; again, high sensilla density appears to have preceded the evolution of eusociality. Also studies examining the relationship of sociality and investment in insect mushroom bodies, brain centres that participate in olfactory associative learning, olfactory processing, and sensory integration, did not reveal a clear pattern. A comparative study of wasp brain morphology indicates that sociality has not increased but reduced the investment into the mushroom body [13]. However, other studies have shown that mushroom bodies of social reproductives are larger than those of solitary reproductives in a facultatively eusocial sweat bee [14], and that mushroom body development is driven by social interactions in ants [15].

100

What is a complex communication system?

119

A major challenge in testing the ‘social complexity hypothesis’ is to find an accurate measurement of communicative complexity. A number of traits have been used as proxies of complexity in communication systems. However, some traits such as the number of sensilla or the size of brains are not necessarily increasing the quantity of information that is communicated, but rather the quality: sensitivity, precision, and speed of information processing [16]. The large antennae of male moths, for example, have evolved to achieve a stunning sensitivity to the typically not very complex female sex pheromone, whose single message is ‘I am here’ [17,18]. In the same vein, larger numbers of CHCs on an insect’s cuticle and a larger number of OR genes might not necessarily have evolved to communicate a larger number of messages either. In a recognition context, more CHCs and OR genes may simply allow for a more reliable discrimination between individuals through a larger number of possible different odour blend configurations, without increasing the number of messages. This could be important when individuals need to discriminate between many different individuals or multiple different groups of individuals [19].

120

As good physiological and morphological proxies for communicative complexity are difficult to find, we suggest ultimate analyses of the interaction between communication and social evolution to focus on the actual messages that are sent, which requires comprehensive ethological studies.

142

Lack of data for hypothesis testing and the benefits of studying the other insect societies

148

Another major problem that we currently face in testing the social complexity hypothesis is our limited knowledge of communication in insects in general. In most cases, except some well-studied models (honeybee,

150

www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118

121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141

143 144 145 146 147

149

151 152 153

COIS 453 1–7

Sociality and communicative complexity Nehring and Steiger 3

183

burying beetle), we have no comprehensive overview over the messages that are communicated within a species. Insects often do not only rely on one but different signalling channels, from olfactory to visual and acoustic communication [20]. Analysing all signals that are produced is thus a demanding task, especially when considering that the most dominant signalling channel in insects, the olfactory one, is technically challenging to analyse [17]. The chemical identity of queen pheromones remained unknown for the overwhelming majority of eusocial species for a long time, and even though some progress has been made in the meantime (e.g. [21]), we still only know the molecules involved for a few species. A particular problem is also the disbalance across taxa and degrees of sociality. In general, it is often neglected that primitive aggregations or small family groups, consisting of parents and offspring, are already characterised by relatively complex communication processes [22]. The step from the other social insects or even solitary insects to highly eusocial ones is not necessarily as steep as one might think. To support this point, we present some examples of olfactory communication that are typically attributed to eusocial insects, but can also be found in the other insect societies. However, while these examples are impressive, they are for the most part anecdotal in that we do not know how common the different types of communication are across species. A more comprehensive dataset of communication in the other insect societies would greatly advance our understanding of the role of communication in social evolution.

184

Aggregation pheromones

185

The highly eusocial honeybees use aggregation pheromones from the mandibular gland to coordinate swarming. Interestingly, the queen mandibular gland pheromone has been co-opted: queens also use it to attract males, attract workers to form the queen retinue, and to keep workers from reproduction [although other substances are involved as well [23]]. Whenever pheromones are co-opted for multiple messages, the machinery for sending and receiving of messages will actually become simpler, which supports the idea that highly eusocial insects need not be the pinnacle when it comes to the complexity of communication systems.

154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182

186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196

197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207

Even without elaborate interactions between group members, aggregations can increase individual fitness in comparison to a purely solitary life (Allee effects). For instance, group living generally reduces individual predation risk (selfish herd) and can protect from desiccation [18]. As a more specific example, bark beetles can often not overcome tree defences individually and depend on a species-specific set of volatile compounds to attract conspecifics, so that they can attack living or dying trees in large groups and then use them as a resource for reproduction [24]. www.sciencedirect.com

Since Allee effects are very simple and require no elaborate interactions among individuals, aggregation pheromones, which attract conspecifics independent of sex, are phylogenetically very basal and found in species as diverse as bed bugs [25], schooling shrimps [26], nematodes [27], and even amoebozoans that aggregate through chemotaxis to cAMP [28].

208

Alarm pheromones

215

Alarm pheromones are among the most common pheromones of insects [29]. In large eusocial colonies, individuals under attack can produce volatile alarm pheromones that recruit others for help. The cues are evolutionary derived from substances leaking from wounded bodies [30], or are defence substances used to repel attacking predators. The formic acid that ant workers spray to defend themselves, for example, also recruits their nestmates to help with defence [31]. In smaller and perhaps more ephemeral societies (e.g. in leaf litter), however, alarm pheromones cause colonies do disperse to avoid the threat (‘panic alarms’ [32]). This behaviour would be adaptive for gregarious insects as well.

216

Aphids feeding on leaves produce farnesenes upon threat, which cause conspecifics and sometimes even individuals of other species to move away or drop off leaves [29]. Alarm pheromones are further known from thrips [33], collembolans [30], bed bugs [34], and other arthropods, such as mites [35].

230

Because alarm pheromones have a highly functional evolutionary origin, and have benefits that can be realised independent of complex social structures, even in loose non-kin groups, there is no reason to expect alarm signalling to be specific to eusocial insects. If anything, the behavioural responses to the pheromones may be rewired to cause a more complex, context-based response.

236

Fertility signals

243

In eusocial insects, the reproductive division of labour between queens and workers is often maintained by means of pheromones that honestly reflect the reproductive state. Although those queen pheromones can belong to different chemical classes, studies indicate that CHCs often play a key role [21,36]. Recent theoretical considerations suggest a transition of reproductive communication from cue-like signature mixtures, to learned fertility signals, to innate queen pheromones that evolved across eusocial insects [37]. Consequently, according to this hypothesis a chemical repertoire reflecting reproductive state was already available early on, for example to be used for mate choice. In fact, there are also subsocial and even many solitary species in which CHCs co-vary with reproductive state, and queen pheromones might have derived from sex pheromones [36]. In burying beetles, which raise their young on vertebrate cadavers and

244

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

209 210 211 212 213 214

217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229

231 232 233 234 235

237 238 239 240 241 242

245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260

COIS 453 1–7

4 Social insects section

284

provide elaborate pre-hatching and post-hatching care, CHC profiles of males and females have been shown to change when the beetles are breeding [38]. The information is used by the adults to recognise a breeding partner and discriminate against infanticidal intruders to the brood chamber [39]. How receivers respond to the CHCs certainly differs from eusocial insects: breeder CHCs do not suppress female reproduction. Nevertheless, it exemplifies that information about reproductive state is available and used in subsocial species. Interestingly, during the intensive period of parental care, female burying beetles are known to emit a volatile, methyl geranate, which reliably reflects their temporary infertility during this time [40]. The pheromone has been shown to suppress sexual activity of male partners. An infertility or sterility pheromone has also been reported in worker bumblebees [41]. Here, workers appear to signal to the queen and other workers that they will refrain from producing any eggs, which reduces aggression. Again, the examples highlight that fertility or infertility cues and signals are not specific to eusocial insects; only the type of receiver and its behavioural response deviate depending on the form of interaction and structure of the group.

285

Solicitation pheromones

286

We know from some eusocial insects that larvae produce chemical substances to solicit food from nurses. In honey bees Apis mellifera, young and old larvae emit pheromones that differ in composition and regulate how much food nurses provide, to meet the nutritional requirements of the larvae [42,43]. Young larvae emit the volatile (E)b-ocimene, whereas older larvae predominantly produce a so-called brood ester pheromone, a blend of 10 ethyl and methyl fatty acid esters. However, such solicitation pheromones are not unique to eusocial groups, but also present in some subsocial insects. In fact, theory predicts the existence of begging signals for all brood caring organisms, where parent and offspring (or helpers and brood) interact and offspring rely on specific resources provided by the parents [44,45]. In the burrower bug, Sehirus cinctus, for example, nymphs release a blend of monoterpenes, which regulates maternal provisioning. Mothers exposed to volatiles from nymphs in poor condition provide significantly more food than those exposed to volatiles from well-fed nymphs [46]. In the European earwig Forficula auricularia CHCs signal the nymph’s nutritional state. However, earwig mothers preferentially feed well-fed nymphs [47].

261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283

287 288 289 290 291

292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299

300 301 302 303 304 305 306

307 308 309 310 311 312 313

Nestmate recognition/kin recognition/individual recognition

Cooperation can only be successful when altruism either benefits relatives or is reciprocated by the receivers. For both interactions to be reliable, some sort of recognition system is necessary to recognise individuals that ‘deserve’ altruism. Eusocial insect workers are selected to direct Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

work efforts towards their closest relatives. However, they rarely recognise kinship itself, but simply treat all colony members as kin. This can be demonstrated by transferring young workers, who typically lack nestmate recognition cues, to other colonies, where they blend in and treat their new unrelated nestmates as kin (reviewed in [48]). Individual odours are both genetically and ecologically determined, but cue mixing within the colony leads to a uniform odour that is well delineated against the odours of other colonies [49]. Only few cases of imperfect cue blending are documented, in which genetic lineages within the same colony can in theory be identified based on their odour [50–52]. Evidence that workers are actually able to distinguish genetic lineages (kin recognition) and behave nepotistically is rare. It comes from primitively eusocial ants and paper wasps [53,54] rather than highly eusocial insects. Interestingly, nestmate recognition is lost in some of the largest eusocial insects known: Unicolonial ants with their supercolonies often abandoned nestmate recognition altogether [55].

314

Kin recognition has also been demonstrated to facilitate kin-directed cooperation in groups of juveniles in solitary or gregarious arthropods: When groups consist of relatives and non-relatives and resources are scarce, non-relatives are preferentially cannibalised in mites and earwigs [56,57]. Subsocial spiders prefer to reside with cuticular extracts of kin rather than non-kin [58]. In the mite case, it has been documented that kin recognition is learned and not based on hard-wired recognition templates, which might suggest that nestmate recognition and kin recognition rely on similar proxy mechanisms. The odours used for kin recognition are likely to be the same than those used in nestmate recognition: blends of CHCs, which will then be perceived by similar sets of olfactory receptors both in solitary and social insects.

334

A different context for kin recognition is mate choice: Information about kinship can prevent inbreeding, and it is known that cockroaches [59] and solitary parasitoid waps [60] use CHCs to preferentially mate with non-kin. There may be cases in which the processes differ from those discussed above, for example when volatile sex pheromones are involved (for example in the beewolf [61]), or in case templates for mate choice are genetically determined rather than learned.

349

Evidence for individual recognition in insects is very rare. It is difficult to prove because it is often confounded with group-level recognition [62]. However, we know that ant queens in primitive Pachycondyla societies use odours to recognise each other individually, which supports the stability of dominance hierarchies ([63,there is a similar case but with visual cues in paper wasps, 64]). Interestingly, this ability is only predicted for these primitive societies and absent from large eusocial insect colonies [65].

358

www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333

335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348

350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357

359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367

COIS 453 1–7

Sociality and communicative complexity Nehring and Steiger 5

375

The overall pattern is thus that some sort of kin recognition is common for all levels of social complexity, with perhaps the highest information content at intermediate levels: In small societies of primitively eusocial species, where dominance is established through individual interactions and conflict over reproduction is high, group-level, kin-level, and individual recognition are all employed simultaneously.

376

Recruitment/trail pheromones

377

393

Trail pheromones are only expected to evolve in cooperative groups where individuals forage for patchy resources on foot: they evolved more than once in ants and are well studied in termites as well [20]. Flying eusocial insects can use volatiles to attract nestmates to food sources (e.g. Nasanov pheromone in honeybees [23]). Trail pheromones are also known from group-living non-eusocial insects such as tent caterpillars [18]. In eusocial or subsocial groups that do not forage but live in their food, such as on-piece nesting termites or burying beetles, trails are not expected. However, we know that parasites do eavesdrop on trail pheromones and it has been speculated that trail pheromones evolved from individuals eavesdropping on the footprints or faeces of their competitors inevitably deposit [66–68], in which case the receiver systems for trail following might have been already in place before the evolution of sociality.

394

Alternative hypotheses

395

Although the ‘social complexity hypothesis’ sounds intuitive, we have to emphasise that also alternative hypotheses on the link between sociality and communication have been suggested. Comprehensively studying intermediate levels of sociality in the Other Insect Societies will allow us to differentiate between the social complexity hypothesis and two alternatives that have been put forward. Instead of sociality being a driver of communicative complexity, a complex or very sensitive and accurate communication system may be a necessary precursor that allows social evolution to occur (‘precursor hypothesis’, Figure 1b [8]). The evolution of individual or group-level recognition, a necessity for stable groups, might only be possible after other evolutionary forces caused CHC profiles to become complex enough to allow for reliable recognition. This would also be in line with the current picture that within the hymenoptera there was an OR gene family expansion prior to the evolution of sociality [9]. However, as Zhou et al. point out, we currently do not have reliable OR gene annotations for enough species to conduct a formal phylogenetically balanced analysis of this question.

368 369 370 371 372 373 374

378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392

396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416

417 418 419 420 421

A second alternative hypothesis is that the investment in recognition processes and brains may peak at intermediate levels of sociality and decrease in highly eusocial insects (‘conflict hypothesis’, Figure 1c) [13,69]. In many primitively eusocial species, reproductive monopoly is www.sciencedirect.com

only maintained through dominance hierarchies, the maintenance of which relies on individual recognition or at least on the ability to judge the reproductive state of others. This conflict causes particular selection on receivers to pick up any fertility or other cues that might give away cheaters. In brood-caring species, such as the biparental burying beetles, conflicts between the sexes, siblings, and parents and offspring are omnipresent and may have driven the evolution of rather sophisticated recognition and communication processes [70]. This contrasts with obligate eusocial insects and their permanent reproductive division of labour between queen and sterile worker castes, where conflict over direct reproduction is low [71]. In line with the social conflict hypothesis, primitive societies are often based on the capacity to recognise both individuals and group members [13,72]. Highly eusocial insects, in contrast, rely on class-level recognition, which is a less sophisticated recognition process than for example individual recognition [19].

422

Conclusions

441

The hypothesis that the complexity of communication systems is driven by the evolution of complex social systems is an intuitive one but so far lacks evidence in insects. We suggest that we currently do not have the data that would be necessary to unveil the true connection between social and communicative complexity. So far, our best shot are comparative studies of facultatively social species or taxonomically narrow groups with well-documented life histories and communication systems (e.g. [12,14]). Broad comparative studies are bound to fail because sociality is not a binary variable but a continuum on which we cannot properly place most of the species due to missing data. Communicative complexity is likely to vary gradually along this continuum, with sometimes surprisingly high complexity in the other insect societies. Social evolution should influence the quantity and quality of messages that are communicated, which has been exhaustively analysed only for very few species. The current sample is highly biased towards eusocial Hymenoptera.

442

With this short review we would like to emphasise that studying the other social insects will help us to better understand the evolutionary connection between social and communicative complexity. That way we will fill in the blanks on the sociality continuum and generate valuable phylogenetically independent data points for the lower and intermediate levels of complexity.

462

Conflict of interest statement

469

Nothing declared.

Acknowledgements

423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440

443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461

463 464 465 466 467 468

Q2470 471

472 We thank Joel Meunier and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments and we acknowledge funding provided by the German Research Q3473 474 Foundation (DFG) to SS (STE 1874/3-3 and STE 1874/7-1).

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

COIS 453 1–7

6 Social insects section

475 476 477

References and recommended reading Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:  of special interest  of outstanding interest

478

1.

Bradbury J, Vehrencamp S: Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 2011.

17. Nehring V, Wyatt TD, d’Ettorre P (Eds): Noise in Chemical Communication. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2013.

531 532

18. Wyatt TD: Pheromones and Animal Behavior: Chemical Signals and Signatures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2014.

533 534

19. Tibbetts EA, Dale J: Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol Evol 2007, 22:529-537.

535 536

20. Leonhardt SD, Menzel F, Nehring V, Schmitt T: Ecology and evolution of communication in social insects. Cell 2016, 164:1277-1287.

537 538 539

479 480 481

2.

Freeberg TM, Dunbar RI, Ord TJ: Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philos Trans R Soc London 2012, 367:1785-1801.

3.

Dunbar R: Evolution of the social brain. Science 2003, 302:11601161.

21. Van Oystaeyen A, Oliveira RC, Holman L, van Zweden JS, Romero C, Oi CA, d’Ettorre P, Khalesi M, Billen J, Wa¨ckers F et al.: Conserved class of queen pheromones stops social insect workers from reproducing. Science 2014, 343:287-290.

540

482 483 484 485 486

4.

Perez-Barberia FJ, Shultz S, Dunbar RI: Evidence for coevolution of sociality and relative brain size in three orders of mammals. Evolution 2007, 61:2811-2821.

22. Steiger S, Sto¨kl J: Pheromones involved in insect parental care and family life. Curr Opin Insect Sci 2017.

543 544

Bouchet H, Blois-Heulin C, Lemasson A: Social complexity parallels vocal complexity: a comparison of three non-human primate species. Front Psychol 2013:4. A meta-analysis of hymenopteran CHCs shows that all classes of CHCs were already present in the early hymenopterans, with the most diverse CHC profiles found in the solitary parasitoid wasps.

23. Bortolotti L, Costa C: Chemical communication in the honey bee society. In Neurobiology of Chemical Communication. Edited by Mucignat-Caretta C. CRC Press; 2014:143-206.

545 546

24. Symonds M, Gitau-Clarke C: The evolution of aggregation pheromone diversity in bark beetles. Adv Insect Physiol 2016, 50:195-234.

547 548 549

6.

25. Gries R, Britton R, Holmes M, Zhai H, Draper J, Gries G: Bed bug aggregation pheromone finally identified. Angew Chem Int Ed 2015, 54:1135-1138.

550 551 552

26. Chak ST, Bauer R, Thiel M: Social behaviour and recognition in decapod shrimps, with emphasis on the Caridea. In Social Recognition in Invertebrates. Edited by Aquiloni L, Tricarico E. Springer; 2015:57-84.

553 554

27. Srinivasan J, von Reuss SH, Bose N, Zaslaver A, Mahanti P, Ho MC, O’Doherty OG, Edison AS, Sternberg PW, Schroeder FC: A modular library of small molecule signals regulates social behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Biol 2012, 10: e1001237.

555

28. O’Day DH, Keszei A: Signalling and sex in the social amoebozoans. Biol Rev 2012, 87:313-329.

559 560

29. Verheggen FJ, Haubruge E, Mescher MC: Alarm Pheromones – Chemical Signaling in Response to Danger.Vitamins & Hormones . Elsevier; 2010:215-239.

561 562 563

30. Purrington FF, Kendall PA, Bater JE, Stinner BR: Alarm pheromone in a gregarious poduromorph collembolan (Collembola: Hypogastruridae). The Great Lakes Entomologist 1991, 24:3.

564 565 566 567

31. Ho¨lldobler B, Wilson EO: The Ants. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1990.

568

32. Wilson EO, Regnier FE: The evolution of the alarm-defense system in the formicine ants. Am Nat 1971, 105:279-289.

569 570

33. de Bruijn PJ, Sabelis MW, Egas M: Antipredator responses to alarm pheromone in groups of young and/or old thrips larvae. Ethology 2017 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/eth.12600.

571 572 573

34. Gries G: Chemical ecology. In Advances in the Biology and Management of Modern Bed Bugs. Edited by Doggett S, Miller D, Lee C-Y. John Wiley & Sons; 2018:163.

574

35. Raspotnig G: Chemical alarm and defence in the oribatid mite Collohmannia gigantea (Acari: Oribatida). Exp Appl Acarol 2006, 39:177-194.

575 576 577

36. Oi CA, van Zweden JS, Oliveira RC, Van Oystaeyen A, Nascimento FS, Wenseleers T: The origin and evolution of social insect queen pheromones: novel hypotheses and outstanding problems. Bioessays 2015, 37:808-821.

578

37. Smith AA, Liebig J: The evolution of cuticular fertility signals in eusocial insects. Curr Opin Insect Sci 2017, 22:79-84.

581 582

38. Steiger S, Peschke K, Francke W, Mu¨ller JK: The smell of parents: breeding status influences cuticular hydrocarbon pattern in the burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides. Proc R Soc B 2007, 274:2211-2220.

583 584 585 586

487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494

5. 

Blumstein DT, Armitage KB: Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with grounddwelling sciurid alarm calls. Am Nat 1997, 150:179-200.

496 497 498 499

Blomquist GJ, Bagne`res A-G: Insect Hydrocarbons: Biology, Biochemistry, and Chemical Ecology. Cambridge University Press; 2010. Novel olfactory receptor gene annotations analyzed on a phylogenetic background. While the ants possess extremely large OR repertoires, the repertoires of solitary wasps are not necessarily short of those of social bees.

500 501 502

8. Kather R, Martin SJ: Evolution of cuticular hydrocarbons in the  Hymenoptera: a meta-analysis. J Chem Ecol 2015, 41:871-883. The first large-scale functional characterization of ORs in ants finds the 9exon family ORs to be narrowly tuned to individual substances.

495

503 504 505 506 507 508

509 510 511 512 513 514 515

7. 

9. 

Zhou X, Rokas A, Berger SL, Liebig J, Ray A, Zwiebel LJ: Chemoreceptor evolution in Hymenoptera and its implications for the evolution of eusociality. Genome Biol Evol 2015, 7:24072416. A comprehensive study of olfactory sensilla density across the phylogeny of halictid bees shows that high sensilla densities might be a facilitator rather than a consequence of social evolution. 10. Roux J, Privman E, Moretti S, Daub JT, Robinson-Rechavi M,  Keller L: Patterns of positive selection in seven ant genomes. Mol Biol Evol 2014, 31:1661-1685. The study shows that brain investment does not increase, but decrease with the emergence of sociality in wasps. 11. Pask GM, Slone JD, Millar JG, Das P, Moreira JA, Zhou X, Bello J, Berger SL, Bonasio R, Desplan C et al.: Specialized odorant receptors in social insects that detect cuticular hydrocarbon cues and candidate pheromones. Nat Commun 2017, 8:297.

516 517

12. Wittwer B, Hefetz A, Simon T, Murphy LEK, Elgar MA, Pierce NE, Kocher SD: Solitary bees reduce investment in communication compared with their social relatives. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2017, 114:6569-6574.

518 519 520 521 522

13. O’Donnell S, Bulova SJ, DeLeon S, Khodak P, Miller S, Sulger E: Distributed cognition and social brains: reductions in mushroom body investment accompanied the origins of sociality in wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Proc R Soc B 2015, 282.

523 524 525 526

14. Smith AR, Seid MA, Jimenez LC, Wcislo WT: Socially induced brain development in a facultatively eusocial sweat bee Megalopta genalis (Halictidae). Proc Biol Sci 2010, 277:21572163.

527 528

15. Seid MA, Junge E: Social isolation and brain development in the ant Camponotus floridanus. Naturwissenschaften 2016, 103:42.

529 530

16. Chittka L, Niven J: Are bigger brains better? Cur Biol 2009, 19: R995-R1008. Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

www.sciencedirect.com

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002

541 542

556 557 558

579 580

COIS 453 1–7

Sociality and communicative complexity Nehring and Steiger 7

587 588 589

39. Steiger S, Whitlow S, Peschke K, Mu¨ller JK: Surface chemicals inform about sex and breeding status in the biparental Burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides. Ethology 2009, 115:178-185.

55. Helantera¨ H, Strassmann JE, Carrillo J, Queller DC: Unicolonial ants: where do they come from, what are they and where are they going? Trends Ecol Evol 2009, 24:341-349.

636 637 638

590 591 592

40. Engel KC, Sto¨kl J, Schweizer R, Vogel H, Ayasse M, Ruther J, Steiger S: A hormone-related female anti-aphrodisiac signals temporary infertility and causes sexual abstinence to synchronize parental care. Nat Commun 2016, 7:11035.

56. Schausberger P: Kin recognition by juvenile predatory mites: prior association or phenotype matching? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2007, 62:119-125.

639 640 641

593 594 595

41. Amsalem E, Twele R, Francke W, Hefetz A: Reproductive competition in the bumble-bee Bombus terrestris: do workers advertise sterility? Proc R Soc B 2009, 276:1295-1304.

57. Dobler R, Ko¨lliker M: Kin-selected siblicide and cannibalism in the European earwig. Behav Ecol 2010, 21:257-263.

642 643

596

58. Grinsted L, Bilde T, d’Ettorre P: Cuticular hydrocarbons as potential kin recognition cues in a subsocial spider. Behav Ecol 2011, 22:1187-1194.

644 645 646

597 598

42. Maisonnasse A, Lenoir J-C, Beslay D, Crauser D, Le Conte Y: Eb-ocimene, a volatile brood pheromone involved in social regulation in the honey bee colony (Apis mellifera). PLoS ONE 2010, 5:e13531.

599 600 601

43. Traynor KS, Le Conte Y, Page RE Jr: Age matters: pheromone profiles of larvae differentially influence foraging behaviour in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Anim Behav 2015, 99:1-8.

59. Lihoreau M, Rivault C: Kin recognition via cuticular hydrocarbons shapes cockroach social life. Behav Ecol 2009, 20:46-53.

647 648 649

602 603

44. Godfray HCJ: Signalling of need by offspring to their parents. Nature 1991, 352:328-330.

60. Metzger M, Bernstein C, Hoffmeister TS, Desouhant E: Does kin recognition and sib-mating avoidance limit the risk of genetic incompatibility in a parasitic wasp? PLoS ONE 2010, 5:e13505.

650 651 652

604 605 606

45. Mas F, Ko¨lliker M: Maternal care and offspring begging in social insects: chemical signalling, hormonal regulation and evolution. Anim Behav 2008, 76:1121-1131.

653 654 655 656 657

607 608 609 610

61. Herzner G, Schmitt T, Heckel F, Schreier P, Strohm E: Brothers smell similar: variation in the sex pheromone of male European Beewolves Philanthus triangulum F. (Hymenoptera: Crabronidae) and its implications for inbreeding avoidance. Biol J Linn Soc 2006, 89:433-442.

46. Ko¨lliker M, Chuckalovcak JP, Haynes KF, Brodie ED: Maternal food provisioning in relation to condition-dependent offspring odours in burrower bugs (Sehirus cinctus). Proc R Soc B 2006, 273:1523-1528.

62. Grafen A: Do animals really recognize kin? Anim Behav 1990, 39:42-54.

658 659

611 612 613

47. Mas F, Haynes KF, Ko¨lliker M: A chemical signal of offspring quality affects maternal care in a social insect. Proc R Soc B 2009, 276:2847-2853.

63. D’Ettorre P, Heinze J: Individual recognition in ant queens. Curr Biol 2005, 15:2170-2174.

660 661

614

64. Tibbetts EA: Visual signals of individual identity in the wasp Polistes fuscatus. Proc Biol Sci 2002, 269:1423-1428.

662 663

48. Nehring V, Dani FR, Calamai L, Turillazzi S, Bohn H, Klass K-D, d’Ettorre P: Chemical disguise of myrmecophilous cockroaches and its implications for understanding nestmate recognition mechanisms in leaf-cutting ants. BMC Ecol 2016, 16:35.

65. Dreier S, D’Ettorre P: Social context predicts recognition systems in ant queens. J Evol Biol 2009, 22:644-649.

664 665

66. Leonhardt SD: Chemical ecology of stingless bees. J Chem Ecol 2017, 43:385-402.

666 667

67. Wilms J, Eltz T: Foraging scent marks of bumblebees: footprint cues rather than pheromone signals. Naturwissenschaften 2008, 95:149-153.

668 669 670

68. Moser JC: Inquiline roach responds to trail-marking substance of leaf-cutting ants. Science 1964, 143:1048-1049.

671 672

69. Gronenberg W, Riveros AJ: Social brains and behavior: past and present. In Organization of Insect Societies: From Genome to Sociocomplexity. Edited by Gadau J, Fewell J. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2009:377-401.

673 674

615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622

49. Van Zweden JS, Brask JB, Christensen JH, Boomsma JJ, Linksvayer TA, d’Ettorre P: Blending of heritable recognition cues among ant nestmates creates distinct colony gestalt odours but prevents within-colony nepotism. J Evol Biol 2010, 23:1498-1508.

623

50. Arnold G, Quenet B, Cornuet J-M, Masson C, De Schepper B, Estoup A, Gasqui P: Kin recognition in honeybees. Nature 1996, 379:498.

624 625 626

51. Helantera¨ H, d’Ettorre P: A comparative study of egg recognition signature mixtures in Formica ants. Evolution 2015, 69:520-529.

627 628 629

52. Nehring V, Evison SE, Santorelli LA, d’Ettorre P, Hughes WO: Kininformative recognition cues in ants. Proc R Soc B 2011, 278:1942-1948.

70. Steiger S: Recognition and family life: recognition mechanisms in the biparental burying beetle. In Social Recognition in Invertebrates. Edited by Aquiloni L, Tricarico E. Springer International Publishing; 2015:249-266.

675 676

630 631 632

53. Helantera¨ H, Aehle O, Roux M, Heinze J, d’Ettorre P: Familybased guilds in the ant Pachycondyla inversa. Biol Lett 2013, 9:20130125.

71. Queller DC, Strassmann JE: Beyond society: the evolution of organismality. Philos Trans R Soc B 2009, 364:3143-3155.

677 678

633 634 635

54. Leadbeater E, Dapporto L, Turillazzi S, Field J: Available kin recognition cues may explain why wasp behavior reflects relatedness to nest mates. Behav Ecol 2013, 25:344-351.

72. McNally L, Jackson AL: Cooperation creates selection for tactical deception. Proc R Soc B 2013, 280.

679 680

www.sciencedirect.com

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2018, 28:1–7

Please cite this article in press as: Nehring V, Steiger S: Sociality and communicative complexity: insights from the other insect societies, Curr Opin Insect Sci (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cois.2018.04.002