The effects of dietary copper deficiency and psychological stress on blood pressure in rats

The effects of dietary copper deficiency and psychological stress on blood pressure in rats

Physiology& Behavior,Vol. 49, pp. 309--314. PergamonPress plc, 1991. Printed in the U.S.A. 0031-9384/91 $3.00 + .00 The Effects of Dietary Copper De...

575KB Sizes 0 Downloads 52 Views

Physiology& Behavior,Vol. 49, pp. 309--314. PergamonPress plc, 1991. Printed in the U.S.A.

0031-9384/91 $3.00 + .00

The Effects of Dietary Copper Deficiency and Psychological Stress on Blood Pressure in Ratsl LESLIE M. KLEVAY 2 AND EDWARD

S. H A L A S

United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service Grand Forks Human Nutrition Research Center, P.O. Box 7166, University Station, Grand Forks, ND 58202 Department of Psychology, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, ND 58202 R e c e i v e d 15 M a r c h 1990

KLEVAY, L. M. AND E. S. HALAS. The effects of dietary copper deficiency and psychological stress on blood pressure in rats. PHYSIOL BEHAV 49(2) 309-314, 1991.--In each of two experiments, adult, male Sprague-Dawley rats were deprived of copper and were subjected to the chronic stress of close confinement. A 2 x 2 factorial design was used because both copper deficiency and stress have been implicated in the regulation of blood pressure and are implicated in a major consequence of human hypertension-ischemic heart disease. Copper deficiency was verified by a decrease in copper in several organs. Both copper deficiency and stress increased blood pressure; results were independent. Sodium in heart was increased by deficiency in both experiments, but was increased in brain in only the second experiment. The combination of stress and deficiency produced an increase in mortality in one of two experiments. A decrease in cholesterol in plasma due to stress is consistent with earlier data from rats but is in contrast to data from humans. Both stress and copper deficiency produce potentially adverse changes in cardiovascular physiology and the chemistry of brain, heart and other organs. These results may be germane to humans because stress is frequent and some diets are low in copper. Cholesterol

Copper deficiency

Hypertension

Ischemic heart disease

A chronic increase in blood pressure is one of the prime predictors of risk of ischemic heart disease, the leading cause of death in the U.S. (4,19). There are numerous hormonal, metabolic, dietary, and neural factors that contribute to the regulation of blood pressure. The factors that stand out in the production of hypertensive illness in humans are more limited, however. Whelton and Patterson mention increased ingestion of salt, excessive caloric intake and emotional stress (52). Kaplan increases this triad by including a group of cooperating hormones and uses the word stress without an accompanying adjective (21). Diets in the United States seem to be low in copper. The National Research Council of the National Academy of Sciences (3) estimated the safe and adequate daily intake of copper to be 2-3 mg. A recent estimate of dally copper intakes based on ten surveys in which dietary copper was measured by chemical analysis revealed that only 14% of daily intakes exceed 2 mg and 35% are below 1 mg (31). The most important consequence of these diets is likely to be ischemic heart disease. First mentioned more than a decade ago (23,24), the association between copper metabolism and this ill-

Sodium

Stress

ness has grown (26, 28, 31). Nearly 50 anatomical, chemical and physiological characteristics of people with ischemic heart disease can be attributed to insufficient copper. The possible contribution of copper deficiency to human hypertension has received recent emphasis (26,31). Adult rats deficient in copper have increased blood pressure (27,39); women fed diets low in copper respond to the stress of sustained hand grip exercise with an extra increase in blood pressure that is prevented by copper supplementation (37). Although stress has long been implicated in hypertensive illness, its mechanism is unknown (10,15). Perhaps hypertension occurs only in combination with other factors such as salt intake, etc. (15, 17, 18) or in association with genetic predisposition (5, 15, 17, 18, 36). Page (44) has proposed the mosaic theory in which no single factor is uniquely responsible for hypertension; in contrast, combined effects of several factors produce illness. This complexity involving stress has led to a search for a suitable animal research model. The use of aversive reinforcement in classical (8, 48, 4 9 ) a n d operant (9, 11, 12, 14, 16)conditioning p a r a d i g m s did not c o n s i s t e n t l y produce h y p e r t e n s i o n in

lMention of a trademark or proprietary product does not constitute a guarantee or warranty of the product by the U.S. Department of Agriculture, and does not imply its approval to the exclusion of other products that may also be suitable. 2Requests for reprints should be addressed to Leslie M. Klevay, M.D., S.D. in Hyg., USDA, ARS, Grand Forks Human Nutrition Research Center, P.O. Box 7166, University Station, Grand Forks, ND 58202. 309

310

KLEVAY ANI) HAl.AS

TABLE 1

TABLE 2

COMPOSITION OF THE DIET (gt

BLOOD PRESSURE (mmHg)

Sucrose Egg white Corn oil Jones Foster salt mix* Cellulose Vitamin ADE mixt Water soluble vitamin mix~ Choline chloride

623.5 200.0 90.0 40.0 30.0 10.0 5.0 1.5

The details of the dietary formulation are published (31, 33, 49). This diet contains all nutrients known to be essential for rats. *Specially formulated without copper and zinc; these nutrients are supplied as drinking solutions (31,42). +Alpha tocopherol, calciferol and vitamin A palmitate in corn oil. ~:Biotin, cyancobalamine, folic acid, menadione, niacinamide, calcium pantothenate, pyridoxine, riboflavin and thiamin in sucrose.

normotensive animals. An avoidance-avoidance conflict procedure, a very potent stressor, raised blood pressure in normotensive rats only slightly (5); the same experimental procedures yielded much higher blood pressures (35) when rats with a borderline genetic susceptibility to hypertension were used, The use of rat strains with inherited hypertension has been useful (10,42), Our success (27) in raising blood pressure of rats with diets low in copper, the complexity of blood pressure regulation, and the prominence of stress in hypertension etiology prompted us to study rats subjected to stress and fed diets low in copper in all possible combinations: stress vs. no stress and supplemented vs, insufficient copper. We deliberately chose to use rats not known to be susceptible to hypertension to provide the most vigorous test of the hypothesis that stress can produce effects on blood pressure in addition to those of copper deficiency. EXPERIMENT 1 METHOD

Animals and Diets Sixty male weanling Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan SpragueDawley, WI) were matched into four equal groups by mean (58 g) body weight. They were fed a purified diet (Table 1) based on 62% sucrose, 20% egg white and 10% corn oil (23, 25, 41). Although the diet contains all the nutrients known to be essential for rats, it is inadequate in copper and zinc and must be supplemented. Supplementation was provided as a drinking solution (23,24) containing 10 ~g zinc and 2 ixg copper per ml (as acetate and sulfate, respectively). Rats were housed in individual cages of stainless steel under conditions similar to those described elsewhere (33).

Apparatus Plexiglas restraint cages were used. These were made from a cylinder 20 cm long and 9 cm in diameter with a 3 mm wall. The cylinder was cut and a plane floor attached parallel to the axis so that the height of the cage was 6.7 cm. Slots were cut in the curved top of the cage at 12, 15, and 19 cm which allowed a guillotine door to be used to restrict the rat and prevent it from turning around. The restraint cages also were used when blood

Week of Restrainl 0

I

2

3

Group I:CuS, NR

121 _ 2.7 (15)

123 +_ 3.6 (151

118 _ 3.(t (151

114 ± 2.2 (15)

Group 2:CuS, R

122 ± 2.1 (15)

125 -+ 1.7 (15)

126 +- 1.8 (15)

123 + 1.7 (14)

Group 3: CuD, NR

119 -+ 2.9 (15)

122 ± 2.5 (15)

118 + 2.0 I15~

119 _+ 2.2 (15)

Group 4: CuD, R

122 -+ 2.5 (15)

123 = 2.t (13)

128 + 2.1 (10)

131 _+ 3.1 (7)*

Mean ~ S.E.; CuS = copper supplemented, CuD=copper deficient, NR = nonrestrained, R = restrained; (n) = number of surviving rats for that week. *Two rats died shortly after BPs were recorded.

pressure was measured.

Chemical Analysis Cholesterol in plasma was measured by fluorescence (6); copper, potassium, sodium and zinc in organs were measured by inductively coupled plasma emission spectrometry (ICP-6500, PerkinElmer Corp. Norwalk, CT) after digestion with nitric acidhydrogen peroxide (40).

Procedures When the rats reached a mean weight of 280 g, blood pressures (BP) were measured without anesthesia by the tail cuff method. The method has been described in detail (27.31). Data were recorded with an Electro-Sphygmomanometer and pneumatic pulse transducer (Narco Bio-Systems, Houston, TX). Although no significant differences in blood pressure were found at this time, a few rats were exchanged between groups to make group blood pressures more similar. Then copper was omitted from the drinking solutions of the two groups with the lower blood pressures. Twenty-four days after copper deprivation was begun, stress in the form of physical restraint (45) was applied to half of the groups. A 2 × 2 factorial design was used. Group 1 was copper supplemented (CuS) and was not restrained (NR). Group 2 was copper supplemented and was restrained (R). Group 3 was copper deficient (CUD) and was not restrained while Group 4 was copper deficient and was restrained. For 23 consecutive days, the rats in Groups 2 and 4 were removed from their home cages starting at 9:00 a.m. and were placed prone in the restraint cages for 45 minutes in an adjoining room at 22°C. Then they were returned to their home cages. The blood pressures for all rats were measured weekly several hours after restraint. At the end of the experiment, all surviving rats were killed and organs were removed after intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbital. Blood was obtained by cardiac puncture and saved for chemical analyses. Rats that died prematurely were examined for gross pathology. Organ and plasma data were analyzed using 2 x 2 analyses of variance (ANOVA) to test for effects of diet and restraint. Blood pressure data from surviving rats were analyzed using a repeated measures ANOVA which tested for effects of diet and restraint across the 3 and 4 weeks of Experiments 1 and 2, respectively. F-values and their associated degrees of freedom are

DIETARY COPPER, STRESS AND BLOOD PRESSURE

311

TABLE 3 COPPER IN ORGANS, iJ,g/g, DRY WEIGHT Adrenal Gland Group 1: CuS, NR Group 2: CuS, R Group 3: CuD, NR Group 4: CuD, R

1.75 1.71 0.75 0.71

Brain

± 0.29 - 0.09 --- 0.07 - 0.12

9.0 8.7 8.2 8.1

__. 0.32 ___ 0.23 ___ 0.23 -4- 0.23

Heart 12.1 12.5 8.3 9.1

Kidney

--- 1.12 --- 0.87 - 0.89 ± 0.71

11.4 12.0 8.0 7.9

Liver

--- 0.58 ___ 0.90 --- 0.32 ± 0.25

12.8 10.8 3.4 6.5

-4- 0.88 --- 1.15 --- 0.71 +-- 2.33

Mean --- S.E.; CuS -- copper supplemented, CuD=copper deficient, NR= nonrestrained, R=restrained.

reported in the text for significant main effects and interactions (47). RESULTSAND DISCUSSION Analysis of variance of blood pressure was computed across time for rats surviving to week three. Data are shown in Table 2. The only significant main effect was that of restraint, F(1,47)= 15.0, p = 0 . 0 0 0 3 . Because the largest differences would be expected in the third week, an ANOVA was computed for that week. Significant main effects for both restraint, F(1,47)=22.1, p = 0.0001, and copper deficiency, F(1,47)=8.2, p = 0 . 0 0 6 3 , were found. There were no significant interactions at any time. Group 4, which was both copper deficient and restrained, had the highest mean BP (131 mmHg) while Group 1, the control group, had the lowest mean BP (114 mmHg). Rats adequate in copper and restrained (Group 2) and those deficient but unrestrained (Group 3) had BPs between the other groups. Premature deaths were not evenly distributed among the groups. Only 5 (of 15) rats survived among those rats (Group 4) subjected to both deficiency and restraint. One rat died in Group 2. Although there was no statistical interaction between restraint and deficiency for BP, the high mortality in Group 4 suggests a biological interaction. Hemorrhage was prominent among the restrained, deficient rats; blood was found in the bladder, gut and peritoneum (two rats each) and in the scrotum (one rat); these findings may be due to a weakened vascular system insufficiently resistant to increased blood pressure. Similar hemorrhages (unpublished) have been found occasionally in our experiments. Metallic elements were measured in several organs of all surviving rats. As expected (2, 13, 20, 34, 40, 41, 46), copper deficiency significantly reduced concentration of copper in all organs (Table 3). The smallest significant decrease was that in brain, 9%, F(1,44)=4.7, p = 0 . 0 3 5 . Decreases in other organs ranged from 29% in heart to 59% in adrenal. Copper deficiency in-

creased cardiac sodium by 6%, F(1,44)= 5.7, p<0.025, but had no significant effect on sodium in other organs. Potassium was decreased 2.6% in heart, F(1,44)=4.3, p<0.05, by restraint and was increased 5% in kidney by deficiency, F(1,44)=4.4, p<0.05. The only significant effect of restraint on any metallic element in any organ was this decrease in cardiac potassium. No significant differences in cholesterol in plasma or in heart or adrenal weight were found (Table 4). EXPERIMENT 2 Because of the high mortality in the group deficient in copper and subjected to restraint in Experiment 1, we decided to increase the size of this group. It was hoped that sufficient numbers of animals would survive the experiment to protect against any statistical bias resulting from the deaths of the weaker, or more susceptible animals. METHOD Animals, analyses, apparatuses, diets and procedures were similar to those of the first experiment. Initial matching was into 5 groups of 15 with a mean weight of 48 g. When the rats reached a mean weight of 275 g, blood pressure was measured. Then copper was omitted from the drinking solution of the three groups with the lowest mean blood pressure and the groups with the two lowest mean blood pressure were combined into one group of 28. A small sample of hair was shaved from the backs of the animals and was discarded. At the end of the experiment, hair was obtained from these areas for analysis. Aortas and other organs were obtained as in the first experiment. Restraint was continued one week longer in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1. RESULTSAND DISCUSSION Analysis of variance of blood pressure was computed across

TABLE 4 MEAN WEIGHTOF BODY AND ADRENALGLAND,CONCENTRATIONOF CHOLESTEROLIN PLASMAAND HEARTWEIGHT Body (g) Group 1: CuS, NR Group 2: CuS, R Group 3: CuD, NR Group 4: CuD, R

367 357 333 337

_ 6.5 --+ 7.2 - 5.7 -+ 9.8

Adrenal Gland (rag) 50.4 50.8 49.3 58.9

_ 3.2 __. 1.5 _+ 2.4 ~ 4.7

Cholesterol (mg/dl) 114 104 143 115

---

8.6 2.8 14.4 6.9

Heart (g) 1.24 1.21 1.30 1.28

___ 0.03 _ 0.02 ___ 0.04 --. 0.06

Mean _.+ S.E.; CuS=copper supplemented, CuD=copper deficient, NR= nonrestrained, R = restrained.

312

KLEVAY AND HALAS

TABLE 5 BLOOD PRESSURES (mmHg) Week of Restraint

Group 1: CuS, NR Group 2: CuS, R Group 3: CuD, NR Group 4: CuD, R

0

1

2

3

4

120 ± 4.3 (14) 117 ± 4.6 (15) t16 _+ 3.8 (15) 115 ± 3.5 (28)

118 ± 2.6 (14) 107 + 4.2 (14) 114 + 3.6 (15) 106 ± 2.2 (27)

130 + 5.7 (14) 129 -+ 5.3 (14/ 129 ± 3.3 (14) 128 ± 2.6 (27)

124 __ 4.2 (14) 135 ± 4.6 (14) 122 ± 3.7 (14) 137 + 3.8 (27)

1t5 ± 3.1 (14) 131 ± 4.7 t141 132 ± 4.0 (13) 138 -+ 2.8 (27)

Mean +__ S.E.; CuS = copper supplemented, CuD = copper deficient, NR = nonrestrained, R = restrained; (n) = number of surviving rats.

time for rats surviving to week four. Data are shown in Table 5. The only significant main effect was for time, F ( 4 , 6 1 ) = 2 5 . 5 , p = 0 . 0 0 0 1 . There were significant interactions between copper status and time, F(4,61) = 3.4, p = 0.015, and between restraint and time, F ( 4 , 6 1 ) = 8.3, p = 0 . 0 0 0 1 , indicating that it takes time before either restraint or deficiency can affect blood pressure. None of the other interactions were significant. Analysis of variance computed in week four revealed both copper deficiency, F(1,63) = 9.8, p < 0 . 0 0 3 , and restraint, F ( 1 , 6 3 ) = 8.2, p-<0.006, significantly elevated BP. There was no significant interaction. As in Experiment 1, the highest mean blood pressure was in the restrained, deficient group and the lowest mean was in the nonrestrained, supplemented group. Premature mortality was much less in Experiment 2. An unrestrained, deficient rat was found dead with nasal hemorrhage and serosanguinous fluid in the thoracic cavity. A second, similar rat died as blood pressure was being measured. A deficient rat died during restraint after nasal hemorrhage was noticed. Three of the four deaths in the experiment were deficient. Thus hemorrhage was a sign commonly found among deficient rats that died in both experiments; only one of these nine was unrestrained. The majority of the early deaths (11 of 15) in both experiments was in the deficient group subjected to restraint although a minority of the rats (43 of 132) was assigned to this group. Copper deficiency was verified by the general decrease in organ copper and the increase in both blood pressure and cholesterol in plasma. This latter phenomenon has been confirmed in at least 13 independent laboratories (31) since data linking the metabolism of copper and cholesterol were published in 1973 (23). The increase in blood pressure of adult rats made deficient in

copper is a more recently recognized phenomenon (27,39). Data on copper in organs are shown in Table 6. Rats deprived of copper had significantly less copper in all organs except aorta (data not shown). Again brain had the smallest decrease (12%), but this time liver had the largest decrease (75%). Cardiac sodium again was increased (6%) by deficiency, F(1,65) = 7.9, p < 0 . 0 0 7 . In contrast to Experiment 1, deficiency provided a marginal decrease in cardiac potassium, F ( 1 , 6 5 ) = 3 . 5 , p < 0 . 0 7 ; however, restraint was without effect (p<0.5). Zinc was increased (5%) in brain, F ( 1 , 6 6 ) = 2 4 . 3 , p = 0 . 0 0 0 1 , and decreased (14%) in liver, F ( 1 , 6 6 ) = 11.5, p < 0 . 0 0 2 , by restraint and deficiency, respectively. Sodium in brain was increased almost 3% by deficiency, F(1,66) = 4 . 9 , p < 0 . 0 3 . Potassium in brain was increased 2% by restraint, F(1,66) = 11.0, p < 0 . 0 0 2 . Data on the concentration of cholesterol in plasma (Tables 4 and 7) were similar in both experiments although statistical significance was reached only in Experiment 2. As expected, deficiency increased cholesterol ( p = 0 . 0 0 0 1 ) . Restraint decreased cholesterol (p<0.03). Hearts and adrenal glands were enlarged by deficiency ( p = 0 . 0 0 0 1 ) . Body weight was unaffected. Much of our experimental data is in agreement with published clinical data. Aalbers (1) found that sodium was increased and potassium was decreased in the normal ventricular wall of people who died of cardiac infarction. He also found a decrease in liver zinc. The Chipperfields (7) found decreased potassium in the cardiac muscle of victims of ischemic heart disease who died suddenly. These epidemiologic data are consonant with the increase in cardiac sodium in both experiments, the decrease in cardiac potassium due to stress in the first experiment, the marginal decrease in cardiac potassium due to deficiency in the second exper-

TABLE 6 COPPER IN ORGANS, p,g/g. DRY WEIGHT Adrenal Gland Group 1: CuS, NR Group 2: CuS, R Group 3: CuD, NR Group 4: CuD, R

1.34 1.73 0.55 0.51

±± ±

0.09 0.18 0.07 0.04

Brain 8.3 8.6 7.4 7.5

+_ 0.16 ± 0.13 - 0.14 --- 0.10

Hair 10.4 10.4 4.7 5.2

± 1.10 + 0.75 - 1.33 --- 0.59

Heart 16.7 17.1 7.3 7.5

__. 1.37 z 1.09 ± 0.72 ±- 0.43

Kidney 19.3 18.9 10.9 11.6

± ± ± ±

1.61 0.86 0.55 0.26

Mean _ S.E.; CuS = copper sufficient, CuD = copper deficient, NR = nonrestrained, R = restrained.

Liver 9.3 10.2 2.4 2.5

_+ 0.55 +- 0.49 ± 0.22 +- 0.24

DIETARY COPPER, STRESS AND BLOOD PRESSURE

313

TABLE 7 MEAN WEIGHT OF BODY AND ADRENAL GLAND, CONCENTRATION OF CHOLESTEROL IN PLASMA AND HEART WEIGHT Body (g) Group 1: CuS, NR Group 2: CuS, R Group 3: CuD, NR Group 4: CuD, R

355 325 336 310

Adrenal Gland (mg)

-4- 10.0 _+ 23.8 --- 7.4 --- 12.6

45.1 45.2 54.2 50.4

--- 1.2 +__ 2.2 --_ 1.4 __. 1.0

Cholesterol (mg/dl) 95 86 190 130

--- 3.1 --- 2.4 __+ 34.1 --+ 11.5

Heart (g) 1.16 1.14 1.45 1.33

+ 0.03 --_ 0.03 + 0.12 ± 0.04

Mean _+ S.E.; CuS=copper supplemented, CuD=copper deficient, NR= nonrestrained, R = restrained.

iment, and the decrease in liver zinc due to deficiency in the second experiment. We generally (2,32) but not invariably (20) have found a decrease in liver zinc in copper deficiency. GENERAL DISCUSSION Increased mortality and hemorrhage in deficient animals subjected to stress is not surprising. Blood vessels of poor quality (51) due to defective connective tissue (26,46) from deficiency may leak when blood pressure is elevated due to stress. Synthesis of some connective tissue in arteries is dependent on lysyl oxidase, a copper metalloenzyme (43). Furthermore, copper deficiency impairs the coagulability of blood (38) which could contribute to the leakage. The greater mortality in the deficient group subjected to stress in Experiment 1 in comparison to Experiment 2 cannot be explained by differences in diet because dietary copper was very similar between the experiments (over-all mean 0.98 mg copper per kg of diet). Groups of rats obtained at different times probably vary somewhat in the ability to resist the effects of the regimen. Even within a group of rats fed the diet in a study of mortality in which 75% were dead from deficiency in 73 days, those remaining lived 40 to 415 days longer (Klevay, unpublished data). Cardiac enlargement from copper deficiency has been found numerous times (2, 13, 22, 50). Enlargement was found in both experiments (p<0.0002) if heart weight was expressed as a percent of body weight which corrects for the smaller size of deficient rats. Failure to find enlarged hearts in Experiment 1 without

this correction may have been due to the low number of hearts available for study due to mortality. Enlarged hearts often are associated with hypertension (52) or other heart damage. Rats subjected to two insults may be more vulnerable than rats subjected to only one (29). Moore et al. (40) found that mortality was more frequent and occurred earlier when rats were both deficient in copper and were fed extra salt. The increase in blood pressure occurred earlier in the present experiments than in similar experiments involving only copper deficiency in which no mortality occurred as of 86 days when mean blood pressure due to deficiency was 158 ___2.4 m m of mercury (27). The rats in this experiment and the others (27,40) were all of the Sprague-Dawley strain which is not known to be susceptible to hypertension and is thought to be protected against the hypertensive effects of stress (10,36). Ischemic heart disease is one of the major sequelae of hypertension in humans (19). Some of the effects of stress on this illness are indirect (e.g., via hypertension), others are direct. Copper deficiency, copper metabolism, hypertension and ischemic heart disease are associated (vide supra) (26, 30, 31, 37). In summary, blood pressure and mortality were increased in adult rats both by stress and by copper deficiency. The effects on blood pressure were independent; the effects on mortality appeared to be synergistic. Results are germane to the pathophysiology of ischemic heart disease because stress is a prime factor in the induction of human hypertension, and hypertension is a risk factor for ischemic heart disease. Environmental stress and human diets low in copper may contribute to this illness.

REFERENCES 1. Aalbers, T. G. Cardiovascular diseases and trace elements. Dieren, Netherlands: Drukkerij Blok En Zonen; 1984:59, 152, 230. 2. Alien, K. G. D.; Klevay, L. M. Cholesterolemia and cardiovascular abnormalities in rats caused by copper deficiency. Atherosclerosis 29:81-93; 1978. 3. Anonymous. Recommended dietary allowances. Food and Nutrition Board, National Research Council, 9th ed. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences; 1980:151-154. 4. Anonymous. Vital statistics of the United States, Mortality, Vol. 2, Part A, 1984; DHHS Pub. No. (PHS) 87-1122. Public Health Service, Hyattsville: U.S. Government Printing Office; 1987:Table 1-6. 5. Buchholz, R. A.; Lawler, J. E.; Barker, G. F. The effects of avoidance and conflict schedules on the blood pressure and heart rate of rats. Physiol. Behav. 26:853-863; 1981. 6. Carpenter, K. J.; Gotsis, A.; Hegsted, D. M. Estimation of total cholesterol in serum by a micro method. Clin. Chem. 3:233-238; 1957. 7. Chipperfield, B.; Chipperfield, J. R. Discussion in metal content of the heart muscle in death from ischemic heart disease. Am. Heart

95:732; 1978. 8. Dahl, L. K.; Knudson, K. D.; Heine, M.; Leitl, G. Hypertension and stress. Nature 219:735-736; 1968. 9. Findley, J. D.; Brady, J. V.; Robinson, W. W.; Gilliam, W. J. Continuous cardiovascular monitoring in the baboon during long-term behavioral performances. Commun. Behav. Biol. 6:49-58; 1971. 10. Folkow, B. Physiological aspects of primary hypertension. Physiol. Rev. 62:347-504; 1982. 11. Forsyth, R. P. Blood pressure responses to long-term avoidance schedules in the restrained rhesus monkey. Psychosom. Med. 31: 30(0309; 1969. 12. Goldstein, D. S.; Hams, A. H.; Brady, J. V. Sympathetic adrenergic blockade effects upon operantly conditioned blood pressure elevations in baboons. Biofeed. Self-Regul. 2:93-105; 1977. 13. Gubler, C. J.; Cartwright, G. E.; Wintrobe, M. M. Studies on copper metabolism. XX. Enzyme activities and iron metabolism in copper and iron deficiencies. J. Biol. Chem. 224:533-546; 1957. 14. Hams, A. H.; Gilliam, W. J.; Findley, J. D.; Brady, J. V. Instrumental conditioning of large magnitude daily, 12-hour blood pres-

314

sure elevations in the baboon. Science 182:175-177; 1973. 15. Herd, J. A.; Falkner, B. ; Anderson, D. E.; Costa, P. D.; Dembroski, T. M.: Hendrix, G. H.; Henry, J. P.; Kaplan, J. R.; Light, K. C.: Schneiderman, N.; Shepherd, J. T. Task Force 2: Psychological factors in hypertension. Circulation 76:89-94; 1987. 16. Herd, J. A.: Morse, W.; Kelleher, R.; Jones, L. Arterial hypertension in squirrel monkeys during behavioral experiments. Am. J. Physiol. 217:24-25; 1969. 17. Hinkle, L. E. Stress and disease: The concept after 50 years. Soc. Sci. Med. 25:561-566; 1987. 18. Huie, P. E.; Hatton, D. C.; Muntzel, M. S. Psychosocial stress, dietary calcium and hypertension in the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Physiol. Behav. 40:425-429; 1987. 19. Insull, W., Jr. Coronary risk handbook. New York: American Heart Association; 1973:3-25. 20. Johnson, W. T.; Nordlie, R. C.; Klevay, L. M. Glucose-6-phosphatase activity in copper-deficient rats. Biol. Tr. Element Res. 6: 369-378; 1984. 21. Kaplan, N. M. Endocrine hypertension. In: Wilson, J. D.; Foster, D. W., eds. Williams textbook of endocrinology, 7th ed. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders; 1985:966-988. 22. Kelly, W. A.; Kesterson, J. W.; Carlton, W. W. Myocardial lesions in the offspring of female rats fed a copper deficient diet. Exp. Mol. Pathol. 20:40-56; 1974. 23. Klevay, L. M. Hypercholesterolemia in rats produced by an increase in the ratio of zinc to copper ingested. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 26:10601068; 1973. 24. Klevay, L. M. Coronary heart disease: the zinc/copper hypothesis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 28:764-774; 1975. 25. Klevay, L. M. The biotin requirement of rats fed 20% egg white. J. Nutr. 106:1643-1646; 1976. 26. Klevay, L. M. The role of copper, zinc, and other chemical elements in ischemic heart disease. In: Rennart, O. M.; Chan, W-Y., eds. Metabolism of trace metals in man. vol. 1. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 1984:129-157. 27. Klevay, L. M. Hypertension in rats due to copper deficiency. Nutr. Rep. Int. 35:999-1005; 1987. 28. Klevay, L. M. Ischemic heart disease. A major obstacle to becoming old. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 3:361-372; 1987. 29. Klevay, L. M. Four ways of becoming ill. Med. Hypotheses 27:6570; 1988. 30. Klevay, L. M. lschemic heart disease as copper deficiency. In: Kies, C., ed. Copper bioavailability and metabolism. New York: Plenum Publishing; 1990:197-208. (Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., vol. 258.) 31. Klevay, L. M. Ischemic heart disease: Toward a unified theory. In: Lei, K. Y.; Carr, T. P., eds. Role of copper in lipid metabolism. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 1990:233-267. 32. Klevay, L. M.; Milne, D. B.; Wallwork, J. C. Comparison of some indices of copper deficiency in growing rats. Nutr. Rep. Int. 31: 963-980; 1985. 33. Klevay, L. M.; Petering, H. G.; Stemmer, K. L. A controlled environment for trace metal experiments on animals. Environ. Sci. Technol. 5:1196-1199: 1971.

KLEVAY AND HALAS

34. Kopp, S. J.; Klevay, L. M.; Feliksik, J. M. Physiological and metabolic characterization of a cardiomyopathy induced by chronic copper deficiency. Am. J. Physiol. 245:H855-H866: 1983. 35. Lawler, J. E.; Barker, G. F.; Hubbard, J. W.; Allen, M. F. The effects of conflict on tonic levels of blood pressure in the genetically borderline hypertensive rats. Psychophysiology 17:363-379; 1980. 36. Lawler, J. E.; Barker, G. F.; Hubbard, J. W.; Schaub, R. G. Effects of stress on blood pressure and cardiac pathology in rats with borderline hypertension. Hypertension 3:496-505; 1981. 37. Lukaski, H. C.; Klevay, L. M.; Milne, D. B. Effects of dietary copper on human autonomic cardiovascular function, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 58:74-80; 1988. 38. Lynch, S. M.: Klevay, L. M. Effects of copper deficiency on coagulation factor activities in mice. FASEB J. 4:in press; 1990. 39. Medeiros, D. M. Hypertension in the Wistar-Kyoto rat as a result of post-weaning copper restriction. Nutr. Res. 7:231-235; 1987. 40. Moore, R. J.: Hall, C. B.; Carlson, E. C.; Lukaski, H. C.; Klevay, L. M. Acute renal failure and fluid retention and kidney damage in copper-deficient rats fed a high-NaCl diet. J. Lab. Clin. Med. 113: 516-524; 1989. 41. Moore, R. J.; Klevay, L. M. Effect of copper deficiency on blood pressure and plasma and tung angiotensin-converting enzyme activity in rats. Nutr. Res. 8:489; 1988. 42. Okamoto, K.; Aoki, K. Development of a strain of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Jpn. Circulat. J. 27:282-293; 1963. 43. Owen, C. A., Jr. Biochemical aspects of copper. Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes Publications; 1982:11. 44. Page, I. H. Hypertension mechanisms. Orlando: Grune & Stratton; 1987.

45. Pare, W. P.; Glavin, G. B. Restraint stress in biomedical research: A review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 10:339-370; 1986. 46. Radhakrishnamurthy, B.; Ruiz, H.; Dalferes, E. R., Jr.; Klevay, L. M.; Berenson, G. S. Composition of proteoglycans in the aortas of copper-deficient rats. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 190:98-104; 1989. 47. SAS Institute Inc. SAS user's guide: Statistics. 5th ed. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.; 1985. 48. Shapiro, A. P.; Melhado, J. Factors affecting development of hypertensive vascular disease after renal injury in rats. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 96:619-623; 1957. 49. Shapiro, A. P.; Melhado, J. Observations on blood pressure and other physiologic and biochemical mechanisms in rats with behavioral disturbances. Psychosom. Med. 20:303-313; 1958. 50. Shields, G. S.; Coulson, W. F.; Kimball, D. A.; Carries, W. H.; Cartwright, G. E.; Wintrobe, M. M. Studies on copper metabolism XXXII. Cardiovascular lesions in copper-deficient swine. Am. J. Pathol. 41:603-621; 1962. 51. Underwood, E. J. Trace elements in human and animal nutrition. 4th ed. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1977. 52. Whelton, P. K.; Patterson, R. R. Systemic hypertension. In: Harvey, A. McG.; Johns, R. J.; McKusick, V. A.; Owens, A. H., Jr.; Ross, R. S., eds. The principles and practice of medicine. 21st ed. Norwalk: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1984:278-297.