Strain vulnerability and resiliency in the chick anxiety–depression model

Strain vulnerability and resiliency in the chick anxiety–depression model

Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/p...

931KB Sizes 1 Downloads 56 Views

Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Physiology & Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/phb

Strain vulnerability and resiliency in the chick anxiety–depression model Kristen A. Hymel a,⁎, Amy L. Salmeto a, Melissa J. Loria a, Stephen W. White a, Kenneth J. Sufka a,b,c a b c

Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, Oxford, MS 38677, USA Department of Pharmacology, University of Mississippi, Oxford, MS 38677, USA Research Institute of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Mississippi, Oxford, MS 38677, USA

H I G H L I G H T S • • • • •

Strain differences were assessed in the chick anxiety–depression model. Social isolation produced differences in onset of depression thresholds. Black Australorp strain was determined to be a vulnerable genetic line. Production Red strain was determined to be a resilient genetic line. Intermediate responsivity was the Silver Laced Wyandotte strain.

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Received 16 January 2013 Received in revised form 23 July 2013 Accepted 25 July 2013 Keywords: Chick Depression Endophenotype Strain Vulnerability Resiliency

a b s t r a c t Increasing research is focused on genetic contributions to variability in stress-related endophenotypes in humans and animal model simulations. The current study sought to identify strain vulnerabilities and resiliencies to an isolation-stressor in the chick anxiety–depression model. Nine different strains of socially raised chicks were tested in isolated or non-isolated conditions for 90 min in which distress vocalization (DVoc) rates were collected and then transformed to depression-like phase threshold (@ 25, 50, 75 and 95%) latencies. In general, chicks in the non-isolated condition displayed relatively low DVoc rates throughout the test session, despite some variability in initial rates. Chicks in the isolated condition displayed relatively high DVoc rates in the first 3 min, indicative of an anxiety-like state, which declined by approximately 50% within 10–25 min in all strains and remained stable thereafter, indicative of a depression-like state. Contrast effects revealed that, relative to all other strains, the Black Australorp strain displayed shorter and the Producrain displayed longer depression threshold latencies, respectively. Of the remaining strains, the Silver Laced Wyandotte displayed depression thresholds that best represent an intermediate stress response. These findings identify vulnerable and resilient strains for examining depression-related endophenotypes in the chick anxiety–depression model. © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Anxiety and depression are common and debilitating clinical disorders. While many patients show clinical improvement with anxiolytics and antidepressants, these drugs may produce unpleasant side effects, and a significant number of patients are unaffected by current pharmacotherapeutic options [1–4]. Understanding the neurochemical underpinnings and treatment targets relies, in part, on valid animal model simulations. Common rodent models of anxiety and depression include the elevated plus maze and the forced swim test, respectively. However, these paradigms fail to model the current understanding that anxiety and depression are related clinical syndromes [5–7]. ⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Peabody Building, University of Mississippi, Oxford, MS 38677, USA. Tel.: +1 662 915 5390. E-mail address: [email protected] (K.A. Hymel). 0031-9384/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.07.013

The chick anxiety–depression model [8] is a hybrid simulation of both syndromes in a single paradigm. The procedure involves an isolation stressor that initially produces high distress vocalization (DVoc) rates characteristic of an anxiety-like state [9–11]. This is followed by lower DVoc rates, characteristic of a depression-like state (i.e., behavioral despair) [12]. This model possesses face, construct and predictive validity in that these phases are pharmacologically dissociated in that diverse compounds possessing anxiolytic effects (e.g., chlordiazepoxide, clonidine and imipramine) attenuate the high DVoc rates during the anxiety-like phase while compounds possessing antidepressant effects (e.g., imipramine, maprotiline and fluoxetine) attenuate the reduction in DVoc rates during the depression-like phase [8,11,12]. Common stress and depression biomarkers are present in the model and include elevated corticosterone and interleukin-6 (IL-6) levels [8,11]. As a drug screening paradigm, the chick model has outperformed traditional rodent depression models [13,14] by avoiding two false positives (e.g., memantine and antalarmin) [15] which were ineffective in clinical trials

K.A. Hymel et al. / Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129

[16,17]. Further adding to the validity, the model displays cognitive biases in a manner parallel to those displayed in individuals presenting anxiety (i.e., more pessimism) and depressive disorders (i.e., more pessimism and less optimism) [18] the latter of which was reversed by the antidepressant imipramine [19]. Valid animal model simulations should also possess etiological homologies. Two factors associated with stress vulnerability are environmental and genetic mediators. Common environmental mediators associated with depressive episodes include abuse, neglect [20], low socioeconomic status and divorce [21], among others. Indeed, the chick anxiety–depression model has shown sensitivity to environmental manipulation [22], where chicks housed in an enriched homecage (e.g., perches, grit, mirrors and string) displayed a delayed onset of behavioral despair to the isolation stressor compared to chicks housed in standard caging. These findings are consistent with environmental enrichment delaying the onset of behavior despair in rodent models [23,24]. One genetic mediator associated with depression in humans includes polymorphisms (one or two copies of the short (S) allele) of the 5HTT gene [25,26]. Two approaches to studying genetic mediators to stress vulnerability in rodent models of depression are using selective inbred lines (e.g., Flinders Sensitive Line) [27,28] and gene knockout models (e.g., 5-HTT knockout lines) [29,30]. Selective breeding in chickens also yield variations in emotionality [31], stress responsivity and biological mechanisms [32]. For example, differences in feeding, comforting behaviors, adrenal gland size, corticosterone, 5-HT levels and 5-HT1A receptor expression between strains selected for varying commercial productivity and survivability following transportation stress [32]. The existing literature validating the chick anxiety–depression model was derived from a single white leghorn strain [33]. However, a wide variety of chick strains are available to explore stress vulnerability in this neuropsychiatric simulation. The ability to identify a stress-vulnerable (and possibly resilient) strain would provide yet another homology between the chick anxiety–depression model and the clinical syndrome. Moreover, such work could then afford the exploration of complex interactions between gene and environmental mediators that mitigate stress vulnerability and/or responsivity to pharmacological treatment. 2. Methods 2.1. Subjects and housing characteristics Nine different strains of cockerels (Gallus gallus, Ideal Poultry Cameron, TX, USA) were received 2 days post hatch and housed in a 34 × 57 × 40 cm stainless steel cages with 12–13 chicks per cage. Strains were selected on the basis of feather pigmentation diversity and hatch availability within the time parameters of this study. Pigmentation [34] and associated genes [35] have been shown related to physiological and behavioral differences in aves, and include aggressiveness [35,36] and exploratory and social behaviors [35]. The strains included were Ameraucana, Barred Rock, Black Australorp, Buff, White Leghorn 236, New Hampshire Red, Production Red, Rhode Island Red and Silver Laced Wyandotte. Food (Purina Start and Grow, St Louis, Missouri, USA) and water was available ad libitum through one quart gravity-fed feeders (Murray MacMurray; Model 4BGFJ) and waterers (Murray MacMurray; Model 4YQW0). Room temperature was maintained at 29 ± 1 °C and overhead illumination was maintained on a 12-h light–dark cycle (7 am–7 pm). 2.2. Isolation apparatus A six-unit test apparatus containing Plexiglas viewing chambers (25 × 25 × 22 cm) situated in sound-attenuating enclosures was used to collect isolation-induced distress vocalizations. The units were illuminated using 25 W light bulbs and ventilated by an 8-cm diameter rotary fan (Model FP-108AXS1; Commonwealth Industrial Corp. Taipei, Taiwan). Miniature video cameras (Model PC60XP; SuperCircuits,

125

Inc., Liberty Hill, Texas, USA) mounted at floor level in the corner of the enclosures and routed through a multiplexer (Model PC47MC; SuperCircuits, Inc.) allowed for animal observation. Distress vocalizations were collected via microphones [Model 3-675-001 (modified for AC current); Lafayette Instruments, Lafayette, Indiana, USA] mounted on the ceiling of the Plexiglas chamber and routed through a USB interface via custom-designed software. 2.3. Procedure Three strains were tested per session over three consecutive weeks. At ages 5–6 days post hatch, the chicks were removed from their home-cage, color-coded with a felt marker and weighed to identify potential outliers (i.e., low weights). The chicks were then placed into the isolation apparatus alone (isolated condition) or with two conspecifics and two 20.3 × 20.3 cm mirrors positioned alongside the walls (non-isolated condition). Distress vocalizations were recorded for 90 min; however, due to software collection error the test session ended at 89 min. Therefore, the depression-like phase constituted the last 59 min of the test session rather than 60 min. The animals were tested once and returned to their home cage following these experimental procedures. The chicks that served as conspecifics were used twice for the two squads that constituted the non-isolated condition. These protocols were approved by the University of Mississippi's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol no. 11-013). 2.4. Dependent measures DVocs were transformed into a rate/min function and collapsed across 3-min blocks before analyses. Separate ANOVAs (1- and 2-way) were conducted to examine the effects of isolation condition and isolation time for each strain. In order to assess the differences of onset of the depression-like phase, we calculated the time point at which each chick's DVoc rate/min from the anxiety-like phase (0–3 min) had declined by 25, 50, 75 and 95% to the rate/min of the depression-like phase (30–89 min). To elaborate, DVoc rates were compared minute by minute over consecutive 3-min blocks to determine the time point at which the average rate of that block was at or below these four thresholds; the middle time point of that block was operationally defined as the onset latency into 25, 50, 75 or 95% of the depression-like phase. 3. Results The effects of social separation stress on DVoc rates for each strain are presented in Fig. 1, panels A–I. In general, chicks in the nonisolated condition displayed fewer DVocs than isolated chicks throughout the test session. Several strains (e.g., Ameraucana, Black Australorp and New Hampshire Red) displayed unexpectedly higher DVocs (e.g., 35–80/3-min block) in this treatment condition which rapidly declined to comparable rates of other strains within 15–30 min of the test session. In contrast, chicks in the isolated condition initially displayed relatively high DVocs (N100/3-min block) which declined within 20 min to approximately 50% of the initial rate and remained stable thereafter. Consistent with these observations, 2-way between–within ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of isolation condition for each strain, F(1,638) = 51.60–168.32, ps b 0.0001, significant main effects of test session time for each strain F(29,638), 2.66–10.03, ps b 0.0001, and significant isolation × time interaction for all but one (i.e., New Hampshire Red) strain F(29,638), 1.17–4.78, ps b 0.0001. To illustrate differences in DVoc rate between the anxiety-like (0–3 min) and the depression-like phase (30–89 min), paired t-tests were conducted for each strain in the isolated condition. For each strain, a significant decrease in mean DVoc rate/min was observed between the phases, ts(11) = 4.85–10.67, ps b 0.0005.

126 K.A. Hymel et al. / Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129 Fig. 1. Mean (±SEM) distress vocalizations (DVocs) for isolated and non-isolated treatment conditions presented in 3-min blocks for nine strains (panels A–I). Sample sizes were n = 12. All isolated conditions revealed a significant decline in mean Dvoc rates from the anxiety-like phase (0–3 min) to the depression-like phase (30–89 min).

K.A. Hymel et al. / Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129

127

present similar vulnerability or resiliency to factors related to the experimental procedure (e.g., experimenter handling, apparatus novelty and exposure to non-cagemate conspecifics) separate from the isolation stress manipulation. To examine this, correlations between each strain's mean DVoc rates in non-isolated chicks in the first 3-min block and depression threshold latency in isolated chicks were conducted at each threshold (see Fig. 3A–D). Moderate to robust correlations were observed at all four depression thresholds and were significant at the 75%, r = −0.713, p = 0.015, and 95% thresholds, r = −0.822, p = 0.003. 4. Discussion

Fig. 2. Depression onset threshold for nine chick strains. Values represent mean (±SEM) time in minutes. See Methods section for formula. Sample sizes were n = 12.

In order to determine strain differences in stress vulnerability that would account for different base rates of DVocs, we transformed these data into onset of depression latency thresholds (see Fig. 2). At the 25% threshold, no strain differences were observed. However at the 50% threshold three distinct groups emerged. Seven of the strains clustered into one homogenous group while the Black Australorp had a shorter latency threshold and the Production Red had a longer latency threshold. This pattern also presented at the 75% and 95% thresholds. To examine strain differences in the onset of depression thresholds, 9 planned contrasts were performed to compare a single strain to the aggregate mean of the remaining 8 strains. These planned contrasts revealed two separate strains that differed from the aggregate mean; the Production Red strain displayed a longer latency at the 50% threshold (p = 0.001) and the Black Australorp strain displayed shorter latencies at all four thresholds (ps = 0.045–0.005). Interestingly, some strain variability in DVoc rate was observed under the non-isolated condition. We hypothesized that specific strains expressing vulnerability or resiliency in depression thresholds may also

The current study sought to identify strain vulnerabilities and resiliencies to an isolation-stressor in the chick anxiety–depression model. Nine different chick strains were tested in isolated and non-isolated conditions and distress vocalizations (DVocs) were collected. Isolation induced DVocs across strains were consistent with previous studies. Chicks in the anxiety-like phase (i.e., first 3 min) displayed high DVocs which was followed by ≃50% reduction of DVocs in the depression-like phase (i.e., last 59 min) [8]. Strain vulnerability and resiliency were revealed in the onset of depression threshold latencies. The Black Australorp strain displayed a shorter latency (i.e., vulnerability) at the 50% depression threshold, whereas the Production Red strain displayed longer latencies (i.e., resiliency) at all depression thresholds relative to the aggregate means of the comparative strains. This dependent measure aligns with the previous research in the chick model [22], as well as in similar measures in rodent models [23,24], in which depression onset was delayed by enriched housing environments. Thus, the observation of strain differences in the chick anxiety–depression model, which parallels to that of the rodent behavioral despair models [37–40], adds an important validation step in this neuropsychiatric simulation. One interesting finding was strain variation in DVocs in non-isolated conditions. We believe this also reflects strain mediated stress vulnerability or resiliency to some experimental procedures such as experimenter

Fig. 3. Relationship between DVoc rates in non-isolated chicks in the first 3-min block and depression threshold latency in isolated chicks for each depression threshold (panels A–D). Correlations were significant at the 75% and 95% thresholds, ps b 0.05.

128

K.A. Hymel et al. / Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129

related stress and test apparatus novelty. For example, previous research has shown that early handling and apparatus habituation reduces isolation stress responses in chicks [41]. In another study, non-isolated chicks receiving imipramine showed modest but measurable antidepressantlike effects on DVocs compared to vehicle controls [19]. Further, and perhaps more importantly, the present data show strong correlations between individual strains' initial rates of DVocs under non-isolated conditions and depression threshold latencies in their isolated counterparts. This observation provides convergent validity of strain mediated stress vulnerability and resilience in the chick anxiety–depression model. Historically, breeding programs in this research arena tended to focus on developing rodent lines exhibiting stress vulnerability [27]. More recent research has yielded murine in-bred lines that express high, intermediate and low stress responsivity [42,43]. Likewise, the present research identified not only a vulnerable (Black Australorp) and a resilient (Production Red) chick strain, but also a number of strains with intermediate stress responsivity. Within this group, the strain that deviated the least from the aggregate in depression threshold means was the Silver Laced Wyandotte. While the current research is the first to study strain differences in a behavioral despair model, others have reported strain differences in behavioral and physiological responses to other stressors. A study examining tonic immobility revealed that White Leghorns remained immobile for approximately 2–3 times longer than Babcock B-390, Production Red and Plymouth Rock strains. In addition, the White Leghorns also displayed longer ambulation latencies and lower overall ambulation scores compared to Production Reds during an open-field test. These findings suggest a greater emotionality component in the White Leghorn strain [31]. The mechanisms that underlie differences to isolation stress in these nine genetic lines are unknown. However, in a follow up study, social separation initially increased hippocampal BDNF levels in the vulnerable Black Australorps whereas levels in the resilient Production Reds remained stable [44]. These findings suggest that the vulnerable Black Australorps display poorer homeostatic stress control mechanisms that ultimately affect neurogenesis. Studies from other laboratories suggest numerous additional candidates in mediating strain differences in stress response and include corticosterone, serotonin and 5-HIAA, among others. For example [45], differences in serotonin receptor function have been identified in strains selected for high- (HGPS) versus lowgroup (LGPS) productivity and survivability. Increased feather pecking, a stress-related behavior, was observed under 5-HT1A antagonism in the LGPS strain; in contrast increased feather pecking was observed under 5-HT1B antagonism in the HGPS strain. Further studies using selective receptor agonists and antagonists in Black Australorps and Production Reds are likely to reveal many additional markers that highlight mechanisms that differentiate these lines. Many studies have observed strain differences in emotionality, behavioral patterns, stress responsivity and homeostatic mechanisms to improve scientific understanding and animal welfare and production in commercial industries. However, until now, none have used strain vulnerability and resiliency in chicks as a preclinical screening tool in a simulation of a neuropsychiatric condition. The identification of a stress vulnerable, resilient and intermediate strain not only further validates the chick anxiety–depression model, but may also provide a valuable comparative tool in future studies to examine genetic, endocrine, behavioral, cognitive, pharmacological sensitivity and environmental mediators of stress responsivity and, as such, may contribute to a better understanding of the etiology, symptomatology and treatment of depressive disorders. References [1] Davidson JRT, Connor KM. Treatment of anxiety disorders. In: Schatzberg AF, Nemeroff CB, editors. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of psychopharmacology. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.; 2004. p. 913–34.

[2] Krishnan KRR. Monoamine oxidase inhibitors. In: Schatzberg AF, Nemeroff CB, editors. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of psychopharmacology. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.; 2004. p. 303–14. [3] Nelson JC. Tricyclic and tetracyclic drugs. In: Schatzberg AF, Nemeroff CB, editors. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of psychopharmacology. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.; 2004. p. 207–30. [4] Rosenbaum JF, Tollefson GD. Fluoxetine. In: Schatzberg AF, Nemeroff CB, editors. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of psychopharmacology. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.; 2004. p. 231–46. [5] Frazer A, Morilak DA. What should animal models of depression model? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:515–23. [6] Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P. Grooming analysis algorithm for neurobehavioural stress research. Brain Res Protoc 2004;13(3):151–8. [7] Kalueff AV, Wheaton M, Murphy DL. What's wrong with my mouse model? Advances and strategies in animal modeling of anxiety and depression. Behav Brain Res 2007;179:1–18. [8] Sufka KJ, Feltenstein MW, Warnick JE, Acevedo EO, Webb HE, Cartwright CM. Modeling the anxiety–depression continuum hypothesis in domestic fowl chicks. Behav Pharmacol 2006;17:681–9. [9] Panksepp J, Vilberg T, Bean NJ, Coy DH, Kastin AJ. Reduction of distress vocalization in chicks by opiate-like peptides. Brain Res Bull 1978;3:663–7. [10] Panksepp J, Meeker R, Bean NJ. The neurochemical control of crying. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1980;12:437–43. [11] Warnick J, Huang C, Acevedo E, Sufka K. Modeling the anxiety–depression continuum in chicks. J Psychopharmacol 2009;23(2):143–56. [12] Lehr E. Distress call reactivation in isolated chicks: a behavioral indicator with high selectivity for antidepressants. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1989;97:145–6. [13] Kos T, Popik P. A comparison of the predictive therapeutic and undesired side-effects of the NMDA receptor antagonist, memantine, in mice. Behav Pharmacol 2005;16: 155–61. [14] Nielsen DM, Carey GJ, Gold LH. Antidepressant-like activity of corticotropin-releasing factor type-1 receptor antagonists in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 2004;499:135–46. [15] Sufka K, Warnick J, Pulaski C, Slauson S, Kim Y, Rimoldi J. Antidepressant efficacy screening of novel targets in the chick anxiety–depression model. Behav Pharmacol 2009;20(2):146–54. [16] Zarate CA, Singh JB, Quiroz JA, De Jesus G, Denicoff KK, Luckenbaugh DA, et al. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of memantine in the treatment of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:153–5. [17] Schechter LE, Ring RH, Beyer CE, Hughes ZA, Khawaja X, Malberg JE, et al. Innovative approaches for the development of antidepressant drugs: current and future strategies. Neuro Rx 2005;2:590–611. [18] Salmeto AL, Hymel KA, Carpenter EC, Brilot BO, Bateson M, Sufka KJ. Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety–depression model. Brain Res 2011;1373:124–30. [19] Hymel KA, Sufka KJ. Pharmacological reversal of cognitive bias in the chick anxiety depression model. Neuropharmacology 2012;62:161–6. [20] Spatz Widom C, Dumont K, Czaja SJ. A prospective investigation of major depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64(1):49–56. [21] Gilman SE, Kawachi I, Fitzmaurice GM, Buka SL. Family disruption in childhood and risk of adult depression. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160(5):939–46. [22] Kim EH, Sufka KJ. The effects of environmental enrichment in the chick anxiety depression model. Behav Brain Res 2011;221:276–81. [23] Brenes Saenz JC, Villagra OR, Fornaguera Trias J. Factor analysis of forced swimming test sucrose preference test and open field test on enriched social and isolated reared rats. Behav Brain Res 2006;169:57–65. [24] Cirulli F, Berry A, Bonsignore LT, Capone F, D'Andrea I, Aloe L, et al. Early life influences on emotional reactivity: evidence that social enrichment has greater effects than handling on anxiety-like behaviors, neuroendocrine responses to stress and central BDNF levels. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010;34:808–20. [25] Hamet P, Tremblay J. Genetics and genomics of depression. Metabolism 2005;54(5): 10–5. [26] Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 2003;301(5631):386–9. [27] Overstreet DH. The Flinders Sensitive Line rats: a genetic animal model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1993;17(1):51–68. [28] Overstreet DH, Friedman E, Mathé AA, Yadid G. The Flinders Sensitive Line rat: a selectively bred putative animal model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29(4–5):739–59. [29] Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Abnormal behavioral phenotypes of serotonin transporter knockout mice: parallels with human anxiety and depression. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54(10):953–9. [30] Holmes A, Yang RJ, Lesch KP, Crawley JN, Murphy DL. Mice lacking the serotonin transporter exhibit 5-HT1A receptor-mediated abnormalities in anxiety-like and exploratory behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003;28(12):2077–88. [31] Gallup GG, Ledbetter DH, Maser JD. Strain differences among chickens in tonic immobility: evidence for an emotionality component. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1976;90(11):1075–81. [32] Cheng HW, Jefferson L. Different behavioral and physiological responses in two genetic lines of laying hens after transportation. Poult Sci 2008;87(5):885–92. [33] Hymel KA, Salmeto AL, Kim EH, Sufka KJ. Development and validation of the chick anxiety depression continuum model. In: Warnick JE, Kalueff AV, editors. Translational neuroscience and its advancement of animal research: advancement, challenges, and research ethics. Nova Science Publishers; 2010. p. 83–110. [34] Lee F, Keeler CE. Pigment variations and their correlates in birds. Auk 1951;68(1): 80–5.

K.A. Hymel et al. / Physiology & Behavior 120 (2013) 124–129 [35] Karlsson A-C, Kerje S, Andersson L, Jensen P. Genotype at the PMEL17 locus affects social and explorative behavior in chickens. Br Poult Sci 2010;51: 170–7. [36] Karlsson A-C, Mormede P, Kerje S, Jensen P. Genotype on the pigmentation regulating PMEL17 gene affects behavior in chickens raised without physical contact with conspecifics. Behav Genet 2011;41(2):312–22. [37] Österlund MK, Overstreet DH, Hurd YL. The Flinders Sensitive Line rats, a genetic model of depression, show abnormal serotonin receptor mRNA expression in the brain that is reversed by 17β-estradiol. Mol Brain Res 1999;74(1–2): 158–66. [38] Kanemaru K, Diksic M. The Flinders Sensitive Line of rats, a rat model of depression, has elevated brain glucose utilization when compared to normal rats and the Flinders Resistant Line of rats. Neurochem Int 2009;55(7): 655–61. [39] Murphy DL, Qian L, Engel S, Wichem C, Andrews A, Lesch KP, et al. Genetic perspectives on the serotonin transporter. Brain Res Bull 2001;56(5):487–94.

129

[40] Carroll JC, Boyce-Rustay JM, Millstein R, Yang R, Wiedholz LM, Murphy DL, et al. Effects of mild early life stress on abnormal emotion-related behaviors in 5-HTT knockout mice. Behav Genet 2007;37(1):214–22. [41] Feltenstein MW, Ford NG, Freeman KB, Sufka KJ. Dissociation of stress behaviors in the chick social-separation-stress procedure. Physiol Behav 2002;75:675–9. [42] Touma C, Bunck M, Glasl L, Nussbaumer M, Palme R, Stein H, et al. Mice selected for high versus low stress reactivity: a new animal model for affective disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008;33(6):839–62. [43] Pillai AG, de Jong D, Kanatsou S, Krugers H, Knapman A, Heinzmann JM, et al. Dendritic morphology of hippocampal and amygdalar neurons in adolescent mice is resilient to genetic differences in stress reactivity. PLoS One 2012;7(6):e38971. [44] Loria MS, White SW, Robbins SA, Salmeto AL, Hymel KA, Murthy SN, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor response in vulnerable and resilient genetic lines in the chick anxiety–depression model. Behav Brain Res 2013;245:29–33. [45] Dennis RL, Chen ZQ, Cheng HW. Serotonergic mediation of aggression in high and low aggressive chicken strains. Poult Sci 2008;87(4):612–20.