Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 166 (3rd series) Cingulate Cortex B.A. Vogt, Editor https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-64196-0.00009-1 Copyright © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved
Chapter 9
The cingulate cortex and spatial neglect A.M. BARRETT1,2,3*, ANDREW ABDOU1,4,5, AND MEGHAN D. CAULFIELD3,6 Center for Stroke Rehabilitation Research, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ, United States
1
2
Kessler Institute for Rehabilitation, West Orange, NJ, United States
3
Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rutgers New Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ, United States 4
Burke Rehabilitation Hospital, Mamaroneck, NY, United States 5
Montefiore Health System, Bronx, NY, United States
6
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Villanova University, Villanova, PA, United States
Abstract Spatial neglect is asymmetric attention, orienting, and action causing functional disability. It is linked to higher-order cortical sensory processing; however, spatial motor “Aiming” processing is critical to fundamental, adaptive environmental movement and daily life function. The cingulate cortex, in particular the anterior cingulate and anterior midcingulate cortex, is strongly linked to spatial Aiming deficits and likely to predict daily life disability in spatial neglect. The authors review the impact and mechanisms of spatial neglect and then describe specific symptoms associated with spatial neglect that are theoretically linked to cingulate cortical functions or associated with lesions extending to cingulate regions in a wellcharacterized spatial neglect cohort. The treatment implications for a link between cingulate cortex spatial Aiming neglect and therapies that improve spatial action, arousal, and persistence are discussed. Clinicians may want to consider theoretically motivated treatments targeted at specific symptoms as well as use treatments supported for spatial neglect based on unselected and uncharacterized groups.
OVERVIEW The cingulate cortex plays a key role in maintaining spatial cognitive systems critical for everyday life and function. Analyzing its contributions to a healthy spatial system is important for both theoretical and pragmatic reasons. Theoretical information about cingulatemediated processes in spatial function can motivate researchers and clinicians to take a structured approach to the role of this brain region in spatial cognitive function. However, knowledge about cingulate-related networks can also generate new information about spatial neglect directly relevant for scientific-guided treatments. Because researchers often limit their consideration of spatial neglect to the classic clinical features of the
syndrome associated with other brain regions, and do not take into account the specific characteristics associated with cingulate cortical damage in spatial neglect, they may not be well prepared to address the clinical problem. A general definition of spatial neglect is asymmetric orienting and action causing functional disability (Barrett & Burkholder, 2006). It has a staggering public health impact in personal and social cost; thus cingulaterelated spatial neglect is a major problem and a burden to public health. Further, we can predict that cingulatelinked mechanisms will play a role in some, but not other, stages of information processing in spatial cognition. A spatial cognitive model that fractionates spatial motor
*Correspondence to: A.M. Barrett, MD, FAAN, FANA, FASNR, Center for Visual and Cognitive Rehabilitation Research (151R), Atlanta VA Health System, 1601 Clairmont Road, Decatur, GA 30033, United States. Tel: +1-404-321-6111, E-mail:
[email protected]
130
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
Aiming from Where, perceptual-attentional and representational spatial cognitive processing stages (Barrett, 2014) may be particularly useful as we theoretically examine the spatial deficits observed after cingulate damage. In this chapter, our first aim is to describe why it is useful to think about spatial systems that both take in (spatial Where, perceptual-attentional systems) and act upon spatial information (output or spatial motor-intentional, Aiming systems). Specific Where and Aiming stages of spatial cognitive processing are dissociable, and we can observe unique symptoms associated with spatial Where and Aiming neglect after brain injury. Spatial Aiming and motor-exploratory behaviors (Heilman, 2004; Goedert et al., 2012, 2014; Barrett & Muzaffar, 2014) may be defined as feedforward, motor-intentional, action- and movement-related spatial behaviors. Our second aim will be to examine evidence from the animal literature that spatial Aiming bias has a neuroanatomic basis. Watson et al. (1973) initiated brain–behavior analysis of spatial neglect after cingulate cortical lesions based on stages of processing. These authors proposed that alerting to environmental sensory stimulation requires intact corticofugal pathways connecting cortical oculomotor and sensory association areas to the midbrain reticular formation, via cingulate pathways. They supported this hypothesis by demonstrating that four monkeys with supplementary motor and cingulate cortical lesions failed to respond to bilateral simultaneous tactile stimulation, while they were capable of responding to single stimuli (extinction to double simultaneous stimulation, a form of spatial neglect). Interestingly, in four of the five monkeys, motor responses to a single tactile stimulus were spatially misdirected. Thus, they demonstrated a possible association between cingulate lesions and an animal model of spatial Aiming bias, which they later explored in depth in monkeys with frontal lobe lesions (Watson et al., 1978). Third, we will explore the association between the anterior cingulate and anterior midcingulate cortex (ACC and aMCC), reward and engagement, and spatial Aiming neglect. We will review how dopaminergic activation of the ACC and aMCC may be a common element linking spatial Aiming systems with the anterior part of the cingulate cortex. The ACC may support spatial Aiming functions and also arousal and persistence. We will review specific animal and human literature supporting arousal and persistence deficits as a core component of spatial neglect. In the section “Spatial Aiming Neglect and the Anterior Cingulate and Anterior Midcingulate Cortices,” we also summarize data drawn from a dataset of well-characterized patients with spatial neglect. Consistent with the critical role of the cingulate cortex in spatial Aiming functions, we found more severe spatial neglect deficits in the patient group with cingulate cortical damage, and the deficits prominently included motorexploratory bias. This suggests that spatial neglect
symptoms closely related to spatial Aiming neglect may be quite common in patients with cingulate injury. Patients with spatial neglect and cingulate cortical injury also had more severe spatial neglect on standardized testing. The MCC region may also be linked to specific spatial Aiming neglect symptoms, related to arm and hand motor recovery, and hemispatial changes in arm and hand kinesis. The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) may have a relationship with different spatial Aiming neglect symptoms: defective motor response inhibition, directional hypokinesia of eye movements, and asymmetric approach behaviors in far space. Finally, we consider how this knowledge of dysfunctional spatial Aiming and arousal and persistence after cingulate cortical damage can be used to plan spatial neglect treatment. Standards for spatial neglect rehabilitation were derived from studies that broadly included patients with many lesion locations as well as diverse spatial neglect symptoms. Thus, although there are professional guidelines recommending care approaches for spatial neglect, we do not have protocols available specifically targeted at the cognitive deficits that occur after cingulate lesions. Our treatment summary offers choices for clinicians who wish to use a targeted, theoretically based approach to the rehabilitation of spatial neglect in patients with cingulate brain injury, addressing spatial Aiming neglect and arousal/persistence deficits.
IMPACT OF SPATIAL NEGLECT, AND STAGES-OF-PROCESSING COGNITIVE MODEL As we review information about cingulate-associated spatial neglect, we need to think about the very substantial impact of spatial neglect on public health and our society. In this section, we first examine the scope of the clinical problem of spatial neglect and its personal and social costs. We then describe a theoretical approach to spatial cognitive deficits, based upon separating input from output stages of spatial cognitive information processing. Rather than focusing exclusively on higher-order visual or perceptual-attentional sensory input systems, as has been traditional (Barrett, 2014), we will consider how cingulate-linked systems may participate in spatial Aiming functions (outputs of the spatial cognitive system). Spatial neglect occurs in the weeks after stroke in at least 50% of right brain–damaged and 30% of left brain–damaged survivors (Gainotti et al., 1972; Denes et al., 1982; Fullerton et al., 1986; Stone et al., 1993; Kalra et al. 1997; McGlone et al., 1997; Ringman et al., 2004; Hreha et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2015a,b,c). Chen et al. (2015a,b,c) reported that in patients receiving inpatient rehabilitation facility (IRF) care, spatial neglect impedes rehabilitation outcomes: burden of care due to
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT stroke was more than 20% greater in patients with spatial neglect than in stroke survivors who did not have spatial neglect. Despite receiving more days of intensive rehabilitation, patients with spatial neglect made 13% fewer gains in functional independence than patients who did not have spatial neglect. Patients with spatial neglect experienced a slow rate of functional improvement, more than 50% slower than patients without spatial neglect. Patients with spatial neglect fell 6.5 times more often than patients without spatial neglect did. Spatial neglect prolonged IRF hospitalization by about 10 days. In this study, the cost of caring for patients with spatial neglect was also likely much higher. Total average IRF charges per person may be estimated at $1600 per day (Mayer et al., 2003). Thus, at least an additional $16,000 worth of additional costs due to prolonged length of stay could have been associated with spatial neglect in this study. Patients with spatial neglect at IRF admission were 45% less likely than those with no symptoms to go home at the end of IRF care, with more severe spatial neglect predicting long-term placement (Chen et al., 2015a,b, c). Informal family caregivers of stroke survivors with spatial neglect may also spend much more time in supervising and caring for them. In one study (Chen et al., 2017), family caregivers spent more than three times
131
as much time in patient care as caregivers of stroke survivors without spatial neglect did—more than 20 h daily. Asymmetric behaviors observed in spatial neglect (see Table 9.1 and Fig. 9.1) may reflect the function of more than one stage of spatial cognitive processing, and stages of processing may reflect functions of different neuroanatomic and neuropharmacologic brain networks. There are three major categories of deficits that can be identified in spatial neglect. Spatial cognition, like all aspects of cognition, requires information to be acquired for processing via the senses and attention systems. However, information is also internally stored. Finally, systematic higher-order output-response processing also takes place as part of spatial cognition. Thus, an extremely useful overall model for spatial information processing describes how spatial information is taken into the system (spatial perceptual-attentional Where function), how it is stored internally in the form of visual images or maps (spatial representational Where function), and then how movement and action is shaped, tuned, modulated, and executed in 3D space (spatial Aiming function). An input–output, stagewise processing model can be used to study speech and language, limb praxis, and many other cognitive functions that involve both perceptual-related and motor-related
Table 9.1 Spatial cognitive processing stages and associated deficits in spatial neglect
Spatial processing stage Where, perceptual-attentional awareness (Heilman and Valenstein, 2012)
Where, representational imagery (Bisiach and Luzzatti, 1978; Halligan et al., 1992; Coslett, 1997; Bonato et al., 2016)
Aiming, motor-intentional action (Meador et al., 1986; Heilman, 2004; Barrett, 2014)
Key behaviors demonstrating bias in spatial neglect Extinction to double simultaneous stimulation Decreased vigilance in the neglected space Allochiria Right-biased visual imagery Neglect dyslexia Right bias with mental number line Directional hypokinesia Hemispatial hypokinesia Limb hypokinesia Asymmetric motor response inhibition
Frequently observed functional impairments Accidents in or outside the home Difficulty eating entire meal Failure to respond in social conversation and interaction Poor environmental navigation Illusions, difficulty recognizing objects in unfamiliar orientation, or poor visual resolution Difficulty reading Calculation errors affecting money management Veering while ambulating, either walking or in a wheelchair Augmented hemiparesis
Summarized from Barrett, A.M., Goedert, K.M., Basso, J.C., 2012. Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps. Nat Rev Neurol 8 (10), 567–577. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2012.170; Riestra, A.R., Barrett, A., 2013. Rehabilitation of spatial neglect. Handb Clin Neurol 110, 347–355. doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-52901-5.00029-0; functional impairments listed are empiric and based on clinical teaching and observations, requiring further systematic study.
132
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
Fig. 9.1. Paper-and-pencil testing characteristic of left spatial neglect after right brain injury. At top (A), on the left, a cube presented for the patient to copy (top right) is only reproduced partially (right side only). At bottom (B), an array of lines for cancellation (Albert, 1973) presented on an 8.500 1100 horizontal sheet reveals omissions, more left-sided and more marked in near than in far space.
processing (Shallice and Cooper, 2011; Barrett, 2014). However, such a model is not yet widely employed to discuss spatial cognition. Clinical characteristics of spatial neglect may be associated with specific stages of processing (see Table 9.1). Spatial Where, perceptual-attentional errors can be attributed to asymmetric unawareness, asymmetrically abnormal habituation, asymmetric decreased vigilance, or asymmetric distraction (Goedert et al., 2012). The phenomenon of extinction, in which the patient fails to perceive a contralesional stimulus only when it is presented simultaneously with a stimulus on the ipsilesional side, may be best explained by the limited capacity of perceptual-attentional resources and is a classic sign of Where spatial cognitive dysfunction (Heilman, et al., 2000; Marzi et al., 2001; Riestra et al., 2001, 2002; Hillis et al., 2006; Beversdorf et al., 2008). Unfortunately, there is not much data yet available describing groups of subjects who have been characterized on both laboratory spatial Where and spatial Aiming tasks to understand patterns of performance of these groups on daily life functional assessment. For example, it is logical that spatial Where neglect may be the cause of errors when the patient does not notice family or clinicians
approaching in the neglected space but then greets them as soon as they cross the midline and are in the preferred body space. Where spatial errors of the representative type may occur when patients attempt to direct a helper in fetching personal effects from another room, yet only advise the helper about the objects that are positioned on the preferred side as one enters the room and omit mention of any objects that are on the neglected side. Presumably, these objects were neglected “in their mind’s eye.” Spatial Aiming errors, in their turn, might have a functional relevance in patients who lean or push toward the good side, adversely affecting posture or transfers (Shinsha and Ishigami, 1999; Riestra and Barrett, 2013). To understand how we might reduce the cost and burden of spatial neglect, we may need a system to analyze spatial neglect behaviors that relates closely to biologic factors for intervention. Although more work is needed to confirm these associations, it is quite possible that people with spatial neglect may have functional disability that arises specifically from unawareness, from inaccurate internal spatial representations, or from asymmetric orienting movements and action (Thomas and Barrett, 2019). For example, patients with spatial neglect after a right brain stroke may only eat from the right side of
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT a plate, leaving much of the meal untouched. This might reflect a problem with detecting food on the left side of the plate. However, there is an alternate explanation. The patients might see the food on the left side of the plate and although they may be aware that it is there, they may not be able to direct actions leftward toward the food in eating—their movement repertoire may only include rightward spatial orientation. Because the only spatial movements they can initiate are rightward movements, they may be “stuck,” repeatedly taking food from the right side of the plate. The Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party in the United Kingdom (2016) warns that this bias may have a direct and adverse disabling effect, that people with stroke and spatial neglect should “be monitored to ensure that they do not eat too little through missing food on one side of the plate.” Patients with spatial neglect may also fail to dress the left side of their body, or may favor right space when navigating a wheelchair. Again, although these errors are often interpreted as perceptual or attentional (Riestra and Barrett, 2013), any of these errors might reflect either asymmetric input or asymmetric spatial output (or a combination of the two; Barrett and Burkholder, 2006; Goedert et al., 2012). It is important to consider which stage of processing is responsible for the abnormal behavior, because improvement of disability may depend directly upon whether the patient receives a treatment that is effective at altering that specific stage of spatial processing (Barrett et al., 2012). Different neuroanatomic regions might play different roles in the networks supporting the three-stage spatial cognitive processing model. Posterior, temporal–parietal cortical sensory association networks may be critical to support information input (Where, perceptual-attentional spatial cognition) or storage (Where, representational spatial cognition; Na et al., 1998), while anterior or motor-related subcortical networks may be more critical to output cognition (Spatial-motor aiming; Heilman and Valenstein, 1972; Watson et al., 1978; Na et al., 1998; Barrett et al., 1999; Sapir et al. 2007).
SPATIAL AIMING NEGLECT AND THE ANTERIOR CINGULATE AND ANTERIOR MIDCINGULATE CORTICES Dopaminergic spatial Aiming A family of characteristic symptoms are associated with spatial motor Aiming neglect and linked to dopaminergic activation. Dopaminergic brain activation, in turn, relates closely to reward networks involving the ACC and aMCC. Thus, there may be a close relationship between dysfunction in these regions, reward learning, and spatial Aiming neglect. We will first review the asymmetric motor behaviors observed in spatial Aiming neglect and their dopaminergic correlates. We will then discuss
133
the relationship of these functions to ACC and aMCC damage in spatial neglect. There are a number of specific deficits we can discuss under the general category of spatial Aiming errors. A major deficit to consider is directional hypokinesia, a failure of intentional movement directed toward the neglected contralesional hemispace (Heilman et al., 1985; Bisiach et al., 1990; Coslettet al., 1990; Barrett, Crucian, and Heilman, 1999; Sapir et al., 2007). Directional hypokinesia can affect either hand and it can also affect the eyes, the trunk, and the whole body, and directional movement deficits in spatial Aiming neglect affecting the eyes, hand, arm, trunk, and whole body also include bradykinesia or hypometria (Heilman, 2004). Other specific dysfunctional movement patterns noted in spatial Aiming neglect include hemispatial hypokinesia, defined as lateralized differences in movement gain, force, or rapidity of movements depending upon the hemispace in which they are performed (Heilman and Valenstein, 1979; Goedert et al., 2012), and limb hypokinesia, which comprises abnormalities of movement performed by the contralesional limbs, beyond that accounted for by hemiparesis alone (Meador et al., 1986; Triggs et al., 1994). In addition to these deficits, patients with spatial Aiming neglect also can exhibit asymmetric perseveration, asymmetric motor impersistence, and asymmetric abnormal motor response inhibition (Kwon and Heilman, 1991, Heilman, 2004, Nys et al., 2006, Khurshid et al., 2009, Barrett, 2014). Motorexploratory behaviors, such as the ability to avoid collisions while steering and ambulating in a wheelchair, were not directly correlated with a measure of directional hypokinesia in one study (Goedert et al., 2012); however, motorexploratory behaviors are also likely part of a spatial Aiming constellation of functions. More work needs to be performed to clarify the interrelationships between these spatial-motor Aiming activities. A number of studies linked the disruption of ascending dopaminergic systems with spatial neglect and its recovery, based on animal models (Ungerstedt, 1976; Deuel and Collins, 1983) and the benefit of dopaminergic medication on generic measures of spatial neglect in human studies (Fleet et al., 1987; Mukand et al., 2001; Gorgoraptis et al., 2012). The ability to respond and turn in a contralesional direction is impaired by dopamine depletion (Carli et al. 1989), a movement bias that is distinct from a contralesional perception deficit in animals (Hoyman et al., 1979). Contralesional movement initiation is also delayed after striatal dopamine depletion in rats trained to react to sensory stimuli by making instrumental responses or head-orienting movements (Apicella et al., 1991). Contralesional orienting can be restored by dopaminergic pharmacotherapy (Schwarting and Huston, 1996). In humans, spatial movements in spatial neglect can be specifically manipulated with dopaminergic pharmacotherapy, linked with
134
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
the spatial Aiming symptoms noted earlier, directional and hemispatial hypokinesia, and motor-exploratory behaviors (bromocriptine: Grujic et al., 1998; Barrett et al., 1999; Chaudhari et al., 2013; apomorphine: Geminiani et al., 1998). There are two links between spatial Aiming in spatial neglect and the cingulate cortex. First, the ACC/aMCC receives rich dopaminergic input (Gallagher et al., 2015), and dopaminergic subcortical–cortical connectivity to frontal and cingulate areas is important to reward and implicit learning (Jones, 1986; Jessup et al., 2010; Bissonette and Roesch, 2016; Umemoto et al., 2017). Lecce et al. (2015) proposed that the ACC may modulate visual-motor spatial behavior based on motivational/ hedonistic inputs. This group reported that a patient with spatial neglect showed severely disrupted contralesional reward learning after injury to the ACC. Consistent with this idea, the ACC is a part of a brain network activated when emotional salience enhances stimulus detection in spatial neglect (Grabowska et al., 2011). Although these papers studied patients with extremely large lesions and cingulate injury may have been a minor part of the total damage, this work still suggests that rewarding detection of salience may partly depend on processing in the ACC/aMCC regions, and in the context of handrelated motor control and pain processing, the aMCC may be very important to stimulus-driven spatial movements of the contralesional arm and hand based on pain or discomfort (Misra and Coombes, 2015). Pereira et al. (2010) demonstrated that activity in the aMCC is increased during target detection preceded by emotionally unpleasant material, with activation depending upon whether or not the participant made an overt motor response to the event. The information mentioned previously potentially links the ACC/aMCC to hemispatial and limb kinesia in spatial Aiming neglect. Weiss et al. (2003) and Ko et al. (2009) demonstrated that spatial decision and action tasks closely associated with spatial Aiming are associated with right ACC and aMCC activation, along with activation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and left sensorimotor cortex. Prism adaptation is a treatment for spatial neglect. It involves several sessions of repeated goal-directed hand and arm movements toward a visual target. As patients look through baseright, yoked, wedge prisms, their field of view of each eye is displaced to the right. The visual shift creates a rightward movement error, which upon repeated trials is eliminated via adaptation. Once the goggles are removed, a persistent effect of the prism adaptation is strongly associated with functional improvement. Neuroimaging and neurophysiologic studies support the idea that prism adaptation therapy (PAT) affects spatial cognition and improves both impairment and daily life task performance in spatial neglect (Luaute, et al., 2006;
Barrett et al., 2012; Champod et al., 2018). Tsujimoto et al. (2018) showed that altered spatial bias after prism adaptation changes functional connectivity (FC) affecting the ACC. The study found that the FC between the right frontal eye field (FEF) and the right intraparietal sulcus was significantly decreased after PAT and that between the right FEF and the right ACC and aMCC was significantly increased after PAT and recovered within 1 h. Prism adaptation training was previously demonstrated to have a primary effect on spatial bias via altering spatial Aiming in both healthy subjects (Fortis et al., 2011a,b) and patients with spatial neglect (Fortis et al., 2011a,b). Thus, the Tsujimoto et al. (2018) study strongly suggests that the ACC is a key part of the brain network supporting spatial-motor processing, which is dysfunctional in spatial Aiming neglect.
The cingulate cortex in a cohort of patients with spatial neglect In the previous section, we suggested that ACC and aMCC may be specifically relevant to spatial Aiming and motor-intentional function. We investigated that question further via retrospective analysis in a dataset of patients who participated in a number of past research protocols investigating spatial neglect, from 2006 to the present (Clinicaltrials.gov identifiers: NCT00990353, NCT00350012, NCT00989430). These patients, who enrolled in studies of the spatial neglect syndrome after stroke had brain images from their clinical care evaluated for lesion location. Our analysis indicates that deficits of patients with spatial neglect and cingulate cortical lesions, most of which affected the ACC and MCC, were more severe and prominently include motorexploratory bias.
PARTICIPANTS We retrospectively analyzed behavioral data from 153 patients (72 female, 81 male, M ¼ 64.9 years). As part of a previous research looking at the behavioral and neuropsychologic characteristics of spatial neglect, patients completed neuropsychologic testing with the Behavioral Inattention Test-conventional subtest (BIT-c; Wilson et al., 1987), the Catherine Bergego Scale (CBS; Azouvi et al., 2003), and the Folstein Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975).
LESION MAPPING AND LOCALIZATION All participants provided informed consent consistent with the oversight of our Kessler Foundation Institutional Review Board. We examined clinical brain scans, which were obtained with participant authorization. When more than one scan was available, the scan obtained closest to the spatial neglect assessment was
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT used. The brain scans were mapped using a “doublestrain” method that has been outlined in detail previously (Goedert et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2014). Briefly, reliability-trained technicians (blind to behavioral classifications) manually mapped individual lesions onto the axial plane of a standard brain template (Montreal Neurological Institute) using MRIcron software (Rorden et al., 2007). Normalized lesion maps were then evaluated via a consensus conference led by an independent neurologist to ensure accuracy. Lesion volume (in cubic centimeters) was calculated from the lesion map and overlaid onto a standard template for comparisons between groups. We coded cingulate damage for each patient’s lesion to identify whether the lesion crossed into the cingulate boundaries and which subregions of the cingulate cortex were involved using both Brodmann areas and Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) demarcations using MRIcron. Each lesion was then overlaid onto a standard brain template and the volume of cingulate lesions in subregions was extracted for analysis.
RESULTS Cingulate damage is associated with more severe spatial neglect The Behavioral Inattention Test-conventional subtest (BIT-c) is a paper-and-pencil measure of spatial neglect containing several subtests (cancellation tasks, figure copy, drawing, line bisection). BIT-c scores were available in 147 patient records. We separated patient data from these patients into a cingulate + group if any part of the brain lesion crossed into any subregion of the cingulate cortex (53 patients) and we coded patient data into a cingulate group if there was no portion of the lesion in cingulate cortex (97 patients). A univariate ANOVA comparing groups (cingulate+, cingulate ) on BIT-c performance and using total lesion volume as a covariate indicated significant differences between the groups, F(1,147) ¼ 4.767, P < 0.05. Fig. 9.2 demonstrates that BIT-c scores were lower in the cingulate + group, suggestive of more severe spatial neglect. All of the BIT-c items except drawing were more impaired in the patients with spatial neglect, after Bonferroni correction (see Table 9.2 for summary). The Catherine Bergego Scale (CBS) is a functional performance test that captures additional variance in daily activity disability, as compared with the BIT-c (Goedert et al., 2012). One-hundred and forty-two of the patients with spatial neglect in the cohort had CBS scores in the dataset; these scores can be divided into mild neglect (0–9), moderate neglect (10–19), and severe neglect (21– 30). We divided patients into four groups by CBS score: no neglect, mild, moderate, and severe. Patients with a CBS ¼ 0 were in the spatial neglect cohort
135
because they had spatial neglect on the BIT-c; the two tests are not always concordant. Using these groups in conjunction with cingulate lesion information, we conducted a chi square test to determine whether neglect severity was related to cingulate lesions. The relation between CBS group and cingulate group was significant, x2 (3, N ¼ 142) ¼ 19.21, P < 0.001; a greater proportion of patients who had severe neglect also had lesions that included the cingulate region (Fig. 9.3). Thus, this analysis confirms that cingulate stroke in our cohort was associated with more severe spatial neglect. Performance testing with the Catherine Bergego Scale suggests cingulate damage is linked to motor-exploratory errors The CBS consists of 10 items that are examiner scored by watching patients perform functional tasks, on a scale of 0 (no neglect) to 3 (severely impaired). To determine whether cingulate lesions contribute to motorexploratory symptoms of spatial neglect, we conducted independent samples t-tests. Table 9.3 shows the items of the CBS and scores between groups. Goedert et al. (2012) suggested that some CBS items are more correlated with perceptual-attentional spatial neglect and other CBS items are more correlated with motor-exploratory spatial neglect symptoms. Motor-exploratory CBS items in this prior study included the ability to dress the contralesional body, navigate in familiar places like the hospital or home, care for and anticipate the position of the contralesional body, and avoid collisions with people or objects (in bold in Table 9.3). As noted earlier, the patients with brain lesions extending into the cingulate cortex had more severe spatial neglect. When we evaluated items that were different in subjects with and without cingulate lesions, with a Bonferroni correction, three items accounted mainly for this increase in severity. Because two of the three items that accounted for increased severity in people with cingulate lesions were motor exploratory, this suggests that the patients had greater spatial-motor deficits. They also may have had greater spatial Aiming bias, since directional hypokinesia, limb hypokinesia, and hemispatial hypokinesia are likely to be closely related to motor exploration. The finding that the cingulate regions of greatest lesion overlap appear to be the ACC and aMCC regions (see Fig. 9.3) is consistent with an association of moderate–severe spatial neglect, spatial-motor deficits, and the ACC/aMCC cortex. Limitations and future directions Goedert et al. (2012) derived a factor structure for symptoms defining motor-exploratory behaviors; however, this was based on a group of patients with diverse lesions (some of whom were included in the present lesion
136
A.M. BARRETT ET AL. 100 90 80
BIT score (mean)
70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 No cingulate lesion
Cingulate lesion
Fig. 9.2. BIT-c scores of patients whose lesions did not include the cingulate (n ¼ 97; left darker bar) indicate that their scores are higher (less impaired) than those having a lesion that crosses into any part of the cingulate (n ¼ 53; right lighter bar). The BIT-c is scored out of 146, and scores <130 are consistent with spatial neglect. Error bars represent 1 SEM. Table 9.2 BIT-c subtest scores for groups with and without cingulate involvement in lesions BIT-c subtest
No cingulate lesion score (SD)
Cingulate lesion score (SD)
P
Lines Letters Stars Line crossing Figure shape copying Representational drawing
4.35 (3.02) 23.64 (12.82) 33.20(17.72) 27.92 (11.02) 1.67 (1.49) 1.61 (1.16)
2.53 (2.66) 16.96 (16.90) 24.00 (16.90) 22.25 (11.35) 0.96 (1.32) 1.18 (1.26)
<0.001 a 0.003 a 0.002 a 0.003 a 0.004 a 0.036
a
Indicates significant independent samples t-test with Bonferroni correction. BIT-c, Behavioral Inattention Test-conventional.
cohort). It is possible that the factor structure for symptoms defining spatial-motor exploratory and even spatial Aiming behaviors may be different when considering only patients with cingulate lesions; evaluating validated Where and Aiming assessments and the CBS in such a group would be needed to confirm the factor structure. If a different factor structure were derived in cingulate lesions and spatial neglect, including asymmetric eating, the third item, this might reflect impairment of different components of eating skills, or qualitatively different impairment of the same skills, since eating has been demonstrated to be multistep and multidimensional functional activity (Hanna-Pladdy et al., 2003). Prospective research on patients with cingulate damage and spatial neglect, comparatively characterizing their deficits, is needed.
AROUSAL/PERSISTENCE AND THE ANTERIOR CINGULATE CORTEX The ACC may also support arousal and persistence. The anterior cingulate cortex is regarded as an essential regulating component of the alertness network and has also been associated with anticipation and preparation of attentional activity (LaBerge and Buchsbaum, 1990; Sturm et al., 1999; Thimm et al., 2006). Arousal and persistence deficits (distinct from perseveration or motor impersistence deficits that are spatial asymmetric) are closely related to the deficits of spatial neglect and commonly cooccur with the spatial neglect syndrome (Robertson, 2001). Generalized body hypokinesia is also a clinical sign cooccurring with spatial Aiming neglect (Riestra and Barrett, 2013).
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT
137
Cingulate lesion+, mild neglect (n = 11) R
Percentage of overlap 75%
R
A Cingulate lesion+, moderate neglect (n = 16) R
R
B Cingulate lesion+, severe neglect (n = 21) R
R 25%
C Fig. 9.3. Lesion overlay maps created for spatial neglect severity groups (mild (A), moderate (B), severe (C)). Analyses indicate that those participants with severe neglect also had a greater proportion of lesions that included the cingulate region (C). The right hemisphere is presented on the left side; colors range from 25% (blue) to 75% (red) of participants with overlapping lesions. Lines on the sagittal reference image (top right of each panel) indicate location of axial slices and lines on the coronal reference image (bottom right of each panel) indicate location of the sagittal slices.
An investigation on the neural correlates of spatial and nonspatial attentional subcomponents such as alerting, orienting, and reorienting of attention concluded that although behavioral data yielded clear benefits of spatial cueing, event-related data does not provide any compelling evidence for a parietal involvement in spatial
orienting. Instead, activations were found in the aMCC (Thiel et al., 2004). This suggests that aMCC-linked brain networks are responsible for this deficit when it occurs in spatial neglect. A large body of evidence indicates that the aMCC is one of the crucial brain regions engaged in the evaluative processes and in inhibiting
138
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
Table 9.3 Subtest scores on the CBS for groups with and without cingulate lesions CBS items
No cingulate lesion score (SD)
Cingulate lesion score (SD)
P
Dressing Grooming Gaze Limb awareness Auditory Collisions Navigation Personal belongings Meals Cleaning after meal
1.33 (0.97) 0.98 (0.89) 1.56 (0.93) 1.50 (1.07) 1.11 (1.02) 1.77 (1.00) 1.29 (1.18) 1.18 (1.01) 1.02 (0.96) 0.98 (0.99)
1.88 (1.10) 1.24 (0.96) 1.96 (1.02) 1.94 (1.07) 1.37 (1.02) 2.00 (1.18) 1.98 (1.22) 1.57 (1.14) 1.70 (1.08) 1.30 (0.98)
0.003 a 1.06 0.017 0.020 0.148 0.249 0.001 a 0.038 <0.001 a 0.077
a
Indicates significant independent samples t-test with Bonferroni correction. CBS, Catherine Bergego Scale. See Azouvi et al. (2003) for a full instrument description. CBS items bolded are motor exploratory associated (Goedert et al., 2012, see text).
responses toward less desirable but easily obtainable goals in favor of more desirable goals that may also require more physical and/or mental effort (Wang et al., 2017). Therapeutic studies also support improved spatial neglect as a result of improved arousal and alertness in ACC-linked brain systems. Thimm et al. (2006) hypothesized that 3-week alertness training will increase frontal and parietal activations either ipsilesionally or contralesionally, reflecting a training-induced functional reactivation associated with training-induced amelioration of neglect symptoms. In an fMRI study they evaluated 7 right-handed patients with cortical and subcortical right hemisphere vascular lesions. All presented with stable neglect symptoms and intrinsic alertness deficit for at least 3 months. Results showed that six out of the seven subjects showed behavioral improvement on at least one of the neglect subtests immediately after training. The alertness training comprised two subroutines (“racing car” and “motorcycle”) of the attention training program “AIXTENT.” They concluded that AIXTENT alertness training game is an effective approach for the rehabilitation of spatial neglect; however, it may be necessary to prolong or even continually maintain training. Functional recovery was based on a reactivation of ipsilesional and contralesional cortical areas, including the ACC, right superior and middle frontal gyrus, other medial frontal regions, angular gyrus, and precuneus (Thimm et al., 2006). In patients having apathy but not evaluated for spatial neglect, Sasaki et al. (2017) studied 13 patients with chronic stroke assigned to either high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over the region spanning from the MCC to medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) or sham stimulation for 5 days. The degree of
change in an apathy scale score was significantly greater in the rTMS group than that in the sham stimulation group (Sasaki et al., 2017).
MIDCINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL AIMING: LIMB KINESIS The midcingulate cortex (MCC), especially the cingulate premotor areas in the cingulate sulcus, may be particularly relevant to spatial hand and arm movements and thus limb kinesis in spatial Aiming. In the posterior MCC, learned integration of eye and head orienting toward a significant stimulus and negative affect or sensation may take place (Clark et al., 2010). The caudal cingulate premotor area also plays a major role in tuning the force and direction of movement in orienting the head and body in space (Vogt, 2016). Buklina (2002) reported that neglect was associated with cingulate-located arteriovenous malformations in 8 patients, with 4 having lesions in the MCC and 4 in the PCC. Limb hypokinesia, bradykinesia, and hypometria are all motor-intentional disorders we categorize with spatial Aiming deficits (Thomas and Barrett, 2019). As we note in Section 7, constraint-induced therapy, in which patients with hemiparesis are treated by massed practice with the paretic arm while constraint of the nonparetic arm helps to prevent substitutive use of that limb, could be considered a therapy to improve limb hypokinesia or limb-specific bradykinesia/hypometria. As such, it is interesting that Takebayashi et al. (2018) reported that improvements in arm motor assessment with constraint therapy were significantly correlated with white matter structural integrity of the motor cingulate cortex. Funayama et al. (2008) also reported limb-specific neglect symptoms in a patient with a pericallosal artery
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT stroke, infarction of much of the corpus callosum, and cortical infarction of the MCC extending to a small part of the anterior PCC. Their patient performed paper-andpencil tests markedly differently depending upon whether the contralesional hand was used, consistent with disconnection observed in callosal neglect (Heilman and Adams, 2003); however, unlike previous patients with callosal neglect, her deficit was persistent. The authors speculated that the right cingulate lesion contributed to persistent left hemispatial kinesia with the right hand. Further information is needed to determine whether spatial Aiming deficits associated with MCC lesions are due to cortical or white matter lesions or a combination of the two, and what lesion localizations are always associated with risk. Finally, further research on the functional impact of MCC lesions in patients with spatial neglect, for example, performing bimanual tasks, is needed. Limb-specific spatial Aiming deficits would be expected to interfere with motor recovery of a paretic limb (Barrett and Muzaffar, 2014). If the MCC plays a role in motor intention (spatial Aiming) relevant to activation, use, and practice when the arm and hand is impaired, we might expect that damage to this region would predict recovery in arm motor rehabilitation studies. In patients with subcortical stroke, Li et al. (2016) and Ward et al. (2006) both reported that activation of ipsilesional MCC and white matter integrity in this region was associated with better motor task performance. Unfortunately, no spatial Aiming tasks that are sensitive to hemispatial or limb hypokinesia were used in these studies to elucidate whether improved Aiming function contributed to motor recovery.
RETROSPLENIAL AND POSTERIOR CINGULATE CORTEX IN SPATIAL APPROACH BEHAVIORS The retrosplenial cortex (RSC) extends around the splenium of the corpus callosum. Most of the RSC is found on the inferior aspect of the cingulate gyrus. It is active during the resting, mind-wandering state (Clark et al., 2010). Retrosplenial cortex has general associations with spatial function, including navigation and spatial long-term memory (Cona and Scarpazza, 2019). Takahashi (2004) reported that right RSC and adjacent PCC were associated with spatial amnesia for topographic features, with familiar locations (buildings and landscapes) recognized, though the positional relationship of these locations was lost. Patients with isolated memory impairment coupled with slight verbal and visuospatial impairments show reduced cerebral blood flow in the PCC (Clark et al., 2010). In a meta-analysis extracted from more than 11,000 functional activation
139
studies in healthy controls, Huang et al. (2019) found the RSC to be associated with “spatial” function. Although their model was based on averaged regions of interest that may not have preserved anatomic distinction of cingulate regions across subjects, this suggests that the perisplenial region is a candidate hub for visual–spatial processing. Further, the PCC may have a specific relationship with spatial Aiming in spatial neglect: motor response inhibition, asymmetric eye movements, and approach behaviors in far space. Activity among the neurons within the PCC increases following saccadic eye movements, sensitive to the direction of displacement rather than the spatial location of the target (Mesulam, 1999). Neurons in PCC of macaque monkeys discharge in response to visual stimuli and following saccadic eye movements, and functional imaging in humans has described activation in PCC associated with visually guided saccades (Olson et al., 1996; Mort et al., 2003). The PCC may selectively mediate the emergence of cue-induced visuospatial expectancy, a process that entails the redistribution of motivational relevance within the extrapersonal space. This sensitivity of the PCC to cues over targets in spatial attention-shifting tasks suggests that oculomotor approach behaviors based on internal expectancies may critically involve this region. Barrett et al. (2000) and Heilman et al. (1990) reported that patients with lesions affecting the thalamicretrosplenial network for stimulus-evoked oculomotor exploration had abnormal approach behaviors in far space; if a lateral distractor appeared in far space, they would make abnormal movements toward it. Heilman et al. (1990) reported that the approach behaviors affected both oculomotor and limb movements; Barrett et al.’s (2000) patient was biased when directing limb movements. In both cases, the authors felt that retrosplenial-cingulate-thalamic-dorsolateral frontal cortical connectivity was disrupted by a brain lesion, releasing approach behaviors toward the contralesional space. Although less commonly reported than spatial neglect affecting near space, asymmetric performance in far extrapersonal space has been reported in spatial neglect (Mennemeier et al., 1992; Halligan et al., 2003; Committeri et al., 2007). Detailed information about the functional consequences of this deficit is not yet available. Both the patient with retrosplenial injury (Heilman et al., 1990) and the patient with anterior thalamic stroke (Barrett et al., 2000) had driving disability; spatial Aiming in far space may be commonly associated with this deficit. Other activities important to daily life function such as rapid ambulation or ambulating in a busy environment may also be adversely affected by approach behaviors in far space. More research is needed
140
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
to establish whether spatial approach behaviors selective to far space after PCC and retrosplenial injury are spatial Aiming in their character, their functional skill associations, and best treatments to improve daily life performance.
A SPATIAL NEGLECT TREATMENT APPROACHES Practice guidelines A best practice to identify patients with spatial neglect across the severity continuum is to employ standardized assessment with an instrument demonstrated to predict functional disability (Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party, 2016). In patients with spatial neglect after cingulate cortical damage, severe spatial neglect may be immediately apparent during bedside observations. Patients may persistently orient rightward, sitting and lying with the trunk, eyes, and head turned to the side of their brain injury, noticeably failing to orient even to significant left-sided events such as a loud noise (Adair & Barrett, 2008). Milder spatial neglect may not be as noticeable, especially if the patient is bedbound or in an unchanging, familiar environment. For example, if a patient has a right-sided trunk, eye, and head movement bias such that the patient orients rightward about 60%–70% of the time, a clinician may not be with the patient long enough to note the asymmetry of behavior; in a hospital room, it also may not seem abnormal for the patient to be more oriented in one spatial direction because the placement of the window, television, etc., are fixed relative to a patient lying in bed. However, because of the increased safety risk and rehabilitation outcome differences that affect even patients with milder spatial neglect symptoms, we need to identify them during routine acute and postacute care. The Catherine Bergego Scale (Azouvi et al., 2006; Chen et al., 2012, 2015a) is an examiner-related scale, rating a patient’s observed asymmetry while executing daily life functional tasks. Patients are scored based on the ability to orient, respond, and act leftward, and this scale has excellent external and predictive validity. If a performance scale with external validity is used to screen for spatial neglect, this may address underimplementation of treatment for spatial neglect, because a practice that is currently common is to assign treatment based on the individual clinician’s knowledge and judgment rather than a fixed criterion. Performing a functional assessment will increase uniformity of implementation and allow clinicians to broaden their concept of spatial neglect beyond a rigid concept of spatial neglect of this disorder as a problem with vision or attention (Barrett et al., 2012).
Special considerations for patients with damage to cingulate cortex In the earlier sections, we suggested that spatial Aiming neglect, or spatial neglect affecting motor-intentional, action, and movement systems, may be particularly characteristic of the deficits people with cingulate cortical damage experience when they have disorders of spatial cognition. Implementing the systematic approach mentioned earlier is particularly important to identify spatial neglect associated with cingulate cortical damage. Our group (Riestra and Barrett, 2013; Thomas and Barrett, 2019) suggested that spatial Aiming deficits, motor-exploratory problems, and problems with arousal may be particularly difficult for clinicians to identify. Despite its high prevalence after stroke, spatial neglect is undetected in more than 60% of patients during routine clinical care (Edwards et al., 2006). If cingulateassociated spatial neglect manifests with spatial-motor rather than the typical visual or perceptual omission errors, a systematic, uniform process of assessment that includes functional performance will be even more important for rapid diagnosis and acute treatment. Although the strongest evidence for spatial Aiming deficits may implicate aMCC, where the cingulate premotor areas are located in the cingulate sulcus, there is also some association between the cingulate premotor areas and limb hypokinesis, as discussed earlier. In treating patients with spatial neglect after cingulate cortical lesions, the association between this neuroanatomic system and spatial Aiming deficits produces a second concern for the clinician. That concern is that “evidence-based” approaches recommended for spatial neglect patients did not take into account the differences in response to treatment that might be observed across patients with different symptom patterns (Barrett et al., 2012; Champod et al., 2019). This is an overall problem with using randomized controlled trials, meta-analyses, and systematic reviews to determine the care of individual patients: a treatment with best outcomes for groups of average patients is often a poor choice for a patient who is atypical in one or more major characteristics (Berguer, 2004).
Rationale for treatment selection: Translational stratification A recent Cochrane review (Bowen et al., 2013) concluded that there is not yet sufficient evidence for selecting one spatial neglect treatment over another, based on currently available randomized controlled trials and other systematic treatment studies. However, this review does not consider translational stratification to be relevant to treatment effectiveness; like most Cochrane reviews, it examines aspects of research rigor
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT (e.g., blinding, randomization) as the possible primary source of differences in treatment outcomes between randomized controlled trials of treatments. We disagree with this philosophy (Goedert et al., 2013). Based on our extensive experience with evaluating and treating patients with spatial neglect after stroke, we observed subject heterogeneity to be the result of true differences in subject characteristics and of problems with reliability or validity of outcome evaluation. In this case, whether spatial Where or spatial Aiming brain systems are more responsible for spatial neglect-related disability—a fundamental difference in the neurologic characteristics of the deficit being treated—must be specifically measured and stratified; otherwise, variance in outcomes drastically reduces the ability to detect a treatment effect. We observed this in three systematic studies (Chen et al., 2014; Goedert et al., 2014, 2018), in which we observed that (1) patients with spatial Aiming neglect responded more readily to prism adaptation therapy (PAT), (2) patients with frontal lobe lesions also responded more readily to PAT, and (3) measures sensitive to functional movements in daily life performance, which we anticipated would be the functional correlate of spatial Aiming bias, were best to stratify stroke patients. As we described in Barrett et al. (2012) and Riestra and Barrett (2013), a failure to evaluate patients with spatial Aiming neglect separately from other spatial neglect patients in randomized controlled trials could well explain the lack of effectiveness observed in several studies of spatial neglect treatment and, in fact, may also explain Bowen et al.’s (2013) decision not to recommend specific treatment approaches in the Cochrane review. Because spatial cognitive impairments differ depending upon the dysfunctional stage of processing, a treatment acting on spatial Where spatial cognitive systems (for example, increasing the gain or enhancing the signal-noise ratio for left-sided stimuli to address leftsided unawareness) may have absolutely no effect on spatial Aiming neglect. Delivering this treatment to a diverse group of patients with spatial neglect, many of whom have primarily spatial Aiming deficits, could result in the incorrect conclusion that it is ineffective, even if the group with primarily perceptual-attentional Where deficits respond well. The averaged group outcome in this case could be entirely determined by the kind of deficits the patients who are included in the study group have. Unfortunately, we do not yet have large-scale studies of spatial Where and spatial Aiming characteristics in spatial neglect to aid in ensuring that representative groups are included in spatial neglect treatment trials. However, in the case of spatial neglect associated with cingulate cortical lesions, based on our retrospective analysis of spatial neglect patient data, we feel motor-exploratory deficits are likely to account for significant disability.
141
Thus even though professional guidelines provide several options for spatial neglect treatment based on benefit in randomized controlled trials (Table 9.4, left side), we suggest that the clinician strongly consider (1) prism adaptation treatment and (2) limb activation therapy for patients with spatial neglect and lesions in the ACC and MCC. Although only a small amount of preliminary information is available supporting pharmacologic intervention, we also recommend that clinicians consider this approach in selected cases and consider applying it with appropriate caution, using either dopaminergic or stimulant medications.
PRISM ADAPTATION THERAPY Prism adaptation has been used for many years to study motor learning and implicit/procedural processing (Redding and Wallace, 2006). A rehabilitation approach using prism adaptation, PAT, is a treatment technique having patients with spatial neglect after right brain stroke wear yoked optical wedge prisms that shift what they see 11.4 degrees rightward (Frassinetti et al., 2002). An occluder blocks the first part of their arm and hand movement from view (Goedert et al., 2012, 2018) and patients complete 20-min sessions of visually guided movement practice. In the protocol used in our program (Fig. 9.4; Barrett and Houston, 2019; Barrett et al., 2019), patients mark lines and circles in left, central, and right space. Prisms are worn only for 20-min sessions; the entire protocol of treatment is 10 sessions over 14 days. Both the American Heart Association (AHA) and the American Occupational Therapy Association list this treatment in their practice guidelines for spatial neglect (Table 9.4); the AHA provides recommendation category Class IIa, Level A (reasonable to recommend treatment, supported by multiple randomized clinical trials) for this guideline for cognitive rehabilitation treatment of spatial neglect, although other spatial neglect treatments were included in AHA recommendations as a generic list. The impact of PAT on daily life disability was reviewed by Champod et al. (2018), who listed more than 20 controlled studies reporting improvement in functional disability including improved reading, ambulation, and self-care via either direct assessment or selfreport with standardized instruments reliable and validated for use in stroke. The prism adaptation process consists of three steps: (1) pointing to targets without glasses in the direction of visual targets to obtain reference values (pretest); (2) 50–60 aiming movements in the direction of visual targets with prisms that deviate the environment about 10 degrees to the right (prismatic exposure); at this stage, the movements are deviated toward the right and then the subject progressively corrects errors; and (3) aiming
142
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
Table 9.4 Conventional spatial neglect treatment (left two columns), based on practice guidelines, is not specific to impaired spatial cognitive mechanisms
Conventional spatial neglect treatment
Targeted approach to improve spatial Aiming and arousal/ activation
Reference
Approach
Reference
Approach
Wolf and Nilsen (2015) Winstein et al. (2016) Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party (2016)
Prism adaptation training (Frassinetti et al., 2002; Chen et al., 2014)
Prism adaptation training (Fortis et al., 2011a,b; Goedert et al., 2014)
Veterans Administration/ Dept. of Defense (2010) Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party (2016)
Compensatory self-mediated cuing strategies (Niemeier et al., 2001; Walker et al., 2012)
Limb activation (Robertson and North, 1993; Làdavas et al. 1997; Eskes and Butler, 2006)
Veterans Administration/ Dept. of Defense (2010) Wolf and Nilson (2015) Winstein et al. (2016) Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party (2016)
Visual scanning training (Katz et al., 2005; Polanowska et al., 2009)
Dopaminergic and noradrenergic medications (Gorelick et al., 2011; Riestra and Barrett, 2013)
Reduce spatial motor Aiming bias by improving directional hypokinesia Reduce spatial motor Aiming bias by improving hypokinesia of the contralesional limb Improve arousal, generative behavior, persistence
In treating patients with cingulate cortical lesions, this may reduce functional benefit. Both practice guidelines and a targeted approach to spatial neglect treatment (right two columns) support prism adaptation training; however, other approaches, not mentioned in practice guidelines (right column), target cingulate-related deficits. See Champod, Eskes, and Barrett (2019) and Riestra and Barrett (2013) for further reading.
Fig. 9.4. Prism adaptation treatment (PAT) procedure, using the Kessler Foundation Prism Adaptation Treatment (KF-PAT®) Portable Kit, patent pending. Figure credit: Andrew Abdou.
toward visual targets without the prisms to measure the aftereffects (Frassinetti et al., 2002). In other words, patients with left neglect after right brain stroke initially wear prisms that displace the visual scene rightward, causing them to misreach to the right of a target. After repeated pointing, and once they have adapted such that they reach accurately, the prisms are removed, leaving patients with a leftward pointing error (Li and Malhotra, 2015). Standard programs intended to improve spatial neglect have one or two daily prism adaptation sessions over a period of 1–2 weeks (Frassinetti et al., 2002). Because studies suggest that patients with spatial Aiming neglect and frontal lobe lesions may experience the most
benefit from treatment with PAT, it is appropriate to reassess patients to confirm treatment benefit. Prism adaptation treatment has the advantage of requiring little training to administer, and patients do not need to understand and accept behavioral modification because the approach trains actions implicitly rather than relying on strategy or knowledge building. Limb activation Limb activation therapy (Robertson and North, 1993; Robertson et al., 2002) and the massed practice to the paretic limb of constraint-induced movement therapy
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT
143
(CIMT) (Mark and Taub, 2002) could all be considered treatments to increase a propensity to move with strokeaffected limbs, and thus potential treatments for limb akinesia. For limb activation therapy, patients are encouraged to make a small movement with the contralesional arm or body; this can be reinforced using a device (Robertson et al., 2002; Fong et al., 2013); however, a device is not required—verbal urging to use the “bad” arm is also effective (Eskes and Butler, 2006). Movements are encouraged and reinforced for repeated sessions: Robertson et al. (2002) used 45-min sessions weekly for 12 weeks. In practice, we have found these treatments difficult to implement for patients with spatial neglect. In using limb activation, active, effective movements on the contralesional side require patient agency and active initiation; thus considerable time can be spent in discussing with patients, who have no awareness of the deficit, why they are being asked to perform these actions, reducing time available for the therapy itself. Passive movement of the limb is not helpful (Robertson and North, 1993). Thus the treatment is very time-intensive for therapists and caregivers, who also require special training to implement it consistently. The use of limb activation training in spatial neglect is not necessarily limited to a formal limb activation protocol. Punt et al. (2011) investigated the effect of spatial cueing and limb activation on wheelchair use. They found improved midline navigation when the joystick was mounted on the contralesional side. This suggests that limb activation–based therapeutic benefits can be obtained during routine wheelchair training, based on optimal configuration of the wheelchair joystick equipment.
time, on-medication assessment specific to spatial Aiming deficits has only been performed in 5 patients treated with dopamine agonists (Geminiani et al., 1998; Barrett et al., 1999); this needs to be completed in groups of patients and response on functional parameters should be compared with spatial Aiming deficits, patient lesion location, and time since stroke as well as spatial neglect and stroke severity. The patient reported in Barrett et al. (1999) was not tested using a functional performance assessment on medication; however, his performance on line bisection, which in past research (Friedman, 1990) was associated with functional disability, was worse on medication. Grujic et al. (1998) noted that three patients exhibited increased ipsilesional exploratory eye movements on a laboratory task; these patients were not functionally characterized. Thus we do not know if their spatial bias met criteria for spatial neglect at baseline, if the finding was a laboratory phenomenon only, or if different changes in functional disability occurred on medication that may have been dissociated from performance on the laboratory task. The paradoxical worsening on laboratory tasks noted by Barrett et al. (1999) and Grujic et al. (1998), although only reported in a preliminary fashion, suggests that clinicians using dopaminergic medication should periodically reassess patients as they are titrating dopaminergic medications and certainly reassess patients on the goal dose to confirm beneficial effects. We strongly recommend using a functional performance assessment such as the Catherine Bergego Scale to obtain results maximally relevant to daily life disability.
DOPAMINERGIC PHARMACOTHERAPY
Guanfacine, an alpha-2 adrenergic agonist used to treat hypertension and attention deficit disorder and available in the United States in generic form only, may specifically improve motor-exploratory deficits, which we previously related to functional disability (Goedert et al., 2012) and which may be related to spatial motor Aiming systems. The medication improved visual search in spatial neglect patients; however, it was effective in patients without frontal damage (Li and Malhotra, 2015). Malhotra et al. (2006) also showed lesion-specific efficacy of guanfacine: it improved leftward exploration in two patients with temporoparietal lesions, but not in another patient with a frontal lesion. These authors proposed that increasing dorsolateral prefrontal cortex– mediated vigilance may improve spatial neglect symptoms in patients with motor-exploratory deficits and posterior cortical injuries (Malhotra et al., 2006). More work needs to be done investigating whether other subcortical lesions, e.g., white matter injury, interfere with or interact with guanfacine effect.
Dopaminergic pharmacotherapy may improve spatial neglect in humans, and neglect symptoms sensitive to dopaminergic supplementation may be identified with intentional premotor exploratory function (Fleet et al., 1987; Coslett et al., 1990; Tegner and Levander, 1991; Barrett et al., 1999; Mukand et al., 2001; Mapstone et al., 2003; Heilman, 2004). Rotigotine, a dopamine agonist with relatively high affinity for D1 receptors (Gorgoraptis et al., 2012), improved visual search and selective attention, without improving spatial representational functions such as working memory. However, these authors did not examine tasks in which spatial Aiming bias could be dissociated from spatial Where perceptual or representational errors. In other studies, dopaminergic medication improved only certain symptoms of spatial neglect; it might also worsen neglect in some patients (Geminiani et al. 1998; Grujic et al., 1998; Barrett et al., 1999). At this
STIMULATION OF AROUSAL
144
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
There is very preliminary information available for a few other stimulants; however, group studies in patients with spatial neglect and patients classified by spatial Aiming versus spatial Where bias are not yet available. In a study of healthy volunteers, modafinil, but not methylphenidate, decreased the rightward bias in a perceptual task apparently mediated by an increase in right hemisphere–mediated alertness (Dodds et al., 2009). Methylphenidate, which affects both norepinephrine and dopamine, had favorable results in a case report, but its effects were inferior and shorter acting than those of bromocriptine (Hurford et al., 1998). There has been reported improvement in magnitude estimation using modafinil, a psychostimulant with probable dopaminergic effects (Volkow et al., 2009) in 1 patient with spatial neglect symptoms (Woods et al., 2006). If clinicians choose to try these agents (for example, because of lack of availability of prism adaptation or intolerance of dopaminergic medication), they should exercise great caution, as when using any nonstandard approach for which only case study and case series data are available. Reassessing spatial function and functional disability on the medication is a minimum standard for good clinical practice in these cases.
CONCLUSIONS In this chapter, we described the spatial neglect syndrome, its impact, and a translational approach to analysis and treatment of the problem based on analyzing the stage of cognitive processing that is dysfunctional in different subgroups of patients. This is potentially useful because stratification by impaired mechanism is likely to reveal different responses to restorative treatments based on the targeted action of the treatment (Goedert et al., 2014). We then discussed cingulate regions and their associated clinical characteristics relevant to spatial neglect. We retrospectively examined our own dataset of patients with brain lesion mapping and spatial neglect assessment, and our data suggests an association between spatial-motor, exploratory deficits and cingulate lesions in patients with severe spatial neglect. Finally, we described evidence for a therapeutic approach for spatial neglect generally and how a clinician may wish to modify this approach to prioritize therapies that optimally treat the symptoms of neglect likely to be associated with cingulate lesions. We discuss targeted neglect treatments that may be optimal when patients have lesions affecting a given anatomic subregion of the cingulate cortex. A relationship is strongly suspected between lesions of the ACC and spatial-motor deficits, including spatial Aiming neglect. There is also a relationship between spatial motor Aiming and the dopaminergic system (Fleet et al., 1987; Coslett et al., 1990; Tegner and Levander, 1991; Barrett et al., 1999; Mukand et al., 2001;
Mapstone et al., 2003; Heilman, 2004). Moreover, the ACC contributes to arousal and persistence. Thus, interventions, including pharmacologic therapy, which stimulate arousal and sustained attention, may play an important role in treating spatial neglect with cingulate cortical lesions. A movement toward understanding brain networks— the way that multiple brain regions interact functionally— may have led to reduced interest in semiotic, symptom- and mechanism-based research on cognitive disorders and their treatments. We hope that with the rise in precision medicine—which in its essence is patient centered—more information will be generated about specific spatial cognitive processing associated with cingulate systems and about specific limitations in activity and participation. Then, biologic scientists and clinicians can work together to understand and remediate specific functional deficits that become the focus of treatment.
REFERENCES Adair JC, Barrett AM (2008). Spatial neglect: clinical and neuroscience review: a wealth of information on the poverty of spatial attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1142: 21–43. https:// doi.org/10.1196/annals.1444.008. Albert ML (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology 23 (6): 658–664. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.23.6.658. Apicella P, Legallet E, Nieoullon A et al. (1991). Neglect of contralateral visual stimuli in monkeys with unilateral striatal dopamine depletion. Behav Brain Res 46 (2): 187–195. Azouvi P, Olivier S, de Montety G et al. (2003). Behavioral assessment of unilateral neglect: study of the psychometric properties of the Catherine Bergego scale. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 84 (1): 51–57. https://doi.org/10.1053/ apmr.2003.50062. Azouvi P, Bartolomeo P, Beis JM et al. (2006). A battery of tests for the quantitative assessment of unilateral neglect. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24 (4-6): 273–285. Barrett AM (2014). Perceptual-attentional “where” and motorintentional “aiming” spatial systems. In: A Chatterjee, HB Coslett (Eds.), The roots of cognitive neuroscience: behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, pp. 171–186. Barrett AM, Burkholder S (2006). Monocular patching in subjects with right-hemisphere stroke affects perceptualattentional bias. J Rehabil Res Dev 43 (3): 337–346. Barrett AM, Houston KE (2019). Update on the clinical approach to spatial neglect. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 19 (5): 25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-019-0940-0. Barrett AM, Muzaffar T (2014). Spatial cognitive rehabilitation and motor recovery after stroke. Curr Opin Neurol 27 (6): 653–658. https://doi.org/10.1097/wco. 0000000000000148. Barrett AM, Crucian GP, Schwartz RL et al. (1999). Adverse effect of dopamine agonist therapy in a patient with motorintentional neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 80 (5): 600–603.
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT Barrett AM, Schwartz RL, Crucian GP et al. (2000). Attentional grasp in far extrapersonal space after thalamic infarction. Neuropsychologia 38 (6): 778–784. Barrett AM, Goedert KM, Basso JC (2012). Prism adaptation for spatial neglect after stroke: translational practice gaps. Nat Rev Neurol 8 (10): 567–577. https://doi.org/10.1038/ nrneurol.2012.170. Barrett AM, Diaz-Segarra N, Gillen R et al. (2019). A care pathway for post-stroke spatial neglect: initial analysis of a practice-based rehabilitation research network. Neurology 92 (15): P5.7-006. Berguer R (2004). The evidence thing. Ann Vasc Surg 18 (3): 265–270. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10016-004-0044-3. Beversdorf DQ, Hughes JD, Heilman KM (2008). Functional MRI of the primary somatosensory cortex in extinction to simultaneous bilateral tactile stimuli due to right temporal lobe stroke. Neurocase 14 (5): 419–424. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/13554790802422112. Bisiach E, Luzzatti C (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex 14 (1): 129–133. Bisiach E, Geminiani G, Berti A et al. (1990). Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology 40 (8): 1278–1281. Bissonette GB, Roesch MR (2016). Neurophysiology of reward-guided behavior: correlates related to predictions, value, motivation, errors, attention, and action. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 27: 199–230. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 7854_2015_382. Bonato M, Saj A, Vuilleumier P (2016). Hemispatial neglect shows that “before” is “left”. Neural Plast 2016: 2716036. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/2716036. Bowen A, Hazelton C, Pollock A et al. (2013). Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 7: CD003586. https://doi.org/10. 1002/14651858.CD003586.pub3. Buklina SB (2002). The unilateral spatial neglect phenomenon in patients with arteriovenous malformations of deep brain structures. Neurosci Behav Physiol 32 (6): 555–560. Carli M, Jones GH, Robbins TW (1989). Effects of unilateral dorsal and ventral striatal dopamine depletion on visual neglect in the rat: a neural and behavioural analysis. Neuroscience 29 (2): 309–327. Champod AS, Frank RC, Taylor K et al. (2018). The effects of prism adaptation on daily life activities in patients with visuospatial neglect: a systematic review. Neuropsychol Rehabil 28: 491–514. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011. 2016.1182032. Champod AS, Eskes GA, Barrett AM (2019). Neuropsychological rehabilitation. In: RM Lazar, JR Festa (Eds.), Neurovascular neuropsychology. Springer, New York (in press). Chaudhari A, Shah P, Goedert K et al. (2013). Classifying patients with spatial neglect by ‘aiming’ versus ‘where’ spatial bias may explain differential response to bromocriptine treatment. In: Presented at the 2013 American Academy of Neurology Meeting, San Diego, CA, Neurology, 80(Meeting Abstracts 1), P03.261.
145
Chen P, Hreha K, Fortis P et al. (2012). Functional assessment of spatial neglect: a review of the Catherine Bergego Scale and an introduction of the Kessler Foundation Neglect Assessment Process. Top Stroke Rehabil 19 (5): 423–435. https://doi.org/10.1310/tsr1905-423. Chen P, Goedert KM, Shah P et al. (2014). Integrity of medial temporal structures may predict better improvement of spatial neglect with prism adaptation treatment. Brain Imaging Behav 8 (3): 346–358. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s11682-012-9200-5. Chen P, Chen CC, Hreha K et al. (2015a). Kessler Foundation Neglect Assessment Process uniquely measures spatial neglect during activities of daily living. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 96 (5): 869–876.e861. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.10.023. Chen P, Hreha K, Kong Y et al. (2015b). Impact of spatial neglect in stroke rehabilitation: evidence from the setting of an inpatient rehabilitation facility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 96 (8): 1458–1466. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.apmr.2015.03.019. Chen P, Hreha K, Kong Y et al. (2015c). Impact of spatial neglect on stroke rehabilitation: evidence from the setting of an inpatient rehabilitation facility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 96 (8): 1458–1466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2015.03.019. Chen P, Fyffe DC, Hreha K (2017). Informal caregivers’ burden and stress in caring for stroke survivors with spatial neglect: an exploratory mixed-method study. Top Stroke Rehabil 24 (1): 24–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357. 2016.1186373. Clark DL, Mendez MF, Boutros NN (2010). Limbic system: cingulate cortex. In: DL Clark, MF Mendez, NN Boutros (Eds.), The brain and behavior: An introduction to behavioral neuroanatomy, third edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 197–215. Committeri G, Pitzalis S, Galati G et al. (2007). Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. Brain 130 (Pt. 2): 431–441. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl265. Cona G, Scarpazza C (2019). Where is the "where" in the brain? A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on spatial cognition. Hum Brain Mapp 40: 1867–1886. https://doi. org/10.1002/hbm.24496. Coslett HB (1997). Neglect in vision and visual imagery: a double dissociation. Brain 120 (Pt. 7): 1163–1171. Retrieved from https://doi.org/. Coslett HB, Bowers D, Fitzpatrick E et al. (1990). Directional hypokinesia and hemispatial inattention in neglect. Brain 113 (Pt. 2): 475–486. Denes G, Semenza C, Stoppa E et al. (1982). Unilateral spatial neglect and recovery from hemiplegia: a follow-up study. Brain 105 (Pt. 3): 543–552. Deuel RK, Collins RC (1983). Recovery from unilateral neglect. Exp Neurol 81 (3): 733–748. Dodds C, Muller U, Manly T (2009). Effects of psychostimulants on alertness and spatial bias in healthy participants. J Cogn Neurosci 21 (3): 529–537. https://doi.org/ 10.1162/jocn.2009.21046. Edwards DF, Hahn MG, Baum CM et al. (2006). Screening patients with stroke for rehabilitation needs: validation of
146
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
the post-stroke rehabilitation guidelines. Neurorehabil Neural Rep 20 (1): 42–48. Eskes GA, Butler B (2006). Using limb movements to improve spatial neglect: the role of functional electrical stimulation. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24 (4-6): 385–398. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/. Fleet WS, Valenstein E, Watson RT et al. (1987). Dopamine agonist therapy for neglect in humans. Neurology 37 (11): 1765–1770. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12 (3): 189–198. Fong KN, Yang NY, Chan MK et al. (2013). Combined effects of sensory cueing and limb activation on unilateral neglect in subacute left hemiplegic stroke patients: a randomized controlled pilot study. Clin Rehabil 27 (7): 628–637. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215512471959. Fortis P, Chen P, Goedert KM et al. (2011a). Effects of prism adaptation on motor-intentional spatial bias in neglect. Neuroreport 22 (14): 700–705. https://doi.org/10.1097/ WNR.0b013e32834a3e20 (b). Fortis P, Goedert KM, Barrett AM (2011b). Prism adaptation differently affects motor-intentional and perceptual-attentional biases in healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 49 (9): 2718–2727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.020 (a). Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F et al. (2002). Longlasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain 125 (Pt. 3): 608–623. https://doi.org/ 10.1093/brain/awf056. Friedman PJ (1990). Spatial neglect in acute stroke: the line bisection test. Scand J Rehabil Med 22: 101–106. Fullerton KJ, McSherry D, Stout RW (1986). Albert’s test: a neglected test of perceptual neglect. Lancet 1 (8478): 430–432. Funayama M, Muramatsu T, Kato M (2008). Differential hand-neglect after a callosal lesion. Cogn Behav Neurol 21 (4): 246–248. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0b013e31 8185e681. Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R (1972). Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 35 (4): 545–550. Gallagher CL, Bell B, Palotti M et al. (2015). Anterior cingulate dopamine turnover and behavior change in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav 9 (4): 821–827. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11682-014-9338-4. Geminiani G, Bottini G, Sterzi R (1998). Dopaminergic stimulation in unilateral neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65 (3): 344–347. Goedert KM, Chen P, Botticello A et al. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of neglect assessment reveals motor-exploratory predictor of functional disability in acute-stage spatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 93 (1): 137–142. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2011.06.036. Goedert KM, Boston RC, Barrett AM (2013). Advancing the science of spatial neglect rehabilitation: an improved statistical approach with mixed linear modeling. Front Hum Neurosci: 7: 211. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.300211.
Goedert KM, Chen P, Boston RC et al. (2014). Presence of motor-intentional aiming deficit predicts functional improvement of spatial neglect with prism adaptation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 28 (5): 483–493. https://doi. org/10.1177/1545968313516872. Goedert KM, Chen P, Foundas AL et al. (2018). Frontal lesions predict response to prism adaptation treatment in spatial neglect: a randomised controlled study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 20: 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2018. 1448287. Gorelick PB, Scuteri A, Black SE et al. (2011). Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 42 (9): 2672–2713. https://doi.org/10.1161/STR. 0b013e3182299496. Gorgoraptis N, Mah YH, Machner B et al. (2012). The effects of the dopamine agonist rotigotine on hemispatial neglect following stroke. Brain 135 (Pt. 8): 2478–2491. https:// doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws154. Grabowska A, Marchewka A, Seniow J et al. (2011). Emotionally negative stimuli can overcome attentional deficits in patients with visuo-spatial hemineglect. Neuropsychologia 49 (12): 3327–3337. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.006. Grujic Z, Mapstone M, Gitelman DR et al. (1998). Dopamine agonists reorient visual exploration away from the neglected hemispace. Neurology 51 (5): 1395–1398. Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Wade DT (1992). Left on the right: allochiria in a case of left visuo-spatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55 (8): 717. https://doi.org/ 10.1136/jnnp.55.8.71. Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC et al. (2003). Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci 7 (3): 125–133. Hanna-Pladdy B, Heilman KM, Foundas AL (2003). Ecological implications of ideomotor apraxia: evidence from physical activities of daily living. Neurology 60 (3): 487–490. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.60.3.487. Heilman KM (2004). Intentional neglect. Front Biosci 9694–705. Heilman KM, Adams DJ (2003). Callosal neglect. Arch Neurol 60 (2): 276–279. Heilman KM, Valenstein E (1972). Frontal lobe neglect in man. Neurology 22660–664. Heilman KM, Valenstein E (1979). Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect. Ann Neurol 5 (2): 166–170. https:// doi.org/10.1002/ana.410050210. Heilman KM, Valenstein E (2012). Clinical neuropsychology, fifth edn. Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett HB et al. (1985). Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology 35 (6): 855e859. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Watson RT et al. (1990). Frontal hypermetabolism and thalamic hypometabolism in a patient with abnormal orienting and retrosplenial amnesia. Neuropsychologia 28 (2): 161–169.
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Watson RT (2000). Neglect and related disorders. Semin Neurol 20 (4): 463–470. https:// doi.org/10.1055/s-2000-13179. Hillis AE, Chang S, Heidler-Gary J et al. (2006). Neural correlates of modality-specific spatial extinction. J Cogn Neurosci 18 (11): 1889–1898. https://doi.org/10.1162/ jocn.2006.18.11.1889. Hoyman L, Weese GD, Frommer GP (1979). Tactile discrimination performance deficits following neglect-producing unilateral lateral hypothalamic lesions in the rat. Physiol Behav 22 (1): 139–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/00319384(79)90415-3. Hreha K, Eller M, Barrett AM (2010). Treating post-stroke spatial neglect establishing a clinical research-clinical care partnership program. Adv Occup Ther Pract 26 (7): 16. Huang Y, Hullfish J, De Ridder D et al. (2019). Meta-analysis of functional subdivisions within human posteromedial cortex. Brain Struct Funct 224 (1): 435–452. https://doi. org/10.1007/s00429-018-1781-3. Hurford P, Stringer AY, Jann B (1998). Neuropharmacologic treatment of hemineglect: a case report comparing bromocriptine and methylphenidate. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 79 (3): 346–349. Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party (2016). Stroke guidelines, Retrieved from, https://www.rcplondon.ac.uk/ guidelines-policy/stroke-guidelines. Jessup RK, Busemeyer JR, Brown JW (2010). Error effects in anterior cingulate cortex reverse when error likelihood is high. J Neurosci 30 (9): 3467–3472. https://doi.org/ 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4130-09.2010. Jones EG (1986). Neurotransmitters in the cerebral cortex. J Neurosurg 65 (2): 135–153. https://doi.org/10.3171/ jns.1986.65.2.0135. Kalra L, Perez I, Gupta S et al. (1997). The influence of visual neglect on stroke rehabilitation. Stroke 28 (7): 1386–1391. Katz N, Ring H, Naveh Y et al. (2005). Interactive virtual environment training for safe street crossing of right hemisphere stroke patients with unilateral spatial neglect. Disabil Rehabil 27 (20): 1235–1244. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/09638280500076079. Khurshid S, Longin H, Crucian GP et al. (2009). Monocular patching affects inattention but not perseveration in spatial neglect. Neurocase 15 (4): 311e317. Ko JH, Ptito A, Monchi O et al. (2009). Increased dopamine release in the right anterior cingulate cortex during the performance of a sorting task: a [11C]FLB 457 PET study. Neuroimage 46 (2): 516–521. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.neuroimage.2009.02.031. Kwon SE, Heilman KM (1991). Ipsilateral neglect in a patient following a unilateral frontal lesion. Neurology 41 (12): 2001–2004. LaBerge D, Buchsbaum MS (1990). Positron emission tomographic measurements of pulvinar activity during an attention task. J Neurosci 10 (2): 613–619. Ladavas E, Berti A, Ruozzi E et al. (1997). Neglect as a deficit determined by an imbalance between multiple spatial representations. Exp Brain Res 116 (3): 493–500.
147
Lecce F, Rotondaro F, Bonni S et al. (2015). Cingulate neglect in humans: disruption of contralesional reward learning in right brain damage. Cortex 62: 73–88. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.008. Li K, Malhotra PA (2015). Spatial neglect. Pract Neurol 15: 333–339. Li Y, Chen Z, Su X et al. (2016). Functional lateralization in cingulate cortex predicts motor recovery after basal ganglia stroke. Neurosci Lett 613: 6–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.neulet.2015.12.051. Luaute J, Halligan P, Rode G et al. (2006). Prism adaptation first among equals in alleviating left neglect: a review. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24 (4-6): 409–418. Malhotra PA, Parton AD, Greenwood R et al. (2006). Noradrenergic modulation of space exploration in visual neglect. Ann Neurol 59 (1): 186–190. https://doi.org/ 10.1002/ana.20701. Mapstone M, Weintraub S, Nowinski C et al. (2003). Cerebral hemispheric specialization for spatial attention: spatial distribution of search-related eye fixations in the absence of neglect. Neuropsychologia 41 (10): 1396–1409. Mark VM, Taub E (2002). Constraint-induced movement therapy for chronic stroke hemiparesis and other disabilities. Restor Neurol Neurosci 22: 317–336. Marzi CA, Girelli M, Natale E et al. (2001). What exactly is extinguished in unilateral visual extinction? Neurophysiological evidence. Neuropsychologia 39 (12): 1354–1366. Mayer NH, Pelensky J, Whyte J et al. (2003). Characterization and correlates of medical and rehabilitation charges for traumatic brain injury during acute rehabilitation hospitalization. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 84 (2): 242–248. https:// doi.org/10.1053/apmr.2003.50101. McGlone J, Losier BJ, Black SE (1997). Are there sex differences in hemispatial visual neglect after unilateral stroke? Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 10 (2): 125–134. Meador KJ, Watson RT, Bowers D et al. (1986). Hypometria with hemispatial and limb motor neglect. Brain 109 (Pt. 2): 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/109.2.293. Mennemeier M, Wertman E, Heilman KM (1992). Neglect of near peripersonal space. Evidence for multidirectional attentional systems in humans. Brain 115 (Pt. 1): 37–50. Mesulam MM (1999). Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354 (1387): 1325–1346. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1999.0482. Misra G, Coombes SA (2015). Neuroimaging evidence of motor control and pain processing in the human midcingulate cortex. Cereb Cortex 25 (7): 1906–1919. https://doi. org/10.1093/cercor/bhu001. Mort DJ, Perry RJ, Mannan SK et al. (2003). Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man. Neuroimage 18 (2): 231–246. Mukand JA, Guilmette TJ, Allen DG et al. (2001). Dopaminergic therapy with carbidopa L-dopa for left
148
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
neglect after stroke: a case series. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 82 (9): 1279–1282. https://doi.org/10.1053/apmr.2001.25149. Na DL, Adair JC, Williamson DJ et al. (1998). Dissociation of sensory-attentional from motor-intentional neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 64 (3): 331–338. Niemeier JP, Cifu DX, Kishore R (2001). The lighthouse strategy: improving the functional status of patients with unilateral neglect after stroke and brain injury using a visual imagery intervention. Top Stroke Rehabil 8 (2): 10–18. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/10.1310/7ukk-hj0fgdwf-hhm8. Nys GM, van Zandvoort MJ, van der Worp HB et al. (2006). Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of perseverative responses in subacute stroke. Brain 129 (Pt. 8): 2148–2157. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/ awl199. Olson CR, Musil SY, Goldberg ME (1996). Single neurons in posterior cingulate cortex of behaving macaque: eye movement signals. J Neurophysiol 76 (5): 3285–3300. https:// doi.org/10.1152/jn.1996.76.5.3285. Pereira M, Oliveira L, Erthal F et al. (2010). Emotion affects action: midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10 (1): 94–106. https://doi.org/10.3758/ cabn.10.1.94. Polanowska K, Senio´w J, Paprot E et al. (2009). Left-hand somatosensory stimulation combined with visual scanning training in rehabilitation for post-stroke hemineglect: a randomised, double-blind study. Neuropsychol Rehabil 19 (3): 364–382. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/ 10.1080/09602010802268856. Punt TD, Kitadono K, Hulleman J et al. (2011). Modulating wheelchair navigation in patients with spatial neglect. Neuropsychol Rehabil 21 (3): 367–382. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/09602011.2011.559132. Redding GM, Wallace B (2006). Prism adaptation and unilateral neglect: review and analysis. Neuropsychologia 44: 1–20. Riestra AR, Barrett A (2013). Rehabilitation of spatial neglect. Handb Clin Neurol 110: 347–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/ b978-0-444-52901-5.00029-0. Riestra AR, Crucian GP, Burks DW et al. (2001). Extinction, working memory, and line bisection in spatial neglect. Neurology 57 (1): 147–149. Riestra AR, Womack KB, Crucian GP et al. (2002). Is the middle between both halves?: midpoint location and segment size estimation in neglect. Neurology 59 (10): 1580–1584. Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF et al. (2004). Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort. Neurology 63 (3): 468–474. Robertson IH (2001). Do we need the "lateral" in unilateral neglect? Spatially nonselective attention deficits in unilateral neglect and their implications for rehabilitation. Neuroimage 14 (1 Pt. 2): S85–S90. https://doi.org/ 10.1006/nimg.2001.0838. Robertson IH, North N (1993). Active and passive activation of left limbs: influence on visual and sensory neglect. Neuropsychologia 31 (3): 293–300.
Robertson IH, McMillan TM, MacLeod E et al. (2002). Rehabilitation by limb activation training reduces leftsided motor impairment in unilateral neglect patients: a single-blind randomised control trial. Neuropsychol Rehabil 12 (5): 439–454. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 09602010244000228. Rorden C, Karnath HO, Bonilha L (2007). Improving lesionsymptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci 19 (7): 1081–1088. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081. Sapir A, Kaplan JB, He BJ et al. (2007). Anatomical correlates of directional hypokinesia in patients with hemispatial neglect. J Neurosci 27 (15): 4045–4051. https://doi.org/ 10.1523/jneurosci.0041-07.2007. Sasaki N, Hara T, Yamada N et al. (2017). the efficacy of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for improving apathy in chronic stroke patients. Eur Neurol 78 (1-2): 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1159/000477440. Schwarting RK, Huston JP (1996). The unilateral 6-hydroxydopamine lesion model in behavioral brain research. Analysis of functional deficits, recovery and treatments. Prog Neurobiol 50 (2-3): 275–331. Shallice T, Cooper R (2011). The organisation of mind, Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Shinsha N, Ishigami S (1999). Rehabilitation approach to patients with unilateral spatial neglect. Top Stroke Rehabil 6 (1): 1–14. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/ 10.1310/p4pc-kwca-dvyu-h4fp. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ (1993). The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age Ageing 22 (1): 46–52. Sturm W, de Simone A, Krause BJ et al. (1999). Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a frontoparietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 37 (7): 797–805. Takahashi T (2004). Symptomatology of retrosplenial cortex damage: comparison between lesions in the right and left hemisphere. Adv Neurol Sci 48: 649–656. Takebayashi T, Marumoto K, Takahashi K et al. (2018). Differences in neural pathways are related to the short- or long-term benefits of constraint-induced movement therapy in patients with chronic stroke and hemiparesis: a pilot cohort study. Top Stroke Rehabil 25 (3): 203–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749357.2017.1399231. Tegner R, Levander M (1991). Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain 114 (Pt. 4): 1943–1951. Thiel CM, Zilles K, Fink GR (2004). Cerebral correlates of alerting, orienting and reorienting of visuospatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 21 (1): 318–328. Thimm M, Fink GR, Kust J et al. (2006). Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: a behavioural and fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44 (7): 1230–1246. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.008. Thomas JO, Barrett AM (2019). Right hemisphere stroke syndromes. In: R Wilson, P Raghavan (Eds.), Stroke rehabilitation. Elsevier, St. Louis, MO, pp. 71–89. https://doi. org/10.1016/B978-0-323-55381-0.00005-6.
THE CINGULATE CORTEX AND SPATIAL NEGLECT Triggs WJ, Gold M, Gerstle G et al. (1994). Motor neglect associated with a discrete parietal lesion. Neurology 44 (6): 1164e1166. Tsujimoto K, Mizuno K, Nishida D et al. (2018). Prism adaptation changes resting-state functional connectivity in the dorsal stream of visual attention networks in healthy adults: a fMRI study. Cortex119: 594–605. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.cortex.2018.10.018. Umemoto A, HajiHosseini A, Yates ME et al. (2017). Rewardbased contextual learning supported by anterior cingulate cortex. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 17 (3): 642–651. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0502-3. Ungerstedt U (1976). 6-Hydroxydopamine-induced degeneration of the nigrostriatal dopamine pathway: the turning syndrome. Pharmacol Ther B 2 (1): 37–40. Veterans Administration/Department of Defense Management of Stroke Rehabilitation Working Group (2010). VA/DOD Clinical practice guideline for the management of stroke rehabilitation. J Rehabil Res Dev 47: 1–43. Vogt BA (2016). Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases. J Chem Neuroanat 74: 28–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchemneu.2016.01.010. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J et al. (2009). Effects of modafinil on dopamine and dopamine transporters in the male human brain: clinical implications. JAMA 301 (11): 1148–1154. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.351. Walker MF, Sunderland A, Fletcher-Smith J et al. (2012). The DRESS trial: a feasibility randomized controlled trial of a neuropsychological approach to dressing therapy for stroke inpatients. Clin Rehab 26 (8): 675–685. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0269215511431089. Wang S, Hu SH, Shi Y et al. (2017). The roles of the anterior cingulate cortex and its dopamine receptors in self-paced cost-benefit decision making in rats. Learn Behav 45 (1): 89–99. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-016-0243-0. Ward NS, Newton JM, Swayne OB et al. (2006). Motor system activation after subcortical stroke depends on corticospinal system integrity. Brain 129 (Pt. 3): 809–819. https://doi. org/10.1093/brain/awl002. Watson RT, Heilman KM, Cauthen JC et al. (1973). Neglect after cingulectomy. Neurology 23 (9): 1003–1007. Watson RT, Miller BD, Heilman KM (1978). Nonsensory neglect. Ann Neurol 3505–508. Weiss PH, Marshall JC, Zilles K et al. (2003). Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors? Neuroimage 18 (4): 837–846. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan P (1987). Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 68 (2): 98–102. Retrieved from https://doi.org/. Winstein CJ, Stein J, Arena R et al. (2016). Guidelines for adult stroke rehabilitation and recovery: a guideline for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/ American Stroke Association. Stroke 47 (6): e98–e169. Retrieved from, https://doi.org/10.1161/str.00000000 00000098. Wolf TJ, Nilsen DM (2015). Occupational therapy practice guidelines for adults with stroke, American Occupational Therapy Association, Bethesda, Md.
149
Woods AJ, Mennemeier M, Garcia-Rill E et al. (2006). Bias in magnitude estimation following left hemisphere injury. Neuropsychologia 44 (8): 1406–1412. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.12.006.
FURTHER READING Alex KD, Pehek EA (2007). Pharmacologic mechanisms of serotonergic regulation of dopamine neurotransmission. Pharmacol Ther 113 (2): 296–320. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2006.08.004. Barnhart WJ, Makela EH, Latocha MJ (2004). SSRI-induced apathy syndrome: a clinical review. J Psychiatr Pract 10 (3): 196–199. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S (1999). Egocentric frame of reference: its role in spatial bias after right hemisphere lesions. Neuropsychologia 37 (8): 881–894. Beis JM, Andre JM, Baumgarten A et al. (1999). Eye patching in unilateral spatial neglect: efficacy of two methods. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 80 (1): 71–76. Bisiach E, Vallar G, Perani D et al. (1986). Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 24 (4): 471–482. Brownsett SL, Warren JE, Geranmayeh F et al. (2014). Cognitive control and its impact on recovery from aphasic stroke. Brain 137 (Pt. 1): 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1093/ brain/awt289. Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, Whyte J et al. (2007). Amantadine treatment of hemispatial neglect: a double-blind, placebocontrolled study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 86 (7): 527–537. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0b013e31806e3392. Dolan RJ, Fletcher P, Frith CD et al. (1995). Dopaminergic modulation of impaired cognitive activation in the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Nature 378 (6553): 180–182. https://doi.org/10.1038/378180a0. Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH et al. (2001). The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: an fMRI study of normal volunteers. Neuroimage 14 (1 Pt. 2): S59–S67. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0819. Gallagher M, Wilkinson D, Sakel M (2013). Hemispatial neglect: clinical features, assessment and treatment. Br J Neurosci Nurs 9: 273–277. Gialanella B, Monguzzi V, Santoro R et al. (2005). Functional recovery after hemiplegia in patients with neglect: the rehabilitative role of anosognosia. Stroke 36 (12): 2687–2690. https://doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000189627. 27562.c0. Gillen G, Nilsen DM, Attridge J et al. (2015). Effectiveness of interventions to improve occupational performance of people with cognitive impairments after stroke: an evidencebased review. Am J Occup Ther 69 (1): 6901180040p6901180041-6901180049. https://doi.org/10. 5014/ajot.2015.012138. Goedert KM, Zhang JY, Barrett AM (2015). Prism adaptation and spatial neglect: the need for dose-finding studies. Front Hum Neurosci 9: 243.
150
A.M. BARRETT ET AL.
Hermens F, Walker R (2012). Do you look where I look? Attention shifts and response preparation following dynamic social cues. J Eye Mov Res 5 (5): 1–11. Hillis AE, Rapp B, Benzing L et al. (1998). Dissociable coordinate frames of unilateral spatial neglect: “viewercentered” neglect. Brain Cogn 37 (3): 491–526. https:// doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1998.1010. Hullfish J, Abenes I, Kovacs S et al. (2018). Functional connectivity analysis of fMRI data collected from human subjects with chronic tinnitus and varying levels of tinnitus-related distress. Data Brief 21: 779–789. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2018.10.044. Jeannerod M, Rossetti Y (1993). Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable visual function: experimental and clinical evidence. Baillieres Clin Neurol 2 (2): 439–460. Kinsbourne M (1987). Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. Adv Psychol 45: 69–86. Lane RM (1998). SSRI-induced extrapyramidal side-effects and akathisia: implications for treatment. J Psychopharmacol 12 (2): 192–214. https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811980120 0212. Leo RJ (1996). Movement disorders associated with the serotonin selective reuptake inhibitors. J Clin Psychiatry 57 (10): 449–454. Lindenberg R, Nachtigall L, Meinzer M et al. (2013). Differential effects of dual and unihemispheric motor cortex stimulation in older adults. J Neurosci 33 (21): 9176–9183. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.0055-13.2013. Mesulam MM (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10 (4): 309–325. https:// doi.org/10.1002/ana.410100402. Osawa A, Maeshima S (2010). Family participation can improve unilateral spatial neglect in patients with acute right hemispheric stroke. Eur Neurol 63 (3): 170–175. https://doi.org/10.1159/000286517. Parton A, Malhotra P, Husain M (2004). Hemispatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75 (1): 13. Puig-Pijoan A, Giralt-Steinhauer E, Zabalza de Torres A et al. (2018). Underdiagnosis of unilateral spatial neglect in stroke unit. Acta Neurol Scand 138 (5): 441–446. https:// doi.org/10.1111/ane.12998. Ramachandran VS, Altschuler EL, Stone L et al. (1999). Can mirrors alleviate visual hemineglect? Med Hypotheses 52 (4): 303–305. https://doi.org/10.1054/mehy.1997.0651.
Rossetti Y, Rode G (2002). Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder. In: H-O KarnathD Milner, G Vallar (Eds.), The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect, Oxford University Press. Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L et al. (1998). Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature 395 (6698): 166–169. https://doi.org/ 10.1038/25988. Samuelsson H, Hjelmquist EK, Jensen C et al. (1998). Nonlateralized attentional deficits: an important component behind persisting visuospatial neglect? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20 (1): 73–88. https://doi.org/10.1076/ jcen.20.1.73.1481. Small DM, Gitelman DR, Gregory MD et al. (2003). The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. Neuroimage 18 (3): 633–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00012-5. Smania N, Bazoli F, Piva D et al. (1997). Visuomotor imagery and rehabilitation of neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 78 (4): 430–436. Starkstein SE, Robinson RG, Honig MA et al. (1989). Mood changes after right-hemisphere lesions. Br J Psychiatry 155: 79–85. Thimm M, Fink GR, Kust J et al. (2009). Recovery from hemineglect: differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training. Cortex 45 (7): 850–862. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.007. Vocat R, Staub F, Stroppini T et al. (2010). Anosognosia for hemiplegia: a clinical-anatomical prospective study. Brain 133 (Pt. 12): 3578–3597. https://doi.org/10.1093/ brain/awq297. Vogt BA, Laureys S (2005). Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Prog Brain Res 150: 205–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123 (05)50015-3. Webster JS, Jones S, Blanton P et al. (1984). Visual scanning training with stroke patients. Behav Ther 15 (2): 129–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(84)80015-5. Wongpakaran N, van Reekum R, Wongpakaran T et al. (2007). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use associates with apathy among depressed elderly: a case-control study. Ann Gen Psychiatry 6: 7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859x-6-7.