Journal Pre-proof The Association of the Executive Functions with Overweight and Obesity Indicators in Children and Adolescents: A Literature Review Paula Mamrot, Tomasz Han´c
PII:
S0149-7634(18)30859-5
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.021
Reference:
NBR 3521
To appear in: Received Date:
6 November 2018
Revised Date:
25 August 2019
Accepted Date:
26 August 2019
Please cite this article as: Mamrot P, Han´c T, The Association of the Executive Functions with Overweight and Obesity Indicators in Children and Adolescents: A Literature Review, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.08.021
This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier.
The Association of the Executive Functions with Overweight and Obesity Indicators in Children and Adolescents: A Literature Review
Paula Mamrot 1, Tomasz Hanć 1
1
Department of Human Biological Development, Institute of Anthropology, Faculty of
ro of
Biology, Adam Mickiewicz University, ul. Uniwersytetu Poznańskiego 6, 61-614 Poznan, Poland
-p
Address for correspondence: Paula Mamrot, Department of Human Biological
Development, Adam Mickiewicz University, Uniwersytetu Poznanskiego 6, 61-614 Poznań,
lP
Highlights
re
Poland, tel: +48618295729, e-mail:
[email protected]
Systematic review supports significant association of the executive functions with
ur na
excess body mass in children and adolescents The strongest evidence supports the relationship between inhibitory control and higher BMI, being overweight or obese There is a need to systematize the way of defining and testing executive functions in
Jo
the context of studying their relationship with body weight
Abstract
The prevalence of obesity in children and adolescents has become an increasing health problem all over the world. Prior studies suggest there is a relationship between excess body mass in adults and executive functions (EF). The paper analyzes recent studies on the
association of obesity indicators and EF performance in children and adolescents. We analyzed four types of studies: comparison studies with obese and healthy children, crosssectional studies describing dependencies between EF and BMI, follow up studies applying EF as a predictor of overweight/obesity and studies describing the effect of weight reduction on improving EF. We interpreted the results based on the categorization of EF into three main processes: inhibitory control, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and higher-level EF such as reasoning, problem-solving and planning. The strongest evidence supports the relationship
ro of
between poor inhibitory control and higher BMI, overweight or obesity. However, the mechanism of the association is still unclear. A better understanding of the EF-obesity link
-p
may be relevant for the prevention of obesity or help in EF deficits improvement.
Introduction
re
Obesity is currently considered one of the most serious growing health problems
lP
around the world. According to World Health Organization (WHO) reports, in 2016 over 340 million children aged 5-19 years were of excess weight (WHO, 2018). In Europe, the rate of children aged 6-9 with overweight and obesity was 22.7% - 53.05% depending on the country
ur na
(Wijnhoven et al., 2014). Although statistical analysis suggests that in recent years the upward trend in obesity has slowed, the number of obese children is still alarming (Kelsey et al., 2014), especially since childhood obesity increases the likelihood of being obese in adulthood
Jo
(Luppino et al., 2010). Furthermore, being obese affects physical health (Sahoo et al., 2015), as well as causes psycho-social consequences during childhood (Sahoo et al., 2015; Luppino et al., 2010).
Obesity is a disease of multifactorial background, hence it is examined extensively in many aspects: biological (e.g.: genetics) (Locke et al., 2015; Frayling et al., 2007), socioeconomic (Petraviciene et al., 2018; Wagner et al., 2018; Gibbs et al., 2013) or
psychological (Miller et al., 2018; Stoeckel et al., 2017; Beck, 2016; Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2006). However, recently the attention of many researches has been focused on the relationship between obesity and executive functions. Executive functions (EF), also called executive control (Berthelsen et al., 2017), are neurocognitive mental processes involved in goal-directed behavior, planning and monitoring, such us: inhibitory control, shifting, delaying of gratification, selective attention, sustained attention, working memory (Berhelsen et al., 2017). According to neuroimaging studies, executive functions are processes occurring
ro of
at the prefrontal and parietal regions of the brain (Chung et al., 2014). Across the lifespan, EF develop intensively during childhood into adolescence and decrease during aging (Zelazo et al., 2004). The proper functioning of EF is relevant for physical health, as well as mental
-p
health, school achievements and job success (Diamond, 2013). It is thought that the following factors have an influence on EF development: socioeconomic status, preterm birth, physical
re
activity, sleep deprivation, stress, social network or genetic background (Zysset et al., 2018).
lP
Understanding the mechanisms of obesity is important for further prevention from early childhood. Therefore, it is important to synthesize the results of previous research on the relationship between EF and obesity, as it could be useful in early psychosocial intervention
ur na
and in the health education area. According to our knowledge, several systematic reviews of the EF-obesity relationship in adults (Rotge et al., 2017; Veronese et al., 2017; Prickett et al., 2015; Fitzpatrick et al., 2013) or across the lifespan (Yang et al., 2018; Gettens et al., 2017;
Jo
Smith et al., 2011) have been published, however, only a limited number of studies address this association exclusively in children and adolescents. The aim of this study was to assess the relation between EF and obesity in children and youths based on a systematic review of the relevant research. In contrast to previous reviews we paid attention to various obesity indicators, not only the most popular BMI, but also body fat, visceral fat and waist circumference. Furthermore, we applied a different division of executive functions than in the
previous works. Since various names of EF and a variety of diagnostic tools are in use the applied division was relevant for explicit interpretation of the research results. Methods A literature review was conducted in 2018 between the 5th and 15th January using Scopus, Medline and Google Scholar databases. The following key words were used in article titles, abstracts and key words’ search: /executive functions and obesity or overweight or body fatness and children or adolescents/. To be included in the analysis, the articles had to meet
ro of
the following criteria: (i) to investigate the relationship between overweight/ obesity indicators and EF in children and adolescents; (ii) to be a quantitative study, (iii) to be written in English. Studies comparing healthy children and children with a serious medical condition
-p
or mental illness and those focused on adults >18 years were excluded from the review.
The various nomenclature of individual EF is given in the literature (Chung et al.,
re
2014). In the works analyzed in this review the authors used different nomenclature for EF as
lP
well. Although this might indicate the complexity of the EF concept, it brings difficulties in the comparison and consistent interpretation of the results. Therefore, we propose an analysis based on the work of Adele Diamond (2013). To our knowledge, this is one of the most
ur na
comprehensive divisions of EF, based on a wide range literature. She distinguished three main cores of EF which are in the order: inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility, assigning the other executive functions to those three categories. Higher-level
Jo
executive functions such as reasoning, problem-solving and planning are built from these three main EF. Results
In all, 610 articles were found by a combination of the above mentioned keywords. Four hundred and sixty-eight studies were excluded after screening by title. The remaining 142 articles were reviewed by abstracts. After excluding duplicate articles and those which
did not fit the criteria (language, both EF and obesity indicators, participants’ age, article type), there remained 31 articles. Two of them were not possible to review due to lack of full access (Nelson et al., 2017; Bozkurt et al., 2017), and another two failed to meet the criteria as they made no reference to measurement of body mass or fatness but only unhealthy eating behavior (Tryon et al., 2013; Riggs et al., 2012). Finally, 27 papers from 25 studies met all the criteria and were reviewed by full-text. Both the articles of Wirt et al. (2015; 2014) come from the same study, as do the two articles of Kamijo et al. (2014; 2012). However, we
ro of
decided to include them in the review because of the authors’ use of different diagnostic tools and various kinds of analyses in each article, which led to new conclusions. The process of article selection is shown in Figure 1.
-p
In all of the studies the number of 13 030 participants aged 3 to 18 years old was examined. From the 27 papers only half include information about the detailed number of
re
excess weight participants, although in each study Body Mass Index (BMI) was used as an obesity indicator. In each study, 1 to 7 EF were examined. The most frequently studied
lP
function was inhibitory control – 85.18%. In above 85% of studies at least one of the EF was associated with excess body mass. Four types of research are distinguished in this review: (i)
ur na
papers describing differences in EF between children with healthy weight and excess weight (40.74%), (ii) cross-sectional studies describing the association of the EF with BMI (37.04%), (iii) follow up studies describing EF as a predictor of excessive weight gain (18.52%) and (iv)
Jo
a study describing the effects of weight reduction on improving EF (3.70%). Detailed results are presented in Table 1. Table 2 presents the EF examined by the researches and applied methods in detail. The
EF strictly defined as inhibitory control was the most frequently tested one (44.04%). Inhibitory control involves the control of impulses, behavior, thoughts and emotions (Diamond, 2013). Based on the division of A. Diamond (2013), other EF that could be
managed with inhibition such as response inhibition, resistance to interference and selfregulation, were examined in the next 11 publications. This means that almost each study (85.18%) was focused on inhibitory control as an important EF in the context of excess weight. To examine inhibitory control the researchers applied the following methods: The Go/NoGo Task (21.74% of inhibitory control studies), Flanker Task (17.39%), The Stroop Color Word Interference Task (13.04%), The Five Digit Test (8.70%) and tasks based on single reaction time – SRT (8.70%). The following methods were applied once: The Conners’
ro of
Continuous Performance Test – II, The Trial Making Test A and B, The Sky Search, The Fruit Stop Task, The Two-Choice Reaction Task, The Tapping Task, The Task Performance, The Ruff2 and 7 Selective Attention Test, The Symbol Search from WISC-III, The Revised-
Task and The Cognitive Assessment System.
-p
Strategy Application Test, The Sustained Attention Task from KiTAP, The Opposite Words
re
Working memory, the next core of EF, refers to holding and working with information
lP
in mind that is no longer perceptually persistent (Diamond, 2013). For the examination of working memory, the following methods were mostly applied: tests based on counting or remembering backwards (The Digit-span subtest of WISC-III, The Counting Span Task, The
ur na
Digit Span Backwards, the Symbol Digit Modalities Test – 44.44%). Other methods applied in individual studies were The Corsi-Block Tapping Task, The Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory, The McCarhy Scales of Children’s Abilities, The Letter-number
Jo
sequencing and working memory functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task. The third core EF, cognitive flexibility, associated with being mentally flexible in
response to changing stimuli (Diamond, 2013), was examined using The Five Digit Test (28.57%) and the Cognitive Flexibility Task (28.57%). Furthermore, some methods applied only once were as follows: The Flexible Item Selection Task, The Verbal Fluency, The Trial Making Test part B.
Last but not least, the following methods were applied to measure higher-level EF: The Delay of Gratification Task (41.66%), gambling tasks (The Children’s Gambling Task, The Iowa Gambling Task, the Hungry Donkey Task – 25%), The Tower of London Test (16.66%), and methods applied only once - The Choice Delay Task, The Question Based Delay Discounting Test, The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities, The Andre Rey Test, The Cognitive Assessment System, The Zoo map and Similarities. Overweight and obesity measure methods
ro of
In each study BMI was the main indicator of overweight and obesity. The overweight cut off point was set on the 85th percentile. The obesity cut off point was set on the 95th or 97th percentile. Indicators of obesity other than BMI were used in only 7 studies (25.93%). These
-p
were as follows: waist circumference (1 study), visceral adipose tissue (3 studies), body fat (5 studies).
re
Differences in EF between excess body weight and healthy weight children
lP
The most (40.74%) of the articles focused on a comparison of executive function performance between the group of non-overweight children and children with overweight/obesity. From these, 11 out of 27 publications, 37.03% tested the performance of
ur na
inhibitory control (inhibitory control, response inhibition, impulsive disinhibition, selfregulation, sustained attention and visuospatial attention) and significant differences were found in 70.37% of them, in favor of the group with a healthy body mass (Tsai et al., 2016;
Jo
Wirt et al., 2014; Kamijo et al., 2014; Gentier et al., 2013; Fields et al., 2013; Maayan et al., 2011; Pauli-Pott et al., 2010). Cognitive flexibility (switching, attention shifting, shifting) were examined in 3 studies. In all of them overweight or obese participant response was worse than healthy weight participants (Blanco-Gomez et al., 2015; Maayan et al., 2011; Vardejo-Garcia et al., 2010). The different aspects of working memory, tested by 4 researchers, were found to be related with body weight in one of these studies (Maayan et al.,
2011). Finally, the higher-level executive functions such as the ability to delay gratification, decision making, problem solving, planning and reasoning were tested in 4 studies, of which 75% found significant differences between the groups (Qavam et al., 2015; Fields et al., 2013; Vardejo-Garcia et al., 2010). Based on these results it could be concluded that obese children performed tests more slowly than their healthy-weight peers and made more mistakes. Cross-sectional studies describing the association of the EF with body mass Cross-sectional studies which describe the relationship between EF and body mass
ro of
were the second of the most numerous articles of this review (37.04%). Out of these 10 crosssectional studies, 70% found a significant association of body mass index with at least one
EF: inhibitory control (inhibitory control, Huang et al., 2015; Wirt et al., 2015; Kamijo et al.,
-p
2012; processing speed, Schwartz et al., 2013), cognitive flexibility (cognitive flexibility, Schwartz et al., 2013; attention shifting, Groppe et al., 2014), working memory (working
re
memory, Schwartz et al., 2013; verbal working memory and spatial working memory,
lP
Alarcón et al., 2015, updating, Groppe et al., 2014) and higher-level executive function (ability to delay gratification, Bruce et al., 2012). What is more, 20% of the studies found an association of visceral fat with executive functions such as working memory, cognitive
ur na
flexibility, inhibitory control (processing speed, Schwartz et al., 2013) and planning (Davis et al., 2011), even though BMI was not associated with EF test results. In the study of Huang (2015) an additional association of greater waist circumference with worse inhibitory control
Jo
was recorded. In conclusion, the better the cognitive performance, the lower BMI or lower visceral fat.
EF as a predictor of excessive weight gain in follow up studies The studies presented in the two above mentioned categories are of an associative nature and do not give any insight as regards an EF-obesity link. Follow up studies are noteworthy due to their ability to assess the long-term relationship between executive
functions and overweight/obesity in children and adolescents. There were only 18.52% longitudinal studies from the total of articles reviewed which tested the values of EF as predictors of excessive weight gain. In all of these studies significant relationships between greater EF performance and a better chance of being a healthy weight at further time point were found. However, the results of a study by Guxens (2009) showed these relationships to be age dependent. At the age of 4 executive functions were not significantly related to BMI, while 2 years later children with worse results at 4y. were more often characterized by
ro of
overweight. These results suggest the influence of EF on excessive body weight gain. In turn, other researchers’ (Stautz et al., 2016) results showed that the working memory but not
inhibitory control at the age 8 and 10 were associated with being overweight at the age of 13.
-p
The researchers concluded that the better the working memory results, the greater the
likelihood of being a healthy weight child. Likewise, other results of longitudinal research
re
concerned the ability to delay gratification (Bruce et al., 2012; Seeyave et al., 2009). Better
lP
results in the gratification task were important predictors for appropriate body weight several years later. EF training and its impact on weight reduction was examined in one study (Verbeken et al., 2013). The authors tested how an 8-month period of EF training (working
ur na
memory and inhibitory control) could affect weight reduction progress in children and adolescents aged 9-14. For two months after completion of the training the children were more able to lose weight. Overall, the results suggest that low executive functioning in
Jo
childhood is a risk factor for excessive weight gain in the following years of the progressive phase of physical development. It seems, therefore highly likely that interventions focused on EF improvement have the potential for the prevention of obesity and being an effective method of treatment. Effects of weight reduction on improving EF
In one of the studies changes in children’s cognitive functioning were assessed after the reduction of the amount of visceral adipose tissue (Raine et al., 2017). It is worth noticing that, regardless of the amount of total body fat, the reduction of visceral fat tissue was significant predictor of better results in neurocognitive tests, particularly among obese participants. This may suggest the influence of visceral fat on executive functioning. In fact, in the studies of Veit et al. (2014), increasing visceral fat was associated with cortical thinning in adults. According to Isaac et al. (2011), higher visceral adipose tissue in healthy elderly
ro of
people was associated with a lower hippocampus volume and worse cognitive functioning. However, in the scope of this review, the study of Raine et al. (2017) was the only one to test such an intervention. Thus, the conclusions are limited.
-p
Discussion
The aim of this review was to present analyses of recent studies on the relationship
re
between EF and obesity indicators in children and adolescents. Although there are systematic
lP
reviews of the EF-obesity relationship in adults (Rotge et al., 2017; Veronese et al., 2017; Prickett et al., 2015; Fitzpatrick et al., 2013) or across the lifespan (Yang et al., 2018; Gettens et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2011), fewer address this association only in children and
ur na
adolescents. Yang and co-workers (2018) made a broad meta-analysis concerning the EFobesity link at various stages of ontogenesis, however, they exclude the articles in which children were younger than 6 years and they focused in their analysis on BMI as the only
Jo
obesity indicator. A remarkable review about children and adolescents was made by Martin and co-workers (2017), however, the review concerns the relationship between obesity and school achievements, which are influenced by more factors than EF. In turn, Miller (2016) in her review presented a valuable analysis of conceptual EF-obesity models and suggested pediatric strategies for the prevention of obesity in children, although she focused mainly on a one-way hypothesis direction that EF deficits affect body mass. Thus, we have enriched the
review with additional indicators of obesity, such as body fat, visceral fat and waist circumference. What is more, in order to facilitate the interpretation of research, using different EF nomenclature and different diagnostic tools, we have applied the EF classification to the groups proposed by the work of Diamond (2013). After analysis of the 27 papers it can be concluded that cognitive functioning is significantly related to body weight in children and adolescents. This relationship concerns both children and adolescents. According to main cores of EF, inhibitory control was
ro of
associated with obesity indicators in 70.37% comparisons of healthy and excess weight participant studies and in all of the cross-sectional studies. However, as a weight status predictor, inhibitory control was significant in only one study involving EF training
-p
(Verbeken et al, 2013). In turn, the working memory component of EF was significantly
worse in obese participants in only 25% studies, however, in cross-sectional studies working
re
memory was associated with body mass in each one, as well as in the studies using working
lP
memory as a predictor of later weight gain. Finally, the third component of EF, cognitive flexibility, was at a higher level in healthy participants in each study and was associated with body mass or weight status in cross-sectional studies, although the value of cognitive
ur na
flexibility as a predictor of future weight gain is unknown. In general, body mass or body status was associated with at least one EF in 81.82% of 11 comparison studies, 70% of 10 cross-sectional studies, while 100% of 5 longitudinal studies tested EF as a weight status
Jo
predictor, and finally, in one study which described the effect of visceral fat reduction on EF. The mechanism that may underlie the relationship between executive functions and
obesity is still not fully explained. It is not clear whether executive functions affect body mass, or body weight affects executive functions. Cognitive deficits lead to obesity
The authors supporting the hypothesis about the influence of cognitive deficits on weight gain suggest mainly a mechanism based on inhibitory control impairment. Children with poorer self-control may be characterized by obesity-related behaviors such us eating greater amounts of food, eating high fat or high sugar products but lower quantities of vegetables (Pieper and Laugero, 2013), and a sedentary life-style (Wirt et al., 2015). Difficulties with self-control also affect the ability to delay gratification and impact eating more unhealthy food (Bruce et al., 2009). As a consequence, such children are more
ro of
vulnerable to putting on weight. An interesting hypothesis on stress as an intermediate variable between EF and obesity was tested by Tryon and co-workers (2013). The researchers conducted a study on chronic stress exposure and its effect on brain activity and obesogenic
-p
eating habits. According to the authors, chronic stress is related to unhealthy eating habits that may be associated with different brain activity patterns in regions responsible for mediating
re
motivation and decision-making. Thus, chronic stress may alter the brain activity in the
lP
anterior prefrontal cortex (PFC) and dorsolateral PFC, regions associated with executive control and this may lead to obesogenic eating and weight gain (Tryon et al., 2013). Obesity and fatness lead to executive dysregulation
ur na
On the other hand, a large amount of visceral adipose tissue, as the result of a sedentary lifestyle and inappropriate diet, may have an impact on cognitive function through metabolic dysregulation (Raine et al., 2017), changes in brain structure (de Groot et al., 2017)
Jo
such as degeneration of the frontal cortex, associated with EF (Qavam, 2015). According to O’Brien et al. (2017), obesity and a high fat diet have a high impact on the central nervous system (CNS) through increased circulating fat free acids, increased triglyceride level and inflammatory adipokines. It has been demonstrated that a high level of triglycerides and fat free acids lead to increased neurodegeneration (O’Brien et al., 2017). According to the authors, as a consequence, obesity can lead to executive dysregulation and be one of the risk
factors of Alzheimer’s disease or mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. The other possible explanation of an obesity-EF link can be found in the review of Agusti and co-workers (2018). The researchers analyzed the studies on the mutual activity of the gut-brain axis, cognitive function and obesity. They concluded that along with an unhealthy diet and an excessive energy supply that leads to obesity, there are adverse changes in the intestinal microbiota. This, in turn, may affect the neuroendocrine system and contribute to its dysregulation, affecting the executive functions.
ro of
The common background of EF and obesity The third hypothesis regarding the association of EF with obesity is the assumption of common association, but no causation. Of particular interest is an article by Susan Carnell et
-p
al. (2012). In their comprehensive biobehavioral risk model of child and adult obesity the authors noticed a research gap in the area of common genetic variant of obesity and brain
re
response to food, including cognitive traits as inhibitory control. The authors conclude that
lP
structural and functional characteristics of the brain and obesity may share a common genetic background, although these are hypotheses that require further research. Nevertheless, it seems to be supported by the results of the work of Marioni et al. (2016) who found, using the
ur na
method of genome wide association studies, seven common genetic variants from four genes (AKAP6, TOMM40, THEM161B, TNRC6B) associated with both cognitive function and BMI. Some of the researchers indicate specific genes that may be related to both obesity and
Jo
cognitive dysregulation. For instance, according to Bressler et al. (2013), the FTO gene, associated with adiposity and obesity-related behaviors, might also be related to diminished cognitive functions. Another example is the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4). It is thought that SCL6A4 has a significant role in executive functions (Zhao et al., 2013). However, the studies of Zhao et al. (2013b) suggest that promoter methylation of SLC6A4 is also associated with regulating food intake and body mass.
Implications There may be relevant implications in the findings of the reviewed studies. If a low level of executive functioning is associated with a greater risk of excess body mass, some supportive intervention could be implemented for better overweight/obesity prevention and treatment. Better executive functioning might help optimize treatment results through enhancing the maintenance of a healthy and well-balanced diet. Thus, different cognitive training could be used to improve EF skills in children and adolescents. Some of the possible
ro of
interventions previously tested on adults, but also targeted for children, are reviewed in an article of Hayes et al. (2018). She explored different kinds of EF improvement strategies and noticed the need for further studies on their efficiency. These strategies include, for instance,
-p
computer training programs for enhancing inhibitory control and working memory, or
episodic future thinking (EFT). According to the results of our review the most promising EF
re
to implement seems to be inhibitory control, as it is the most widely studied function with the
lP
highest percentage of significance in the context of obesity indicators. However, our results indicate an insufficient amount of research with other EF. In addition, none of the EF works separately – they are closely related to each other (Diamond, 2013). Thus, comprehensive EF
ur na
training should be considered.
Since some findings on visceral fat and body fat suggest that these may be important parameters in the analysis of the EF-obesity link, it is worth expanding the diagnosis of
Jo
obesity in future research with the parameters of body composition. There may be a relationship between adipose tissue and EF, even if it is not related to BMI.
Conclusions There is a significant association between executive dysfunction and excess body mass in children and adolescents. The strongest evidence supports the relationship between poor
inhibitory control and higher BMI, being overweight or obese. However, more longitudinal studies including EF and body mass assessment at different time points are needed to understand the mechanisms of the link and its direction. A better understanding of the EFobesity link may be relevant for the prevention of obesity through EF enhancement or help in EF deficit improvement through body fat reduction programs. Thus, further studies on the association of executive functions with overweight/obesity in children and adolescents are
ro of
warranted.
Disclosures
This work was supported by the National Science Centre (NCN) in Poland [grant
Jo
ur na
lP
re
-p
number OPUS 2016/21/B/NZ5/00492].
References Alarcón, G., Ray, S., Nagel, B. J. (2016). Lower Working Memory Performance in Overweight and Obese Adolescents Is Mediated by White Matter Microstructure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(3), 281–292. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715001265 Agustí, A., García-Pardo, M. P., López-Almela, I., Campillo, I., Maes, M., Romaní-Pérez, M., Sanz, Y. (2018). Interplay between the gut-brain axis, obesity and cognitive function. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 155. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00155 Beck, A. R. (2016). Psychosocial Aspects of Obesity. NASN School Nurse, 31(1), 23–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1942602X15619756 Berthelsen, D., Hayes, N., White, S. L. J., Williams, K. E. (2017) Executive function in adolescence: Associations with child and family risk factors and self-regulation in early childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 903. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00903
ro of
Blanco-Gómez, A., Ferré, N., Luque, V., Cardona, M., Gispert-Llauradó, M., Escribano, J., ClosaMonasterolo, R., Canals-Sans, J. (2015). Being overweight or obese is associated with inhibition control in children from six to ten years of age. Acta Paediatrica, 104(6), 619– 625. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12976 Bozkurt, H., Özer, S., Yılmaz, R., Sönmezgöz, E., Kazancı, Ö., Erbaş, O., Demir, O. (2017). Assessment of Neurocognitive Functions in Children and Adolescents with Obesity. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 6(4), 262–268. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2016.1150184
-p
Bressler, J., Formage, M., Demerath, E. W., Knopman, D. S., Monda, K. L., North, K. E., Penman, A., Mosley, T.H., Boerwinkle, E. (2013). Fat mass and obesity gene and cognitive decline: the Antherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Neurology, 80(1), 92-99. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182768910
lP
re
Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24(3), 337–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ECRESQ.2009.06.001 Bruce, A. S., Black, W. R., Bruce, J. M., Daldalian, M., Martin, L. E., Davis, A. M. (2012). Ability to Delay Gratification and BMI in Preadolescence. Obesity, 19(5), 1101– 1102. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2010.297
ur na
Carnell, S., Kim, Y., Pryor, K. (2012). Fat brains, greedy genes, and parent power: A biobehavioural risk model of child and adult obesity. International Review of Psychiatry, 24(3), 189–199. https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2012.676988 Chung, H.J., Weyandt, L.L., Swentosky, A. (2014). The Physiology of Executive Functioning. In: Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J. (eds) Handbook of Executive Functioning. Springer, New York, NY
Jo
Davis C. L., Cooper, S. (2011). Fitness, fatness, cognition, behavior, and academic achievement among overweight children: Do cross-sectional associations correspond to exercise trial outcomes? Preventive Medicine, 52, 65-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.020 de Groot, C. J., van den Akker, E. L. T., Rings, E. H. H. M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., van der Grond, J. (2017). Brain structure, executive function and appetitive traits in adolescent obesity. Pediatric Obesity, 12(4), 33–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12149 Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-14375 Fields, S. A., Sabet, M., Reynolds, B. (2013). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in obese, overweight, and healthy-weight adolescents. Appetite, 70, 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.089 Fitzpatrick, S., Gilbert, S., Serpell, L. (2013). Systematic Review: Are Overweight and Obese Individuals Impaired on Behavioural Tasks of Executive Functioning? Neuropsychology Review, 23(2), 138–156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9224-7
Frayling, T. M. et al. (2007). A common variant in the FTO gene is associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. Science, 316, 889-894, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1141634. Gentier, I., Augustijn, M., Deforche, B., Tanghe, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Lenoir, M., D’Hondt, E. (2013). A comparative study of performance in simple and choice reaction time tasks between obese and healthy-weight children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(9), 2635– 2641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.016 Gettens, K. M., Gorin, A. A. (2017) Executive function in weight loss and weight loss maintenance: a conceptual review and novel neuropsychological model of weight control. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 40(5), 687-701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-017-9831-5 Gibbs, B.G., Forste, R. (2013). Socioeconomic status, infant feeding practices and early childhood obesity. Pediatric Obesity, 9(2), 135–146. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2047-6310.2013.00155.x Groppe, K., Elsner, B. (2014). Executive function and food approach behavior in middle childhood. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00447
ro of
Guxens, M., Mendez, M. A., Julvez, J., Plana, E., Forns, J., Basagaña, X., Torrent, M., Sunyer, J. (2009). Cognitive function and overweight in preschool children. American Journal of Epidemiology, 170(4), 438–446. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwp140
Hayes, J. F., Eichen D.M., Barch D. M., Wilfley D. E. (2018). Executive function in childhood obesity: Promising intervention strategies to optimize treatment outcomes. Appetite, 124, 10–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.040
re
-p
Huang, T., Tarp, J., Domazet, S. L., Thorsen, A. K., Froberg, K., Andersen, L. B., Bugge, A. (2015). Associations of Adiposity and Aerobic Fitness with Executive Function and Math Performance in Danish Adolescents. Journal of Pediatrics, 167(4), 810815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.07.009
lP
Hughes, S. O., Power, T. G., O’Connor, T. M., Fisher, J. O. (2015). Executive functioning, emotion regulation, eating self-regulation, and weight status in low-income preschool children: How do they relate? Appetite, 89, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.009 Isaac, V., Sim, S., Zheng, H., Zagorodnov, V., Tai, E. S., Chee, M. (2011). Adverse associations between visceral adiposity, brain structure and cognitive performance in healthy elderly. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 3, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2011.00012
ur na
Kamijo, K., Khan, N. A., Pontifex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., Raine, L. B., Evans E. M., Castelli D. M., Hillman, C. H. (2012). The Relation of Adiposity to Cognitive Control and Scholastic Achievement in Preadolescent Children. Pediatric Obesity, 20(12). https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2012.112 Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., Evans E. M., Castelli D. M., Hillman, C. H. (2014). The negative association of childhood obesity to cognitive control of action monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 24(3), 654– 662. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs349
Jo
Kelsey, M. M., Zaepfel, A., Bjornstad, P., Nadeau, K. J. (2014). Age-Related Consequences of Childhood Obesity. Gerontology, 60, 222-228 Locke, A. E., Kahali, B., Berndt, S. I., Justice, A. E., Pers, T. H., Day, F. R., … Speliotes, E. K. (2015). Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature, 518, 197206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature14177 Luppino, F.S., de Wit, L.M., Bouvy, P.F. (2010). Overweight, Obesity, and Depression. A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 67(3), 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.2 Maayan, L., Hoogendoorn, C., Sweat, V., Convit, A. (2011). Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity, 19(7), 1382–1387. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2011.15
Martin, A., Booth, J. N., Laird, Y., Sproule, J., Reilly, J. J., Saunders, D. H. (2018). Physical activity, diet and other behavioural interventions for improving cognition and school achievement in children and adolescents with obesity or overweight. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 1. http://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD009728.pub4 Martin, A., Booth, J. N., McGeown, S., Niven, A., Sproule, J., Saunders, D. H., Reilly, J. J. (2017). Longitudinal Associations Between Childhood Obesity and Academic Achievement: Systematic Review with Focus Group Data. Current Obesity Reports, 6(3), 297–313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13679-017-0272-9 Marioni, R. E., Yang, J., Dykiert, D., Mottus, R., Campbell, A., Davies, G., Hayward, C., Porteous D. J., Visscher, P. M., Deary, I. J. (2016). Assesing the genetic overlap between BMI and cognitive function. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(10), 1477-1482. https://doi.org/10.3389.fnins.2018.00155 Miller, A. L. (2016). Neurocognitive Processes and Pediatric Obesity Interventions: Review of Current Literature and Suggested Future Directions. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 63(3), 447–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PCL.2016.02.006
ro of
Miller, A. L., Gearhardt, A. N., Retzloff, L., Sturza, J., Kaciroti, N., Lumeng, J. C. (2018). Early Childhood Stress and Child Age Predict Longitudinal Increases in Obesogenic Eating Among Low-Income Children. Academic Pediatrics, 18(6), 685-691 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2018.01.007 Nelson, T. D., James, T. D., Hankey, M., Nelson, J. M., Lundahl, A., Espy, K. A. (2017). Early executive control and risk for overweight and obesity in elementary school. Child Neuropsychology, 23(8), 994–1002. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2016.1183606
-p
O’Brien, P.D., Hinder, L. M., Callaghan, B.C., Feldman, E.L. (2017). Neurological Consequences of Obesity. Lancet Neurol. 16(6), pp. 465–477. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30084-4
re
Qavam, S. E., Anisan, A., Fathi, M., Pourabbasi, A. (2015). Study of relationship between obesity and executive functions among high school students in Bushehr, Iran. Journal of Diabetes & Metabolic Disorders, 14, 79. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40200-015-0211-9
lP
Pauli-Pott, U., Albayrak Ö., Hebebrand J. (2010). Association between inhibitory control, capacity and body weight in overweight and obese children and adolescents: dependence on age and inhibitory control component. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 592-603, https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.485980
ur na
Petraviciene, I., Grazuleviciene, R., Andrusaityte, S., Dedele, A., Nieuwenhuijsen, M. J. (2018). Impact of the social and natural environment on preschool-age children weight. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(3). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15030449 Pieper, J. R., Laugero, K. D. (2013). Preschool children with lower executive function may be more vulnerable to emotional-based eating in the absence of hunger. Appetite, 62, 103– 109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.020
Jo
Poon, K. (2017). Hot and Cool Executive Functions in Adolescence: Development and Contributions to Important Developmental Outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2311. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02311 Prickett, C., Brennan, L., Stolwyk, R. (2015). Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: A systematic literature review. Obesity Research & Clinical Practice, 9(2), 93–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.orcp.2014.05.001 Raine, L. B., Khan, N. A., Drollette, E. S., Pontifex, M. B., Kramer, A. F., Hillman, C. H. (2017). Obesity, Visceral Adipose Tissue, and Cognitive Function in Childhood. The Journal of Pediatrics, 187, 134–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.05.023 Rotge, J.‐ Y., Poitou, C., Fossati, P., Aron‐ Wisnewsky, J., and Oppert, J.‐ M. (2017) Decision‐ making in obesity without eating disorders: a systematic review and meta‐ analysis of Iowa gambling task performances. Obesity Reviews, 18: 936–942. doi: 10.1111/obr.12549.
Sahoo, K., Sahoo, B., Choudhury, A. K., Sofi, N. Y., Kumar, R., Bhadoria, A. S. (2015). Childhood obesity: causes and consequences. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care, 4(2), 187–192. http://doi.org/10.4103/2249-4863.154628 Schlam, T. R., Wilson, N. L., Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., Ayduk, O. (2013). Preschoolers’ Delay of Gratification Predicts their Body Mass 30 Years Later. The Journal of Pediatrics, 162(1), 90– 93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.06.049 Schwartz, D. H., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Richer, L., Syme, C., Veillette, S., Pausowa, Z., Paus, T. (2013). Visceral fat is associated with lower executive functioning in adolescents. International Journal of Obesity, 37(10), 1336–1343. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2013.104 Seeyave, D. M., Coleman, S., Appugliese, D. (2009). Ability to Delay Gratification at Age 4 Years and Risk of Overweight at Age 11 Years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, 51, 290299. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.12
ro of
Smith, E., Hay, P., Campbell, L., Trollor, J. N. (2011). A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: Implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obesity Reviews, 12(9), 740–755. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00920.x Stautz, K., Pechey, R., Couturier, D., Deary, I. J. (2016). Do Executive Function and Impulsivity Predict Adolescent Health Behaviour after Accounting for Intelligence? Findings from the ALSPAC Cohort, PLoS ONE, 11(8). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160512
-p
Stoeckel, L. E., Birch, L. L., Heatherton, T., Mann, T., Hunter, C., Czajkowski, S., Onken, L., Berger, P. K. and Savage, C. R. (2017), Psychological and neural contributions to appetite self‐ regulation. Obesity, 25(1), 17-25. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.21789
re
Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Cohen, M. L., Yanovski, S. Z., Cox, C., Theim, K. R., Keil, M., Reynolds J. C., Yanovski, J. A. (2006). A Prospective Study of Psychological Predictors of Body Fat Gain Among Children at High Risk for Adult Obesity. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1203-1209. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-1329
lP
Tsai, C-L., Chen, F-C., Pan, C-Y., Tseng, Y-T. (2016). The Neurocognitive Performance of Visuospatial Attention in Children with Obesity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1033. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01033 Tryon, M. S., Carter, C. S., DeCant, R., Laugero, K. D. (2013). Chronic stress exposure may affect the brain’s response to high calorie food cues and predispose to obesogenic eating habits. Physiology and Behavior, 120, 233–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.010
ur na
Veit, R., Kullmann, S., Heni, M., Machann, J., Häring, H., Fritsche, A., Preissl, H. (2014). NeuroImage: Clinical Reduced cortical thickness associated with visceral fat and BMI. NeuroImage: Clinical, 6, 307–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.013 Verbeken, S., Braet, C., Goossens, L., van der Oord, S. (2013). Executive function training with game elements for obese children: A novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory abilities for weightcontrol. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(6), 290– 299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.02.006
Jo
Verdejo-García, A., Pérez-Expósito, M., Schmidt-Río-Valle, J., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Cruz, F., Pérez-García, M., Lopez-Belmonte G., Martin-Matillas M., Martin-Lagos J. A., Marcos A., Campoy, C. (2010). Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity, 18(8), 1572–1578. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.475 Veronese, N., Facchini, S., Stubbs, B., Luchini, C., Solmi, M., Manzato, E., Sergi G., Maggi S., Cosco T., Fontana, L. (2017). Weight loss is associated with improvements in cognitive function among overweight and obese people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 72, 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.017 Wagner, K. J. P., Bastos, J. L. D., Navarro, A., Gonzalez-Chica, D. A., Boing, A. F. (2018). Socioeconomic status in childhood and obesity in adults: a population-based study. Revista de Saúde Pública, 52, 15. http://doi.org/10.11606/S1518-8787.2018052000123
Wijnhoven, T. M. A., van Raaij, J. M. A., Spinelli, A., Starc, G., Hassapidou, M., Spiroski, I., Breda, J. (2014). WHO European Childhood Obesity Surveillance Initiative: body mass index and level of overweight among 6–9-year-old children from school year 2007/2008 to school year 2009/2010. BMC Public Health, 14(1), 806. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-806 Wirt, T., Hundsdörfer, V., Schreiber, A., Kesztyüs, D., Steinacker, J. M., Komm, T., Grundschule, B. (2014). Eating Behaviors Associations between inhibitory control and body weight in German primary school children. Eating Behaviors, 15(1), 9– 12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.015 Wirt, T., Schreiber, A., Kesztyüs, D., Steinacker, J. M. (2015). Early Life cognitive abilities and body weight: Cross-sectional study of the association of inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and sustained attention with BMI percentiles in primary school children. Journal of Obesity, 2015. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/534651 World Health Organization (2018). WHO Obesity and Overweight, http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/
ro of
Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Guo, C., Liu, Y. (2018). Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 225–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.020 Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychologica, 115, 167-183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005
Jo
ur na
lP
re
-p
Zysset, A. E., Kakebeeke, T. H., Messerli-bürgy, N., Meyer, A. H., Stülb, K., Leeger-Aschmann, C. S., Schmutz, E. A., Arhab, A., Puder, J. J., Kriemler, S., Munsch, S., Jenni, O. G. (2018). Predictors of Executive Functions in Preschoolers: Findings From the SPLASHY Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2060. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02060
ro of
-p
re
lP
ur na
Jo
Table 1. Research testing the relationship between EF and obesity indicators Type First of author analys is
Dat Stud N e y type
Populatio Age n (year s)
Obesity/overwei Executive ght measure function
Results
Obese and healthy weight individuals comparison de Groot et al.
201 C 7
42
Obese (n=23) and matched healthy weight (n=19)
12-16 BMI
Tsai et al.
201 C 6
52 (♀16)
Obese (n=26) and controls (n=26)
9-10
Blanco- 201 C Gomez 5 et al.
515 (♀265)
Obese + 6-10 overweigh t (n=221) and healthy controls
Qavam et al.
♂120
Jo Wirt et al.
201 C 4
(1) Visuospati Obese al children did attention poorer than healthyweight controls in EF tasks (slower reaction time)
-p
BMI; >97th obesity, >95th and <97th overweight; >16th and <95th normal weight
Poorer performanc e of overweight and obese children in switching
Obese 15-18 BMI; >95th (32,5%), obesity; >85th overweigh and < 95th t (30%), overweight normal weight (30%)
(1) Planning and organizing (2) Problem solving
Significant difference between the executive functions in excess and healthy weight students. Obese students have poorer EF than healthy weight peers
Obese 7±0,6 BMI; >97th (n=17), obesity; overweigh >90th and <97th t (n=25), overweight nonoverweigh t (n=454)
(1) Inhibitory control
Obese participants achieved lower level of inhibitory control than non-obese,
re
(1) Inhibitory control (2) Switching (3) Visuomot or ability
lP
498 (♀50,2 %)
No differences between obese and healthy weight adolescents
ro of
BMI; >95th obesity; >5th and <85th healthy weight
ur na
201 C 5
(1) Inhibitory control (2) The ability to delay gratificati on
nonoverweight and overweight children 201 C 4
74
Obese (n=37) and healthy weight (n=37)
7-9
BMI; >87th obesity, BF
(1) Inhibitory control (2) Response accuracy
Obese children performed longer reaction time than healthy weight
Gentier et al.
201 C 3
38 (♀20)
Obese (n=19), healthy weight (n=19)
6-12
BMI BF
(1) Response time
Obese children were slower than healthy weight peers only at Simple Reaction Time Task (EF – response time), not at Choice Reaction Time
Fields et al.
201 C 3
61
Obese 14-16 BMI; >95th (n=21), obesity; >85th overweigh and <95th t (n=20), overweight healthy weight (n=20)
re
-p
ro of
Kamijo et al.
Obese adolescents performed worse at sustained attention task – more omission errors than healthyweight and overweight controls
14-21 BMI; >95th (1) Response obesity or BMI > inhibition 30kg/m2 (2) Attention (3) Cognitive flexibility (4) Verbal fluency (5) Working memory
Obese participants performed worse in all EF measures
ur na
lP
(1) Sustained attention (2) Inhibition control (3) Decision making
91
Obese (n=54) and healthy weight (n=37)
177
Obese 8-15 overweigh t,
Jo
Maayan 201 C et al. 1
PauliPott et al.
201 C 0
BMI; >90th and <95th overweight; >95th obesity;
(1) Inhibitory control
More inattention in obese children and
adolescents , no difference between groups in impulsivity scores 61 (♀24)
Obese, 13-16 BMI; 17-24 overweigh kg/m2 normalt, healthy weight 24-28 weight kg/m2 overweight; 2951 kg/m2 obesity
(1) Response inhibition (2) Shifting (3) Planning (4) Decisionmaking (5) Reasoning (6) Selfregulation (7) Working memory
No difference between excess weight group and healthy weight group at: response inhibition, planning, reasoning and selfregulation. Poorer performanc e of excess weight group at shifting and decisionmaking functions.
152 (♀67)
Obese 12-17 BMI; >95th (n=18), obesity; overweigh >5th and < 85th t (n=46) healthy weight and healthy weight
ur na
Alarcón 201 C et al. 5
lP
EF level with BMI association
re
-p
ro of
Verdejo 201 C -Garcia 0 et al.
201 C 5
Jo
Huang et al.
525 Obese, 12-13 BMI, WC (♀272)s overweigh t and healthy weight
(1) Verbal working (2) memory (3) Spatial working memory
BMI percentile was negatively correlated with verbal and spatial working memory performanc e
(1) Inhibitory control
BMI and WC were negatively associated with inhibitory control, independen tly of aerobic fitness. Positive association
between aerobic fitness and executive function in all children 201 C 5
187 (♀47,6 %)
Obese 15-18 BMI; >95th (26,2%), obesity; >85th overweigh and <95th t (20,9%), overweight normal weight
(1) Delay of gratificati on (2) Response inhibition (3) Cognitive flexibility
No association between EF and excess weight
Wirt et al.
201 C 5
297 (♀52,5 %)
Obese 7±0,6 BMI; >97th (n=4), obesity; >90 and overweigh < 97th t (n=22), overweight; normal <10th weight underweight (n=236), underweig ht (n=35)
(1) Inhibitory control task (2) Cognitive flexibility task (3) Sustained attention task
No association between sustained attention and BMI. Lower inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility were predictors for higher body weight.
Groppe et al.
201 C 4
1657 (♀52,1 %)
Obese, 6-11 overweigh t and normal weight
ur na
lP
re
-p
ro of
Hughes et al.
Jo
Schwart 201 C z et al. 3
983 (♀503)
BMI
Obese, 12-18 VF, BF overweigh t, healthy weight
(1) Attention shifting (2) Updating and monitorin g working memory (3) Inhibitory control (4) Decision making
The negative association between BMI, attention shifting and updating working memory.
(1) Working memory (2) Resistance to interferenc e (3) Cognitive flexibility (4) Processing speed
Female subjects with low VF performed better than female subjects with high VF at working memory tasks, cognitive flexibility
task and processing speed (no difference for men).
201 C 3
29 (♀15)
Obese, 3-6 overweigh t, healthy weight
BMI
Kamijo et al.
201 C 2
126
Obese 7-9 (n=30), overweigh t (n=26), healthy weight (n=70)
Bruce et al.
201 C 2
59 (♀28)
Obese, 8-12 overweigh t, healthy weight
Davis et al.
201 C 1
170 Overweig (♀56%) ht and sedentary healthy weight
No association between EF measures and BMI zscores
BMI; BF; >30,6% and <37,3% overweight; >37,3% obesity
(1) Inhibitory control
Negative association between BMI and inhibitory control
BMI
(4) The ability to delay gratificati on
The higher BMI the poorer the ability to delay gratificatio n
(1) (2) (3) (4)
BMI zscores were not associated with planning and attention, however VF and SAF were positively related with planning.
-p
re
lP
ur na Jo
(1) The ability to delay gratificati on (2) Decision making (3) Inhibitory control (4) Switching
ro of
Pieper and Lauger o
7-11
BMI; >85th overweight; BF, VF, SAF
Planning Attention Sedentary Simultane ous
BMI zscores were negatively associated only with simultaneo us. No association between
EF as a predictor of later weight gain 201 F 3
164 (♀93)
Obese, 4 overweigh t and normal weight
Verbek 201 F en et al. 3
44 (♀22)
Overweig ht
BMI
(1) The ability to delay gratificati on
re
-p
Schlam et al.
ro of
BMI and other executive functions. BF was associated with planning and attention, whereas VF and SAF with planning and simultaneo us.
BMI
(1) Working memory (2) Inhibition
After 8months working memory and inhibition training, children improved their working memory skills and were more capable of maintaining their weight loss at 2 months post training
BMI; >85th overweight
(1) The ability to delay gratificati on
Children who performed worse at age 4 were more likely to be
Jo
ur na
lP
9-14
Seeyav e et al.
200 F 9
805
Obese, 4 overweigh t, healthy weight
Longer time delaying of gratificatio n at age 4 predicted lower BMI 30 years later
overweight at age 11
200 F 9
261
Obese, 4 overweigh t, healthy weight
BMI; >95th obesity; >85th and <95th overweight
Stautz et al.
201 F 6
6069 (♀50,9 %)
Obese, 8-10 overweigh t and healthy weight
BMI (measured at age 13)
(1) Verbal fluency (2) Problem solving (3) Working memory
Executive function scores at age 8 and age 10 predict overweight status at age 13 – the better working memory test results, the greater chance not being overweight
(1) Inhibitory control
The association between reduction in VAT and greater cognitive performanc e, independen t of whole body’s fat changes, particularly among obese participants
-p
(1) Selective attention (2) Attentiona l control (3) Working memory (4) Response Inhibition
re 201 F 7
144
Obese (n=77) and matched healthy weight (n=77)
Jo
ur na
Raine et al.
lP
The effect of fat reduction on EF
8-9
Overweight at 6 years is associated with lower executive functions at age 4 but at age 4 executive functions were not associated with BMI
ro of
Guxens et al.
BMI (BMI-forage-percentiles), VAT, SAT
C – cross-sectional, F – follow-up, BMI – body mass index, BF – body fat, VF – visceral fat, SAF – subcutaneous abdominal fat, VAT – visceral adipose tissue, SAT - subcutaneous abdominal adipose tissue, WC – waist circumference
Table 2. EF methods applied in studies and their association with obesity indicators EF EF tested at the EF measure at the study Authors group* study**
The Stop Signal Task
de Groot et al. (2017)
N
The Flanker Task
Raine et al. (2017)
Y
The Flanker Task
Huang et al. (2015)
Y
The Five Digit Test (FDT)
Blanco-Gomez et al. (2015) Wirt et al. (2015)
N
The Go/NoGo Task (subtest of KiTAP)
Wirt et al. (2014)
Y
Groppe et al. (2014)
N
The Flanker Task
Pieper et al. (2013)
N
The Two-Choice Reaction Task
Verbeken et al (2013)
Y
The Flanker Task
Kamijo et al. (2014)
Y
Kamijo et al. (2012)
Y
The Go/NoGo Task; Interference Task
Pauli-Pott et al. (2011)
Y
The Stop Signal Task
Stautz et al. (2016)
N
The Tapping Task
Hughes et al. (2015)
N
The Stroop Color-Word Interference Test The Five Digit Test (FDT), The Stroop Color-Word Interference Test The Go/NoGo Task
Maayan et al. (2011)
Y
Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010)
N
Fields et al (2013)
N
The Stroop Color-Word Interference Test The simple and four choice reaction time (SRT/CRT)
Schwartz et al. (2013)
Y(♀)
Gentier et al. (2013)
Y
The Task Performance
Kamijo et al. (2012)
Y
The Ruff 2 and 7 Selective Attention Test; the Symbol Search (from the WISC-III) The Revised-strategy application test (R-SAT) The Sky Search task (from TEACh)
Schwartz et al. (2013)
Y(♀)
Verdejo-Garcia et al. (2010) Stautz et al. (2016)
N
ur na
lP
re
-p
The Go/NoGo Task (subtest of KiTAP) The Fruit Stop Task
The Go/NoGo Task
Response inhibition
Impulsive disinhibition
Jo
Resistance to interference Response time/response accuracy
Processing speed
Self-regulation Selective attention
Y
ro of
Inhibitory Control Inhibitory control
The association with obesity indicators
Y
The sustained attention task (subtest of KiTAP)
Wirt et al. (2015)
N
Fields et al. (2013)
Y
The Opposite Words task (from TEA-Ch) Computerized serial reaction time task (SRT), the Posner paradigm The Cognitive Assessment System The Trial Making Test Part A (TMT-A)
Stautz et al. (2016)
N
Tsai et al. (2016)
Y
Davis et al. (2011)
N
Maayan et al. (2011)
Y
The Five Digit Test (FDT)
Blanco-Gomez et al. (2015)
Y
Attention shifting
The Cognitive Flexibility Task
Groppe et al. (2014)
Y
Shifting
The Five Digit Test (FDT)
Y
Cognitive flexibility
The Flexible Item Selection Task (FITS)
Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010) Hughes et al. (2015)
Wirt et al. (2015)
Y
Attentional control Visuospatial attention Attention
Cognitive Flexibility Switching
The Cognitive flexibility task (subtest of KiTAP) The Verbal Fluency
Working Memory
The Counting Span Task
Y(♀)
Maayan et al. (2011)
Y
Stautz et al. (2016)
Y
The Corsi Block-Tapping Task
Verbeken et al. (2013)
Y
The Digit-span subtest (WISCIII) The Wide Range Assessment of Learning and Memory (WRAML) The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MCSA)
Schwartz et al. (2013)
Y(♀)
Maayan et al. (2011)
Y
Guxens et al. (2009)
The Letter-number sequencing
Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010)
Y (6-yearold children) N
Updating (working memory)
The Digit Span Backwards Test
Groppe et al. (2014)
Y
Verbal working memory
Working memory functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task Working memory functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task The symbol digit modalities test (SDMT)
Alarcón et al. (2015)
Y
Alarcón et al. (2015)
Y
Blanco-Gomez et al. (2015)
N
Jo
ur na
lP
Working memory
N
Schwartz et al. (2013)
re
The Trial Making Test part B (TMT-B)
-p
The Conners’ Continuous Performance Test – II (CPT – II)
ro of
Sustained attention
Spatial working memory Visuomotor ability
Higher-Level Executive Functions
Decision making
Problem solving
The Choice Delay Task
de Groot et al. (2017)
N
The Delay of Gratification Task (DOG) and gift delay task The Delay of Gratification Task (DOG) The Delay of Gratification Task (DOG) The Delay of Gratification Task (DOG) The Delay of Gratification Task (DOG)
Hughes et al. (2015)
N
Schlam et al. (2013)
Y
Pieper and Laugero (2013) Bruce et al. (2012)
N
Seeyave et al. (2009)
Y
The Hungry Donkey Task
Groppe et al. (2014)
N
Question Based Delay Discounting Test (DDQ)
Fields et al. (2013)
Y
The Children’s Gambling Task (CGT) The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)
Pieper et al. (2013)
N
Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010) Qavam et al. (2015)
Y
Guxens et al. (2009)
Qavam et al. (2015)
Y (6-yearold children) Y
Davis et al. (2011)
Y
The Tower of London Test
Andre Rey Test, The Tower of London Test The Cognitive Assessment System Zoo map
re
Planning/ planning and organizing
-p
The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MCSA)
ur na
lP
Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010) Reasoning Similarities Vardejo-Garcia et al. (2010) *division based on Diamond (2013); ** the exact EF name from the article; EF - executive function, Y - yes, N - no, KiTAP - the computer-based test battery of attention for children, TEA-Ch - the Test of Everyday Attention for Children, WISC-III - Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III
Jo
Y
ro of
The ability to delay gratification
Y
N N