THE EFFECTS OF SOME CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM DEPRESSANTS ON CONFLICT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS

THE EFFECTS OF SOME CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM DEPRESSANTS ON CONFLICT BEHAVIOR IN DOGS

THE EFFECTS DEPRESSANTS OF SOME ON CENTRAL CONFLICT NERVOUS BEHAVIOR SYSTEM IN DOGS MASAAKI TAMURA Department of Pharmacology, Facultyof Med...

324KB Sizes 0 Downloads 45 Views

THE

EFFECTS

DEPRESSANTS

OF

SOME ON

CENTRAL

CONFLICT

NERVOUS

BEHAVIOR

SYSTEM IN DOGS

MASAAKI TAMURA Department of Pharmacology, Facultyof Medicine, University of Tokyo,Tokyo Receivedfor publicationDecember12, 1962

Conflict behavior induced by punishing the animal's approach to food has been frequently studied in cats or rats as a means for evaluating the effects of drugs upon neurotic conditions. In 1945Masserman and Yum (1) showed that alcohol prevented the development of neurotic behavior in cats and abated the neurotic behavior. A similar result was obtained by Conger (2) in rats. Jacobsen and Skaarup (3), modifying the technique described by Masserman, introduced a method which permits evaluation of drug effects upon behavior with ease. They found that benzilic acid aminoester deriva tives, especially benactyzine, have a clear-cut normalizing effect in cats. Recently Naess and Rasmussen (4), Geller and Seifter (5), and other workers proposed newer procedures which allowed more objective evaluation of drugs upon behavior. This paper describes a method for inducing conflict behavior in dogs, and the effects of some drugs upon the conflict behavior. METHODSAND MATERIALS In this series of experiments 16 adult dogs, 7 female and 9 males, ranging in weight from 6.5 to 15 kg, were used. They were fed on boiled barley containing fish meat once a day at 5 : 00 p.m.

FIG.

田村

正昭

I

Fig. 1 (A) illustrates the outline of experimental cage, 100cm wide, 120cm deep and 100 cm high. Excepting the base and the front side, all the faces were made of wire mesh fixed to the wooden frame of the cage. On the front side of the cage, a food box, 30 x 20 x 15 cm, Fig. 1 (B), and a bar were placed . When the dog pressed the bar, a buz zer signal worked automatically, and when the dog pushed the lid of the food box from the inside of cafe the animal could open it to take food in the box . The food for reward were made of 70% boiled barley and 30% finely minced fish meat . It was placed in the food box in a volume of about 4 cm'. The cage was further equipped with a bell , and two metal plates for punishing the dog by electric shock. Phenobarbital sodium (Phenobal), pentobarbital sodium (Mintal), thiopental sodium (Ravonal), chlorpromazine hydrochloride (Contomin), and benactyzine hydrochloride (Parpon) were used in diluted solution of less than 1 per cent. Ethanol was used as a 30% aqueous solution and meprobamate (Atraxin) as a 10% propylene glycol solution . All drug solutions were injected intravenously. The interval between two successive drug injections was at least one week. The experiments were undertaken 1 min after injec tion in cases of thiopental and ethanol, and 5 min after injection in cases of other drugs . CONDITIONING The they

dogs were trained

learned

box

and

food.

to push

put

buzzer

and

they

was given.

buzzer

signal

generally

the

and

training

then

They

repeated

of food-taking min.

Fig.

to obtain

after

was further

the feeding this

2 shows

the

training

a day.

became

sequence than such

the

response

repeated

cycle

more

the

when

food taking

2 to 3 hours

times,

to open

only

In the final stage

achieved

1 to 5 days,

the

the bar so as to make

food. This conditioned was

food

it to take

learned

to press

First,

lid of the

into

eat the food

signal

they learned the

the

the head

Secondly,

food box

stepwise.

of

When 10 to 15

stabilized.

of activities

100 times

in 15

FIG.

2

examples.

INDUCTION OF CONFLICT BEHAVIOR Having achieved frequently

the conditioned

over a period

this type of feeding, the punishment the electrodes had completed

attached

food-taking

of 2 to 3 months.

of the electric

to the food box (mouth)

10 feeding

response, the dogs were further

trained

When the dogs had been well accustomed to shock, 5-20 volts, was given through

and the floor (paw).

When the dogs

cycles they were given the electric shock concomitantly

with

bell signal.

After

the subsequent

10 normal

feeding cycles a second punishment

was

given, and this was repeated every 10 feeding cycles as far as the dogs continued feed ing. This punishing procedure was applied every day until -the conflict behavior deve loped. Although some individual variation in the pattern was noticed, the following behaviors were common : 1) the dogs performed only one or two, or no feeding cycle, 2) the frequencies

of behaviors

the cage, wandering considerably

such as licking and smelling the floor or the frame of

about in the experimental

increased.

When these abnormal

cage, and lying or sitting down became behaviors

had been established,

the electric

shock was neglected and only the bell signal was used at the end of each 10 cycles of feeding.

Under

re-establish

these circumstances,

the normal

conditioned TABLE

This total

table numbers

it took generally

1.

shows

the

of

behaviors

food taking reaction.

Examples

avarage

of

conflict

numbers

divided

by

of

behavior

behaviors

minutes

An experiment

cycles of feeding, or when the animal

per

dogs.

minute, for

the

that

is to

say,

the

experiment.

were carried

was terminated

required

for the dogs to

Table 1 shows two examples. in

required

The dogs were not starved, and the experiments before meal at 5:00 p.m.

2 to 3 months

out in the afternoon

when the dog completed

more than 5 min to complete

50

a feeding

cycle. The following categories of behavior

were particularly

per min were recorded : feeding cycle, putting the head into the box without buzzer, smelling about in the experimental cage, licking the body or experimental scratching turning

cage,

the body, neck, head, etc., back from the food box and the bar,

extramanipulation

of the bar without

taking the food,

observed and the frequencies

lying or sitting.down, standing

still more than 30 sec.

The last two categories

were counted 2, 3, 4.and so on when the animal

remains

in the

specified states more than 1, 1.5, 2 min and so on. hesitation

of food-taking

violent behaviors

more than 5 sec after pressing

including

barking,

the bar,

biting at experimental

cage, pushing

the door

of the cage, with head or paws, falling

or tumbling,

autonomic

reaction

including

vomiting,

micturition,

defecation,

etc.

RESULTS The assessment on the temporary

of the effects of drugs on conflict behavior recovery

toward

normal

feeding cycle.

was based principally

The effect was graded as +

(marked decrease in the conflict behavior), + (moderate decrease in conflict behavior), 0 (slight or no change) according to the degree of recovery toward the pre-punishment, normal

behavior

(Table 2).

TABLE 2. The effects of phenobarbital

This

table

of behaviors

shows divided

the avarage by minutes

numbers required

of

(30 mg/kg i.v.) on conflict behavior.

behaviors

for the

per

minute,

that

is to say, the total

numbers

experiment.

Two to 3 days before as well as after the experiment with drug, the control ments (conflict behavior) were carried out with physiological saline (0.5 ml/kg) of drug solution. Table 3 shows the effects of some drugs on conflict behavior. Phenobarbital, pentobarbital, and thiopental markedly lessened the degree flict behavior and restored the inhibited food-taking response in the majority

experi instead of con of dogs

TABLE

3a.

The

effect

of

barbiturates

on

conflict

behavior.

In Table 3a, b, c, the total numbers of feeding cycle are given in columns under headings of pre-exp. control, drug i.v. and post-exp. control, and under "behavior" is given the extent of the decrease of the conflict behavior. pre-exp. control : 2-3. days before drug administration, drug i.v.: 5 min after drug administration (phenobarbital, pentobarbital, benacty zine, chlorpromazine, meprobamate), 1 min after drug administration (thiopental, ethanol), post-exp. control : 2-3 days after drug administration.

TABLE: 3b.

The effect

of benactyzine

on conflict

behavior.

(Table 3a). Severe ataxia was produced by these three drugs. conflict behavior, when large doses (40-60 mg/kg) of phenobarbital dogs frequently

failed to press the bar exactly and

buzzer

The

signal.

conditioned

even when the dogs sometimes

food taking

Before producing were administered

the the

put the head into the box without

response,

however,

was never

abolished

fell (Table 4a).

Benactyzine produced no noticeable improvement in the conflict behavior except in one dog (Table 3b). Before producing the conflict behavior, when large doses (1 mg/kg) were given the conditioned food taking response was abolished and the dogs wandered about in the experimental cage. They rarely lay or sit down, and when they lay down they stood up soon (Table 4b). Outside the experimental cage, the dogs treated with large doses (1 mg/kg) of benactyzine dropped the food out of the mouth and some of them vomited or exhibited poor appetite. Chlorpromazine exerted no marked effect on inhibited food taking response (Table 3c). Before producing conflict behavior, when large doses (1-1.5 mg/kg) were adminis tered the dogs spent a large part of time lying down in the experimental cage, but as soon as the animal heard the buzzer signal, the dog stood up and put the head into the

TABLE 3c. The effects of chlorpromazine,

TABLE

4a.

In total

The

Table numbers

effects

of

phenobarbital

4a, b,

c the

avarage

of

behaviors

divided

on

numbers by

meprobamate,

conditioned

of

minutes

ethanol on couflict behavior.

food

behaviors

per

required

for

taking

response

minute, the

that

experiment

and

on

is to

say,

behavior.

the

are showed.

TABLE 4b.

TABLE

4c.

The

The

effects

effects

of

of benactyzine

chlorpromazine

box to eat food, and then repeated experimental

on conditioned

on

conditioned

food taking

food

response

taking

response

and on behavior .

and

on

behavior

two or three feeding cycles (Table 4c) . Outside

.

the

cage, the dogs had a good appetite.

Meprobamate toward normal

improved

the conflict behavior

very much

and temporary

recovery

was seen in most of dogs . Severe ataxia was seen in all. Ethanol also improved the conflict behavior and the inhibited food-taking response , but the effect seemed less obvious than those of barbiturates and meprobamate (Table 3c).

Ataxia Fasting

feeding activity

was considerable

(Table 4c).

of 48 to 72 hours caused no remarkable

change

in conflict behavior .

DISCUSSION

Jacobsen and Skaarup (6) set the switch for signal on the opposite side of the food box in their apparatus. In the present experiments, the bar for buzzer signal and the

food box were set on the same side so that the frequency of turning back from the food box or the bar might be taken as one of abnormal behaviors. The behavioral effects of chlorpromazine in dogs was discriminated from these of benactyzine by the experi ments in which this arrangement was used. In this experimental series once the conflict behavior had been established, most of the dogs performed only one or two, or no feeding cycle, so the improvement of the conflict behavior by drugs is independent of the change in pain threshold. Dogs were used in this experiment. The dog might be too large for the laboratory animal in this type of experiment but is very convenient in some respects : 1) the be haviors are observed in detail ; 2) the dog learns conditioned food taking process easily and the experiments can be carried out without fasting. Since the present study was primarily qualitative, no attempts were made to compare the potency of meprobamate with those of barbiturates. Although the effects of mepro bamate and barbiturates were differentiated from those of chlorpromazine and benac tyzine. In comparison of the results obtained with cats or rats by Masserman, Conger, Jacobsen, etc. (1-9), and results with dogs by this author, there are some similarities as well as differences according to drugs. Regardless of the animal species barbiturates and meprobamate had clear-cut normaliz_neffects on the conflict behavior. Ethanol also possessed a normalizing effect on the conflict behavior, although the effect was not so obvious as that of barbiturates or meprobamate. Chlorpromazine, regardless of animal species, had no effect on the inhibited food taking response. The effect of benactyzine varied with the species of animals, and the results obtained in the dogs were more closely resembling those in rats rather than those in cats. SUMMARY The effects of some central nervous system depressants upon conflict behavior in dogs were studied. Phenobarbital, pentobarbital, thiopental, meprobamate and ethanol restored the in hibited food taking response, but benactyzine and chlorpromazine did not produce the restoring effect. Before inducing conflict behavior, phenobarbital did not abolish the conditioned food taking response even in such large doses as caused severe ataxia and sometimes falling, although the dogs frequently failed to press the bar exactly. Benactyzine and chlorpromazine abolished the conditioned food taking response, and in the doses almost abolishing the conditioned food taking response, the former caused dogs to walk about in the experimental cage and the latter to lie down. Acknowledgement : The author is much indebted to Prof. Dr. H. Kumagai, Dr. F. Sakai, Dr. A. Sakuma and Dr. T. Fukuhara for valuable suggestions and advices.

REFERENCES 1) MASSERMAN,J.H. AND YuM, K.S. : Psychosom.Med. 8, 36 (1946) 2) CONGER,J.J. : Quart. J. Stud. Alc. 12, 1 (1951) 3) JACORSEN,E. AND SKAARUP,Y.: Acta pharm. tox. Kbh. 11, 125 (1955) 4) NAESE,K. AND RASMUSSEN,W.: Ibid. 15, 99 (1958) 5) GELLER, I. AND SEIFTER,J. : Psychopharmacologia1, 482 (1960) 6) JACOBSEN,E. AND SKAARUP,Y.: Acta pharm. tox. Kbh. 11, 117 (1955) 7) MASSERMAN,J.H. AND SIEVER,P.W. : Psychosom.Med. 6, 7 (1944) 8) BAILEY,C.J. AND MILLER, N.E.: J. comp.physiol. Psychol. 45, 205 (1952) 9) MASSERMAN, J.H.:

Amer. J. Psychiat. 101, 389 (1944)