Journal Pre-proofs Full length article Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae) Ivan A. Kerchev PII: DOI: Reference:
S1226-8615(19)30419-4 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aspen.2020.01.011 ASPEN 1505
To appear in:
Journal of Asia-Pacific Entomology
Received Date: Revised Date: Accepted Date:
4 July 2019 15 January 2020 23 January 2020
Please cite this article as: I.A. Kerchev, Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the FourEyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae), Journal of Asia-Pacific Entomology (2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aspen.2020.01.011
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
© 2020 Korean Society of Applied Entomology. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1
Ivan A. Kerchev
2
Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir
3
Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae).
4 5 6
Institute of Monitoring of Climatic and Ecological Systems of the Siberian Branch of Russian Academy of Sciences, Tomsk, 634055, Russia e-mail:
[email protected] Phone / fax: (+7-3822)491855 / (+7-3822) 491978
7 8
Abstract
9
Acoustic signals are an essential part of the multi-modal systems of conspecific
10
communication among insects. The patterns of abiotic factors effects on their communication
11
parameters are of great interest for prognostic purposes in current climatic instability and for
12
practical application in order to manage their populations.
13
The aim of this study was to reveal the dependence of the parameters of acoustic signals
14
produced by bark beetle Polygraphus proximus, an aggressive alien stem pest on the
15
environmental temperature. Male stridulatory signals were recorded in seven temperature
16
settings (5–35 °C), and changes in their temporal parameters were evaluated under laboratory
17
conditions.
18
The obtained results allowed us to reveal the pattern of temperature dependence of
19
signals produced by P. proximus. More than half of the insects were found to actively stridulate
20
at 5 °C. Raising temperature caused an increment in chirp rate that continued to increase up to 30
21
°C. Further temperature increase led to suppression of signaling in most of the tested males. The
22
obtained results showed a number of patterns of temperature effect on the parameters of acoustic
23
signals which might be universal for representatives of different bark beetles genera with various
24
stridulatory apparatus types.
25 26
Key words: bark beetle, temperature effect, acoustic signals, chirp 1
27
Introduction
28
Insects are poikilothermic animals, and much of their activity is directly dependent on
29
environmental conditions (Walker, 1975; Pires and Hoy, 1992; Sanborn, 2004). Temperature
30
affects the rate of biochemical processes in the insect body and the neuromuscular response,
31
which strongly modulates acoustic communication (Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Sanborn, 2005).
32
This area of study is of current relevance because of the need to predict possible effects of global
33
warming on important components of terrestrial and aquatic ecosystems (Pörtner and Knust,
34
2007; Kearney, et al., 2009; Conrad et al., 2017). One of the important issues that influences
35
temperature is determining the mechanism to correctly recognize a signal that is modified by
36
exposure to various temperatures. When choosing a sexual partner, it is assumed that this
37
problem can be solved by insects in two ways Conrad et al., 2017). One way is “temperature
38
coupling”, where a change in the signal is accompanied by parallel changes in receiver
39
preferences resulting from a change in temperature (Gerhardt, 1978). Another possibility is that
40
the receiver preference stays the same, regardless of temperature. In this case, the signals that are
41
less modulated by temperature would be recognized correctly, and in the case of mating signals,
42
will be more attractive (von Helversen and von Helversen, 1981; Ritchie et al., 2001)
43
The influence of temperature fluctuations on the parameters of acoustic signals was
44
studied mostly in a few taxonomic groups that were preferred by bioacoustics specialists:
45
Orthoptera and Hemiptera (Doherty, 1985; Souroukis et. al., 1991; Pires and Hoy, 1992;
46
Korsunovskaya and Zhantiev, 2007; Zhemchuzhnikov and Knyazev, 2015). Narrow selectivity
47
during choosing a model organism caused significant disproportion in the study of the effect of
48
temperature on the insect’s acoustic communication and did not involve the largest orders, such
49
as Coleoptera, Hymenoptera, and Hemiptera (Fonseca and Revez, 2002; Sanborn, 2004; Andrew
50
et al., 2013; Conrad et al., 2017). The range of studied species should be expended because of
51
effects of polysemy in communication activity that is observed even within congeners (Sanborn
52
and Mater, 2000; Fonseca and Revez, 2002; Sueur and Sanborn 2003; Sanborn, 2006).
53
Bark beetles (Scolytinae) are representatives of the family Curculionidae, which is one of
54
the largest beetle families, and they are important agents in the carbon cycle in forest ecosystems
55
(Weed et al., 2015). According to the latest reports, the ability of acoustic signaling in this group
56
is observed in 107 species, which were mainly studied in genera such as Dendroctonus Erichson,
57
Ips De Geer, and Scolytus Geoffroy (Dobai, 2017), which have significant ecological and
58
economic impacts through periodical outbreaks.
2
59
In our study, Polygraphus proximus, a Far Eastern stem pest of the trees from genus
60
Abies (Baranchikov, 2010), was chosen for the experiment. Because this pest is an exceptional
61
threat to the boreal forests of the northern hemisphere, it has been included on the European and
62
Mediterranean Plant Protection Organization Alert list since 2014. The invasion of this species
63
into the territory of Southern Siberia from the Far East led to large-scale degradation of
64
aboriginal dark-coniferous forests (Krivets et al., 2015; Kononov et al., 2016). Currently, P.
65
proximus is a model that is used to study the various ecological effects of forest pest invasions on
66
the environment (Kerchev, 2014; Krivets et al., 2015) and the behavior of bark beetles including
67
acoustic communication (Kerchev, 2019). P. proximus stridulates during different types of
68
intraspecific communication by producing signals that are distinguishable based on temporal
69
parameters. The first signal appears under the elyta-tergal type of stridulating, which is noted in
70
three contexts; these contexts are stress, rivalry behavior, and courtship. During courtship, males
71
also produce the second type of chirps, which are registered only in male−female interactions
72
inside galleries. Pre-copula signals are produced by rubbing the tibia against the elytral margin
73
just before copulation (Kerchev, 2019).
74 75
The aim of this study was to reveal the dependence of acoustic signal parameters on changes in the ambient air temperature in bark beetles.
76 77
Materials and methods
78
Collection and storage of insects
79
For the experiments, overwintering P. proximus imago specimens were collected together
80
with fir bark from their brood trees in May 2018 in the Tomsk Region of the dark-conifer
81
plantation in the Kornilovskoe forest (56°49′15″ N; 85°37′07″ W). Sexual identification was
82
performed based on morphological characteristics. The males were distinguished by the presence
83
of two frontal protuberances, and the females were identified by dense seta on the frons (Stark
84
1952; Kerchev, 2014). Insects were placed individually in separate labelled 5-mL glass tubes 3
85
containing crushed pieces of fir bark, and the tubes were closed on the top with a moistened
86
cotton plug. Before the experiment, the beetles were stored in the TS-1/20 SPU incubator (JSC
87
Smolensk SKTB SPU, Smolensk) at 4°C for 2 days.
88 89 90 91
Design Recordings of male−female interactions were obtained using a microphone that was placed inside a tube (diameter, 1 cm), which represents the arena (Kerchev, 2019).
92
The audio recording was performed using a Behringer condenser microphone (Willich-
93
Münchheide II, Germany) (model: ECM 8000; 15–20 000 Hz), and a Zoom R16 digital recorder
94
(Tokyo, Japan) with a 20 Hz–44.1 kHz frequency range and a 24-bit sampling rate. The recorded
95
signals were saved in the WAV format.
96
Signals were produced using an elytra-tergal stridulatory apparatus (Kerchev, 2019)
97
under seven different temperature conditions, ranging from 5°C to 35°C, which were increased
98
in increments of 5°C for each pair of beetles. The duration of each audio recording was 15
99
minutes. This time period was chosen because it was insufficient for the beetles to intrude into
100
the bark and to change the context of interaction between them.
101
Before recording at each temperature, a continuous pure tone that was centered at the
102
mean dominant frequency (9.8 kHz; 40 dB) was produced by the tone generator (GWIinstec
103
SFG2107; Taiwan, China) and was recorded. The distance from the microphone to the resonator
104
400 RT 160 (40 kHz; 74 dB) in both cases was 1.5 cm. No distortion of the time and frequency
105
parameters of the test signal was detected during temperature changes.
106
The temperature regime under the audio recording was changed every 24 hours. In the
107
intervals between the experiments, insects were placed into tubes and stored at 5°C. To prevent
108
insect damage to each other in the interval between recordings, they were placed individually in
109
numbered 2-mL glass tubes that were closed with a moistened swab. Before each recording, the
4
110
insects underwent a 2-hour adaptation period to the new temperature regime, after which they
111
were released into the arena (Fig. 1).
112 113
Terminology and measurements
114
For each record, indicators such as syllable duration, inter-syllable interval, number of
115
chirps per syllable, chirp rate, chirp duration, inter-chirp interval, number of tooth-strikes, and
116
inter-tooth-strike interval (Fig. 2) were analyzed based on the terminology proposed in previous
117
studies (Ryker and Rudinsky, 1976; Pureswaran et al., 2016; Kerchev, 2019). Individual chirps
118
were identified using the band-limited energy detector in Raven Pro 1.5 (Cornell Lab of
119
Ornithology; Ithaca, NY, USA) (Charif et al., 2010). A syllable and the minimum interval
120
between adjacent syllables were empirically found on the sonogram for each recording at a
121
distance between the chirp series that exceeded the average interval between the minimal groups
122
of chirps (three intervals were used for analysis).
123 124
Statistical analysis
125
Each parameter in each record was measured at five points that were calculated using the
126
RANDBETWEEN function in Microsoft Excel. The mean values that were taken for each
127
recording were analyzed, and a consistency check of the parameter variations was performed
128
using the Kendall coefficient of concordance (W). Signal parameters were compared using the
129
Kruskal–Wallis test (H-test), and for statistically significant differences, multiple comparisons
130
were performed using the FDR correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). The Spearman’s
131
rank correlation coefficient (rs) was used to determine the strength of the relationship between
132
signal parameters and temperature conditions. All of the statistical analyses were performed
133
using Statistica 8.0 (StatSoft Inc.; Tulsa, OK, USA). In all cases, p < 0.05 was considered
134
significant, p < 0.01 highly significant and p < 0.001 very highly significant.
135
5
136
Results
137
During the recordings at 5–30°C, at least 22 out of 37 beetles were found to produce
138
signals (e.g. a set of 10-s records of stridulation by the same male at different temperatures; see
139
the media file (Kerchev, 2020)). When the temperature was increased to 35°C, only eight male
140
beetles continued to stridulate. Only four specimens stridulated during all seven temperature
141
conditions. The control group consisting of four beetles, which was recorded at 22°C over the
142
entire experimental period, consistently retained the ability to signal.
143
Analysis of the signal characteristic variability in males showed a high consistency for
144
such characteristics such as intertooth-strike interval, inter-chirp interval, chirp duration, and
145
chirp rate (Table 1).
146 147
The median concordance was established for the parameters of syllable duration and number of chirps per syllable (Table 1).
148
For multiple comparisons, highly significant differences were established for almost all
149
signal parameters under the tested settings, except for the number of chirps per syllable and the
150
number of tooth-strikes per chirp.
151
Among the analyzed characteristics of the P. proximus signals, a strong correlation
152
between ambient temperature and chirp rate was found (Spearman rank-order correlations (rs =
153
0.8; p <0.05).
154
The average coefficient of the negative correlation with temperature increase was noted
155
for several parameters (Fig. 3), as follows: chirp duration, inter-chirp interval, and inter-tooth-
156
strike interval (rs = −0.6; p<0.05). Weak feedback with temperature was observed for the
157
duration of the inter-syllable interval (rs = −0.4; p <0.05) and syllable duration (rs = −0.3; p
158
<0.05) were observed. No statistically significant correlations with temperature were established
159
for the number of tooth-strikes or chirps per syllable (rs = 0.02; rs = 0.04; p>0.05, respectively).
160 161
Discussion 6
162 163
The correlation between the temperature change and the temporal signal parameters has been established for the first time in bark beetles and for Coleopteran insects in general.
164
There was no statistically significant relationship between syllable duration and the
165
number of chirps per syllable, and it is likely to be more dependent on the context, as described
166
previously (Kerchev, 2019). Temperature fluctuations did not yield significant changes in the
167
number of tooth-strikes per chirp. In the signals that were produced by bark beetles in the genus
168
Dendroctonus, this parameter was directly related to the number of ridges on the pars stridens
169
(Fleming, 2013; Yturalde and Hofstetter, 2015). We previously found that the number of tooth-
170
strikes may vary depending on the context of the produced signal (Kerchev, 2019).
171
A strong correlation between the rate of chirps per unit of time and the increase in
172
temperature was noted previously for many Orthoptera representatives (Doherty, 1985; Beckers
173
and Schul, 2008) and other cold-blooded animals such as amphibians (Gayou, 1984). An
174
increase in the chirp rate also caused a decrease in chirp duration, without changes in the
175
amplitude of abdominal movement, which otherwise would have an influence on the number of
176
tooth-strikes per chirp.
177
As shown by neurophysiological studies of grasshoppers and crickets, the impulses of
178
some spontaneously active neurons in the insect’s central nervous system result from phase
179
rearrangements, which are able to synchronize mainly with conspecific signals. This mechanism,
180
acting as an internal oscillator, makes it possible to separate intraspecific signals with a slight
181
differece in rhythm from the external noise (Zhantiev and Korsunovskaya, 2014). The
182
mechanisms used in mass synchronization of conspecific signals in insects that use another
183
channel for communication are probably of the same neurophysiological nature (e.g. fireflies,
184
Carlson et al., 1976). Thus, significant signal modulations that are noted in the temperature range
185
from 5 to 30°C and the operation of their recognition mechanism to maintain the role of
186
intraspecific communication, most likely work in accordance with the “temperature coupling”
187
theory that was mentioned earlier. 7
188
The number of insects that produced signals (n = 22) shows that the low-temperature
189
setup (5°C) is not a threshold for production of acoustic signals and general activity of the adult
190
species. This temperature is 10°C lower than that at which mass swarming of beetles occurs in
191
the region of invasion (Kerchev, 2014). Thus, acoustic signals are the first way for bark beetles
192
to communicate at the imago stage, and it probably retains priority significance until feeding on
193
the tissues of a fresh host tree.
194
We assume that collective stridulation of males after wintering may serve as a signal to
195
the colony beneath the bark that the environmental temperature has reached values that are
196
suitable for dispersal flight, or as a signal for females to emerge from the brood tree.
197
A dramatically reduced number of stridulating males at 35°C also indicates that this
198
temperature is above the optimum condition limits. In the field, P. proximus avoids colonizing
199
sides of the tree trunk that can be exposed to the direct sunlight.
200
Although none of the tested males that were released into the arena were under the
201
protection of their nest or behaving as if to attack a stranger, the characteristics of their signals
202
(except for syllable parameters) were similar in terms of temporal parameters to the signals of
203
the rivalry or aggression context (Kerchev, 2019). Probably a necessary condition for male
204
stridulation in the context of courtship is the presence of aggregation pheromones, the production
205
of which was excluded by the design of the experiment.
206
Application of the acoustic or vibrational signals in addition to semiochemicals to disrupt
207
the mechanisms of transmission or perception of intraspecific signals is a developing area of pest
208
control (Mazzoni et al., 2018). This approach, as well as methods for identifying wood boring
209
pests by species-specific signals (Mankin et al., 2008), requires a better understanding of the
210
signal parameters depending on variations in environmental factors, and experimental studies
211
can significantly increase our knowledge of these patterns. This will ultimately lead to increased
212
effectiveness of their practical application.
213
8
214 215 216
Conclusions Temporal characteristics of acoustic signals that are produced by P. proximus are temperature dependent.
217
Temperature affects the production of acoustic signals by P. proximus through changes in
218
motility during stridulation, as evidenced by the altered signal characteristics that are dependent
219
on the frequency of insect movements.
220
These results made it possible to establish several patterns in the temperature dependence
221
of acoustic signaling in bark beetles, which might be universal for species with different types of
222
stridulation. However, for other genera, and possibly for species within the genus Polygraphus,
223
tendencies and the dependence on temperature effects can be different and may require an
224
individual approach to target various species.
225 226
Acknowledgements:
227
The author owes an immense gratitude to the assistant Julia A. Tsoi for conducting laboratory
228
recordings. This research was supported by the Russian Science Foundation (17-74-10034).
229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238
References Beckers O.M., and Schul J., 2008. Developmental plasticity of mating calls enables acoustic communication in diverse environments. Proc. Biol. Sci. 275, 1243–1248. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y., 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat .Soc. Series B. 57(1), 289–300. Bertram S.M., 2000. The influence of age and size on temporal mate signaling behavior. Anim. Behav. 60, 333–339. Carlson A.D., Copeland J., Raderman R., Bulloch A.G.M., 1976. Role of interflash intervals in a firefly courtship (Photinus macdermotti). Anim. Behav. 24, 786–792.
9
239 240
Charif R.A, Waack A.M., Strickman L.M., 2010. Raven Pro 1.4 User’s Manual. Revision 11. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, New York, pp. 283–287.
241
Ciceran M., Murray A.-M., Rowell G., 1994. Natural variations in the temporal
242
patterning of calling song structure in the field cricket Gryllus pennsylvanicus: effects of
243
temperature, age, mass, time of day, and nearest neighbour. Can. J. Zool. 72, 38–42.
244
Conrad T., Stöcker C., Ayasse M., 2017. The effect of temperature on male mating
245
signals and female choice in the red mason bee, Osmia bicornis (L.). Ecol. Evol. 7, 8966–8975.
246
https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.3331
247
Dobai A., Sivalinghem S., Guedes R.N.C., Yack J., 2018. Acoustic communication in the
248
pine engraver bark beetle: do signals vary between behavioral contexts? Physiol. Entomol.
249
43(1)30–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/phen.12222
250
Fleming A.J., Lindeman A.A., Carroll A.L., and Yack J.E., 2013. Acoustics of the
251
Mountain Pine Beetle (Dendroctonus ponderosae) (Curculionidae, Scolytinae): Sonic,
252
Ultrasonic, and Vibration Characteristics. Canad. J. Zool. 91(4), 235–244.
253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260
Fonseca P.J., Revez M.A., 2002. Temperature dependence of cicada songs (Homoptera, Cicadoidea). J Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 187, 971–976. Gerhardt H.C., Huber F., 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. University of Chicago Press. Chicago, 531 pp. Gerhardt, H.C. 1978. Temperature coupling in the vocal communication system of the gray tree frog, Hyla versicolor. Science, 199, 992–994. Jacot A., Scheuber H., and Brinkhof M.W.G., 2007. The effect of age on a sexually selected acoustic display. Ethol. 113, 615–620.
261
Kearney M., Shine R., Porter W.P., 2009. The potential for behavioral thermoregulation
262
to buffer “cold-blooded” animals against climate warming. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106,
263
3835–3840. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808913106
10
264
Kerchev I.A., 2014. Ecology of Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle Polygraphus proximus
265
Blandf. (Coleoptera: Curculionidae, Scolytinae) in the West Siberian Region of Invasion. Russ.
266
J. Biol. Invasions 5(3), 176–185.
267
Kerchev I.A., 2019. Context-dependent acoustic signals in the four-eyed fir bark beetle,
268
Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae). Environ. Entomol. 48(1), 181–
269
188. https://doi.org/10.1093/ee/nvy178 Kerchev I.A., 2020. Influence of temperature on stridulation in Polygraphus
270 271
proximus.mp4. figshare. Media. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11611695.v1
272
Kononov A., Ustyantsev K., Blinov A., Fet V., and Baranchikov Y.N., 2016. Genetic
273
diversity of aboriginal and invasive populations of four-eyed fir bark beetle Polygraphus
274
proximus Blandford (Coleoptera, Curculionidae, Scolytinae). Agric. For. Entomol. 18(3), 294–
275
301.
276
Korsunovskaya O.S. and Zhantiev R.D. 2007. Effect of temperature on auditory receptor
277
functions in crickets (Orthoptera, Tettigoniodea). J. Evol. Biochem. Phys. 43, 327–334.
278
https://doi.org/10.1134/S0022093007030076
279
Krivets S.A., Bisirova E.M., Kerchev I.A., Pats E.N., and Chernova N.A., 2015.
280
Transformation of taiga ecosystems in the Western Siberian invasion focus of four-eyed fir bark
281
beetle Polygraphus proximus Blandford (Coleoptera: Curculionidae, Scolytinae). Russ. J. Biol.
282
Invasions 6(2), 94–108.
283
Mankin R.W., Smith M.T., Tropp J.M., Atkinson E.B., and Jong D.Y. 2008. Detection of
284
Anoplophora glabripennis (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) larvae in different host trees and tissues
285
by automated analyses of sound-impulse frequency and temporal patterns. J. Econ. Entomol.
286
101(3), 838–849.
287
Mazzoni V., Nieri R., Anfora G., Eeriksson A.E., Polajnar J., Virant-Doblert M., Lucchi
288
A., 2018. Vibrational signals as semiophysicals: a comparison between pheromonal and
11
289
vibrational mating disruption. Abstract Book Centro Congressi Riva del Garda, Trento, Italy
290
September 4–6. pp. 42.
291
Pires A. and Hoy R.R., 1992. Temperature coupling in cricket acoustic communication. J.
292
Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 171, 69–78. https://
293
doi.org/10.1007/BF00195962 Pörtner H.O, and Knust R., 2007. Climate change affects marine fishes through the
294 295
oxygen limitation of thermal tolerance. Science 315(5808), 95–97
296
Pureswaran D.S., Hofstetter R.W., Sullivan B.T., and Potter K.A., 2016. The role of
297
multimodal signals in species recognition between tree-killing bark beetles in a narrow sympatric
298
zone. Environ. Entomol. 45(3) 582–591. https://doi.org/10.1093/ee/nvw022
299
Ritchie, M. G., Saarikettu, M., Livingstone, S., & Hoikkala, A. 2001. Characterization of
300
female preference functions for Drosophila montana courtship song and a test of the temperature
301
coupling hypothesis. Evolution. 55, 721–727. Ryker L.C., and Rudinsky J.A., 1976. Sound production in Scolytidae: aggressive and
302 303
mating behavior of the mountain pine beetle. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 69, 677–680.
304
Sanborn A.F. 2006. Acoustic signals and temperature. In: Drosopoulos S, Claridge MF,
305
editors. Insect sounds and communication: physiology, behaviour, ecology and evolution. CRC
306
Taylor and Francis. Florida, pp. 111–125. Sanborn A.F., 2004. Thermoregulation and endothermy in the large western cicada
307 308
Tibicen cultriformis (Hemiptera: Cicadidae). J. Therm. Biol. 29(2), 97–101.
309
Sanborn A.F., Mater S., 2000. Thermoregulation and the effect of body temperature on
310
call temporal parameters in the cicada Diceroprocta olympusa (Homoptera: Cicadidae). Comp.
311
Biochem. Physiol., Part A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 125, 141–148.
312
Simmons L.W., 1995. Correlates of male quality in the field cricket, Gryllus campestris
313
L.: age, size, and symmetry determine pairing success in field populations. Behav. Ecol. 6, 376–
314
381. 12
315
Souroukis K., Cade W.H., and Rowell G., 1992. Factors that possibly influence variation
316
in the calling song of field crickets: Temperature, time and male size, age and wing morphology.
317
Can. J. Zool. 70, 950–955.
318 319 320 321 322 323
Stark V.N., 1952. Bark beetles / In: Stackelberg A.A. ed. Fauna of the USSR. Vol.XXXI. M.–L.: Izdatel'stvo Аkademii Nauk USSR, 462 pp. (In Russian) Sueur J., Sanborn A.F., 2003. Ambient temperature and sound power of cicada calling songs (Hemiptera: Cicadidae: Tibicina). Physiol. Entomol. 28, 340–343. von Helversen, D., & von Helversen, O. 1981. Korrespondenz zwischen Gesang und auslösendem Schema bei Feldheuschrecken. Nova Acta Leopold. 54, 449–462.
324
Walker T. 1975. Effects of temperature on rates in poikilotherm nervous systems:
325
Evidence from the calling songs of meadow katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae:Orchelimum)
326
and reanalysis of published data. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol.
327
101:57–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00660119
328
Weed A.S., Ayres V.P., Bentz B.J. 2015. Population Dynamics of Bark Beetles. Vega
329
F.E., Hofstetter R.W (Eds.), Bark beetle. Biology and ecology of native and invasive species.
330
Academic Press of Elsevier, London, pp. 157–176.
331
Yturralde K.M., and Hofstetter R.W., 2015. Characterization of stridulatory structures
332
and sounds of the larger Mexican pine beetle, Dendroctonus approximatus (Coleoptera:
333
Curculionidae: Scolytinae). Fla. Entomol. 98, 516–527
334
Zhantiev R.D., and Korsunovskaya O.S., 2014. On recognition of sound communication
335
signals in bush crickets (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Entomol. Rev. 94(6), 815–819.
336
https://doi.org/10.1134/S0013873814060025
337 338 339 340
13
341 342
Table 1. The results of the analysis of variability of the signal parameters in Polygraphus proximus males depending on temperature. Parameter
343 344
(N)
W
H-test
(r`-values)
(Z`-values)
Inter syllable interval
283
0.14
146.14*
Syllable duration
283
0.5
46.46*
Number of chirps/ syllable
283
0.31
14.3
Chirp rate
278
0.95
176.97*
Chirp duration
277
0.80
114.98*
Inter chirp interval
276
0.89
159.20*
Tooth-strikes/ chirp
274
0.09
12.8
Intertooth-strikes interval
276
0.85
122.96*
*p<0.001, without asterisk – insignificant. In the column H-test the Z-factor values (measure of statistical effect size) are given.
345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358
14
Figure legends:
359 360 361
Fig. 1. The experimental set inside thermostat for registration of Polygraphus proximus
362
signals during male-female interactions (A – condenser microphone in glass tube, B – the arena
363
with beetles inside marked by arrows).
364 365 366
Fig. 2. The graphical descriptions of temporal parameters of stridulatory signals in the four-eyed fir bark beetle measured in this study
367 368
Fig. 3. Temporal characteristics of simple chirps during male-female interactions under
369
tested temperature settings: A. – inter syllable interval; B – the number of chirps per syllable; C–
370
chirp rate; D – chirp duration; E – inter chirp interval; F – number of tooth-strikes; G –
371
intertooth-strikes interval; H – syllable duration.
372 373 374 375
15
376
16
377
378
379
Highlights 17
380 381 382
Stridulation is first way of communication between individuals of bark beetles occurring at the imago stage.
383 384
Temporal parameters of P. proximus acoustic signals are temperature-dependent.
385 386 387
Among the analyzed characteristics of P. proximus signals a high correlation were between temperature and the chirp rate
388 389 390
Presence of females in limited area not inside entrance hole provokes producing of a rivalry signaling by males.
391
18