Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae)

Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae)

Journal Pre-proofs Full length article Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus...

6MB Sizes 0 Downloads 22 Views

Journal Pre-proofs Full length article Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae) Ivan A. Kerchev PII: DOI: Reference:

S1226-8615(19)30419-4 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aspen.2020.01.011 ASPEN 1505

To appear in:

Journal of Asia-Pacific Entomology

Received Date: Revised Date: Accepted Date:

4 July 2019 15 January 2020 23 January 2020

Please cite this article as: I.A. Kerchev, Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the FourEyed Fir Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae), Journal of Asia-Pacific Entomology (2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aspen.2020.01.011

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

© 2020 Korean Society of Applied Entomology. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1

Ivan A. Kerchev

2

Temperature-Dependent Parameters of Stridulatory Signals in the Four-Eyed Fir

3

Bark Beetle, Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae).

4 5 6

Institute of Monitoring of Climatic and Ecological Systems of the Siberian Branch of Russian Academy of Sciences, Tomsk, 634055, Russia e-mail: [email protected] Phone / fax: (+7-3822)491855 / (+7-3822) 491978

7 8

Abstract

9

Acoustic signals are an essential part of the multi-modal systems of conspecific

10

communication among insects. The patterns of abiotic factors effects on their communication

11

parameters are of great interest for prognostic purposes in current climatic instability and for

12

practical application in order to manage their populations.

13

The aim of this study was to reveal the dependence of the parameters of acoustic signals

14

produced by bark beetle Polygraphus proximus, an aggressive alien stem pest on the

15

environmental temperature. Male stridulatory signals were recorded in seven temperature

16

settings (5–35 °C), and changes in their temporal parameters were evaluated under laboratory

17

conditions.

18

The obtained results allowed us to reveal the pattern of temperature dependence of

19

signals produced by P. proximus. More than half of the insects were found to actively stridulate

20

at 5 °C. Raising temperature caused an increment in chirp rate that continued to increase up to 30

21

°C. Further temperature increase led to suppression of signaling in most of the tested males. The

22

obtained results showed a number of patterns of temperature effect on the parameters of acoustic

23

signals which might be universal for representatives of different bark beetles genera with various

24

stridulatory apparatus types.

25 26

Key words: bark beetle, temperature effect, acoustic signals, chirp 1

27

Introduction

28

Insects are poikilothermic animals, and much of their activity is directly dependent on

29

environmental conditions (Walker, 1975; Pires and Hoy, 1992; Sanborn, 2004). Temperature

30

affects the rate of biochemical processes in the insect body and the neuromuscular response,

31

which strongly modulates acoustic communication (Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Sanborn, 2005).

32

This area of study is of current relevance because of the need to predict possible effects of global

33

warming on important components of terrestrial and aquatic ecosystems (Pörtner and Knust,

34

2007; Kearney, et al., 2009; Conrad et al., 2017). One of the important issues that influences

35

temperature is determining the mechanism to correctly recognize a signal that is modified by

36

exposure to various temperatures. When choosing a sexual partner, it is assumed that this

37

problem can be solved by insects in two ways Conrad et al., 2017). One way is “temperature

38

coupling”, where a change in the signal is accompanied by parallel changes in receiver

39

preferences resulting from a change in temperature (Gerhardt, 1978). Another possibility is that

40

the receiver preference stays the same, regardless of temperature. In this case, the signals that are

41

less modulated by temperature would be recognized correctly, and in the case of mating signals,

42

will be more attractive (von Helversen and von Helversen, 1981; Ritchie et al., 2001)

43

The influence of temperature fluctuations on the parameters of acoustic signals was

44

studied mostly in a few taxonomic groups that were preferred by bioacoustics specialists:

45

Orthoptera and Hemiptera (Doherty, 1985; Souroukis et. al., 1991; Pires and Hoy, 1992;

46

Korsunovskaya and Zhantiev, 2007; Zhemchuzhnikov and Knyazev, 2015). Narrow selectivity

47

during choosing a model organism caused significant disproportion in the study of the effect of

48

temperature on the insect’s acoustic communication and did not involve the largest orders, such

49

as Coleoptera, Hymenoptera, and Hemiptera (Fonseca and Revez, 2002; Sanborn, 2004; Andrew

50

et al., 2013; Conrad et al., 2017). The range of studied species should be expended because of

51

effects of polysemy in communication activity that is observed even within congeners (Sanborn

52

and Mater, 2000; Fonseca and Revez, 2002; Sueur and Sanborn 2003; Sanborn, 2006).

53

Bark beetles (Scolytinae) are representatives of the family Curculionidae, which is one of

54

the largest beetle families, and they are important agents in the carbon cycle in forest ecosystems

55

(Weed et al., 2015). According to the latest reports, the ability of acoustic signaling in this group

56

is observed in 107 species, which were mainly studied in genera such as Dendroctonus Erichson,

57

Ips De Geer, and Scolytus Geoffroy (Dobai, 2017), which have significant ecological and

58

economic impacts through periodical outbreaks.

2

59

In our study, Polygraphus proximus, a Far Eastern stem pest of the trees from genus

60

Abies (Baranchikov, 2010), was chosen for the experiment. Because this pest is an exceptional

61

threat to the boreal forests of the northern hemisphere, it has been included on the European and

62

Mediterranean Plant Protection Organization Alert list since 2014. The invasion of this species

63

into the territory of Southern Siberia from the Far East led to large-scale degradation of

64

aboriginal dark-coniferous forests (Krivets et al., 2015; Kononov et al., 2016). Currently, P.

65

proximus is a model that is used to study the various ecological effects of forest pest invasions on

66

the environment (Kerchev, 2014; Krivets et al., 2015) and the behavior of bark beetles including

67

acoustic communication (Kerchev, 2019). P. proximus stridulates during different types of

68

intraspecific communication by producing signals that are distinguishable based on temporal

69

parameters. The first signal appears under the elyta-tergal type of stridulating, which is noted in

70

three contexts; these contexts are stress, rivalry behavior, and courtship. During courtship, males

71

also produce the second type of chirps, which are registered only in male−female interactions

72

inside galleries. Pre-copula signals are produced by rubbing the tibia against the elytral margin

73

just before copulation (Kerchev, 2019).

74 75

The aim of this study was to reveal the dependence of acoustic signal parameters on changes in the ambient air temperature in bark beetles.

76 77

Materials and methods

78

Collection and storage of insects

79

For the experiments, overwintering P. proximus imago specimens were collected together

80

with fir bark from their brood trees in May 2018 in the Tomsk Region of the dark-conifer

81

plantation in the Kornilovskoe forest (56°49′15″ N; 85°37′07″ W). Sexual identification was

82

performed based on morphological characteristics. The males were distinguished by the presence

83

of two frontal protuberances, and the females were identified by dense seta on the frons (Stark

84

1952; Kerchev, 2014). Insects were placed individually in separate labelled 5-mL glass tubes 3

85

containing crushed pieces of fir bark, and the tubes were closed on the top with a moistened

86

cotton plug. Before the experiment, the beetles were stored in the TS-1/20 SPU incubator (JSC

87

Smolensk SKTB SPU, Smolensk) at 4°C for 2 days.

88 89 90 91

Design Recordings of male−female interactions were obtained using a microphone that was placed inside a tube (diameter, 1 cm), which represents the arena (Kerchev, 2019).

92

The audio recording was performed using a Behringer condenser microphone (Willich-

93

Münchheide II, Germany) (model: ECM 8000; 15–20 000 Hz), and a Zoom R16 digital recorder

94

(Tokyo, Japan) with a 20 Hz–44.1 kHz frequency range and a 24-bit sampling rate. The recorded

95

signals were saved in the WAV format.

96

Signals were produced using an elytra-tergal stridulatory apparatus (Kerchev, 2019)

97

under seven different temperature conditions, ranging from 5°C to 35°C, which were increased

98

in increments of 5°C for each pair of beetles. The duration of each audio recording was 15

99

minutes. This time period was chosen because it was insufficient for the beetles to intrude into

100

the bark and to change the context of interaction between them.

101

Before recording at each temperature, a continuous pure tone that was centered at the

102

mean dominant frequency (9.8 kHz; 40 dB) was produced by the tone generator (GWIinstec

103

SFG2107; Taiwan, China) and was recorded. The distance from the microphone to the resonator

104

400 RT 160 (40 kHz; 74 dB) in both cases was 1.5 cm. No distortion of the time and frequency

105

parameters of the test signal was detected during temperature changes.

106

The temperature regime under the audio recording was changed every 24 hours. In the

107

intervals between the experiments, insects were placed into tubes and stored at 5°C. To prevent

108

insect damage to each other in the interval between recordings, they were placed individually in

109

numbered 2-mL glass tubes that were closed with a moistened swab. Before each recording, the

4

110

insects underwent a 2-hour adaptation period to the new temperature regime, after which they

111

were released into the arena (Fig. 1).

112 113

Terminology and measurements

114

For each record, indicators such as syllable duration, inter-syllable interval, number of

115

chirps per syllable, chirp rate, chirp duration, inter-chirp interval, number of tooth-strikes, and

116

inter-tooth-strike interval (Fig. 2) were analyzed based on the terminology proposed in previous

117

studies (Ryker and Rudinsky, 1976; Pureswaran et al., 2016; Kerchev, 2019). Individual chirps

118

were identified using the band-limited energy detector in Raven Pro 1.5 (Cornell Lab of

119

Ornithology; Ithaca, NY, USA) (Charif et al., 2010). A syllable and the minimum interval

120

between adjacent syllables were empirically found on the sonogram for each recording at a

121

distance between the chirp series that exceeded the average interval between the minimal groups

122

of chirps (three intervals were used for analysis).

123 124

Statistical analysis

125

Each parameter in each record was measured at five points that were calculated using the

126

RANDBETWEEN function in Microsoft Excel. The mean values that were taken for each

127

recording were analyzed, and a consistency check of the parameter variations was performed

128

using the Kendall coefficient of concordance (W). Signal parameters were compared using the

129

Kruskal–Wallis test (H-test), and for statistically significant differences, multiple comparisons

130

were performed using the FDR correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). The Spearman’s

131

rank correlation coefficient (rs) was used to determine the strength of the relationship between

132

signal parameters and temperature conditions. All of the statistical analyses were performed

133

using Statistica 8.0 (StatSoft Inc.; Tulsa, OK, USA). In all cases, p < 0.05 was considered

134

significant, p < 0.01 highly significant and p < 0.001 very highly significant.

135

5

136

Results

137

During the recordings at 5–30°C, at least 22 out of 37 beetles were found to produce

138

signals (e.g. a set of 10-s records of stridulation by the same male at different temperatures; see

139

the media file (Kerchev, 2020)). When the temperature was increased to 35°C, only eight male

140

beetles continued to stridulate. Only four specimens stridulated during all seven temperature

141

conditions. The control group consisting of four beetles, which was recorded at 22°C over the

142

entire experimental period, consistently retained the ability to signal.

143

Analysis of the signal characteristic variability in males showed a high consistency for

144

such characteristics such as intertooth-strike interval, inter-chirp interval, chirp duration, and

145

chirp rate (Table 1).

146 147

The median concordance was established for the parameters of syllable duration and number of chirps per syllable (Table 1).

148

For multiple comparisons, highly significant differences were established for almost all

149

signal parameters under the tested settings, except for the number of chirps per syllable and the

150

number of tooth-strikes per chirp.

151

Among the analyzed characteristics of the P. proximus signals, a strong correlation

152

between ambient temperature and chirp rate was found (Spearman rank-order correlations (rs =

153

0.8; p <0.05).

154

The average coefficient of the negative correlation with temperature increase was noted

155

for several parameters (Fig. 3), as follows: chirp duration, inter-chirp interval, and inter-tooth-

156

strike interval (rs = −0.6; p<0.05). Weak feedback with temperature was observed for the

157

duration of the inter-syllable interval (rs = −0.4; p <0.05) and syllable duration (rs = −0.3; p

158

<0.05) were observed. No statistically significant correlations with temperature were established

159

for the number of tooth-strikes or chirps per syllable (rs = 0.02; rs = 0.04; p>0.05, respectively).

160 161

Discussion 6

162 163

The correlation between the temperature change and the temporal signal parameters has been established for the first time in bark beetles and for Coleopteran insects in general.

164

There was no statistically significant relationship between syllable duration and the

165

number of chirps per syllable, and it is likely to be more dependent on the context, as described

166

previously (Kerchev, 2019). Temperature fluctuations did not yield significant changes in the

167

number of tooth-strikes per chirp. In the signals that were produced by bark beetles in the genus

168

Dendroctonus, this parameter was directly related to the number of ridges on the pars stridens

169

(Fleming, 2013; Yturalde and Hofstetter, 2015). We previously found that the number of tooth-

170

strikes may vary depending on the context of the produced signal (Kerchev, 2019).

171

A strong correlation between the rate of chirps per unit of time and the increase in

172

temperature was noted previously for many Orthoptera representatives (Doherty, 1985; Beckers

173

and Schul, 2008) and other cold-blooded animals such as amphibians (Gayou, 1984). An

174

increase in the chirp rate also caused a decrease in chirp duration, without changes in the

175

amplitude of abdominal movement, which otherwise would have an influence on the number of

176

tooth-strikes per chirp.

177

As shown by neurophysiological studies of grasshoppers and crickets, the impulses of

178

some spontaneously active neurons in the insect’s central nervous system result from phase

179

rearrangements, which are able to synchronize mainly with conspecific signals. This mechanism,

180

acting as an internal oscillator, makes it possible to separate intraspecific signals with a slight

181

differece in rhythm from the external noise (Zhantiev and Korsunovskaya, 2014). The

182

mechanisms used in mass synchronization of conspecific signals in insects that use another

183

channel for communication are probably of the same neurophysiological nature (e.g. fireflies,

184

Carlson et al., 1976). Thus, significant signal modulations that are noted in the temperature range

185

from 5 to 30°C and the operation of their recognition mechanism to maintain the role of

186

intraspecific communication, most likely work in accordance with the “temperature coupling”

187

theory that was mentioned earlier. 7

188

The number of insects that produced signals (n = 22) shows that the low-temperature

189

setup (5°C) is not a threshold for production of acoustic signals and general activity of the adult

190

species. This temperature is 10°C lower than that at which mass swarming of beetles occurs in

191

the region of invasion (Kerchev, 2014). Thus, acoustic signals are the first way for bark beetles

192

to communicate at the imago stage, and it probably retains priority significance until feeding on

193

the tissues of a fresh host tree.

194

We assume that collective stridulation of males after wintering may serve as a signal to

195

the colony beneath the bark that the environmental temperature has reached values that are

196

suitable for dispersal flight, or as a signal for females to emerge from the brood tree.

197

A dramatically reduced number of stridulating males at 35°C also indicates that this

198

temperature is above the optimum condition limits. In the field, P. proximus avoids colonizing

199

sides of the tree trunk that can be exposed to the direct sunlight.

200

Although none of the tested males that were released into the arena were under the

201

protection of their nest or behaving as if to attack a stranger, the characteristics of their signals

202

(except for syllable parameters) were similar in terms of temporal parameters to the signals of

203

the rivalry or aggression context (Kerchev, 2019). Probably a necessary condition for male

204

stridulation in the context of courtship is the presence of aggregation pheromones, the production

205

of which was excluded by the design of the experiment.

206

Application of the acoustic or vibrational signals in addition to semiochemicals to disrupt

207

the mechanisms of transmission or perception of intraspecific signals is a developing area of pest

208

control (Mazzoni et al., 2018). This approach, as well as methods for identifying wood boring

209

pests by species-specific signals (Mankin et al., 2008), requires a better understanding of the

210

signal parameters depending on variations in environmental factors, and experimental studies

211

can significantly increase our knowledge of these patterns. This will ultimately lead to increased

212

effectiveness of their practical application.

213

8

214 215 216

Conclusions Temporal characteristics of acoustic signals that are produced by P. proximus are temperature dependent.

217

Temperature affects the production of acoustic signals by P. proximus through changes in

218

motility during stridulation, as evidenced by the altered signal characteristics that are dependent

219

on the frequency of insect movements.

220

These results made it possible to establish several patterns in the temperature dependence

221

of acoustic signaling in bark beetles, which might be universal for species with different types of

222

stridulation. However, for other genera, and possibly for species within the genus Polygraphus,

223

tendencies and the dependence on temperature effects can be different and may require an

224

individual approach to target various species.

225 226

Acknowledgements:

227

The author owes an immense gratitude to the assistant Julia A. Tsoi for conducting laboratory

228

recordings. This research was supported by the Russian Science Foundation (17-74-10034).

229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238

References Beckers O.M., and Schul J., 2008. Developmental plasticity of mating calls enables acoustic communication in diverse environments. Proc. Biol. Sci. 275, 1243–1248. Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y., 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat .Soc. Series B. 57(1), 289–300. Bertram S.M., 2000. The influence of age and size on temporal mate signaling behavior. Anim. Behav. 60, 333–339. Carlson A.D., Copeland J., Raderman R., Bulloch A.G.M., 1976. Role of interflash intervals in a firefly courtship (Photinus macdermotti). Anim. Behav. 24, 786–792.

9

239 240

Charif R.A, Waack A.M., Strickman L.M., 2010. Raven Pro 1.4 User’s Manual. Revision 11. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, New York, pp. 283–287.

241

Ciceran M., Murray A.-M., Rowell G., 1994. Natural variations in the temporal

242

patterning of calling song structure in the field cricket Gryllus pennsylvanicus: effects of

243

temperature, age, mass, time of day, and nearest neighbour. Can. J. Zool. 72, 38–42.

244

Conrad T., Stöcker C., Ayasse M., 2017. The effect of temperature on male mating

245

signals and female choice in the red mason bee, Osmia bicornis (L.). Ecol. Evol. 7, 8966–8975.

246

https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.3331

247

Dobai A., Sivalinghem S., Guedes R.N.C., Yack J., 2018. Acoustic communication in the

248

pine engraver bark beetle: do signals vary between behavioral contexts? Physiol. Entomol.

249

43(1)30–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/phen.12222

250

Fleming A.J., Lindeman A.A., Carroll A.L., and Yack J.E., 2013. Acoustics of the

251

Mountain Pine Beetle (Dendroctonus ponderosae) (Curculionidae, Scolytinae): Sonic,

252

Ultrasonic, and Vibration Characteristics. Canad. J. Zool. 91(4), 235–244.

253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260

Fonseca P.J., Revez M.A., 2002. Temperature dependence of cicada songs (Homoptera, Cicadoidea). J Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 187, 971–976. Gerhardt H.C., Huber F., 2002. Acoustic communication in insects and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions. University of Chicago Press. Chicago, 531 pp. Gerhardt, H.C. 1978. Temperature coupling in the vocal communication system of the gray tree frog, Hyla versicolor. Science, 199, 992–994. Jacot A., Scheuber H., and Brinkhof M.W.G., 2007. The effect of age on a sexually selected acoustic display. Ethol. 113, 615–620.

261

Kearney M., Shine R., Porter W.P., 2009. The potential for behavioral thermoregulation

262

to buffer “cold-blooded” animals against climate warming. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106,

263

3835–3840. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808913106

10

264

Kerchev I.A., 2014. Ecology of Four-Eyed Fir Bark Beetle Polygraphus proximus

265

Blandf. (Coleoptera: Curculionidae, Scolytinae) in the West Siberian Region of Invasion. Russ.

266

J. Biol. Invasions 5(3), 176–185.

267

Kerchev I.A., 2019. Context-dependent acoustic signals in the four-eyed fir bark beetle,

268

Polygraphus proximus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae). Environ. Entomol. 48(1), 181–

269

188. https://doi.org/10.1093/ee/nvy178 Kerchev I.A., 2020. Influence of temperature on stridulation in Polygraphus

270 271

proximus.mp4. figshare. Media. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11611695.v1

272

Kononov A., Ustyantsev K., Blinov A., Fet V., and Baranchikov Y.N., 2016. Genetic

273

diversity of aboriginal and invasive populations of four-eyed fir bark beetle Polygraphus

274

proximus Blandford (Coleoptera, Curculionidae, Scolytinae). Agric. For. Entomol. 18(3), 294–

275

301.

276

Korsunovskaya O.S. and Zhantiev R.D. 2007. Effect of temperature on auditory receptor

277

functions in crickets (Orthoptera, Tettigoniodea). J. Evol. Biochem. Phys. 43, 327–334.

278

https://doi.org/10.1134/S0022093007030076

279

Krivets S.A., Bisirova E.M., Kerchev I.A., Pats E.N., and Chernova N.A., 2015.

280

Transformation of taiga ecosystems in the Western Siberian invasion focus of four-eyed fir bark

281

beetle Polygraphus proximus Blandford (Coleoptera: Curculionidae, Scolytinae). Russ. J. Biol.

282

Invasions 6(2), 94–108.

283

Mankin R.W., Smith M.T., Tropp J.M., Atkinson E.B., and Jong D.Y. 2008. Detection of

284

Anoplophora glabripennis (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) larvae in different host trees and tissues

285

by automated analyses of sound-impulse frequency and temporal patterns. J. Econ. Entomol.

286

101(3), 838–849.

287

Mazzoni V., Nieri R., Anfora G., Eeriksson A.E., Polajnar J., Virant-Doblert M., Lucchi

288

A., 2018. Vibrational signals as semiophysicals: a comparison between pheromonal and

11

289

vibrational mating disruption. Abstract Book Centro Congressi Riva del Garda, Trento, Italy

290

September 4–6. pp. 42.

291

Pires A. and Hoy R.R., 1992. Temperature coupling in cricket acoustic communication. J.

292

Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 171, 69–78. https://

293

doi.org/10.1007/BF00195962 Pörtner H.O, and Knust R., 2007. Climate change affects marine fishes through the

294 295

oxygen limitation of thermal tolerance. Science 315(5808), 95–97

296

Pureswaran D.S., Hofstetter R.W., Sullivan B.T., and Potter K.A., 2016. The role of

297

multimodal signals in species recognition between tree-killing bark beetles in a narrow sympatric

298

zone. Environ. Entomol. 45(3) 582–591. https://doi.org/10.1093/ee/nvw022

299

Ritchie, M. G., Saarikettu, M., Livingstone, S., & Hoikkala, A. 2001. Characterization of

300

female preference functions for Drosophila montana courtship song and a test of the temperature

301

coupling hypothesis. Evolution. 55, 721–727. Ryker L.C., and Rudinsky J.A., 1976. Sound production in Scolytidae: aggressive and

302 303

mating behavior of the mountain pine beetle. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 69, 677–680.

304

Sanborn A.F. 2006. Acoustic signals and temperature. In: Drosopoulos S, Claridge MF,

305

editors. Insect sounds and communication: physiology, behaviour, ecology and evolution. CRC

306

Taylor and Francis. Florida, pp. 111–125. Sanborn A.F., 2004. Thermoregulation and endothermy in the large western cicada

307 308

Tibicen cultriformis (Hemiptera: Cicadidae). J. Therm. Biol. 29(2), 97–101.

309

Sanborn A.F., Mater S., 2000. Thermoregulation and the effect of body temperature on

310

call temporal parameters in the cicada Diceroprocta olympusa (Homoptera: Cicadidae). Comp.

311

Biochem. Physiol., Part A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 125, 141–148.

312

Simmons L.W., 1995. Correlates of male quality in the field cricket, Gryllus campestris

313

L.: age, size, and symmetry determine pairing success in field populations. Behav. Ecol. 6, 376–

314

381. 12

315

Souroukis K., Cade W.H., and Rowell G., 1992. Factors that possibly influence variation

316

in the calling song of field crickets: Temperature, time and male size, age and wing morphology.

317

Can. J. Zool. 70, 950–955.

318 319 320 321 322 323

Stark V.N., 1952. Bark beetles / In: Stackelberg A.A. ed. Fauna of the USSR. Vol.XXXI. M.–L.: Izdatel'stvo Аkademii Nauk USSR, 462 pp. (In Russian) Sueur J., Sanborn A.F., 2003. Ambient temperature and sound power of cicada calling songs (Hemiptera: Cicadidae: Tibicina). Physiol. Entomol. 28, 340–343. von Helversen, D., & von Helversen, O. 1981. Korrespondenz zwischen Gesang und auslösendem Schema bei Feldheuschrecken. Nova Acta Leopold. 54, 449–462.

324

Walker T. 1975. Effects of temperature on rates in poikilotherm nervous systems:

325

Evidence from the calling songs of meadow katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae:Orchelimum)

326

and reanalysis of published data. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol.

327

101:57–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00660119

328

Weed A.S., Ayres V.P., Bentz B.J. 2015. Population Dynamics of Bark Beetles. Vega

329

F.E., Hofstetter R.W (Eds.), Bark beetle. Biology and ecology of native and invasive species.

330

Academic Press of Elsevier, London, pp. 157–176.

331

Yturralde K.M., and Hofstetter R.W., 2015. Characterization of stridulatory structures

332

and sounds of the larger Mexican pine beetle, Dendroctonus approximatus (Coleoptera:

333

Curculionidae: Scolytinae). Fla. Entomol. 98, 516–527

334

Zhantiev R.D., and Korsunovskaya O.S., 2014. On recognition of sound communication

335

signals in bush crickets (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Entomol. Rev. 94(6), 815–819.

336

https://doi.org/10.1134/S0013873814060025

337 338 339 340

13

341 342

Table 1. The results of the analysis of variability of the signal parameters in Polygraphus proximus males depending on temperature. Parameter

343 344

(N)

W

H-test

(r`-values)

(Z`-values)

Inter syllable interval

283

0.14

146.14*

Syllable duration

283

0.5

46.46*

Number of chirps/ syllable

283

0.31

14.3

Chirp rate

278

0.95

176.97*

Chirp duration

277

0.80

114.98*

Inter chirp interval

276

0.89

159.20*

Tooth-strikes/ chirp

274

0.09

12.8

Intertooth-strikes interval

276

0.85

122.96*

*p<0.001, without asterisk – insignificant. In the column H-test the Z-factor values (measure of statistical effect size) are given.

345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358

14

Figure legends:

359 360 361

Fig. 1. The experimental set inside thermostat for registration of Polygraphus proximus

362

signals during male-female interactions (A – condenser microphone in glass tube, B – the arena

363

with beetles inside marked by arrows).

364 365 366

Fig. 2. The graphical descriptions of temporal parameters of stridulatory signals in the four-eyed fir bark beetle measured in this study

367 368

Fig. 3. Temporal characteristics of simple chirps during male-female interactions under

369

tested temperature settings: A. – inter syllable interval; B – the number of chirps per syllable; C–

370

chirp rate; D – chirp duration; E – inter chirp interval; F – number of tooth-strikes; G –

371

intertooth-strikes interval; H – syllable duration.

372 373 374 375

15

376

16

377

378

379

Highlights 17

380 381 382



Stridulation is first way of communication between individuals of bark beetles occurring at the imago stage.

383 384



Temporal parameters of P. proximus acoustic signals are temperature-dependent.

385 386 387



Among the analyzed characteristics of P. proximus signals a high correlation were between temperature and the chirp rate

388 389 390



Presence of females in limited area not inside entrance hole provokes producing of a rivalry signaling by males.

391

18