FRONTIERS IN NEUROENDOCRINOLOGY ARTICLE NO.
18, 292–353 (1997)
FN970153
The Neuroendocrinology of Thirst and Salt Appetite: Visceral Sensory Signals and Mechanisms of Central Integration Alan Kim Johnson and Robert L. Thunhorst Departments of Psychology and Pharmacology and The Cardiovascular Center, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242
This review examines recent advances in the study of the behavioral responses to deficits of body water and body sodium that in humans are accompanied by the sensations of thirst and salt appetite. Thirst and salt appetite are satisfied by ingesting water and salty substances. These behavioral responses to losses of body fluids, together with reflex endocrine and neural responses, are critical for reestablishing homeostasis. Like their endocrine and neural counterparts, these behaviors are under the control of both excitatory and inhibitory influences arising from changes in osmolality, endocrine factors such as angiotensin and aldosterone, and neural signals from low and high pressure baroreceptors. The excitatory and inhibitory influences reaching the brain require the integrative capacity of a neural network which includes the structures of the lamina terminalis, the amygdala, the perifornical area, and the paraventricular nucleus in the forebrain, and the lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPBN), the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), and the area postrema in the hindbrain. These regions are discussed in terms of their roles in receiving afferent sensory input and in processing information related to hydromineral balance. Osmoreceptors controlling thirst are located in systemic viscera and in central structures that lack the blood–brain barrier. Angiotensin and aldosterone act on and through structures of the lamina terminalis and the amygdala to stimulate thirst and sodium appetite under conditions of hypovolemia. The NTS and LPBN receive neural signals from baroreceptors and are responsible for inhibiting the ingestion of fluids under conditions of increased volume and pressure and for stimulating thirst under conditions of hypovolemia and hypotension. The interplay of multiple facilitory influences within the brain may take the form of interactions between descending angiotensinergic systems originating in the forebrain and ascending adrenergic systems emanating from the hindbrain. Oxytocin and serotonin are additional candidate neurochemicals with postulated inhibitory central actions and with essential roles in the overall integration of sensory input within the neural network devoted to maintaining hydromineral balance. KEY WORDS: Angiotensin II; anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V); baroreceptors; circumventricular organs; ingestive behaviors; parabrachial nucleus. r 1997 Academic Press
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Alan Kim Johnson, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, 11 Seashore Hall E, Iowa City, IA 52242-1407, fax: 319/335-0191.
0091-3022/97 $25.00 Copyright r 1997 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
292
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
293
INTRODUCTION
The major components of body fluids are in a state of constant flux. Even in benign environments, terrestrial animals constantly lose water and sodium. Reflex and behavioral responses are both necessary to correct water and sodium imbalances and maintain body fluid homeostasis. Reflex mechanisms employ the autonomic nervous system and endocrine responses [e.g., aldosterone; vasopressin (VP)] to modify renal losses of sodium and water in states of dehydration. Behavioral responses include the seeking out and drinking of water and the ingestion of sodium-containing foods or fluids. The ingestion of water and sodium is the only means to repair deficiencies of body water and body sodium. Thirst and salt appetite are the motivational states that drive animals to find and consume water and salty substances and are defined operationally by measuring this consumption under appropriate experimental conditions. The autonomic and endocrine reflexes and the behaviors that minimize the consequences of body fluid deficits take effect with different latencies. At the onset of an imbalance of body fluids, sympathetic reflexes exert their actions within seconds, and significant endocrine effects are felt within minutes. In contrast, the consequences of behavioral mechanisms at the cellular level are realized only tens of minutes to hours later. Reflex mechanisms, in essence, serve to maintain cardiovascular function until behaviors can be mobilized for the ultimate restoration of homeostasis. The activation and coordination of reflexive and behavioral responses that maintain body fluid homeostasis require the integrative action of the central nervous system (CNS). The brain must be informed second by second of the balance and distribution of body fluids. Although the reflexes and behaviors that maintain body fluid homeostasis differ in their forms of expression, they employ common mechanisms. Autonomic/endocrine reflexes and hydromineral-related ingestive behaviors are activated by similar afferent signals to the brain. The same CNS structures and neurochemical systems process information from the afferent signals. For example, circulating angiotensin II (ANG II) increases sympathetic outflow, stimulates VP release, and causes drinking and sodium ingestion via actions in overlapping, if not identical, areas of the brain. The purpose of this review is to summarize the current state of understanding of the stimuli and signaling mechanisms that apprise the CNS of body fluid status and to discuss how the brain processes such information to generate thirst and salt appetite. The neural and endocrine mechanisms that activate thirst and salt appetite to maintain body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis are viewed as complementary to reflex hormonal and autonomic mechanisms. There are now numerous examples of how cardiovascular reflex mechanisms are relevant to the afferent control of thirst and salt appetite and vice versa.
FIN 153
294
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST BACKGROUND Body Fluid Compartments and Their Experimental Manipulation
Water accounts for approximately 60% of the body’s weight (122), with two-thirds of body water within cells and one-third in interstitial spaces and plasma. The 2:1 ratio of intracellular to extracellular water largely reflects an osmotic equilibrium achieved by the relative concentrations of impermeable ions found in the two compartments. The concentration of extracellular sodium is the primary ionic factor determining this osmotic equilibrium. Thus, the addition (through ingestion) or loss (through excretion, salivary loss, respiration, perspiration, etc.) of either water or sodium to/from the body alters not only net fluid balance but also osmotic equilibrium, thereby changing the relative distribution of fluid between the compartments. Some disturbances of body water and sodium give rise to the general sensation of thirst and to drinking behavior. The double depletion hypothesis, proposed by Alan Epstein and James Fitzsimons (79, 105), incorporated the idea that loss of water from either the intracellular or the extracellular compartment is sufficient to generate sensations of thirst and drinking behavior. Losses of water from the two compartments are hypothesized to generate different classes of sensory signals (e.g., osmotic, hormonal, neural) that utilize different afferent pathways (e.g., blood, nerves) to inform the brain of the status of body water. There are continuous demands placed on body fluids by obligatory water and sodium losses from normal renal excretory functions and ‘‘insensible’’ loss of water from respiration and transpiration through the skin. Feeding places demands on body water reflecting the need to dilute ingested solutes and other products of digestion and absorption (glucose, amino acids, etc.). Thirst caused by loss of fluid from the intracellular space occurs, for example, after the ingestion of salty foods or fluids. The sodium in salty substances causes an osmotically driven loss of water from cells into the extracellular space creating a deficit of intracellular water. Thirst caused by depletion primarily of extracellular fluid occurs after hemorrhage, vomiting, diarrhea, and sweating. The extracellular fluid lost in these circumstances is dilute with respect to intracellular fluid, so there is some shifting of water from the intracellular compartment to the extracellular compartment. Pure water loss occurs through respiration and by transpiration of water through the skin (i.e., ‘‘insensible’’ loss), resulting in losses of water from both compartments. Thirst from insensible loss occurs under arid conditions, such as breathing dry heated air. Although physiological disturbances may sometimes selectively deplete only one compartment to yield a single stimulus culminating in drinking, it is likely that day-to-day thirst results from the central integration of multiple stimuli of different classes. The loss of extracellular fluid results in deficiences of sodium as well as water. The condition of sodium deficiency is associated with a ‘‘peculiar sensa-
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
295
tion in the mouth’’ (205) that is sometimes interpreted as thirst, but is not relieved by drinking water (206). This sensation is called salt appetite. Recent evidence suggests that humans express salt appetite after reductions in plasma volume and sodium concentration by indicating increased palatability of hypertonic sodium chloride solutions (311). However, humans do not couple the unique sensations of salt appetite with the need to ingest sodium as strongly as they do thirst with the need to ingest water. For example, people who excrete large quantities of sodium in their urine due to adrenal insufficiency (e.g., Addison’s disease) or to salt-wasting renal disorders are more likely to develop a craving for salty foods than for salt itself (261, 291, 315, 356). They may simply report that the salty foods have a pleasant taste (261). Anorexia, aberrations of taste, and fatigue are also common symptoms of sodium deficiency in humans (20, 201, 205–207, 231, 276). The ability to selectively deplete the intracellular and extracellular fluid compartments has greatly advanced understanding of the afferent signaling mechanisms responsible for generating the CNS states associated with the motivation to ingest water and sodium. Thirst caused by depletion of the intracellular compartment is achieved by introducing into the extracellular space substances, such as NaCl (127), that are excluded from cells by the semipermeable properties of the cell plasma membrane, thus drawing water out of cells. Relative depletion of the extracellular compartment, or hypovolemia, is produced by experimental procedures that remove isotonic extracellular fluid so as to reduce circulating blood volume without causing loss of fluid from cells. For example, Fitzsimons (102) produced thirst in rats by injecting large molecular weight substances (e.g., polyethylene glycol, gum acacia) under the skin. The oncotic pressure at the injection site of such colloids progressively sequesters extracellular fluid. Since the sequestered fluid is isotonic, osmotic equilibrium is not affected, and there is no movement of intracellular water. Extracellular fluid depletion can also be produced by removal of sodium from the extracellular spaces by glucose dialysis (50, 88), thus diluting the extracellular fluid and shifting water into the cells. Similar shifts in body fluid are produced by maintaining animals on sodium-deficient diets (141, 310). It has been recognized for several decades that individuals suffering dehydration from heat or exercise stop drinking water before blood volume is fully restored. In order to restore blood volume, it is necessary to also provide a source of sodium ions. The phenomenon of ceasing to drink water before extracellular volume is restored was first defined as voluntary dehydration by Adolph and associates (3), but has also been referred to as involuntary dehydration (131). Stricker and colleagues (292, 302) have modeled this inhibition of drinking in the hypovolemic rat and have demonstrated that the attenuated water intake can be resolved by the ingestion of sodium (300, 302). Typically when given the choice, hypovolemic rats consume water (i.e., display thirst) in preference to concentrated saline solutions for the first several hours after extracellular fluid loss. Only later will such animals begin to consume concentrated saline solutions. Over the long term, hypovolemic animals ingest water
FIN 153
296
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
and concentrated NaCl solutions in a ratio that produces the equivalent of isotonic saline, which is, of course, the ideal concentration to restore a normovolemic state. The experimental production of salt appetite dates to Richter’s pioneering work demonstrating that adrenalectomy produces ingestion of sodium solutions in the rat (259). Adrenalectomy removes the principal source of the mineralocorticoid hormone, aldosterone, resulting in loss of sodium in the urine, saliva, and feces. Thus, the absence of salt-retaining mineralocorticoid hormones results in compensatory salt-ingesting behaviors. However, pharmacological doses of mineralocorticoids also stimulate a powerful salt appetite (260). This appears paradoxical in the light of the marked retention of sodium that is initiated by aldosterone administration. Thus, either the absence or the excess of mineralocorticoid hormones causes sodium intake (119). Salt appetite is also produced by the same hypovolemic treatments that stimulate thirst. Administration of diuretics (148), hyperoncotic colloids given ip or sc (307), and glucose dialysis (88) reliably stimulate salt appetite in rats. In sheep, saliva can be diverted from a parotid gland to create a considerable loss of sodium and, in turn, impressive intakes (65).
SYSTEMIC STIMULI, RECEPTOR SYSTEMS AND AFFERENT SIGNALING PATHWAYS Osmoreceptors and Sodium Receptors
Gilman (127) concluded that any hypertonic solute that was excluded from cells was a potent stimulus for thirst. The classic studies by Verney (345) bolstered the concept of effective osmolality (i.e., increased extracellular osmolality produced by solutes that do not cross the cell membrane) by demonstrating that intracarotid injections of highly concentrated (i.e., supraphysiological) sodium chloride, dextrose, or sodium sulfate, but not urea, caused antidiuresis. A more precise demonstration of the importance of effective osmoles in producing antidiuresis was also accomplished by administering very low concentrations of NaCl and sucrose by the same intraarterial route. At osmolar concentrations that could be considered physiological, both substances produced antidiuresis. Observing this effect, Verney introduced the term osmoreceptors to describe the cells controlling VP release that shrink in response to hyperosmotic agents (345). A new understanding of the nature of the blood–brain barrier and the rate of exchange of water and hypertonic substances across it prompted Andersson (6) to suggest that the Verney osmoreceptor was really a sodium receptor. Andersson pointed out that systemically administered hypertonic solutions, irrespective of the nature of their molecular characteristics, increase the sodium concentration of brain extracellular fluid because of the movement of water out of the CNS. In support of this idea, Andersson and colleagues (7, 244) demonstrated that intracerebroventricular (icv) infusions of hypertonic glucose and
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
297
hypertonic sucrose, in contrast to hypertonic saline, were ineffective osmotic agents for stimulating thirst and antidiuresis. However, McKinley and colleagues (208) subsequently pointed out that administering solutions composed of only hyperosmolar glucose and sucrose dilutes extracellular sodium, possibly disrupting neural function nonspecifically. McKinley et al. (208, 210) supported this idea in experiments using icv infusions of hypertonic solutions made by adding sucrose to artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF). The result was that the hypertonic sucrose solution made with ACSF was a potent osmotic stimulus in contrast to the hypertonic sucrose solution made with water. However, it is notable that hypertonic solutions composed of sucrose or fructose in ACSF were still less effective stimuli than equiosmolar hypertonic solutions made with NaCl added to ACSF. Therefore, these investigators (210) argued that both osmo- and sodium receptors may be present in brain. The remarkable discovery by Andersson (5) that injection of hypertonic saline into the brain of goat elicited drinking, coupled with the work of Verney (345), focused early research on identifying osmo- and Na1 receptors in the brain. However, recent work has emphasized the importance of osmo- and Na1 receptors located in the periphery, particularly in the splanchnic-hepatoportal circulation (see 33 for review). Studies by Haberich and his colleagues (135) drew attention to the importance of hepatic osmo- and Na1 receptors for the maintenance of body fluid balance. Haberich (135) demonstrated that water produced diuresis more effectively when administered into the portal vein than systemically via the vena cava. Furthermore, portal injection of hypertonic saline was more effective than systemic delivery in evoking antidiuresis. Vagal afferent nerve activity is altered by hyper- and hypoosmotic injections into the portal vein (2, 8, 236, 265). Baertschi and Vallet (12) recorded increased firing rates from fibers in the hypothalamo-neurohypophyseal tract in response to hyperosmotic solutions administered into the portal vein, and similar treatment elevates plasma levels of VP (12, 49). In rats, VP is released in response to intragastric administration of hypertonic saline solutions in quantities that increase osmolality in the splanchnic vein and hepatoportal regions, but not systemically. Infusions of hypertonic NaCl into the portal vein cause an hepatorenal reflex in several species as shown by increased natriuresis and a fall in renal nerve activity (62, 230, 246). It has been argued that such reflex responses are due to sodium receptors in the hepatoportal region because administration of hypertonic sucrose into the portal vein has no effect on renal nerve activity (230). Taken together such results indicate the presence of receptors in the liver, portal vein, and/or the mesenteric veins that drain the upper small intestine (48). There is a role for peripheral osmo- and Na1 receptors in the control of thirst and salt appetite. Blake and Lin (28) observed that the quantity of isotonic saline consumed by rats following 24 h of water deprivation was reduced by hepatic portal vein infusions of hypertonic saline, and this reduction was abolished by right vagotomy. Tordoff and colleagues (333, 334) have also shown that salt appetite in sodium deficient rats was reduced by saline infusions into the portal vein at concentrations as low as isotonic (0.15 M). Recently, Kraly
FIN 153
298
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
and co-workers (182) demonstrated that water intake is elicited by intragastric administration of hypertonic saline at concentrations that do not increase systemic osmolality. Total subdiaphragmatic vagotomy nearly abolishes drinking after such dipsogenic intragastric infusions (182). There are important functional implications of the presence of osmoreceptors located in the splanchnic region. As Haberich (135) has pointed out, the portal circulation provides an ‘‘advanced post in a dangerous front line between the enteral and parenteral spaces of the body.’’ It is clearly advantageous to have these early warning mechanisms that activate reflex and behavioral responses to buffer large changes in both systemic and brain osmolality due to ingested solute. However, these mechanisms would not be of notable advantage when osmolality is increased throughout the body as a result of renal or evaporative water loss.
Baroreceptors
Hypovolemia activates autonomic and endocrine reflexes that mitigate the negative effects of disrupted body fluid homeostasis. In the face of progressively declining cardiac output that accompanies even modest depletions, sympathetic reflexes maintain arterial pressure (134) by increasing heart rate and causing vasoconstriction. Sympathetic responses to mild hypovolemia also increase proximal tubular reabsorption of sodium mediated by the action of norepinephrine (NE) on a1-noradrenergic receptors (67). Hemorrhage activates several endocrine mechanisms including the release of renin, VP, oxytocin (OXY), aldosterone, epinephrine, NE, adrenocorticotropic hormone, and glucocorticoids. These renal, adrenal, and pituitary hormones work with the autonomic nervous system to decrease water and sodium loss, adjust the distribution of water between intra- and extracellular fluid compartments, and modify regional hemodynamics to optimize available resources. Reflex and the behavioral responses that defend against hypovolemia are activated by reduced activity of vascular baroreceptor afferents and increased circulating ANG II. Systemic vascular receptors are located on both the venous and the arterial sides of the circulation. The vascular baroreceptors located in the heart, great veins, and pulmonary circulation are referred to as cardiopulmonary receptors. The application of the single descriptor, cardiopulmonary receptors, to define these sensory elements is misleading because they comprise several types of receptors in various locations that have both myelinated and unmyelinated afferent fibers. The most thoroughly investigated cardiopulmonary receptors are those located in the great veins and atria that detect changes in blood volume. Another set of important cardiopulmonary receptors is located in the cardiac ventricles. The rate and magnitude of extracellular fluid loss determine whether hypovolemia is accompanied by hypotension. At the onset of hypovolemia, sympathetically mediated vasoconstriction maintains blood pressure in the face of falling cardiac output. However, there appears to be a threshold at which extracellular
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
299
fluid loss (and/or hypotension) produces an abrupt withdrawal of sympathetic tone to the vasculature and an accompanying marked bradycardia (277). This seemingly paradoxical sympatholytic response has been attributed by Thoren (314) to activation of cardiac ventricular receptors as the heart contracts around an empty ventricular chamber. It is thought that the same ventricular receptors are stimulated by agents such as serotonin (5-HT) acting on 5-HT3 receptors, nicotine and bradykinin (343, also see 369 for review). The shift from sympathetic activation to sympathetic inhibition which accompanies the progression from compensated to uncompensated hypovolemia appears to have a behavioral counterpart. As will be discussed later in this review, modest reductions (i.e., 20–40 mm Hg) of blood pressure below normal resting levels increase salt appetite. In contrast, experimental manipulations which produce severe hypotension inhibit salt appetite (see 170 for review of this issue). Additional sets of baroreceptors located in the aortic arch and carotid sinus (i.e., sinoaortic baroreceptors) respond to changes in arterial pressure. The role of these arterial baroreceptors has been well characterized in the reflex control of sympathetic activity and hormone (e.g., VP and renin) release, but a role for these afferent mechanisms in the behavioral control of fluid balance is just beginning to be explored (170, 254, 306). Reductions in venous return to the heart that result in so-called unloading of cardiac baroreceptors stimulate drinking in dogs. Reductions in venous return can be produced experimentally by constriction of a cuff placed around the vena cava (254, 319) or by inflation of a balloon surgically implanted inside the vena cava (109). In either case, blood flow through the vena cava is impeded, producing a fall in venous return and often a reduction in arterial blood pressure. Drinking in response to constriction of the vena cava appears to be mediated by baroreceptor afferents because this drinking is reduced in dogs that have undergone surgical transection of the nerves supplying either the cardiopulmonary or the sinoaortic baroreceptors (254) and is abolished by combined denervation of both sets of baroreceptors. In the absence of input from cardiopulmonary and sinoaortic baroreceptors, constriction of the vena cava does not cause drinking even though angiotensin levels are substantially elevated (254). Zimmerman et al. (368) demonstrated that crushing the left atrial appendage with its associated cardiac receptors reduced water drinking of hypovolemic sheep. These findings are consistent with a stimulatory role of these neural afferents for drinking in response to extracellular fluid loss. Loading of cardiac baroreceptors can be mimicked by inflation of an intravascular balloon placed at the junction of the left pulmonary vein and the left atrium. The mechanical stimulation of these receptors reduces spontaneous water intake in dogs and also reduces water intake in response to isoproterenol treatment, iv infusions of hypertonic saline, and water deprivation (228). Similar results for extracellular dehydration-related drinking have been obtained in rats after inflation of a balloon placed at the junction of the superior vena cava and right atrium (175). These results suggest that cardiopulmonary receptors provide signals that inhibit water intake when body fluids are
FIN 153
300
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
expanded. In dog, temporary reductions of cardiopulmonary activity by vagal blockade increase water intake in response to reduced venous return (109). An understanding of the role of systemic baroreceptor mechanisms in thirst and sodium appetite is very limited compared to current knowledge about autonomic and endocrine reflexes controlling fluid balance. Furthermore, the relatively slow progress in elucidating baroreceptor mechanisms in the control of thirst and salt appetite can be contrasted with more rapid advances in the understanding of hormonal signals of thirst and salt appetite. Particularly relevant is the discovery of the potent dipsogenic (103) and natriorexigenic (36, 37) actions of ANG II.
The Peripheral (i.e., Endocrine) Renin–Angiotensin System (RAS) and Thirst and Salt Appetite
The steady stream of evidence implicating circulating ANG II as a major afferent signal for initiating thirst and salt appetite has largely been the result of the seminal work of Fitzsimons (103) and his colleagues. In early experiments attempting to identify the vascular receptors for hypovolemic thirst, Fitzsimons (103) discovered a renal thirst mechanism. Normovolemic rats drink substantial quantities of water when cardiac return is reduced by ligating the inferior vena cava or when renal blood flow is compromised by coarctation of the descending aorta above the renal arteries. These dipsogenic responses are markedly diminished by nephrectomy (103). As further evidence for a role of the kidney in thirst, Fitzsimons demonstrated that renal extracts contained a thirst factor which he determined by bioassay to be identical to renin (103). It was subsequently shown that systemically administered renin (103) and ANG II (110) are dipsogenic. Hypotension, hypovolemia, and various pharmacological (e.g., isoproterenol, phentolamine) treatments which activate the RAS induce drinking (see 104, 107 for review). Fitzsimons and Simons (110) initially infused high doses of ANG II iv (0.05 to 3.0 nmol/kg/min for periods of 30 to 310 min) to generate drinking. Using refined behavioral methods, Hsiao et al. (144) demonstrated that drinking could be induced reliably in rats with an iv dose of approximately 25 pmol/kg/ min. While this dose is still large and probably in the high physiological range, it is markedly lower than the initial quantities used to induce thirst. Mann et al. (198) infused ANG II iv at the rates used by Hsiao et al. (144) and found that resting plasma levels of ANG II increased from approximately 100 to roughly 200 pmol/ml during the infusion. In the time since ANG II was first implicated as a dipsogen, there have been several challenges to the idea that the octapeptide has a physiological role in drinking (1, 10, 247, 293, 295, 341). Concerns raised by various investigators have included (i) the need to administer large systemic doses of renin or ANG II to induce drinking; (ii) reports by some investigators that only small increases in intake are obtained with renin or ANG II; and (iii) the alternative possibility that circulating ANG II in certain thirst-inducing paradigms (e.g., isoproter-
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
301
enol) is not directly dipsogenic but plays a permissive role by maintaining blood pressure at levels compatible with drinking behavior. In this light, it is instructive to consider how the plasma levels of ANG II generated by various thirst-inducing manipulations or during physiological states in which drinking is stimulated compare with those generated by Hsiao and associates (144; see Fig. 1). For example, circulating levels of ANG II reach roughly 350, 400, 400, and 875 pmol/ml plasma by 8 h after sc treatment with 5 ml of 20% polyethylene glycol, 48 h of water deprivation, 30 min after isoproterenol treatment (30 µg/kg, sc), and 1 h after caval ligation, respectively (166, 198). These data indicate that at the time that animals would normally initiate drinking in response to these dipsogenic treatments, plasma levels of angiotensin surpass the empirically established dipsogenic threshold for iv infused ANG II. Therefore, it is logically consistent that circulating ANG II makes a contribution to the drinking response after these dipsogenic treatments (168, 199). In
FIG. 1. Summary indicating plasma levels of angiotensin II (ANG II) obtained following various challenges to activate the endogenous renin–angiotensin system. These are presented on the regression line of the relationship between infused ANG II dose (abscissa) and circulating level of octapeptide (ordinate). Intact-25 pmol/kg/min and Intact-100 pmol/kg/min indicate levels of circulating ANG II after 60-min infusions of the indicated dose into unanesthetized rats. Lower crosshatched areas represent the range of control values of plasma ANG II. Upper crosshatched area represents approximate ANG II plasma level at dipsogenic threshold. The threshold at which ANG II induces drinking with iv infusion was established by Hsaio et al. (144). Abbreviations: DI, diabetes insipidus; RH, renal hypertension; PEG, polyethylene glycol; ISOP, isoproterenol. From Johnson and Thunhorst (170) with permission of the publisher.
FIN 153
302
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
addition as will be considered in a later section, it is also important to note that arterial pressure is a critical factor that influences the dipsogenic potency of ANG II (82, 170). The role of the endocrine RAS in salt appetite has also been controversial (107). Surgical procedures that alter the activity of the RAS such as nephrectomy, adrenalectomy, renal artery constriction, or ureteric ligation have marked effects on sodium intake. However, it is not clear if the effects of these surgical procedures on sodium intake can be attributed to changes in renin secretion and the production of ANG II. Thus, the effectiveness of nephrectomy in completely abolishing salt appetite induced by hypovolemia may be due to the loss of the source of renin, to anuria, or to general debilitation (111, 304). The robust salt appetite following excision of the adrenal glands with the consequent loss of mineralocorticoid may be directly related to increased renin secretion and formation of ANG II, indirectly related to a massive loss of sodium in the urine, to changes in salivary sodium content (118), or to reduced sodium concentration at sodium receptors in the brain (352). Clipping the renal arteries produces salt appetite preceded by increased renin secretion (226). However, clipping of the renal arteries also produces hypertension, so the salt appetite produced in these circumstances may be a pathophysiological response. Ligation of the ureters suppresses salt appetite, despite markedly increasing renin secretion, but it is not known if the attenuation of salt appetite is due primarily to alterations of renal function or to the release of unidentified inhibitory factors (111, 220). Drug treatments that target the endocrine RAS produce ambiguous effects on salt appetite. For example, salt appetite of sodium-depleted rats has not been shown to be prevented by pharmacological blockade of the renal RAS with ANG II receptor antagonist (272). Doses of systemically administered angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) inhibitor that reduce the salt appetite of sodium deficient rats are typically so high that their suppressive actions have been attributed to inhibition of the brain RAS rather than the peripheral RAS (224, 272). Intravenous infusions of ANG II do not reliably stimulate salt appetite in sodium-replete rats (95, 112, 272) or rabbits (312), although they may in sheep (353). In any case, the sodium ingestion produced by iv ANG II has been attributed to indirect effects including angiotensin-induced pressure natriuresis and slowly developing negative sodium balance (312, 353, 362). The early evidence supported the assertion that the central, or brain, RAS is more important than the endocrine RAS in producing salt appetite. Centrally administered ANG II readily stimulates salt appetite without prior natriuresis and negative sodium balance (11, 36, 37, 46, 113, 114). Furthermore, centrally administered ACE inhibitors and ANG II receptor antagonists easily inhibit depletion-induced salt appetite (43, 224, 274, 354). Circulating ANG II was ascribed a minor role in the production of salt appetite. For example, Epstein and colleagues (80, 272, 362) proposed in the synergy hypothesis of salt appetite that the role of renally derived ANG II is in essence only to stimulate the secretion of aldosterone following sodium depletion and that brain-derived
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
303
ANG II synergized with aldosterone to produce salt appetite through their combined actions within the CNS. The first convincing demonstrations that systemically administered ANG II produces salt appetite not attributable to its indirect effects were provided by Weisinger and colleagues in sodium deficient cow (27), sheep (351), rabbit (313), and mouse (348). The sodium-deficient animals were treated with doses of ACE inhibitors sufficient to prevent the endogenous formation of ANG II both peripherally and centrally. In all cases, blockade of the endogenous RAS reduced or abolished salt appetite. Subsequently, systemic administration of ANG II to these ACE-blocked, sodium-deficient animals either partially or fully restored salt appetite without producing additional sodium loss. These results were taken as evidence that during times of sodium deficiency increased levels of circulating ANG II could stimulate salt appetite, probably by acting on brain areas that lie outside of the blood–brain barrier, namely, the circumventricular organs (CVOs). Recent work has extended the findings of Weisinger and co-workers to the sodium-deficient rat. In this demonstration, rats were depleted of sodium by diuretics and then infused iv either with captopril (CAP; 2.5 mg/h) or the vehicle (5% dextrose in water [D5W]; 322). Upon presentation of 0.3 M NaCl, rats infused with D5W showed a robust salt appetite (Fig. 2) while rats infused with CAP had no salt appetite at all. Importantly, both groups of rats showed normal water intakes and pressor responses to icv injections of angiotensin I (ANG I) after the salt appetite tests. This confirmed that the brain RAS remained unblocked in CAP-treated rats and indicated that selective blockade of the endocrine RAS is sufficient to abolish depletion-induced salt appetite. Subsequent work showed that the salt appetite response of sodium-depleted, CAP-blocked rats is reestablished within minutes by iv infusion of ANG II (99). Together, these findings indicate that salt appetite after sodium depletion in rats is dependent on the presence of circulating ANG II. Therefore, rather than being of minimal importance, the endocrine RAS may be a critical factor for stimulating salt appetite after periods of sodium depletion in rats as well as in other species. To suggest that the endocrine RAS has an essential role in depletion-induced salt appetite does not minimize the importance of a brain RAS in this behavior. If the peripheral and central RASs function in series, as has been proposed for thirst and other endocrine and autonomic responses (152, 188), then both may be required for the expression of salt appetite (320, 322).
Arterial Blood Pressure and Blood Volume Control of Thirst and Salt Appetite
New experimental strategies have made it possible to begin to establish the relative roles of hormonal and baroreceptor signals in the mediation of fluid ingestion. Furthermore, increased attention is being paid to interactions be-
FIN 153
304
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
FIG. 2. Cumulative intake of 0.3 M NaCl solution beginning 18 h after furosemide-induced sodium depletion in groups of naive rats receiving continuous intravenous infusions of 5% dextrose in water (D5W) vehicle or 2.5 mg/h captopril (CAP) during the entire period of depletion and testing. Values are means 6 SE. Captopril infusion completely suppressed sodium depletioninduced salt appetite. From Thunhorst and Fitts (322) with permission of the publisher.
tween these two classes of afferent signals in the control of body fluid regulation (82, 170, 299, 326, 328). ANG II has been regarded as a dipsogenic hormone since the demonstration by Fitzsimons and Simons (110) that its iv administration produces drinking in rats. Although the dipsogenic response to iv infusions of ANG II has been confirmed in several species by many investigators (54, 106, 285, 337), the often weak nature of the dipsogenic response has been considered evidence against a physiological role of the renal RAS in the control of drinking behavior (293, 295, 341). For example, Van Eekelen and Phillips (341) note that the doses of iv ANG II that are required to produce even modest dipsogenic responses result in plasma levels of the hormone that are in the high physiological range. However, evidence (82, 87, 263) now indicates that the dipsogenic potency of iv ANG II has been underestimated due to the confounding influences of acute hypertension on drinking behavior. In fluid-replete animals in normal hydromineral balance, exogenous ANG II often produces nonphysiological increases of arterial pressure (82). Under natural conditions involving hypovolemia, the reflex increase in circulating levels of endogenous ANG II serves to maintain arterial
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
305
pressure at approximately normotensive levels. A greater experimental drinking response to iv infusion of ANG II is unmasked if arterial pressure is normalized by the coadministration of hypotensive agents (87, 263; see Fig. 3). The pressure-induced inhibition of water drinking in response to ANG II appears to be mediated by cardiopulmonary or arterial baroreceptors, as the combined surgical removal of both sets of these neural sensors in dogs permits a greater drinking response to iv ANG II (178). In rats, removal of only the arterial baroreceptors does not affect the drinking response to systemically administered angiotensin (173, 257). Therefore, the data suggest that cardiopulmonary baroreceptors or some combination of cardiopulmonary and arterial baroreceptor signals act as inhibitory modulators of drinking behavior when arterial pressure is elevated.
FIG. 3. Effects of minoxidil on mean arterial pressure (MAP) and water intake of rats infused intravenously with a combination of ANG II and captopril for 90 min. Captopril prevented the endogenous formation of ANG II during testing. Vertical arrow indicates time of injection of minoxidil or isotonic saline control. Rats drank significantly more water during infusion with ANG II when MAP was reduced by minoxidil. Values are mean 6 SE. *Significantly greater than that of saline control, p , 0.05. The number of rats is in parentheses. From Robinson and Evered (263) with permission of the publisher.
FIN 153
306
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
Elevation of arterial pressure also inhibits drinking in response to centrally administered ANG II. Ganglionic blockade simultaneously augments pressor responses and decreases drinking responses to central injections of ANG II in dogs (183). Rats of the Brattleboro strain drink more in response to central injections of ANG II compared to Long–Evans rats (136), possibly reflecting their genetic lack of VP and consequent diminished pressor responses to central injections of ANG II (136, 167). Moderate reductions in arterial pressure below normal resting levels increase drinking in response to centrally administered ANG II (326). In these studies, rats received central infusions of relatively low doses of ANG II (4 or 16 ng/h) with arterial pressure at resting levels or reduced by iv infusion of minoxidil (25 µg/kg/min). With a high dose of ACE inhibitor (CAP, 0.33 mg/min) included in both infusions to prevent the endogenous formation of ANG II during testing, centrally administered ANG II produced dose-related increases in water intake at resting levels of arterial pressure (Fig. 4), and notably the dipsogenic response to centrally administered ANG II doubled under hypotensive conditions. If the level of arterial pressure modulates angiotensin-induced drinking, it is reasonable to hypothesize that carotid sinus and aortic arch baroreceptors are
FIG. 4. Effects of iv captopril and minoxidil on water intakes in response to icv ANG II. Rats received iv infusions of captopril (CAPTOPRIL) or minoxidil in combination with captopril (MINOXCAP) beginning at t0. Separate groups received icv infusions of ANG II at 4 (A) or 16 (B) ng/h starting at t60 (broken lines). Each dose of icv ANG II produced water intakes that were greater during MINOXCAP treatment, when arterial pressure was reduced below normal resting levels, than during captopril treatment, when pressure was normal. Water intakes to icv ANG II were significantly dose-related. From Thunhorst and Johnson (326) with permission of the publisher.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
307
involved. However, deafferenting the arterial baroreceptors did not disrupt the ability of arterial pressure to modulate drinking in response to ICV ANG II (330; Fig. 5). The potential contribution of other systemic baroreceptors (e.g., cardiopulmonary baroreceptors, renal baroreceptors; 290) in mediating the influences of arterial pressure on fluid ingestion remains to be investigated. Little is known about potential interactions between hormonal and baroreceptor signals controlling salt appetite. Substantial evidence suggests that ANG II and aldosterone stimulate salt appetite but that neither hormone is strictly essential for the response (118, 296), so that other mechanisms must regulate salt appetite, including cerebral sodium sensors (352) and possibly baroreceptors. Evidence for a role of baroreceptors in salt appetite is largely circumstantial. Almost without exception, such evidence relies on producing hypovolemic/ hypotensive conditions known to alter baroreceptor activity, but few experiments have asked whether animals lacking baroreceptors show salt appetite in response to hypovolemic/hypotensive conditions. Similarly, the finding that distention of the right atrium in the rat inhibits salt appetite has been interpreted as evidence for inhibitory neural signals arising from atrial barore-
FIG. 5. Effects of iv captopril, minoxidil, and phenylephrine on water intake in response to icv infusions of ANG II (16 ng/h) in sham and sinoaortic denervated (SAD) rats. Water intake was significantly increased when mean arterial pressure (MAP) was reduced below normal resting levels during iv minoxidil plus captopril treatment and was significantly decreased when MAP was elevated during phenylephrine plus captopril treatment, compared with water intake during vehicle infusions of captopril. Sinoaortic denervation did not affect water intake in response to icv infusion of ANG II. From Thunhorst et al. (330) with permission of the publisher.
FIN 153
308
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
ceptors under conditions of volume expansion; alternatively, this may reflect release of inhibitory hormones from the atrium itself (335). To help resolve this issue, it must be determined whether distention of the atrium inhibits salt appetite in animals with deafferented atrial baroreceptors. One difficulty in accepting a role for baroreceptors in the initiation of salt appetite has been the temporal dissociation of hypovolemia and salt appetite (107, 299). Hypovolemic treatments often produce water drinking with delays of only 30–60 min, but sodium ingestion typically commences hours after treatment and only after water intake has occurred. This temporal delay between the hypovolemic stimulus and salt appetite may be an experimental artifact. Stricker (296, 297) suggests that rats maintained on standard laboratory diet with its high sodium content have a reserve of body sodium that acts as a buffer against the extracellular-depleting effects of hypovolemia. When rats are maintained on a sodium-deficient diet for 2–4 days before study, they may show salt appetite with the onset of hypovolemia. The question remains as to how the reserve of body sodium delays the expression of salt appetite when hypovolemia readily produces the ingestion of water. Compared with rats on standard diets, those on sodium-deficient diets have reduced plasma volume and plasma sodium concentrations (296, 301) and increased levels of plasma renin activity and aldosterone (304). When challenged by hypovolemia, sodium-deficient rats have greater reductions in plasma volume, larger increases in plasma renin activity and aldosterone, and more difficulty maintaining arterial pressure in the face of the accruing deficits than rats maintained on standard diet (301, 304). The increased activity of the renin–angiotensin–aldosterone axis in rats on sodium-deficient diets supports the synergy hypothesis for the generation of salt appetite. Furthermore, the difficulty these rats have maintaining arterial pressure, combined with the greater reductions in plasma volume in response to hypovolemia, is consistent with the idea that baroreceptors may contribute facilitatory signals or remove inhibitory inputs under these conditions. Rats maintained on sodium-deficient diets also have blunted secretion of OXY in response to hypovolemia (303), which may reflect reduced activity of central mechanisms inhibiting sodium ingestion. Animals develop salt appetite more rapidly in response to hypovolemia if they are simultaneously made moderately hypotensive. Stricker (297) demonstrated that rats treated with polyethylene glycol showed immediate salt appetite when injected with a low dose of ACE inhibitor. A rapidly developing salt appetite is also produced by administering CAP to rats made hypovolemic by injection of the diuretic furosemide (FURO; 97, 204, 327). These are two examples of ‘‘paradoxically enhanced’’ salt appetite (to be discussed in a later section), and the sodium ingestion in these circumstances is likely due to increased formation of ANG II within brain CVOs (97, 204, 297, 327). It is noteworthy that in both situations peripheral ACE blockade causes a reduction in arterial pressure (123, 327) which may be essential for the rapid induction of salt appetite (327). When infusions of phenylephrine are used to maintain arterial pressure at basal levels during combined treatment with FURO and
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
309
CAP, the salt appetite response is selectively blunted, although water drinking is quite normal (327). It is currently unclear if hypotension facilitates the appearance of salt appetite by changing neural input to critical CNS sites or by further increasing renin secretion to augment formation of ANG II. There are hints in the literature that acute increases in arterial pressure may inhibit salt appetite, as they do thirst, but this possibility has not been adequately explored. For example, iv infusions of ANG II that elevate arterial pressure approximately 40 mm Hg above baseline slightly reduced the salt appetite of sodium-deficient sheep (32). In hypotensive sodium-deficient rats, saline drinking was stimulated soon after the start of ANG II infusion but abated as arterial pressure rose above normal basal levels (99). A pressuremediated inhibition of salt appetite could partly explain why it takes such prolonged iv infusions of ANG II to stimulate salt appetite in fluid-replete rats. There is one report that denervation of the arterial baroreceptors reduces salt appetite in sodium-deplete rats by half (331). It is not known if this reduction in salt appetite following denervation is due directly to the absence of baroreceptor signals, indirectly to altered hormonal responses (e.g., reduced renin secretion), or both.
INTEGRATIVE ACTIONS OF THE CNS Brain Neural Networks and the Reflex and Behavioral Controls of Body Fluid Homeostasis
Mechanisms of Afferent Signaling: Neural and Humoral Inputs Information reflecting the status of body fluids is imparted to the CNS by both afferent nerves and circulating factors. The nucleus of the tractus solitarius (NTS) serves as the major port of access to the brain for most of the systemically derived input carried in the IXth and Xth cranial nerves (239). Small molecules or highly lipophilic substances (e.g., aldosterone) generated in the periphery communicate directly with the brain, and other substances that are excluded from the CNS by the blood–brain barrier (e.g., insulin) may use special transport mechanisms to enter the brain. However, some of the most important systemic signals generated in response to changes in body fluid status, such as the peptides ANG II and atrial natriuretic peptide (ANP), are excluded by the blood–brain barrier and must act via CVOs. In vertebrates, there are several small periventricular structures which lack a blood–brain barrier (see 211, 347 for review of CVOs). Early investigations implicated many of the CVOs (e.g., median eminence, posterior pituitary, pineal) as sites of brain neuroendocrine release into vascular portal systems or the systemic circulation. Additionally, there is substantial evidence that three CVOs, the subfornical organ (SFO), the organum vasculosum of the lamina terminalis (OVLT), and the area postrema (AP), function as receptors of blood-borne information. Much of the insight into the sensory role of these
FIN 153
310
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
structures was acquired in the course of studying the function of the CNS in body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis. The SFO, OVLT and AP can be appropriately referred to as sensory CVOs (154, 164).
The Nature of Neural Processing in Motivated States Such as Thirst and Salt Appetite Fundamental concepts underlying the processing of sensory information by the central nervous system have changed markedly in the past 20 years. There has also been considerable progress in defining the organization and chemical characteristics of many CNS pathways. Neuroanatomical tract tracing techniques and functional mapping methods, such as ablation, electrical stimulation, metabolic activation (e.g., 2-deoxy glucose method), and expression of immediate early genes (e.g., c-fos) have implicated numerous brain structures in the processing of systemically derived information related to intra- and extracellular volume and arterial blood pressure. Most notable among these regions are the (i) AP, (ii) NTS, (iii) caudal and rostral ventrolateral medulla, (iv) parabrachial nuclei, (v) SFO, (vi) periventricular tissue of the anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V) [i.e., ventral median preoptic nucleus (vMePO), OVLT, periventricular preoptic nuclei], (vii) both the magnocellular and the parvocellular components of the hypothalamic paraventricular nuclei, (viii) supraoptic nuclei, (ix) amygdala (particularly central nucleus and medial nucleus), (x) bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), (xi) lateral hypothalamus/perifornical region, (xii) the zona incerta, (xiii) dorsomedial and ventromedial hypothalamus, (xiv) locus coeruleus, (xv) dorsal motor complex of the vagus, and (xvi) intermediolateral cell column of the spinal cord. This work has established a collection of structures and their connections that constitute a body fluid-related neural network that carries and integrates information pertinent to the control of fluid balance. Although every structure and pathway in the brain can, in principle, influence information processing in this neural network, some regions are relatively more important for handling visceral input related to hydromineral balance. Neural networks have several features that are well suited for the control of homeostatic behaviors and physiological mechanisms controlling body fluid balance. The organization of the body fluid-related neural network, in principle, allows visceral information reflecting fluid balance to be broadly distributed from the several relatively small loci that act as neural and endocrine ports of access into the brain. Also, the property of parallel distributed processing of information typical of neural networks provides redundancy so that damage to a portion of the system may not be catastrophic. It is likely that this system is inherently modifiable, exhibiting plasticity associated with recovery of function, and capable of storing information. The central resetting of baroreceptor reflexes is a classic example of plasticity in control of the cardiovascular system, and this capacity for modification is probably a consequence of func-
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
311
tional properties of the neural network maintaining arterial blood pressure. Similar plasticity is likely to exist in the circuitry subserving thirst and salt appetite. For example, chronically enhanced sodium preference and salt appetite may result from a single episode of an acute sodium deficiency (89, 273). Several neurotransmitters/neuromodulators associated with the body fluidrelated neural network have been identified. These include (i) glutamate, (ii) g-aminobutyric acid (GABA), (iii) acetylcholine, (iv) NE, (v) 5-HT, (vi) ANG II, (vii) VP, (viii) OXY, and (ix) corticotrophin releasing hormone. The brain renin–angiotensin and noradrenergic systems are broadly distributed in the CNS (21, 187). Together, these two neurochemical systems innervate most of the structures implicated in the body fluid-related neural network. One can conceive of many of the aforementioned neurotransmitters/neuromodulators inputting into nodes of this network as components of local circuits associated with specific processing of facilitory or inhibitory information. There is an emerging body of data indicating a regional localization of function (processing modules) in the neural network subserving thirst and salt appetite. In particular, forebrain structures including those located along the lamina terminalis appear to play a facilitory role in the generation of thirst and sodium appetite. A second set of findings implicates a hindbrain system, particularly the AP, the NTS, and the parabrachial nucleus (56, 283) as part of the neural substrate providing inhibitory modulation of these behavioral responses under conditions of expanded extracellular fluid volume. These excitatory and inhibitory systems undoubtedly converge and interact at many nodes within the body fluid-related neural network, although for heuristic purposes the following discussion is organized to describe these facilitory and inhibitory circuits independently.
Facilitory Neural Circuits and Neurochemical Systems
Role of Structures Along the Lamina Terminalis in Body Fluid Regulation Early in embryonic development, the rostral end of the neural tube is closed by a layer of ependymal cells. Technically this band of cells comprises the lamina terminalis and in mature vertebrates constitutes the anterior wall of the third cerebral ventricle. Adjacent to the lamina terminalis are three midline nuclei—the SFO, the MePO, and the OVLT—and the anterior commissure. Morphological studies of the SFO by Dietrickx (68) and Palkovitz (245) led them to the prescient speculation that this CVO might be involved in sodium and water homeostasis. Substantial support for this idea was provided by the work of Simpson and Routtenberg (286), who demonstrated that stimulation of the SFO with ANG II produces reliable drinking with extremely low doses of the peptide. However, there is evidence that the SFO is not the only site of dipsogenic action for ANG II in the brain. For example, rats will drink in
FIN 153
312
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
response to icv ANG II after SFO lesions provided that cerebrospinal fluidborne peptide reaches the rostral portion of the ventral third ventricle (i.e., the preoptic recess) (38). Ablation studies demonstrated that lesions destroying rostral nuclei surrounding the ventral third ventricle abolish drinking to both systemically and centrally injected ANG II (41, 42, 157, 158). Such lesions also have several other major effects on the regulation of body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis (40–42, 158). The specific periventricular tissue destroyed by this ablation, which has been defined as an AV3V lesion, encompasses the OVLT, the vMePO, the periventricular preoptic nuclei, and the periventricular nuclei at the rostral level of the anterior hypothalamus. The name AV3V was chosen to describe the lesion because its constellation of functionally defined effects is not localizable to a single anatomical structure. Both the acute and the chronic effects of AV3V lesions on body fluid and cardiovascular regulation have been studied extensively and various aspects have been summarized in several reviews (34, 57, 149, 151–153, 157, 160–163, 366). Only a few of the most salient features will be discussed here. Some prominent effects of AV3V lesions are summarized in Table 1. Immediately after placement of AV3V lesions, animals show an acute period of adipsia and do not release VP in the face of severe hypovolemia and hypertonicity. Animals with AV3V lesions will die of dehydration typically within 5–7 days unless they are hydrated. Although rats with AV3V lesions do not consume TABLE 1 Prominent Effects of AV3V Lesions Acute effects Adipsia Impaired secretion of vasopressin Severe weight loss and if untreated, debilitation and death due to dehydration Chronic effects Reductions in body weight Recovery of ad libitum drinking Impaired drinking responses to experimental challenges Attenuated water deprivation-induced drinking Abolished cellular dehydration-induced drinking Extracellular thirst Abolished angiotensin II-induced drinking Attenuated isoproterenol-induced drinking Attenuated caval ligation-induced drinking Attenuated polyethylene glycol treatment-induced drinking Altered ad libitum sodium intake Impaired sodium depletion-induced salt appetite Impaired vasopressin secretion to hypertonic saline and ANG II Impaired natriuresis Impaired pressor responses to hypertonic saline and ANG II Protection against most forms of experimentally-induced hypertension Hypernatremia
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
313
water acutely, they continue to eat and will readily drink palatable fluids (i.e., sucrose, saccharin, and liquid diets) in sufficient quantities to survive. Gradually, over the course of 7 to 10 days, the initially adipsic rats can be ‘‘weaned’’ back to water. Nearly all animals maintained through the acute postlesion period recover ad libitum water intake and sustain themselves in the benign laboratory environment. On average, the mean daily water intake of a group of ‘‘recovered’’ animals with AV3V lesions is comparable to that of animals with sham lesions. However, on a day to day basis, the consumption of individual animals during the chronic phase varies widely (190). For the rest of their lives, animals with AV3V lesions show severe impairments in thirst, VP release, and pressor responses to several cardiovascular and fluid regulatory challenges. Especially affected are reflex and behavioral responses to humoral factors (i.e., ANG II and increased plasma osmolality). Although animals with AV3V lesions respond to sustained hypovolemic challenges as long as there is a significant contribution from vascular baroreceptors (41), drinking is still impaired (190), presumably due to the loss of response to ANG II which contributes to hypovolemic thirst (166). As in intact rats, recovered animals with AV3V lesions have a high probability of drinking in association with food intake (i.e., meal-associated thirst). However, unlike control animals, ‘‘recovered’’ AV3V animals do not demonstrate a strong positive correlation between meal size and the amount of water consumed (16). Rats with AV3V lesions appear to lack the fine control of water intake under ad libitum conditions which may reflect impaired responsiveness to the humoral (i.e., osmolality and ANG II) signals of hydrational status.
Osmoreceptors and the AV3V Based on functional studies showing that rats with AV3V lesions have impaired drinking, antidiuretic responses, and pressor responses in the face of plasma hyperosmolality or to hypertonic stimuli, it was proposed that the AV3V region houses osmoreceptive cells (41, 158, 165). One classic qualification for establishing a region as a site housing osmoreceptors is that electrolytic lesions of the region should impair drinking and/or antidiuretic responses to hyperosmotic stimulation. Although this criterion is important, it is not sufficient to define the location of osmoreceptors. Electrolytic lesions do not distinguish between destruction of cell soma (i.e., in this case, osmoreceptors) and fibers of passage. However, the hypothesis of the AV3V as a site containing osmoreceptors received further support from additional functional studies which compared the sensitivity of the lateral preoptic area, a region implicated earlier as osmosensitive (29, 248), with the effects of injections into the AV3V. Injections into the optic recess of the third ventricle were markedly more effective in inducing drinking than lateral preoptic administrations (39). Experiments by Thrasher and colleagues in dog and McKinley and coworkers in sheep that employed both systemic and icv administration of
FIN 153
314
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
osmo-active solutions led these investigators to conclude that cephalic osmoreceptors probably reside within one or more of the CVOs (210, 317). As noted above, the OVLT is the CVO consistently destroyed by AV3V lesions, and AV3V lesions are effective in markedly attenuating or abolishing both osmotically evoked antidiuretic responses mediated by VP release (151, 165) and cellular dehydration-induced drinking (39, 41). Consequently, the OVLT emerged as a logical choice for a structure housing osmoreceptors mediating antidiuretic hormone release and thirst. Given its small size and location, it is difficult to ablate the entire OVLT in the rat without encroaching on adjacent structures. Consequently, investigators have resorted to in vitro methods to study the OVLT in rats or have employed larger animals. One early study used hypothalmo-neurohypophyseal explants from rats with AV3V lesions. The explants were cut in the horizontal plane at the dorsal edge of the OVLT thereby eliminating input from more dorsal structures. In contrast to control explants with intact AV3V regions, the hypothalmo-neurohypophyseal tissue from rats with AV3V lesions (i.e., with nothing dorsal to the OVLT) did not release VP in response to osmotic stimulation with hypertonic ACSF (287). This provided further evidence of involvement of the OVLT, rather than dorsal (i.e., MePO and SFO), lamina terminalis structures as a site for osmoreceptors. Recently, Bourque and co-investigators (see 33 for review) studied rat OVLT neurons in hypothalamic explants under Ca21-free conditions and individual cells after acute dissociation. The application of hypertonic solutions to both of these in vitro preparations depolarizes cells of the OVLT, providing further evidence of the osmosensitivity of OVLT neurons. The depolarization of OVLT neurons results from activation of nonselective cationic channels (33). In vivo studies in sheep and dog support the in vitro rat studies implicating the OVLT as osmosensitive. Ablations restricted to the OVLT markedly raise the threshold and reduce the magnitude of responses for drinking and VP release to hyperosmotic stimulation (209, 318). However, it is necessary to ablate additional AV3V tissue surrounding the OVLT to completely abolish cellular dehydration-induced drinking and VP release (209, 318; see 150, 153 for review and discussion). There is evidence indicating that there are osmosensitive cells in other CVOs (143, 192, 284) as well as in structures located within the blood–brain barrier (33, 202). For example, Bourque et al. (33) presented electrophysiological evidence similar to that described above for osmosensitive units in both the SFO and the MePO. Mangiapane and colleagues (197) have shown that ablation of the MePO of the rat produces deficits in osmotically stimulated drinking and VP release. King and Baertschi (177) using electrolytic lesions argue that input from splanchnic/hepatic osmo- or sodium receptors which ascends the neuraxis via the NTS and ventrolateral medulla projects to the MePO before it proceeds to hypothalamic magnocellular vasopressinergic neurons. Taken together, current data implicate AV3V regions beyond the OVLT in osmoreception and/or the processing of osmotic information.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
315
Angiotensin Action in the Lamina Terminalis Because lesions of both SFO and AV3V attenuate angiotensin-induced pressor responses, drinking and VP release, the relationship between these two areas of the lamina terminalis has been investigated extensively. Tract tracing studies (187, 193, 222, 223) have been especially illuminating by establishing the major neural connections between these two regions. The most prominent pathway in this local circuitry is a large descending projection originating in the SFO that travels down the lamina terminalis into the vMePO and OVLT. In these more ventral regions, close appositions between apparent fiber terminals and cell bodies are frequently observed. This suggests that the MePO and OVLT receive direct input from the SFO. Such anatomical evidence, along with additional functional data, such as the sensitivity of the AV3V region to icv-injected ANG II (38) and the capacity of icv angiotensin receptor antagonists to block blood-borne ANG II-induced drinking (169), has led to a model in which ANG II may act in two different modes along the lamina terminalis (188). First, ANG II acts at the SFO as a hormone with the blood-borne peptide acting on receptors to activate a descending angiotensinergic pathway with terminals ending in several forebrain structures including the vMePO. Second, ANG II is released from the terminals of these descending projections and acts as a neurotransmitter/neuromodulator. This model is also supported by functional studies showing that knife cuts of descending fibers from the SFO abolish drinking and attenuate pressor responses to blood-borne ANG II (78, 189, 191). Such transections have little effect on angiotensin-induced responses when the peptide is administered directly into the ventricles of the forebrain where it can gain access to the periventricular tissue of the AV3V (189). Since the time this model was initially proposed, additional data have been generated that are consistent with the hypothesis. These observations include: (i) the presence of high concentrations of angiotensin receptors located in a heavy band throughout the lamina terminalis (219, 252), (ii) in situ hybridization studies indicating the presence of high levels of angiotensinogen messenger RNA in the AV3V (196), and (iii) additional electrophysiological studies (235, 316, 336) supplementing earlier work (e.g., 90, 133, 180) indicating that there are ANG II sensitive cells located at sites all along the lamina terminalis. In light of an increased understanding of the relationship of the AV3V to other CNS regions that control hydromineral balance, it is possible to understand why AV3V lesions produce such profound, global effects on body fluid and cardiovascular regulation. Lesions of the AV3V are likely to destroy (i) sensory receptors (e.g., osmoreceptors), (ii) fibers of passage, and (iii) cells involved in integration of hydromineral-related sensory input from multiple brain and systemic receptor systems (Fig. 6). Neuroanatomical studies indicate that the OVLT, MePO, and SFO are richly interconnected with one another. Cell bodies in the SFO, MePO, and OVLT are the origins of descending pathways which project directly to several additional forebrain structures. Functional evidence associated with the structures of these terminal fields accounts for many
FIN 153
316
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
FIG. 6. Sensory circumventricular organs (CVOs) associated with the lamina terminalis and descending projections to forebrain regions involved in autonomic, neuroendocrine, and behavioral control of body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis. OVLT, organum vasculosum of the lamina terminalis. From Johnson (154) with permission of the publisher.
known actions of brain angiotensin in the control of thirst, sympathetic activation, and the secretion of VP, OXY, and adrenocorticotropic hormone.
Descending Angiotensinergic and Ascending Adrenergic Systems, and the Integrative Actions of Structures of the Ventral Lamina Terminalis Rats with brain lesions induced by icv injection of the neurotoxin 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) exhibit many of the same signs as rats with AV3V lesions. Specifically, both have (i) chronic reductions in body weight, (ii) transient postsurgical adipsia, (iii) permanent impairments to cellular and extracellular thirst challenges, (iv) pressor deficits and reduced VP release in response to ANG II and hypertonic saline administration, and (v) a failure to develop many forms of experimental hypertension. It is reasonable to consider whether electrolytic AV3V lesions and 6-OHDA ablations produce damage to similar brain structures and to determine which catecholaminergic substrate (dopamine vs. NE) is critical for producing these effects. Initial experiments focused on determining if the depletion of NE or dopamine is critical. Varying doses of
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
317
6-OHDA were administered in conjunction with catecholamine uptake blockers to produce selective widespread depletions of dopamine, NE, or both catecholamines. These studies demonstrated that damage to brain noradrenergic systems was primarily responsible for impairments in angiotensin-induced drinking and pressor responses (17). Additional studies using local injections of 6-OHDA into tissue along the lamina terminalis (i.e., SFO, MePO, OVLT; 18, 19, 58) demonstrated that neurotoxin administration into the OVLT and vMePO impaired drinking and pressor responses evoked by ANG II. However, the presence of NE innervation of the ventral lamina terminalis was not critical for drinking and pressor responses produced by sc hypertonic saline or icv carbachol. ANG II-induced pressor and drinking responses have been restored in rats with 6-OHDA lesions of the ventral lamina terminalis by transplantation of NE cell bodies into the parenchyma of the catecholamine-depleted region (212, 213). Similarly, animals with thirst deficits after 6-OHDA lesions drink in response to icv ANG II when pretreated with icv NE (59). Taken together, the outcomes of catecholamine depletion and repletion studies indicate that NE must be present in the tissues of the ventral lamina terminalis in order to obtain drinking and pressor responses induced by exogenous ANG II. Noradrenergic innervation of the AV3V region arises from the A1, A2, and A 6 catecholamine cell groups located in the hindbrain (275). In turn, these brainstem aminergic cell groups receive reciprocal innervation from the ventral lamina terminalis (i.e., MePO; 364). There are several functional studies indicating that brainstem noradrenergic cell groups are activated by hypotension and hypovolemia (74, 227, 255), and it has been hypothesized (57, 152) that hypovolemia and hypotension activate ascending noradrenergic pathways to release NE into the ventral lamina terminalis region. Consistent with this is the observation that hypovolemia induced by sc polyethylene glycol treatment increases NE turnover in the MePO (355). NE has been demonstrated to modulate information processing in several CNS structures including nuclei in primary sensory pathways (e.g., vision, olfaction, 147, 264). At these sites, NE reduces the basal level of firing and thereby enhances the signal-to-noise ratio in response to a stimulus evoking activity in the primary sensory pathway (115, 147, 361). Therefore, NE may have a similar action to facilitate throughput in the angiotensinergic sensory pathway that connects the SFO via the MePO with the rest of the neural network that maintains body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis.
Converting Enzyme Inhibitors and the Paradoxical Enhancement of Thirst and Salt Appetite: The Role of Local ANG II Generation
Lehr et al. (186) observed what they termed ‘‘paradoxical enhancement’’ of drinking while using the competitive ACE inhibitor teprotide (SQ 20, 881) to test the role of the RAS in so-called renin-dependent and renin-independent thirsts. They reasoned that teprotide would block renin-dependent thirst by preventing the formation of ANG II. Unexpectedly, sc injections of teprotide
FIN 153
318
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
increased the drinking response to several purported renin-dependent dipsogens, including isoproterenol, polyethylene glycol, and caval ligation, and to water deprivation. The additional water drinking caused by the sc injection of teprotide was eliminated by concurrent central administration of the ACE inhibitor through icv cannulas. Lehr et al. concluded that water drinking during peripheral treatment with teprotide results from increased delivery of ANG I to the brain of the rat where it is converted to ANG II at some central site. In a footnote to the article, they referenced work by Simpson and Routtenberg (286) demonstrating that the SFO is extremely sensitive to the dipsogenic effect of ANG II and used this evidence to suggest that the SFO is one brain site where such central conversion could occur. Under physiological conditions, most circulating ANG I is converted to ANG II by ACE located in the microvasculature of several structures but most notably the lung (234). Therefore, only small amounts of ANG I are available as substrate for ACE located in peripheral tissues and brain. Systemically administered ACE inhibitors increase the survival of ANG I in the blood which may then act as substrate for ACE located in the brain (280). Obviously, in order for there to be central conversion of ANG I to ANG II to occur, some ACE in brain must remain unblocked during peripheral ACE inhibition. Autoradiographic studies provide some support for this possibility. Two ACE inhibitors (i.e., lisinopril and perindopril) have been studied and shown to spare some portion of ACE activity in the brain, including the CVOs, at doses that greatly reduce ACE activity in lung and abolish ACE activity in plasma (269–271). The phenomenon of paradoxical enhancement of water drinking has been confirmed by many investigators using many different thirst challenges and several different ACE inhibitors (70, 85, 139, 184, 266). Paradoxical enhancement of sodium ingestion (75, 84, 116, 267, 297) and VP secretion (179, 332) have also been observed with peripheral ACE blockade. The effects of ACE inhibitors on water drinking, sodium ingestion, and VP secretion are doserelated and biphasic. The range of doses that generates the biphasic response pattern varies greatly with experimental conditions. Generally, renin-dependent responses are augmented at ‘‘low’’ doses of ACE inhibitor and attenuated at ‘‘high’’ doses. For example, the ACE inhibitor CAP administered systemically in doses ranging from 0.1 to 15 mg/kg greatly increases water drinking to isoproterenol, caval ligation, and water deprivation while doses ranging from 25 to 100 mg/kg suppress drinking in response to these stimuli (13, 83, 108, 174). Similar biphasic effects of ACE inhibitors have been demonstrated for salt appetite (77, 224, 327, 350) and VP secretion (332). Most investigators interpret the biphasic dose effects of peripheral ACE inhibition in the manner of Lehr et al. (186). That is, low doses of ACE inhibitor block formation of systemic ANG II so that increased levels of circulating ANG I enter the brain where it is converted to ANG II. Higher systemic doses or the central administration of ACE inhibitors prevent the central formation of ANG II and thus prevent the exaggerated behavioral responses (69, 76, 85, 86, 108, 186, 224, 262, 279, 309).
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
319
Two models can be derived from the mechanism initially proposed by Lehr et al. (186) to explain paradoxical enhancement of fluid ingestion. The first model will be referred to as the spillover hypothesis. This model argues that blockade of peripheral ACE causes plasma concentrations of ANG I to become so high that ANG I crosses, or spills over, the blood–brain barrier. ANG I that ‘‘leaks’’ into the brain is then converted to ANG II in brain regions located behind the blood–brain barrier to stimulate an exaggerated response. The main problem with the spillover hypothesis is that ANG I is a decapeptide and, like the smaller peptide ANG II (278), is not likely to cross the blood–brain barrier despite its elevated levels during peripheral ACE inhibition. Regardless, it remains to be experimentally demonstrated that there are increased levels of ANG I in brain areas located behind the blood–brain barrier during peripheral ACE inhibition. An alternative to the spillover hypothesis utilizes the presence and properties of CVOs to explain the mechanism of ACE inhibition-induced water intake. Substances in the blood readily penetrate the CVOs because these areas of the brain are highly vascularized and lack an effective blood–brain barrier. According to this model, ANG I achieves high levels in the blood during peripheral ACE inhibition and enters CVOs where it is converted to ANG II. In other words, circulating ANG I accesses the brain in the same manner that circulating ANG II does, across fenestrated capillaries in CVOs. This locally formed ANG II then causes drinking by acting at the same angiotensin receptors in the CVOs that respond to circulating ANG II. Figure 7 summarizes this model which will be referred to here as the CVO conversion hypothesis. This alternative to the spillover hypothesis eliminates the need to postulate that ANG I crosses the blood–brain barrier. Furthermore, the CVOs contain some of the highest levels of ACE activity in the brain (35, 45, 55, 251), making them appropriate brain sites for such conversion to occur. The CVOs possess ANG II receptors (45, 342) that mediate several CNS responses to circulating ANG II. Finally, CVOs have been experimentally implicated in the paradoxical enhancement of water drinking and sodium ingestion. For example, so-called paradoxically enhanced water drinking is suppressed to a greater degree by ACE inhibitor injected directly into the SFO than by injections into the lateral ventricles for access to brain parenchyma in general (324). Both lesions of the SFO (323) and knife-cut transections of the ventral efferent fibers of the SFO (204) abolish paradoxically enhanced water drinking. Infusions of ANG II receptor antagonist in the proximity of the SFO reduce the additional component of water intake following peripheral ACE inhibition (97). Similarly, the paradoxical increase in sodium intake observed in sodium-deficient rats given low doses of ACE inhibitors is suppressed by injections of ACE inhibitor into tissue near the OVLT (98). These findings indicate that paradoxically enhanced water drinking and sodium ingestion both rely on the functional integrity of CVOs and that the site for the central conversion of ANG I to ANG II is likely to be one or both of the forebrain CVOs.
FIN 153
320
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
FIG. 7. Proposed model of the interface between the endocrine and the brain renin–angiotensin systems. The classic endocrine renin–angiotensin system forms blood-borne ANG I from hepatic derived angiotensinogen and renal-derived renin. ANG II, under physiological conditions, is formed from ANG I by angiotensin converting enzyme (ACE) in the lungs. Under pathophysiological or pharmacological conditions, residual amounts of ANG I survive passage through the lungs to arrive at tissue sites. Receptors in CVOs are stimulated either by blood-borne ANG II or by ANG II formed locally within the CVO from blood-borne ANG I. Angiotensinergic neurons then stimulate target areas of the visceral neuraxis.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
321
Evidence from Fos immunoreactivity (Fos-ir) studies supports a role for the lamina terminalis and associated CVOs in mediating paradoxically enhanced water and sodium intake. The early gene product Fos is used as a functional marker of neuronal activity (140). Rowland et al. (268) demonstrated that injection of low doses of ACE inhibitor increased isoproterenol-induced expression of Fos-ir in certain structures of the lamina terminalis. This effect paralleled the enhancement of water intake observed under the same conditions. Administration of higher doses of ACE inhibitor abolishes both water drinking and the expression of Fos-ir in the structures of the lamina terminalis in response to isoproterenol. In other studies, parallel changes in behavior and expression of Fos-ir along the lamina terminalis were obtained in sodiumdepleted animals given high and low doses of ACE inhibitors (366). In these studies, low doses of ACE inhibitor increased Fos-ir in the structures of the lamina terminalis, especially in the SFO and in the OVLT. The increased Fos-ir in the CVOs was abolished by giving higher doses of ACE inhibitor or the specific ANG II Type 1 receptor (AT1) antagonist losartan. These findings indicate that the increased Fos-ir in forebrain structures during peripheral ACE inhibition fits the pattern of the paradoxically enhanced responses caused by peripheral antagonism of ACE. The paradoxical enhancement of thirst and salt appetite may serve as an important model of the actions of a circulating prohormone delivered to a target tissue which contains the trigger enzyme to generate the active hormone. The conventional understanding has been that the central effects of angiotensin are mediated by circulating ANG II and not by ANG II formed within the CVOs from circulating ANG I. However under pathophysiological conditions, such systemic conversion may not be complete, so that residual unconverted ANG I may reach target tissues such as CVOs. Thus mechanisms of the paradoxical enhancement of thirst and salt appetite may be of more than theoretical interest and may have implications for understanding the etiology of hypertension. There is evidence that the central administration of ACE inhibitors reduces arterial blood pressure in many forms of hypertension (145), although the precise central location(s) of such an action has not been identified. Some investigators have recently argued that the critical actor in some forms of hypertension is not circulating ANG II but ANG II produced in specific tissue RASs (e.g., brain, heart, adrenal; 339, 344). Brain CVOs may use circulating ANG I as a precursor to generate local ANG II for immediate action on adjacent AT1 receptors.
Salt Appetite and the Lamina Terminalis The first demonstrations that ANG II produced salt appetite upon central administration utilized injections into the forebrain ventricles (11, 36, 37, 113). This was a fortunate choice of location as it was later determined that salt appetite is stimulated by acute infusions of ANG II only if the area of perfusion includes the forebrain, and not if delivery is limited to the hindbrain (97).
FIN 153
322
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
Additionally, sodium depletion-induced salt appetite is suppressed by infusions of ANG II receptor antagonists into the lateral ventricles of the forebrain, but not by administration of the antagonists into the fourth ventricle of the hindbrain (97). Salt appetite after sodium depletion is abolished in the rat by disconnecting the forebrain from the hindbrain by supracollicular decerebration (132). As has been discussed with thirst, considerable work now points to an essential role of the lamina terminalis of the forebrain for the expression of renin-dependent forms of salt appetite, including salt appetite following sodium depletion. The evidence for an essential role of the endocrine RAS in the expression of sodium depletion-induced salt appetite underscores the importance of work directed at determining the role of forebrain CVOs in the genesis of salt appetite. One plausible mechanism by which circulating ANG II may activate salt appetite is by acting on receptors located in CVOs to stimulate central pathways governing sodium ingestion. The participation of the SFO of the lamina terminalis in salt appetite was examined in the light of its known role in mediating the effects of ANG II. Early attempts to establish a role for the SFO in salt appetite were unsuccessful. Knife-cuts of the major efferent pathways of the SFO did not affect salt appetite produced either by treatment with a combination of deoxycorticosterone acetate (DOCA; 5 mg/kg) and FURO (10 mg/kg) or by maintenance for 4 days on sodium-deficient diet (281). These results show that the SFO does not influence salt appetite via its terminal fields. However, two later reports showed that electrolytic lesions of the SFO diminished salt appetite of rats following sodium depletion (321, 349), indicating that the SFO has a role in renin-dependent salt intake. The divergent conclusions may partly be explained by the duration over which salt appetite was measured in the three studies. The first study (281) reported total saline consumed over 48 h. This considerable period may have permitted additional (i.e., compensatory) mechanisms to normalize saline intakes of animals with transection of SFO efferent fibers (321). The two later studies reported saline intakes in the first 2 h after saline solution was reintroduced to the depleted animals. Not all forms of renin-dependent salt intake are affected by SFO lesions. For example, lesions of the SFO do not eliminate the exaggerated salt intake caused by peripheral ACE inhibition (323). In fact, peripheral ACE inhibition appears to completely normalize the salt appetite response of sodium-deplete rats with SFO lesions (349). Therefore, the available lesion data suggest that the SFO plays a significant role in the salt appetite response to sodium depletion, but not in the exaggerated intake of sodium following ACE inhibition. It was logical to assess the role of the AV3V region in sodium intake because this unique area of the brain is critical for cardiovascular and hydromineral balance (41, 158). Effects of lesions within the AV3V on salt appetite are mixed and depend on the size of the lesion, including the subnuclei that are destroyed, and the means by which salt appetite is produced (e.g., sodium depletion, DOCA). Lesions that encompass the entire AV3V region do not affect ad libitum
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
323
consumption of concentrated saline solution by rats maintained on standard laboratory diet (64), but reduce the relatively greater daily intakes of concentrated saline solutions by rats maintained on sodium deficient diet (15). AV3V lesions also retard the development of sodium depletion-induced salt appetite (64). On the other hand, rats with AV3V lesions show a normal increase in salt ingestion during acute formalin-induced hyponatremia (15). Smaller lesions of the AV3V region that are confined mainly to the OVLT reduce salt intake after sodium depletion by about half (47). Interestingly, lesions that encompass both the OVLT and the adjacent third of the vMePO reduce sodium ingestion in response to renin-dependent forms of salt appetite (i.e., sodium depletion-induced and ACE inhibitor-induced; 101) but increase sodium intake elicited by daily injections of DOCA (96, 101). The mechanism by which these lesions increase DOCA-induced salt appetite is unknown. Lesions of the vMePO that spare the OVLT greatly increase need-free sodium intake in some animals (124). It has thus been proposed that lesions of the MePO remove a mechanism related to OXY secretion that tonically inhibits salt appetite. MePO lesions impair the osmoregulatory stimulation of OXY secretion (125). However, identical lesions do not affect sodium intake after sc polyethylene glycol (124). Brain lesions may disrupt thirst and salt appetite by destroying neural substrates for these behaviors and by altering body fluid and plasma parameters that, in turn, reduce thirst and salt appetite. For example, AV3V lesions render animals chronically hypernatremic and hyperosmotic (41, 64) which could interfere with salt appetite by affecting cerebral sodium/osmoreceptors and endocrine responses to extracellular fluid depletion. Lesions of the SFO cause persistently elevated levels of circulating OXY (92) which may reflect increased activity of central mechanisms inhibitory to salt appetite. Brain stimulation studies circumvent some of the interpretational difficulties inherent in lesion studies. As previously discussed, discrete injections of low doses of ANG II into the SFO induce water drinking (285, 286). The OVLT is also reported to be sensitive to the dipsogenic effects of ANG II (250). In studies in which animals have access to both water and saline solution, Fitts and Masson (98) found that ANG II infused near the SFO stimulates only water drinking, while identical infusions into the OVLT and/or adjacent portion of the MePO nucleus stimulate saline ingestion as well as water drinking. Curiously, the best sodium intake was elicited by infusion of ANG II receptor antagonist into the SFO. Although this sodium intake was attributed to agonist properties of the receptor antagonist, ANG II itself did not stimulate salt intake upon administration into the SFO. It is possible that ANG II acts at the SFO to engage central mechanisms that are inhibitory to salt appetite including OXY secretion (14, 91–93, 301, 303, 306; this hypothesis is discussed in detail below). Discrete, local administration of ACE inhibitor or ANG II receptor antagonist directly into forebrain CVOs selectively reduces water or sodium ingestion depending on the model of salt appetite that is employed and the CVO that is targeted (52, 97, 98). Fluharty and Epstein (113) observed a greater salt appetite response to
FIN 153
324
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
central administration of ANG II in rats treated with aldosterone than in untreated rats. Since levels of ANG II and aldosterone are high in sodiumdeficient animals, they argued that central administration of ANG II to aldosterone-treated rats more closely mimics natural conditions which produce salt appetite than does ANG II infused into sodium-replete rats with low levels of endogenous aldosterone. It would not be surprising if other signals related to sodium deficiency modulate the central actions of ANG II. Thunhorst and Johnson (325) have reported that central infusions of ANG II stimulated salt appetite more readily in hypotensive, sodium-deficient rats than in normotensive, sodium-replete rats. Furthermore, brain lesions of the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus, which blunt the pressor responses to centrally administered ANG II (159), dramatically increase the salt appetite response to ANG II injected into the AV3V area (44). It is therefore possible that prior treatment with aldosterone, reductions in body/plasma sodium, or unloading of baroreceptors may remove inhibitory mechanisms allowing ANG II to more effectively stimulate salt appetite.
Salt Appetite and the Amygdala/Extended Amygdala It has been proposed that the function of the amygdala is to interpret the significance of internal and external stimuli and bias the animals toward the most appropriate adaptive behavior (128, 253). Salt appetite is a homeostatic behavior requiring central integration of both visceral sensory input (humoral and neural) related to sodium balance and somatic sensory input (e.g., taste) for activation of the appropriate behaviors. Most studies on the role of the amygdala in sodium ingestion have focused on the basolateral, central, and medial nuclei of the amygdala. Ablation of the basolateral amygdala reduces salt appetite induced by DOCA treatment (232). Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) nearly abolish ad libitum sodium intake in rats and greatly impair the salt appetite response to DOCA, sodium depletion, sc administration of yohimbine, and icv ANG II (120, 363). The global impairment of salt intake in rats with lesions of the central nucleus does not appear to be the result of disordered gustatory function (121). Rats with such lesions discriminate between different tastes, and their differential responses to tastes are modified by their internal states, yet they fail to consume sodium (282). Lesions of the medial region of the amygdala impair mineralocorticoid-induced salt appetite, but not sodium depletion-induced salt appetite (237, 367). This selective impairment does not seem to result from disruption of connections to the ventral forebrain through the stria terminalis (22). The medial amygdala does not appear to be necessary for the generation of renin-dependent salt appetite, as evidenced by the failure of lesions of medial amygdala to prevent depletion-induced salt appetite. However, tachykinins ingested into medial amygdala inhibit renin-dependent forms of salt appetite (203). Recent reviews (4, 66, 253) have reaffirmed the concept initially proposed by
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
325
Johnston (171) that the amygdala and the BNST were once a single structure which become separated in mammals by development of the internal capsule. Both tract tracing and immunocytochemical studies suggest that the lateral and ventral portions of the BNST are more similar to the CeA and that the medial region of the bed nucleus and medial amygdala are closely related. The ventral and lateral portions of the BNST and the CeA are each reciprocally connected with several nuclei involved in cardiovascular control and fluid balance (142, 225, 289, 365). Taken together, the structural, neurochemical, and functional similarities and interrelationships between the central nucleus and the related portions of the BNST support the idea that these two nuclei are likely to have similar functions. Electrolytic ablation of either the CeA or the BNST causes a significant reduction in sodium intake following systemic administration of the a2-adrenergic receptor antagonist, yohimbine, or 24 h after sodium depletion (363). Neither lesion affects water intake to sc ANG II or to sc hypertonic saline, indicating a specificity of the lesion for salt appetite. Since lesions of the lateral BNST and of the central nucleus each reduced sodium intake by approximately 50%, future studies will be necessary to determine if combined lesions of these structures will result in more complete abolition of the behavior. Inhibitory Neural Circuits and Neurochemical Systems
Early Investigations of the Role of the Hindbrain and the Reflex Controls of Body Fluid Balance The hindbrain is the gateway for neural input from the viscera and it has had a long history in the study of reflex control of the circulation and fluid balance. Claude Bernard in the mid-19th century noted that puncturing the floor of the 4th cerebral ventricle increased urinary excretion (see 61). More recent studies have described activation of hindbrain neurons by systemic infusions of hypertonic saline (51) and elevation of urinary sodium/potassium ratios in rats over the 24-h period following lesions in the region of the obex (288). In the dog, lesions of the AP, the hindbrain sensory CVO, do not seem to produce marked alterations in water and electrolyte balance (359), although electrical stimulation in and around the AP (358) increases excretion of both sodium and chloride and increases urine volume and creatinine clearance in this species. Although such studies clearly implicate the hindbrain in the control of hydromineral balance, the role of this region in the behaviors contributing to such homeostatic processes has only recently been investigated. The AP and Immediately Adjacent Nucleus of the Tractus Solitarius (mNTS) in Salt Appetite and Thirst The AP is a bilaterally symmetrical CVO lying in the caudal aspects of the rhomboid fossa of the 4th ventricle. Its role as a chemosensitive region detect-
FIN 153
326
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
ing circulating toxins which trigger emesis was established in the 1950s by Borison and colleagues (see 221 for review). Thus, the AP was the first of the three sensory CVOs to have a demonstrated receptive function at the blood– brain interface. It was also the first CVO to be convincingly implicated as a central target for circulating ANG II (94, 172, 338). These seminal studies indicate that ANG II acts at the AP to reduce the gain of cardiovascular baroreceptor reflexes to thereby enhance the pressor actions of ANG II. It is extremely difficult to use chemical or electrical stimulation of the AP in unanesthetized animals to study its function because it is small and difficult to access. As a consequence, most of the functional research on the AP in behaving animals has involved ablation studies (53, 71, 73, 146, 242, 346). When discussing the functional effects of AP lesions, it is important to appreciate the fact that none of the results of the current studies in which the entire AP is ablated can be taken as indicative of effects that are attributable solely to the AP. In most if not all of the experiments effectively removing all of the AP, the lesions encroach on adjacent NTS tissue. Even in studies where there has been a scrupulous effort to limit destruction to the AP, there is degeneration of adjacent NTS neuropile reflecting the substantial neural interconnectivity between the AP and the NTS. Careful attention to histological characterization of the extent of NTS involvement in AP lesions is thus essential in correlating AP ablation with functional consequences. The first paper to report an effect of AP lesions on water and sodium ingestion was by Contreras and Stetson (53). These investigators described increased ad libitum consumption of NaCl solutions across a range of concentrations in two-bottle preference tests. Similar effects have been observed by others (53, 71, 146, 181, 346). Although Hyde and Miselis (146) provided evidence that increased sodium intake may be secondary to increased sodium excretion after AP ablation, others have found minimal effects of AP lesions on sodium excretion (53, 346). Contreras and Stetson (53) have emphasized the importance of reducing damage to the NTS in conjunction with AP lesions. In their studies, animals with lesions restricted mainly to the AP (i.e., minimal damage to the NTS) showed the greatest overconsumption of NaCl solutions. A recent study investigating the impact of AP lesions on NaCl intake examined control and ablated rats maintained on ad libitum water and standard laboratory diet exposed to 1, 2, and 3% NaCl solutions in short-term (3-h) intake tests (71). In this acute situation, rats with AP/mNTS lesions consumed significantly greater amounts of each concentration of hypertonic saline (Table 2) on an initial exposure to the solutions than control rats with intact APs. For example, AP/mNTS ablated rats consumed an average of approximately 3 mmol of sodium in the form of 3% NaCl solution. The remarkable nature of the 3-h intakes of NaCl by rats with AP lesions can be appreciated better when one considers that the extracellular fluid volume of a 350-g rat is roughly 90 ml and contains slightly more than 12 mmol of sodium. Therefore, during the 3-h intake period, AP/mNTS ablated rats consumed NaCl equivalent to 25% of their extracellular sodium. Occasionally, individual rats with AP/mNTS lesions have been observed to drink nearly 20 ml of 3% NaCl during such 3-h tests,
FIN 153
327
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
TABLE 2 Three-Hour Intakes (ml) of 1, 2, and 3% Saline by Area Postrema / Medial Nucleus of the Tractus Solitarius (AP/mNTS) Lesion and Sham Lesion Rats Group Saline concentration (%)
Sham lesion
AP/mNTS
1
3.4 6 0.4 (n 5 10) 1.6 6 0.4 (n 5 10) 0.4 6 0.2 (n 5 14)
42.7 6 5.2* (n 5 10) 24.4 6 3.0* (n 5 10) 5.5 6 0.8* (n 5 14)
2 3
Note. Values are means 6 SEM; n 5 number of observations. * Significantly greater than sham lesion group; p , 0.01.
which is nearly sufficient to replace all readily exchangeable extracellular sodium. A further characterization of AP/mNTS lesioned rats indicates that the increased consumption of NaCl is not due to an enhanced renal loss of sodium or water during the test period. Specifically, rats with AP/mNTS lesions that have been bilaterally nephrectomized still show a significant increase in the intake of 3% saline compared to intact controls. It is also worth noting that in the face of such increases in NaCl intake during 3-h tests, it is possible for animals with AP/mNTS lesions to express an even greater intake when they are sodium depleted with FURO and switched to sodium-deficient diet 24 h prior to presentation of the 3% NaCl solution (Fig. 8). Edwards and Ritter (73) were the first to report enhanced water intake in response to dipsogenic challenges after AP lesions. These investigators found that animals with AP/mNTS lesions drank more than controls in response to systemically administered isoproterenol (73) and extracellular depletion with sc polyethylene glycol. Importantly, AP ablated rats with slight damage to the mNTS (i.e., AP/mNTS lesions) do not manifest exaggerated drinking responses to systemically injected hypertonic saline (73) or to centrally administered carbachol (242). As a result, it has been proposed (73, 242) that the AP functions to inhibit water intake in response to extracellular dehydration and that this mechanism may be more closely associated with the control of constant plasma and extracellular fluid volume than with the maintenance of intracellular volume.
The Lateral Parabrachial Nucleus (LPBN) in Thirst and Sodium Appetite Early anatomical tract tracing studies (194, 238, 258) demonstrated that the LPBN receives a substantial portion of the output of the NTS. Such observa-
FIN 153
328
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
FIG. 8. Three-hour, depletion-induced intake of 3% saline by area postrema-medial portion of the nucleus of the tractus solitarius lesion (APX) (n 5 17) and sham lesion (SHAM) rats (n 5 18) before and after sodium depletion induced by furosemide treatment and low-sodium diet. APX rats increased their intake of 3% saline after depletion compared with their intake before depletion (*p , 0.02). Sodium depletion also significantly increased intake of 3% saline by the sham rats (*p , 0.01). From Edwards et al. (71) with permission of the publisher.
tions were responsible for studies exploring the proposition that in hypovolemic/ hypotensive states excitatory information derived from vascular baroreceptors ascends from the NTS to the forebrain via the parabrachial nucleus (241). It was suggested that depriving the forebrain of ascending arterial pressure/ vascular volume information by lesions of the LPBN would result in impaired drinking responses. Contrary to this prediction, lesions located in the lateral portion of the LPBN (i.e., LLPBN) have the opposite effect. Drinking responses to systemic isoproterenol (241) and both sc (241, 242) and icv (242) ANG II are significantly enhanced in animals with lesions in the LLPBN. The same lesion does not cause overdrinking in response to systemic hypertonic saline or icv carbachol (241, 242). The overdrinking responses observed after lesions of the LLPBN are not dependent on the method used to interrupt the flow of neural information. That is, both lidocaine injections into the LPBN (215) and excitotoxic chemical lesions (i.e., ibotenic acid) destroying LPBN cell bodies (72) significantly increase drinking in response to icv ANG II. The fact that destruction of LPBN soma without damage to fibers of passage produces overdrinking is evidence that this nucleus is a site of neural integration for the control of fluid intake (72). Lesions of the LLPBN produce effects on thirst nearly identical to those described for AP/mNTS lesions (73). Both types of lesion enhance drinking in response to extracellular thirst challenges but not to cellular dehydration. These observations have led to the hypothesis that the LLPBN and the
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
329
AP/mNTS relate to one another as components of a hindbrain system which limits extracellular volume expansion (72, 241, 242). Anatomical studies (56, 283, 340) indicate that both the NTS and the AP send major projections to the LPBN. Lanc¸a and van der Kooy (185) emphasized the presence of an ascending serotonergic pathway from the AP to the parabrachial nucleus. Therefore, 5-HT release into the LPBN might inhibit drinking. Consistent with this hypothesis, bilateral administration of 5-HT or the more selective serotonergic 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist [6]-2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI) significantly suppresses water intake in response to icv ANG II. Conversely the relatively nonselective 5-HT1/2 receptor antagonist methysergide injected bilaterally into the LPBN produces a marked enhancement in drinking in response to icv ANG II (216). Although there is a remarkable degree of similarity in the pattern of enhanced water intake in response to extracellular stimuli between rats with AP/mNTS lesions and those with LPBN lesions, the two types of lesions differ in their effects on salt appetite. Whereas, AP/mNTS lesions promote exaggerated ad libitum and experimentally induced NaCl intake (53, 71, 146, 181, 346), LPBN lesions do not increase sodium intake (L. E. Ohman and A. K. Johnson, unpublished observations; G. L. Edwards, personal communication). One reason why there may be a dissociation between salt and water intake for the two types of lesions is that the AP/mNTS and LPBN may each receive different amounts of excitatory vs inhibitory input derived from other sources. Perhaps AP/mNTS lesions destroy mainly inhibitory salt appetite-related substrates, but LPBN ablation removes not only inhibitory input but also a substantial facilitory component as well. On the basis of chemical neuroanatomy of 5-HT described just above, one strategy to dissociate and reveal the nature of inhibitory and excitatory components in the LPBN is to employ direct injections of receptor agonists and antagonists into the LPBN. Such experiments indicate that bilateral administration of methysergide into the LPBN remarkably enhances intakes of 0.3 M NaCl and water. In two-bottle drinking tests, methysergide injections into LPBN increased water and sodium ingestion in response to ANG II administered icv and into SFO and to combined systemic treatments with FURO/CAP (52, 218; see Fig. 9). Furthermore, administration of the 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonists significantly reduced 0.3 M saline intake to FURO/CAP treatment. Therefore, inhibitory serotonergic mechanisms associated with the LPBN appear to limit both of the behaviors that act to restore and expand extracellular fluid volume.
Functional Interpretation of AP/mNTS-LPBN Pathways in the Behavioral Control of Extracellular Volume The results of AP/mNTS and LPBN lesion and injection studies along with the current understanding of input from the periphery suggest that inhibitory signals originating from the vena cava and from atrial baroreceptors arrive at the AP/mNTS and then ascend to the LPBN. The hypothesis that the LPBN
FIN 153
330
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
FIG. 9. (A) Cumulative 0.3 M NaCl intakes. (B) Individual 0.3 M NaCl intakes. (C) Cumulative water intakes. (D) Individual water intakes induced by subcutaneous furosemide (FURO; 10 mg/ kg) 1 captopril (CAP; 5 mg / kg) after previous injection of vehicle or methysergide (4 µg/200 nl) bilaterally into lateral parabrachial nuclei (LPBN). (A, C) Values are means 6 SE. *Significantly different from vehicle pretreatment tested by t tests ( p , 0.05). n 5 8 rats. From Menani et al. (218) with permission of the publisher.
may function to integrate information derived from systemic inhibitory input is supported by a recent study (243) which examined the effects of LPBN lesions on changes in behavior produced by stretch of the superior vena cava and right atrium. As discussed earlier, Kaufman (175) demonstrated that stretching the superior vena cava and right atrium by inflating a small intravascular balloon inhibits drinking in response to extracellular-related thirst challenges, specifically systemically administered isoproterenol and extracellular fluid depletion with sc polyethylene glycol. However, stretching the superior vena cava and right atrium does not inhibit drinking in response to systemic injections of hypertonic saline. The inhibitory effects of superior vena cava and right atrium stretch on drinking to isoproterenol are impaired in animals with LPBN lesions (243; Fig. 10). These observations are consistent with the idea that the LPBN receives and integrates information reflecting extracellular (blood) volume.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
331
FIG. 10. The effects of lateral parabrachial nucleus (LPBN) lesions on the inhibition of drinking by inflation of a balloon at the junction of the right atrium and superior vena cava. Drinking was induced by sc isoproterenol (30 µg / kg) in rats with bilateral lesions of the lateral portion of the LPBN (LLPBN) or sham lesions. As indicated by the significant interaction, at 1 h after treatment, enhanced water intake in rats with LLPBN lesions (X) was not reduced as it was in the case of sham lesion (W) animals.
The input disrupted by lesions of the LPBN may derive from atrial stretch receptors located in the superior vena cava and right atrium region. This input is carried to the CNS by the vagus or possibly by humoral signals generated by atrial stretch. Stretch of the superior vena cava and right atrium releases ANP (176, 214), and this peptide when administered either centrally (100) or systemically (9) inhibits water and sodium intake. Circulating ANP may act on the AP to activate the ascending AP/mNTS to LPBN pathway. Of course, it is also possible that neurally derived signals carried via the vagus and circulating ANP act in concert to prevent excessive fluid intake. The ascending projections from the AP or NTS to the LPBN may contain other neurotransmitters/neuromodulators in addition to 5-HT which modify fluid intake responses. For example, bilateral administration of cholecystokinin (CCK) into the LPBN suppresses sodium intake induced by icv ANG II, whereas the CCK receptor antagonist proglumide administered bilaterally enhances sodium intake (217). Neuroanatomical studies (138) indicate the presence of a CCK pathway projecting from the NTS to the parabrachial nucleus. This pathway may be one link in a series of CCK neurons projecting via the vagus from the periphery to NTS, NTS to LPBN, and LPBN to forebrain which may influence taste and viscerally related homeostatic functions (200). This CCK pathway or similar projections with other neurotransmitters/ neuromodulators could also be related to other visceral inputs, such as those derived from gastric distention which inhibits fluid intake (60). In particular afferent signals from the gastrointestinal tract (e.g., stomach; 60), splanchnic and hepatic circulation (182), and kidneys (329) may travel in this projection.
FIN 153
332
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
The Midline Septal Area in Thirst and Salt Appetite Lesions of the midline septum/diagonal band of Broca produce effects similar to those observed after ablation of the LPBN and AP/mNTS. Harvey and Hunt (137) first reported large increases in ad libitum water intake after septal lesions, and in addition, electrical stimulation of the septal area suppresses drinking induced by water deprivation (129, 229, 360). Animals with septal lesions also overrespond to polyethylene glycol (30, 294, 298) and isoproterenol (31). However, similar to AP/mNTS and LPBN ablated animals, rats with septal lesions typically do not overdrink in response to cellular dehydration (30, 31). Septal lesions, therefore, produce effects on thirst that are quite comparable to those produced by AP/mNTS and LPBN lesions. Furthermore, septal lesions enhance the intake of sodium (130, 233). In a preliminary study, Sullivan and co-workers (308) examined the effects of ibotenic acid destruction of cell bodies in the vertical limb of the diagonal band of Broca, an area contiguous with the medial septum and with similar cell types and neurochemistry. Neurotoxic lesions of the vertical limb of the diagonal band of Broca significantly increase water intake in response to extracellular depletion with polyethylene glycol but do not increase drinking to cellular dehydration with systemic hypertonic saline. Thus, it seems that the midline septum and diagonal band of Broca may be a rostral representation of the AP/mNTS–LPBN inhibitory pathway. If this is true, it is unclear whether information projects directly from the hindbrain circuitry to the forebrain region or whether an ascending multisynaptic pathway is involved.
OXY Systems and Salt Appetite The fact that rats maintained on sodium deficient diet have blunted secretion of OXY during hypovolemia is of theoretical interest given the recently proposed role for a central oxytocinergic system as an inhibitory mechanism for the control of salt appetite (301, 303). Sodium ingestion can vary inversely with circulating levels of OXY; for example, salt appetite does not occur in animals with elevated levels of plasma OXY. Although it was predicted that OXY would suppress salt appetite, peripheral administration of synthetic OXY actually increased sodium intake to polyethylene glycol treatment (305). It was subsequently proposed that circulating levels of OXY simply mirror the activity of a central oxytocinergic system which is causally related to the control of sodium ingestion. For example, reduced levels of circulating OXY in rats maintained on a sodium-deficient diet indicate lower activity of central inhibitory mechanisms, so that, during hypovolemic conditions, there is a greater salt appetite response with shorter latency. A number of additional observations support this hypothesis. Treatments or conditions (e.g., naloxone, hyperosmolality, nausea) that stimulate OXY secretion are associated with reduced salt appetite in response to hypovolemia (25, 301, 305). Treatments which suppress OXY
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
333
secretion (e.g., sodium deprivation, alcohol) increase salt appetite (26, 301). In addition, salt appetite in response to centrally administered ANG II is enhanced by central pretreatment with an OXY receptor blocker (23). Finally, salt appetite in response to hypovolemia and centrally administered ANG II is increased in animals after inactivation of central oxytocinergic systems by administration of a conjugate of oxytocin and the cytotoxin ricin (24).
Central a2-Adrenergic Receptors and Salt Appetite In two preliminary reports, it has been demonstrated that systemic and icv (195, 240) administration of the selective a2-adrenoceptor antagonist yohimbine significantly increases drinking when animals have access to water. More recently, administration of yohimbine has been shown to induce salt appetite with short latency (96, 155, 156, 363). When tested in both one- and two-bottle tests, sc yohimbine (1.0–9.0 mg/kg) produces significant increases in hypertonic NaCl intake. Other a2-adrenoceptor antagonists such as rauwolseine and idazoxan are also effective in inducing a rapid onset salt appetite. The yohimbine-induced salt appetite is inhibited by treatment with the a2-noradrenergic receptor agonist clonidine. Recently we have observed that infusion of low doses of yohimbine into the lateral ventricle significantly increases 2% NaCl intake, which suggests that the a2-adrenoceptor antagonist probably produces its natriorexigenic effect directly through a CNS action. A substantial literature indicates that systemic treatment with a2-receptor agonists, such as clonidine, produces a pattern of sympathetic and endocrine responses that would collectively reduce extracellular fluid volume and lower arterial blood pressure. Given the putative CNS action of clonidine and its use as an antihypertensive therapeutic agent, many studies have been performed to determine its mechanism and site of action. Although clonidine may exert some of its blood pressure lowering effects through actions on the imidazoline receptor (see 81 for review), it is still very likely that a substantial component of its antihypertensive effect derives from action on central a2-adrenoceptors. Microinjection studies implicate the NTS (63) and the ventrolateral medulla (126) as sites in the central neural circuitry where the a2-adrenoceptor agonist acts to decrease sympathetic and increase parasympathetic efferent nerve activity. Other antihypertensive/volume reducing actions of clonidine include reduced renin secretion (249) and lowered VP release (256). Taken together reports that a2-adrenoceptor antagonists increase the ingestion of water and hypertonic NaCl and that systemically administered clonidine inhibits experimental thirst (357) suggest that these a2-adrenoceptorrelated behaviors are consistent with other physiological mechanisms which limit or reduce blood volume and blood pressure. In light of their general actions on both extracellular- and intracellular-depletion-induced drinking responses, Fregly, Rowland and colleagues (117, 356) have hypothesized that a2-adrenoceptor mechanisms lie in a final common path mediating thirst. An
FIN 153
334
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
alternative to this idea is that a2-adrenoreceptor agonists act at several locations on the sensory side of the central body fluid-related neural network to produce a modulatory action on thirst and sodium appetite.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
Signals for the behavioral controls of body fluid homeostasis arise from both the intracellular and the extracellular fluid compartments. The intracellular compartment is monitored by putative osmoreceptors and/or sodium receptors located in the brain and periphery. The integrity of the AV3V is essential for normal behavioral, autonomic, and endocrine responses elicited by increased plasma osmolarity and /or sodium concentration. There are osmosensitive cells located within the AV3V, most notably in the OVLT and MePO. In addition, it is likely that information about extracellular osmolarity and sodium concentration derived from the periphery (e.g., the splanchnic/hepatoportal vasculature) and from other brain-related osmosensitive regions (e.g., SFO, AP) projects to and/or through the vMePO or other sites within the AV3V. Figure 11 summarizes many of the major points addressed in this review related to the afferent stimuli and central circuitry relevant to the behavioral controls of extracellular fluid volume. To the right and above the large box representing the brain are the neural and humoral inputs that communicate the status of body fluid and cardiovascular homeostasis to the CNS. Among these putative stimuli, circulating ANG II is a potent stimulus for thirst. In physiological states, circulating ANG II is a participant in the generation of thirst and its actions are complemented by additional visceral afferent inputs. ANG II addresses the CNS through brain CVOs. Because sensory CVOs such as the SFO have high concentrations of converting enzyme, it is possible that the so-called paradoxical enhancement of drinking and salt intake that occurs with low doses of converting enzyme inhibitor reflects local generation of ANG II within these areas of blood–brain interface. Hypovolemia and / or hypotension results in neural input from vascular baroreceptors to the CNS to enhance activity in central facilitory systems and conversely hypervolemia and / or hypertension activates inhibitory mechanisms. Other types of viscerally derived input from the gastrointestinal tract, splanchnic and hepatoportal circulation, and the kidneys may also contribute to this input. In the brain, angiotensin acting in the SFO has been proposed to result in the activation of several descending projections to key forebrain structures such as the MePO, perifornical area, CeA, supraoptic nucleus, and paraventricular nucleus. These pathways and their termination sites are probably associated with control of the behaviors and reflexes that maintain body fluid balance and cardiovascular homeostasis. Angiotensinergic neurons from the SFO and other lamina terminalis structures descend to innervate several of these forebrain sites (see Fig. 6). Ascending facilitory and inhibitory inputs project into many of
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
335
FIG. 11. Diagram depicting the nature of neural and hormonal inputs into the brain and the central neural pathways that mediate sensory integration of signals for the generation of drinking (thirst) and sodium ingestion (salt appetite). Both inhibitory and excitatory inputs from the periphery derive from arterial and cardiopulmonary baroreceptors and probably other visceral receptors (e.g., gastric, hepatic/portal, renal). Information carried in afferent nerves projects mainly to the nucleus of the tractus solitarius (NTS). Angiotensin (ANG) acts in the form of ANG II on angiotensin type 1 (AT1) receptors in the subfornical organ (SFO). Information reflecting input to the SFO is carried in descending pathways, some of which are likely to use ANG in the mode of a neurotransmitter, to forebrain structures such as those in the anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V). Ascending information to the forebrain is carried in from noradrenergic cell groups (A) in the hindbrain which are activated by arterial and cardiopulmonary receptor input under conditions of hypotension and / or hypovolemia. ANG and noradrenergic (NE) inputs act synergistically in forebrain nuclei. A hindbrain inhibitory pathway originating in the area postrema (AP) and medial NTS ascends to the lateral parabrachial nucleus. This projection uses serotonin (5-HT) as a neurotransmitter and prevents against excessive sodium and water intake to limit excess expansion of blood volume. Inhibitory input is likely to ascend the neuraxis either directly or indirectly (perhaps to or through the midline septum) to interact with forebrain structure in the neural network regulating hydromineral balance.
these forebrain regions, such as those in the ventral lamina terminalis. Synaptic increases of ANG II and NE within key forebrain sites act to reinforce the actions of one another to activate corrective responses to hypotension and hypovolemia. The facilitory actions of ascending noradrenergic input at these forebrain sites are probably mediated through a1-adrenergic receptors. Several recent studies have described the role of inhibitory influences arising in the periphery and activating neural circuitry in the hindbrain. Central structures implicated in this inhibitory system are the AP/mNTS and the LPBN. The inhibitory actions of 5-HT and CCK on the LPBN have been
FIN 153
336
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
associated with a hindbrain projection from the AP/mNTS to the LPBN that has been proposed to protect animals from overexpansion of the extracellular fluid compartment. The midline septum and ascending limb of the diagonal band of Broca may represent a rostral component of the hypothesized AP/mNTS– LPBN inhibitory circuitry. Central OXY and a2-adrenergic mechanisms have also been implicated as having inhibitory roles on the behavioral responses that lead to expanded extracellular volume. The specific structures and pathways associated with OXY and a2-adrenergic inhibition remain to be defined. Integration of information derived from facilitory and inhibitory visceral inputs occurs at many different nodes (nuclei) in a sensory network that regulates hydromineral balance. The outputs of this network control motor pattern generators responsible for the appetitive and consummatory behaviors associated with states that are commonly referred to as thirst and salt appetite.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This review is dedicated to James T. Fitzsimons on the occasion of his retirement from Cambridge University. Preparation of this review was supported in part by grants from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute HL 14388, HL 57472, HL 57472, HL 54292 from the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) NAGW-4358, and from the Office of Naval Research N00014-97-1-0145.
REFERENCES 1.
Abraham SF, Baker RM, Blaine EH, Denton DA, McKinley MJ. Water drinking induced in sheep by angiotensin—A physiological or pharmacological effect? J Comp Physiol Psychol 1975; 88: 503–518. 2. Adachi A, Niijima A, Jacobs HL. An hepatic osmoreceptor mechanism in the rat: Electrophysiological and behavioral studies. Am J Physiol 1976; 231(4): 1043–1049. 3.
Adolph EF and Associates. Physiology of Man in the Desert. New York: Interscience Publishers, 1947. 4. Alheid GF, de Olmos J, Beltramino CA. Amygdala and extended amygdala. In: Paxinos G, Ed. The Rat Nervous System, 2nd ed. San Diego: Academic Press 1995: 495–578. 5. Andersson B. The effect of injections of hypertonic NaCl-solutions into different parts of the hypothalamus of goats. Acta Physiol Scand 1953; 28: 188–201. 6.
Andersson B. Thirst and brain control of water balance. Am Sci 1971; 59: 408–415.
7.
Andersson B, Olsson K, Warner RG. Dissimilarities between the central control of thirst and the release of antidiuretic hormone (ADH). Acta Physiol Scand 1967; 71: 57–64. 8. Andrews WHH, Orbach J. Sodium receptors activating some nerves of perfused rabbit livers. Am J Physiol 1974; 227: 1273–1275. 9.
Antunes-Rodrigues J, McCann SM, Rogers LC, Samson WK. Atrial natriuretic factor inhibits dehydration- and angiotensin II-induced water intake in the conscious, unrestrained rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1985; 82: 8720–8723. 10. Atkinson J, Kaesemann HP, Lambert J, Peters G, Peters-Haefeli L. The role of circulating renin in drinking in response to isoprenaline. J Physiol (London) 1979; 291: 61–73.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
337
11.
Avrith DB, Fitzsimons JT. Increased sodium appetite in the rat induced by intracranial administration of components of the renin-angiotensin system. J Physiol (London) 1980; 301: 349–364.
12.
Baertschi AJ, Vallet PG. Osmosensitivity of the hepatic portal vein area and vasopressin release in rats. J Physiol 1981; 315: 217–230.
13.
Barney CC, Katovich MJ, Fregly MJ. The effect of acute administration of an angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor, captopril (SQ 14,225), on experimentally induced thirsts in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1980; 212: 53–57.
14.
Bartanusz V, Jezova D. Angiotensin II induces reduced oxytocin but normal corticotropin release in rats with lesions of the subfornical organ. Fundam Clin Pharmacol 1994; 8: 539–545.
15.
Bealer SL, Johnson AK. Sodium consumption following lesions surrounding the anteroventral third ventricle. Brain Res Bull 1979; 4: 287–290.
16.
Bealer SL, Johnson AK. Preoptic–hypothalamic periventricular lesions alter food-associated drinking and circadian rhythms. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1980; 94: 547–555.
17.
Bellin SI, Bhatnagar RK, Johnson AK. Periventricular noradrenergic systems are critical for angiotensin-induced drinking and blood pressure responses. Brain Res 1987; 403: 105–112.
18.
Bellin SI, Landas SK, Johnson AK. Localized injections of 6-hydroxydopamine into lamina terminalis-associated structures: Effects on experimentally-induced drinking and pressor responses. Brain Res 1987; 416: 75–83.
19.
Bellin SI, Landas SK, Johnson AK. Selective catecholamine depletion of structures along the ventral lamina terminalis: Effects on experimentally-induced drinking and pressor responses. Brain Res 1988; 456: 9–16.
20.
Best JB, Bett JHN, Coghlan JP, Cran EJ, Scoggins BA. Circulating angiotensin-II and aldosterone levels during dietary sodium restriction. Lancet 1971; 2: 1353–1354.
21.
Bjo¨rklund A, Lindvall O. Catecholaminergic brain stem regulatory systems. In: Mountcastle VB, Bloom FE, Geiger SR, Eds. Handbook of Physiology, Section 1, The Nervous System, Vol. IV. Bethesda: American Physiological Society, 1986: 155–235.
22.
Black RM, Weingarten HP, Epstein AN, Maki R, Schulkin J. Transection of the stria terminalis without damage to the medial amygdala does not alter behavioral sodium regulation in rats. Acta Neurobiol Exp 1992; 52: 9–15.
23.
Blackburn RE, Demko AD, Hoffman GE, Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Central oxytocin inhibition of angiotensin-induced salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1992; 263: R1347– R1353.
24.
Blackburn RE, Samson WK, Fulton RJ, Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Central oxytocin and ANP receptors mediate osmotic inhibition of salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1995; 269: R245–R251.
25.
Blackburn RE, Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Central oxytocin mediates inhibition of sodium appetite by naloxone in hypovolemic rats. Neuroendocrinology 1992; 56: 255–263.
26.
Blackburn RE, Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Acute effects of ethanol on ingestive behavior in rats. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1994; 18: 924–930.
27.
Blair-West JR, Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Weisinger RS. Angiotensin-related sodium appetite and thirst in cattle. Am J Physiol 1988; 255: R205–R211.
28.
Blake WD, Lin KK. Hepatic portal vein infusion of glucose and sodium solutions on the control of saline drinking in the rat. J Physiol 1978; 274: 129–139.
29.
Blass EM, Epstein AN. A lateral preoptic osmosensitive zone for thirst in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1971; 76: 378–394.
30.
Blass EM, Hanson DG. Primary hyperdipsia in the rat following septal lesions. J Comp Physiol Psych 1970; 70(1): 87–93.
FIN 153
338
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
31.
Blass EM, Nussbaum AI, Hanson DG. Septal hyperdipsia: Specific enhancement of drinking to angiotensin in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974; 87(3): 422–439.
32.
Bott E, Denton DA, Weller S. The effect of angiotensin II infusion, renal hypertension and nephrectomy on salt appetite of sodium-deficient sheep. Aust J Exp Biol Med Sci 1967; 45: 595–612.
33.
Bourque CW, Oliet SHR, Richard D. Osmoreceptors, osmoreception, and osmoregulation. Front Neuroendocrinol 1994; 15: 231–274.
34.
Brody MJ, Johnson AK. Role of the anteroventral third ventricle region in fluid and electrolyte balance, arterial pressure regulation, and hypertension. Front Neuroendocrinol 1980; 6: 249–292.
35.
Brownfield MS, Reid IA, Ganten D, Ganong WF. Differential distribution of immunoreactive angiotensin and angiotensin-converting enzyme in rat brain. Neuroscience 1982; 7: 1759– 1769.
36.
Bryant RW, Epstein AN, Fitzsimons JT, Fluharty SJ. Arousal of a specific and persistent sodium appetite in the rat with continuous intracerebroventricular infusion of angiotensin II. J Physiol (London) 1980; 301: 365–382.
37.
Buggy J, Fisher AE. Evidence for a dual central role for angiotensin in water and sodium intake. Nature 1974; 250: 733–735.
38.
Buggy J, Fisher AE, Hoffman W, Johnson AK, Phillips MI. Ventricular obstruction: Effect on drinking induced by intracranial injection of angiotensin. Science 1975; 190: 72–74.
39.
Buggy J, Hoffman WE, Phillips MI, Fisher AE, Johnson AK. Osmosensitivity of rat third ventricle and interactions with angiotensin. Am J Physiol 1979; 236: R75–R82.
40.
Buggy J, Johnson AK. Anteroventral third ventricle periventricular ablation: Temporary adipsia and persisting thirst deficits. Neurosci Lett 1977; 5: 177–182.
41.
Buggy J, Johnson AK. Preoptic–hypothalamic periventricular lesions: Thirst deficits and hypernatremia. Am J Physiol 1977; 233: R44–R52.
42.
Buggy J, Johnson AK. Angiotensin-induced thirst: Effects of third ventricular obstruction and periventricular ablation. Brain Res 1978; 149: 117–128.
43.
Buggy J, Jonklaas J. Sodium appetite decreased by central angiotensin blockade. Physiol Behav 1984; 32: 737–742.
44.
Camargo LA, Saad WA, Renzi A, de Luca LA Jr, Goncalves JR, Menani JV. Hypothalamic lesions increase saline ingestion induced by injection of angiotensin II into AV3V in rats. Am J Physiol 1991; 261: R647–R651.
45.
Chai SY, Allen AM, Adam WR, Mendelsohn FA. Local actions of angiotensin II: Quantitative in vitro autoradiographic localization of angiotensin II receptor binding and angiotensin converting enzyme in target tissues. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 1986; 8(Suppl. 10): S35–S39.
46.
Chiaraviglio E. Effect of renin–angiotensin system on sodium intake. J Physiol (London) 1976; 255: 57–66.
47.
Chiaraviglio E, Perez Guaita MF. Anterior third ventricle (A3V) lesions and homeostasis regulation. J Physiol (Paris) 1984; 79: 446–452.
48.
Choi-Kwon S, McCarty R, Baertschi AJ. Splanchnic control of vasopressin secretion in conscious rats. Am J Physiol 1990; 259: E19–E26. 49. Chwalbinska-Moneta J. Role of hepatic portal osmoreception in the control of ADH release. Am J Physiol 1979; 236(6): E603–E609. 50.
Cizek LJ, Semple RE, Huang KC, Gregersen MI. Effect of extracellular electrolyte depletion on water intake in dogs. Am J Physiol 1951; 164: 415–422. 51. Clemente CD, Sutin J, Silverstone JT. Changes in electrical activity of the medulla on the intravenous injection of hypertonic solutions. Am J Physiol 1957; 188: 193–198. 52. Colombari DSA, Menani JV, Johnson AK. Forebrain angiotensin type 1 receptors and parabrachial serotonin in the control of NaCl and water intake. Am J Physiol 1996; 271: R1470–1476.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
339
53.
Contreras RJ, Stetson PW. Changes in salt intake after lesions of the area postrema and the nucleus of the solitary tract in rats. Brain Res 1981; 211: 355–366.
54.
Cooling MJ, Day MD. Angiotensin-induced drinking in the cat. In: Peters G, Fitzsimons JT, Peterson-Haefeli L, Eds. Control Mechanisms of Drinking. New York: Springer Verlag, 1975: 132–135.
55.
Correa FMA, Plunkett LM, Saavedra JM. Quantitative distribution of angiotensinconverting enzyme (kininase II) in discrete areas of the rat brain by autoradiography with computerized microdensitometry. Brain Res 1986; 375: 259–266.
56.
Cunningham ET Jr, Miselis RR, Sawchenko PE. The relationship of efferent projections from the area postrema to vagal motor and brain stem catecholamine-containing cell groups: An axonal transport and immunohistochemical study in the rat. Neuroscience 1994; 58(3): 635–648.
57.
Cunningham JT, Johnson AK. Models for the integration of humoral and neural factors critical to body fluid homeostasis. In: Yoshida S, Share L, Eds. Recent Progress in Posterior Pituitary Hormones. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1988: 97–105.
58.
Cunningham JT, Johnson AK. Decreased norepinephrine in the ventral terminalis region is associated with angiotensin II drinking response deficits following local 6-hydroxydopamine injections. Brain Res 1989; 480: 65–71.
59.
Cunningham JT, Johnson AK. The effects of central norepinephrine infusions on drinking behavior induced by angiotensin after 6-hydroxydopamine injections into the anteroventral region of the third ventricle (AV3V). Brain Res 1991; 558: 112–116.
60.
Curtis KS, Verbalis JG, Striker EM. Area postrema lesions in rats appear to disrupt rapid feedback inhibition of fluid intake. Brain Res 1996; 726: 31–38.
61.
Cushny AR. The Secretion of the Urine. London: Longmans Green, 1926; 135–137.
62.
Daly JJ, Roe JW, Horrochs P. A comparison of sodium excretion following the infusion of saline into systemic and portal veins in the dog: Evidence for a hepatic role in the control of sodium excretion. Clin Sci 1967; 33: 481–487.
63.
de Jong W, Petty M. Chemical stimulation of the nucleus of the solitary tract and the resulting blood pressure response. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 1982; 4: S77–S80.
64.
De Luca LA Jr, Galaverna O, Schulkin J, Yao S-Z, Epstein AN. The anteroventral wall of the third ventricle and the angiotensinergic component of need-induced sodium intake in the rat. Brain Res Bull 1992; 28: 73–87.
65.
Denton DA. The study of sheep with permanent unilateral parotid fistulae. Q J Expl Physiol 1957; 43: 72–95.
66.
DeOlmos J, Alheid GF, Belatrimino CA. Amygdala. In: Paxinos G, Ed. The Rat Nervous System, Vol. 1, Forebrain and Midbrain. Orlando: Academic Press, 1985: 223–334.
67.
DiBona GF. Neural regulation of renal tubular sodium reabsorption and renin secretion. Fed Proc 1985; 44: 2816–2822.
68.
Dietrickx K. The subfornical organ, a specialized osmoreceptor. Naturwissenschaften 1963; 50: 163–164.
69.
DiNicolantonio R. Angiotensin converting enzyme blockade and thirst. Clin Exp Theor Practice 1984; A6(10&11): 2025–2029.
70.
DiNicolantonio R, Mendelsohn FA, Hutchinson JS. Central angiotensin converting enzyme blockade and thirst. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983; 18: 731–735.
71.
Edwards GL, Beltz TG, Power JD, Johnson AK. Rapid onset ‘‘need-free’’ sodium appetite after lesions of the dorsomedial medulla. Am J Physiol 1993; 264: R1242–R1247.
72.
Edwards GL, Johnson AK. Enhanced drinking after excitotoxic lesions of the parabrachial nucleus in the rat. Am J Physiol 1991; 261: R1039–R1044.
73.
Edwards GL, Ritter RC. Area postrema lesions increase drinking to angiotensin and extracellular dehydration. Physiol Behav 1982; 29: 943–947.
FIN 153
340
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
74.
Elam M, Svensson TH, Thoren P. Differentiated cardiovascular afferent regulation of locus coeruleus neurons and sympathetic nerves. Brain Res 1985; 358: 77–84.
75.
Elfont RM, Epstein AN, Fitzsimons JT. Involvement of the renin–angiotensin system in captopril-induced sodium appetite in the rat. J Physiol (London) 1984; 354: 11–27.
76.
Elfont RM, Fitzsimons JT. Renin dependence of captopril-induced drinking after ureteric ligation in the rat. J Physiol (London) 1983; 343: 17–30.
77.
Elfont RM, Fitzsimons JT. The effect of captopril on sodium appetite in adrenalectomized and deoxycorticosterone-treated rats. J Physiol (London) 1985; 365: 1–12.
78.
Eng R, Miselis RR. Polydipsia and abolition of angiotensin-induced drinking after transections of subfornical organ efferent projections in the rat. Brain Res 1981; 225: 200–206.
79.
Epstein AN. Epilogue: Retrospect and prognosis. In: Epstein AN, Kissileff HR, Stellar E, Eds. The Neuropsychology of Thirst: New Findings and Advances in Concepts. Washington, DC: VH Winston 1973; 315–322.
80.
Epstein AN. Mineralocorticoids and cerebral angiotensin may act together to produce sodium appetite. Peptides 1982; 3: 493–494.
81.
Ernsberger P, Giuliano R, Willette RN, Granata AR, Reis DJ. Hypotensive action of clonidine analogues correlates with binding affinity at imidazole and not alpha-2-adrenergic receptors in the rostral ventrolateral medulla. J Hypertens 1988; 6: S554–S557.
82.
Evered MD. Investigating the role of angiotensin II in thirst: Interactions between arterial pressure and the control of drinking. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 1992; 70: 791–797.
83.
Evered MD, Robinson MM. The renin–angiotensin system in drinking and cardiovascular responses to isoprenaline in the rat. J Physiol (London) 1981; 316: 357–367. Evered MD, Robinson MM. Effects of captopril on salt appetite in sodium-replete rats and rats treated with desoxycorticosterone acetate (DOCA). J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1983; 225: 416–421. Evered MD, Robinson MM. Increased or decreased thirst caused by inhibition of angiotensinconverting enzyme in the rat. J Physiol (London) 1984; 348: 573–588. Evered MD, Robinson MM, Richardson MA. Captopril given intracerebroventricularly, subcutaneously or by gavage inhibits angiotensin-converting enzyme activity in the rat brain. Eur J Pharmacol 1980; 68: 443–449. Evered MD, Robinson MM, Rose PA. Effect of arterial pressure on drinking and urinary responses to angiotensin II. Am J Physiol 1988; 254: R67–R74. Falk JL. Water intake and NaCl appetite in sodium depletion. Psychol Rep 1965; 16: 315–325. Falk JL. Serial sodium depletion and NaCl solution intake. Physiol Behav 1966; 1: 75–77. Felix D, Phillips MI. Inhibitory effects of luteinizing hormone releasing hormone (LH-RH) on neurons in the organum vasculosum lamina terminalis (OVLT). Brain Res 1979; 169: 204–208.
84.
85. 86.
87. 88. 89. 90.
91.
Ferguson AV, Kasting NW. Activation of subfornical organ efferents stimulates oxytocin secretion in the rat. Regul Pept 1987; 18: 93–100. 92. Ferguson AV, Kasting NW. Angiotensin acts at the subfornical organ to increase plasma oxytocin concentrations in the rat. Regul Pept 1988; 23: 343–352. 93.
Ferguson AV, Renaud LP. Systemic angiotensin acts at subfornical organ to facilitate activity of neurohypophysial neurons. Am J Physiol 1986; 251: R712–R717.
94.
Ferrario CM, Gildenberg PL, McCubbin JW. Cardiovascular effects of angiotensin mediated by the central nervous system. Circ Res 1972; 30: 257–262. 95. Findlay ALR, Epstein AN. Increased sodium intake is somehow induced in rats by intravenous angiotensin II. Horm Behav 1980; 14: 86–92. 96. Fitts DA. Effects of lesions of the ventral ventral median preoptic nucleus or subfornical organ on drinking and salt appetite after deoxycorticosterone acetate or yohimbine. Behav Neurosci 1991; 105(5): 721–726.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
341
97.
Fitts DA, Masson DB. Forebrain sites of action for drinking and salt appetite to angiotensin or captopril. Behav Neurosci 1989; 103: 865–872.
98.
Fitts DA, Masson DB. Preoptic angiotensin and salt appetite. Behav Neurosci 1990; 104: 643–650.
99.
Fitts DA, Thunhorst RL. Rapid elicitation of salt appetite by an intravenous infusion of angiotensin II in rats. Am J Physiol 1996; 270: R1092–R1098.
100.
Fitts DA, Thunhorst RL, Simpson JB. Diuresis and reduction of salt appetite by lateral ventricular infusions of atriopeptin II. Brain Res 1985; 348: 118–124.
101.
Fitts DA, Tjepkes DS, Bright RO. Salt appetite and lesions of the ventral part of the ventral median preoptic nucleus. Behav Neurosci 1990; 104: 818–827.
102.
Fitzsimons JT. Drinking by rats depleted of body fluid without increase in osmotic pressure. J Physiol (London) 1961; 159: 297–309.
103.
Fitzsimons JT. The role of a renal thirst factor in drinking induced by extracellular stimuli. J Physiol (London) 1969; 201: 349–368.
104.
Fitzsimons JT. Thirst. Physiol Rev 1972; 52: 468–561.
105.
Fitzsimons JT. Some historical perspectives in the physiology of thirst. In: Epstein AN, Kissileff HR, Stellar E, Eds. The Neuropsychology of Thirst: New Findings and Advances in Concepts. Washington, DC: VH Winston, 1973: 3–33.
106.
Fitzsimons JT. The renin–angiotensin system and drinking behavior. In: Gispen WH, van Wimersma Greidanus TB, Bohus B, de Wied D, Eds. Hormones, Homeostasis and the Brain. Prog Brain Res 1975; 42: 215–233.
107.
Fitzsimons JT. The Physiology of Thirst and Sodium Appetite. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1979.
108.
Fitzsimons JT, Elfont RM. Angiotensin does contribute to drinking induced by caval ligation in rat. Am J Physiol 1982; 243: R558–R562.
109.
Fitzsimons JT, Moore-Gillon MJ. Drinking and antidiuresis in response to reductions in venous return in the dog: Neural and endocrine mechanisms. J Physiol (London) 1980; 308: 403–416.
110.
Fitzsimons JT, Simons BJ. The effect on drinking in the rat of intravenous infusion of angiotensin, given alone or in combination with other stimuli of thirst. J Physiol 1969; 203(1): 45–57.
111.
Fitzsimons JT, Stricker EM. Sodium appetite and the renin–angiotensin system. Nature 1971; 231: 58–60.
112.
Fitzsimons JT, Wirth JB. The renin-angiotensin system and sodium appetite. J Physiol (London) 1978; 274: 63–80.
113.
Fluharty SJ, Epstein AN. Sodium appetite elicited by intracerebroventricular infusion of angiotensin II in the rat. II. Synergistic interaction with systemic mineralocorticoids. Behav Neurosci 1983; 97: 746–758.
114.
Fluharty SJ, Manaker S. Sodium appetite elicited by intracerebroventricular infusion of angiotensin II in the rat. I. Relation to urinary sodium excretion. Behav Neurosci 1983; 97: 738–745.
115.
Foote SL, Bloom FE, Aston-Jones G. Nucleus locus ceruleus: New evidence of anatomical and physiological specificity. Physiol Rev 1983; 63: 844–914.
116.
Fregly MJ. Effect of the angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor, captopril, on NaCl appetite of rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1980; 215: 407–412.
117.
Fregly MJ, Rowland NE. Role for a2-adrenoceptors in experimentally-induced drinking in rats. In: de Caro G, Epstein AN, Massi M, Eds. The Physiology of Thirst and Sodium Appetite. New York: Plenum Press 1984: 509–519.
118.
Fregly MJ, Rowland NE. Role of renin–angiotensin–aldosterone system in NaCl appetite of rats. Am J Physiol 1985; 248: R1–R11.
FIN 153
342
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
119.
Fregly MJ, Waters IW. Effect of mineralocorticoids on spontaneous sodium chloride appetite of adrenalectomized rats. Physiol Behav 1966; 1: 65–74.
120.
Galaverna O, De Luca LA Jr, Schulkin J, Yao SZ, Epstein AN. Deficits in NaCl ingestion after damage to the central nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. Brain Res Bull 1992; 28: 89–98.
121.
Galaverna OG, Seeley RJ, Berridge KC, Grill HJ, Epstein AN, Schulkin J. Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. I. Effects on taste reactivity, taste aversion learning and sodium appetite. Behav Brain Res 1993; 59: 11–17.
122.
Ganong WF. Review of Medical Physiology, 17th ed. Norwalk, Connecticut: Appleton & Lange, 1995.
123.
Gardiner SM, Bennett T. Pressor contributions from angiotensin and vasopressin after polyethylene glycol. Am J Physiol 1986; 251: R769–R774.
124.
Gardiner TW, Jolley JR, Vagnucci AH, Stricker EM. Enhanced sodium appetite in rats with lesions centered on nucleus medianus. Behav Neurosci 1986; 100: 531–535.
125.
Gardiner TW, Verbalis JG, Stricker EM. Impaired secretion of vasopressin and oxytocin in rats after lesions of nucleus medianus. Am J Physiol 1985; 249: R681–R688.
126.
Gillis RA, Gatti PJ, Quest JA. Mechanism of the antihypertensive effect of alpha 2-agonists. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 1985; 7: S38–S44.
127.
Gilman A. The relation between blood osmotic pressure, fluid distribution and voluntary water intake. Am J Physiol 1937; 120: 323–328.
128.
Gloor P. Electrophysiological studies of the amygdala (stimulation and recording): Their possible contributions to the understanding of neural mechanisms of aggression. In: Fields WS, Sweet WH, Eds. Neural Basis of Violence and Aggression. St Louis: Warren H. Green, 1975: 5–38.
129.
Gordon FJ, Johnson AK. Electrical stimulation of the septal area in the rat: Prolonged suppression of water intake and correlation with self-stimulation. Brain Res 1981; 206: 421–430.
130.
Grace JE. Central nervous system lesions and saline intake in the rat. Physiol Behav 1968; 3: 387–393.
131.
Greenleaf JE. Problem: Thirst, drinking behavior, and involuntary dehydration. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1992; 24: 645–656.
132.
Grill HJ, Schulkin J, Flynn FW. Sodium homeostasis in chronic decerebrate rats. Behav Neurosci 1986; 100: 536–543.
133.
Gronan RJ, York DH. Effects of angiotensin II and acetylcholine on neurons in the preoptic area. Brain Res 1978; 154: 172–177.
134.
Guyton AC, Hall JE. Textbook of Medical Physiology, 9th ed. Philadelphia: Saunders, 1996.
135.
Haberich FJ. Osmoreception in the portal circulation. Fed Proc 1968; 27: 1137–1141.
136.
Harland D, Gardiner SM, Bennett T. Cardiovascular and dipsogenic effects of angiotensin II administered i.c.v. in Long–Evans and Brattleboro rats. Brain Res 1988; 455: 58–64.
137.
Harvey JA, Hunt HF. Effect of septal lesions on thirst in the rat as indicated by water consumption and operant responding for water reward. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1965; 59: 49–56.
138.
Herbert H, Saper CB. Cholecystokinin-, galanin-, and corticotropin-releasing factor-like immunoreactive projections from the nucleus of the solitary tract to the parabrachial nucleus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1990; 293: 581–598.
139.
Hertting G, Meyer DK. Effect of converting enzyme blockade on isoprenaline- and angiotensin I-induced drinking. Br J Pharmacol 1974; 52: 381–386.
140.
Hoffman GE, Smith MS, Verbalis JG. c-Fos and related immediate early gene products as markers of activity in neuroendocrine systems. Front Neuroendocrinol 1993; 14: 173–213.
141.
Holmes JH, Cizek LJ. Observations on sodium chloride depletion in the dog. Am J Physiol 1951; 164: 407–414.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
343
142.
Holstege G, Meiners L, Tan K. Projections of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the mesencephalon, pons and medulla oblongata in the cat. Exp Brain Res 1985; 58: 379–391.
143.
Hosutt JA, Rowland N, Stricker EM. Impaired drinking responses of rats with lesions on the subfornical organ. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1981; 95: 104–113.
144.
Hsiao S, Epstein AN, Camardo JS. The dipsogenic potency of peripheral angiotensin II. Horm Behav 1977; 8: 129–140.
145.
Hutchinson JS, Mendelsohn FAO, Doyle AE. Blood pressure responses of conscious normotensive and spontaneously hypertensive rats to intracerebroventricular and peripheral administration of captopril. Hypertension 1980; 2: 546–550.
146.
Hyde TM, Miselis RR. Area postrema and adjacent nucleus of the solitary tract in water and sodium balance. Am J Physiol 1984; 247: R173–R182.
147.
Jahr CE, Nicoll RA. Noradrenergic modulation of dendrodendritic inhibition in the olfactory bulb. Nature 1982; 297: 227–229.
148.
Jalowiec JE. Sodium appetite elicited by furosemide: Effects of differential dietary maintenance. Behav Biol 1974; 10: 313–327.
149.
Johnson AK. Role of the periventricular tissue of the anteroventral third ventricle in body fluid homeostasis. In: Meyer P, Schmitt H, Eds. Nervous System and Hypertension. New York: Wiley, 1979: 106–114.
150.
Johnson AK. Neurobiology of the periventricular tissue surrounding the anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V) and its role in behavior, fluid balance, and cardiovascular control. In: Smith OA, Galosy RA, Weiss SM, Eds. Circulation, Neurobiology and Behavior. New York: Elsevier, 1982: 277–295.
151.
Johnson AK. Role of the periventricular tissue surrounding the anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V) in the regulation of body fluid homeostasis. In: Schrier RW, Ed. Vasopressin. New York: Raven Press, 1985: 319–331.
152.
Johnson AK. The periventricular anteroventral third ventricle (AV3V): Its relationship with the subfornical organ and neural systems involved in maintaining body fluid homeostasis. Brain Res Bull 1985; 15: 595–601.
153.
Johnson AK. Brain mechanisms in the control of body fluid homeostasis. In: Gisolfi CV, Lamb DR, Eds. Perspectives in Exercise Science and Sports Medicine: Fluid Homeostasis During Exercise. Carmel, IN: Cooper Publishing, 1990: 347–419.
154.
Johnson AK. Circumventricular organs. In: Adelman G, Smith B, Eds. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1997, in press.
155.
Johnson AK, Beltz TG, Edwards GL. A rapid onset sodium appetite induced by yohimbine. Soc Neurosci Abstr 1989; 15: 964.
156.
Johnson AK, Beltz TG, Edwards GL. The nature of the sodium appetite induced by yohimbine. FASEB J 1990; 4: A1195.
157.
Johnson AK, Buggy J. A critical analysis of the site of action for the dipsogenic effect of angiotensin II. In: Buckley JP, Ferrario CM, Lokhandwala MF, Eds. Central Actions of Angiotensin and Related Hormones. Elmsford, New York: Pergamon, 1977: 357–386.
158.
Johnson AK, Buggy J. Periventricular preoptic–hypothalamus is vital for thirst and normal water economy. Am J Physiol 1978; 234: R122–R129.
159.
Johnson AK, Buggy J, Fink GD, Brody MJ. Prevention of renal hypertension and of the central pressor effect of angiotensin by ventromedial hypothalamic ablation. Brain Res 1981; 205: 255–264.
160.
Johnson AK, Cunningham JT. Brain mechanisms and drinking: The role of the lamina terminalis-associated systems and extracellular thirst. Kidney Int 1987; 21: S35–S42.
161.
Johnson AK, Cunningham JT, Thunhorst RL. Integrative role of the lamina terminalis in the regulation of cardiovascular and body fluid homeostasis. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1996; 23: 183–191.
FIN 153
344
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
162.
Johnson AK, Edwards GL. The neuroendocrinology of thirst: Afferent signaling and mechanisms of central integration. In: Pfaff DW, Ganten D, Eds. Current Topics in Neuroendocrinology, Vol 10. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, 1990; 149–190.
163.
Johnson AK, Edwards GL. Central projections of osmotic and hypervolaemic signals in homeostatic thirst. In: Ramsay DJ, Booth DA, Eds. Thirst—Physiological and Psychological Aspects. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1991: 149–175.
164.
Johnson AK, Gross PM. Sensory circumventricular organs and brain homeostatic pathways. FASEB J 1993; 7: 678–686.
165.
Johnson AK, Hoffman WE, Buggy J. Attenuated pressor responses to intracranially injected stimuli and altered antidiuretic activity following preoptic–hypothalamic periventricular ablation. Brain Res 1978; 157: 161–166.
166.
Johnson AK, Mann JFE, Rascher W, Johnson JK, Ganten D. Plasma angiotensin II concentrations and experimentally induced thirst. Am J Physiol 1981; 240: R229–R234.
167.
Johnson AK, Phillips MI, Mohring J, Ganten D. Angiotensin-induced drinking in rats with hereditary hypothalamic diabetes insipidus. Neurosci Lett 1977; 4: 327–330.
168.
Johnson AK, Robinson MM, Mann JFE. The role of the renal renin–angiotensin system in thirst. In: de Caro G, Epstein AN, Massi M, Eds. The Physiology of Thirst and Sodium Appetite. New York: Plenum, 1986: 161–180.
169.
Johnson AK, Schwob JE. Cephalic angiotensin receptors mediating drinking to systemic angiotensin II. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1975; 3: 1077–1084.
170.
Johnson AK, Thunhorst RL. Sensory mechanisms in the behavioral control of body fluid balance: Thirst and salt appetite. In: Fluharty SJ, Morrison AR, Sprague JM, Stellar E, Eds. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, Vol. 16. New York: Academic Press, 1995: 145–176.
171.
Johnston JB. Further contributions to the study of the evolution of the forebrain. J Comp Neurol 1923; 35: 337–481.
172.
Joy MD, Lowe RD. Evidence that the area postrema mediates the central cardiovascular response to angiotensin II. Nature 1970; 228: 1303–1304.
173.
Kadekaro M, Creel M, Terrell ML, Lekan HA, Gary HE, Eisenberg HM. Effects of sinoaortic denervation on glucose utilization in the subfornical organ and pituitary neural lobe during administration of angiotensin II. Peptides 1989; 10: 103–108.
174.
Katovich MJ, Barney CC, Fregly MJ, McCaa RE. Effect of an angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor (SQ 14,225) on beta-adrenergic and angiotensin-induced thirsts. Eur J Pharmacol 1979; 56: 123–130.
175.
Kaufman S. Role of right atrial receptors in the control of drinking in the rat. J Physiol 1984; 349: 389–396.
176.
Kaufman S. Renal and hormonal responses to prolonged atrial stretch. Am J Physiol 1990; 258: R1286–R1290.
177.
King MS, Baertschi AJ. Central neural pathway mediating splanchnic osmosensation. Brain Res 1991; 550: 268–278.
178.
Klingbeil CK, Brooks VL, Quillen Jr EW, Reid IA. Effect of baroreceptor denervation on stimulation of drinking by angiotensin II in conscious dogs. Am J Physiol 1991; 260: E333–E337.
179.
Knepel W, Meyer DK. Influence of converting enzyme inhibition on the release of vasopressin induced by angiotensin. Br J Pharmacol 1980; 71: 337–341.
180.
Knowles WD, Phillips MI. Angiotensin II responsive cells in the organum vasculosum lamina terminalis (OVLT) recorded in hypothalamic brain slices. Brain Res 1980; 197: 256–259.
181.
Kosten T, Contreras RJ, Stetson PW, Ernest MJ. Enhanced saline intake and decreased heart rate after area postrema ablations in rat. Physiol Behav 1983; 31: 777–785.
182.
Kraly FS, Kim YM, Dunham LM, Tribuzio RA. Drinking after intragastric NaCl without increase in systemic plasma osmolality in rats. Am J Physiol 1995; 269: R1085–R1092.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
345
183.
Kucharczyk J. Inhibition of angiotensin-induced water intake following hexamethonium pretreatment in the dog. Eur J Pharmacol 1988; 148: 213–219.
184.
Lal J, Atkinson J. Involvement of the renin–angiotensin system in the dipsogenic effect of morphine. Arch Int Pharmacodyn 1985; 278: 273–291.
185.
Lanc¸a AJ, van der Kooy D. A serotonin-containing pathway from the area postrema to the parabrachial nucleus in the rat. Neuroscience 1985; 14: 1117–1126.
186.
Lehr D, Goldman HW, Casner P. Renin–angiotensin role in thirst: Paradoxical enhancement of drinking by angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor. Science 1973; 182: 1031–1034.
187.
Lind RW, Ganten D. Angiotensin. In: Bjorklund A, Hokfelt T, Kuhar MJ, Eds. The Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Neuropeptides in the CNS, Vol 9. New York: Elsevier, 1990: 165–286.
188.
Lind RW, Johnson AK. Central and peripheral mechanisms mediating angiotensin-induced thirst. In: Ganten D, Printz M, Phillips MI, Scholkens BA, Eds. The Renin Angiotensin System in the Brain. Berlin/Heidelberg/New York: Springer-Verlag, 1982: 353–364.
189.
Lind RW, Johnson AK. Subfornical organ–median preoptic connections and drinking and pressor responses to angiotensin II. J Neurosci 1982; 2: 1043–1051.
190.
Lind RW, Johnson AK. A further characterization of the effects of AV3V lesions on ingestive behavior. Am J Physiol 1983; 245: R83–R90.
191.
Lind RW, Ohman LE, Lansing MB, Johnson AK. Transection of subfornical organ neural connections diminishes the pressor response to intravenously infused angiotensin II. Brain Res 1983; 275: 361–364. Lind RW, Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. The subfornical organ and the integration of multiple factors in thirst. Physiol Behav 1984; 32: 69–74. Lind RW, Van Hoesen GW, Johnson AK. An HRP study of the connections of the subfornical organ of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1982; 210: 265–277. Loewy AD, Burton H. Nuclei of the solitary tract: Efferent projections to the lower brain stem and spinal cord of the cat. J Comp Neurol 1978; 181(2): 421–449. Lopez MD, Pfister SL, Brennan TJ, Haywood JR. The effect of yohimbine on thirst. Fed Proc 1983; 42: 610. Lynch KR, Hawelu-Johnson CL, Guyenet PG. Localization of brain angiotensinogen mRNA by hybridization histochemistry. Brain Res 1987; 388(2): 149–158. Mangiapane ML, Thrasher TN, Keil LC, Simpson JB, Ganong WF. Deficits in drinking and vasopressin secretion after lesions of the nucleus medianus. Neuroendocrinol 1983; 37: 73–77. Mann JFE, Johnson AK, Ganten D. Plasma angiotensin II: Dipsogenic levels and angiotensingenerating capacity of renin. Am J Physiol 1980; 238: R372–R377. Mann JFE, Johnson AK, Ganten D, Ritz E. Thirst and the renin–angiotensin system. Kidney Int 1987; 32(Suppl. 21): S27–S34.
192. 193. 194. 195. 196. 197.
198. 199. 200.
Mantyh PW, Hunt SP. Neuropeptides are present in projection neurones at all levels in visceral and taste pathways: From periphery to sensory cortex. Brain Res 1984; 299: 297–312. 201. Marriott HL. Water and salt depletion. In: Pitts RF, Ed. American Lectures in Physiology. Springfield, IL: CC Thomas, 1950. 202. Mason WT. Supraoptic neurones of rat hypothalamus are osmosensitive. Nature 1980; 287: 154–157. 203. Massi M, Gentili L, Perfumi M, de Caro G, Schulkin J. Inhibition of salt appetite in the rat following injection of tachykinins into the medial amygdala. Brain Res 1990; 513: 1–7. 204.
Masson DB, Fitts DA. Subfornical organ connectivity and drinking to captopril or carbachol in rats. Behav Neurosci 1989; 103: 873–880. 205. McCance RA. Experimental sodium chloride deficiency in man. Proc R Soc London 1936; 119b: 245–268.
FIN 153
346
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
206.
McCance RA. Medical problems in mineral metabolism. III. Experimental human salt deficiency. Lancet 1936; 1: 823–830.
207.
McEwen OR. Salt loss as a common cause of ill-health in hot climates. Lancet 1935; 1: 1015.
208.
McKinley MJ, Blaine EH, Denton DA. Brain osmoreceptors, cerebrospinal fluid electrolyte composition and thirst. Brain Res 1974; 70: 532–537.
209.
McKinley MJ, Congiu M, Miselis RR, Oldfield BJ, Pennington G. The lamina terminalis and osmotically stimulated vasopressin secretion. In: Yoshida S, Share L, Eds. Recent Progress in Posterior Pituitary Hormones. New York: Elsevier, 1988: 117–124.
210.
McKinley MJ, Denton DA, Weisinger RS. Sensors for antidiuresis and thirst—osmoreceptors or CSF sodium detectors? Brain Res 1978; 141(1): 89–103.
211.
McKinley MJ, McAllen RM, Mendelsohn FAO, Allen AM, Chai SY, Oldfield BJ. Circumventricular organs: Neuroendocrine interfaces between the brain and the hemal milieu. Front Neuroendocrinol 1990; 11: 91–127.
212.
McRae-Degueurce A, Bellin SI, Landas SK, Johnson AK. Fetal noradrenergic transplants into amine-depleted basal forebrain nuclei restore drinking to angiotensin. Brain Res 1986; 374: 162–166.
213.
McRae-Degueurce A, Cunningham JT, Bellin S, Landas S, Wilkin L, Johnson AK. Fetal noradrenergic cell suspensions transplanted into amine-depleted nuclei of adult rats. Ann NY Acad Sci 1987; 495: 757–759.
214.
Meikle AD, Kaufman S. Stretch-induced reduction in atrial content of natriuretic factor is locally mediated. Am J Physiol 1988; 254: R284–R288. Menani JV, Beltz TG, Johnson AK. Effects of lidocaine injections into the lateral parabrachial nucleus on dipsogenic and pressor responses to central angiotensin II in rats. Brain Res 1995; 695: 250–252. Menani JV, Johnson AK. Lateral parabrachial serotonergic mechanisms: Angiotensininduced pressor and drinking responses. Am J Physiol 1995; 269: R1044–R1049. Menani JV, Johnson AK. Cholecystokinin action on the lateral parabrachial nucleus to inhibit salt appetite. FASEB J 1996; 10: A672. Menani JV, Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. Lateral parabrachial nucleus and serotonergic mechanisms in the control of salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1996; 270: R162–R168. Mendelsohn FAO, Quirion R, Saavedra JM, Aguilera G, Catt KJ. Autoradiographic localization of angiotensin II receptors in rat brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1984; 81: 1575–1579. Michell AR. The Clinical Biology of Sodium. The Physiology and Pathophysiology of Sodium in Mammals. New York: Elsevier Science, 1995. Miller AD, Leslie RA. The area postrema and vomiting. Front Neuroendocrinol 1994; 15: 301–320. Miselis RR. The efferent projections of the subfornical organ of the rat: A circumventricular organ within a neural network subserving water balance. Brain Res 1981; 230: 1–23.
215.
216. 217. 218. 219. 220. 221. 222. 223.
Miselis RR, Shapiro RE, Hand PJ. Subfornical organ efferents to neural systems for control of body water. Science 1979; 205: 1022–1025. 224. Moe KE, Weiss ML, Epstein AN. Sodium appetite during captopril blockade of endogenous angiotensin II formation. Am J Physiol 1984; 247: R356–R365. 225.
Moga MM, Saper CB, Gray TS. Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: Cytoarchitecture, immunohistochemistry, and projection to the parabrachial nucleus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1989; 283: 315–332. 226. Mohring J, Petri M, Mohring B. Salt appetite during the early phase of renal hypertension in rats. Pflugers Arch 1975; 356: 153–158. 227.
Moore SD, Guyenet PG. Effect of blood pressure and clonidine on the activity of A2 noradrenergic neurons. Soc Neurosci Abstr 1983; 9: 549. 228. Moore-Gillon MJ, Fitzsimons JT. Pulmonary vein–atrial junction stretch receptors and the inhibition of drinking. Am J Physiol 1982; 242: R452–R457.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
347
229.
Moran JS, Blass EM. Inhibition of drinking by septal stimulation in rats. Physiol Behav 1976; 17: 23–27.
230.
Morita H, Ishiki K, Hosomi H. Effects of hepatic NaCl receptor stimulation on renal nerve activity in conscious rabbits. Neurosci Lett 1991; 123: 1–3.
231.
Moss KN. Some effects of high air temperatures and muscular exertion upon colliers. Proc R Soc London 1923; 95b: 181–200.
232.
Nachman M, Ashe JH. Effects of basolateral amygdala lesions on neophobia, learned taste aversions, and sodium appetite in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974; 87: 622–643.
233.
Negro Vilar A, Gentil CG, Covian MR. Alterations in sodium chloride and water intake after septal lesions in the rat. Physiol Behav 1967; 2: 167–170.
234.
Ng KK, Vane JR. Fate of angiotensin I in the circulation. Nature 1968; 218: 144–150.
235.
Nicolaidis S, Ishibashi S, Gueguen B, Thornton SN, de Beaurepaire R. Iontophoretic investigation of identified SFO angiotensin responsive neurons firing in relation to blood pressure changes. Brain Res Bull 1983; 10: 357–363.
236.
Niijima A. Afferent discharges from osmoreceptors in the liver of the guinea pig. Science 1969; 166: 1519–1520.
237.
Nitabach MN, Schulkin J, Epstein AN. The medial amygdala is part of a mineralocorticoidsensitive circuit controlling NaCl intake in the rat. Behav Brain Res 1989; 35: 127–134.
238.
Norgren R. Projections from the nucleus of the solitary tract in the rat. Neuroscience 1978; 3: 207–218.
239.
Norgren R. The central organization of the gustatory and visceral afferent systems in the nucleus of the solitary tract. In: Katsuki Y, Norgren R, Sato M, Eds. Brain Mechanisms of Sensation. New York: Wiley, 1981: 143–160.
240.
Ohman L, Callahan M, Kirby R, Johnson AK. Alpha-2 adrenergic antagonist stimulation of water intake in the rat. Fed Proc 1985; 44: 723.
241.
Ohman LE, Johnson AK. Lesions in lateral parabrachial nucleus enhance drinking to angiotensin II and isoproterenol. Am J Physiol 1986; 251: R504–R509.
242.
Ohman LE, Johnson AK. Brain stem mechanisms and the inhibition of angiotensin-induced drinking. Am J Physiol 1989; 256: R264–R269.
243.
Ohman LE, Johnson AK. Role of lateral parabrachial nucleus in the inhibition of water intake produced by right atrial stretch. Brain Res 1995; 695: 275–278.
244.
Olsson K. Studies on central regulation of secretion and antidiuretic hormone (ADH) in the goat. Acta Physiol Scand 1969; 77: 465–474.
245.
Palkovits M. The role of the subfornical organ in the salt and water balance. Naturwissenschaften 1966; 53: 336.
246.
Passo SS, Thornborough JR, Rothballer AB. Hepatic receptors in control of sodium excretion in anesthetized cats. Am J Physiol 1973; 224: 373–375.
247.
Pawloski CM, Fink GD. Circulating angiotensin II and drinking behavior in rats. Am J Physiol 1990; 259: R531–R538.
248.
Peck JW, Novin D. Evidence that osmoreceptors mediating drinking in rabbits are in the lateral preoptic area. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1971; 74: 134–147.
249.
Pettinger WA, Keeton TK, Campbell WB, Harper DC. Evidence for a renal a-adrenergic receptor inhibiting renin release. Circ Res 1976; 38: 338–346.
250.
Phillips MI, Felix D, Hoffman WE, Ganten D. Angiotensin-sensitive sites in the brain ventricular system. In: Cowan WM, Ferrendelli JA, Eds. Soc Neurosci Symposia 1977; 2: 308–339.
251.
Pickel VM, Chan J, Ganten D. Dual peroxidase and colloidal gold-labeling study of angiotensin converting enzyme and angiotensin-like immunoreactivity in the rat subfornical organ. J Neurosci 1986; 6: 2457–2469.
FIN 153
348
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
252.
Plunkett LM, Shigematsu K, Kurihara M, Saavedra JM. Localization of angiotensin II receptors along the anteroventral third ventricle area of the rat brain. Brain Res 1987; 405: 205–212.
253.
Price JL, Russchen FT, Amaral DG. The limbic region III: The amygdaloid complex. In: Bjo¨rklund A, Ho¨kfelt T, Swanson LW, Eds. Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Vol. 5, Integrated Systems of the CNS, Part 1. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1987: 279–388.
254.
Quillen EW Jr, Reid IA, Keil LC. Cardiac and arterial baroreceptor influences on plasma vasopressin and drinking. In: Cowley AW, Liard J-F, Ausiello DA, Eds. Vasopressin: Cellular and Integrative Functions. New York: Raven Press, 1988: 405–411.
255.
Quintin L, Gillon JY, Ghignone M, Renaud B, Pujol JF. Baroreflex-linked variations of catecholamine metabolism in the caudal ventrolateral medulla: An in vivo electrochemical study. Brain Res 1987; 425: 319–336.
256.
Reid IA, Nolan PL, Wolf JA, Keil LC. Suppression of vasopressin secretion by clonidine: Effect of alpha-adrenoreceptor antagonists. Endocrinology 1979; 104(5): 1403–1406.
257.
Rettig R, Johnson AK. Aortic baroreceptor deafferentation diminishes saline-induced drinking in rats. Brain Res 1986; 370: 29–37.
258.
Ricardo JA, Koh ET. Anatomical evidence of direct projections from the nucleus of the solitary tract to the hypothalamus, amygdala and other forebrain structures in the rat. Brain Res 1978; 153: 1–26.
259.
Richter CP. Increased salt appetite in adrenalectomized rats. Am J Physiol 1936; 115: 155–161.
260.
Richter CP. Sodium chloride and dextrose appetite of untreated and treated adrenalectomized rats. Endocrinology 1941; 29: 115–125.
261.
Richter CP. Total self regulatory functions in animals and human beings. Harvey Lectures 1943; 38: 63–103.
262.
Robinson MM, Evered MD. Effects of systemic and intracranial inhibition of angiotensinconverting enzyme on isoproterenol-induced drinking in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 1983; 90: 343–348.
263.
Robinson MM, Evered MD. Pressor action of intravenous angiotensin II reduces drinking response in rats. Am J Physiol 1987; 252: R754–R759.
264.
Rogawski MA, Aghajanian GK. Modulation of lateral geniculate neurone excitability by noradrenaline microiontophoresis of locus coeruleus stimulation. Nature 1980; 287: 731– 734.
265.
Rogers RC, Novin D, Butcher LL. Electrophysiological and neuroanatomical studies of hepatic portal osmo- and sodium-receptive afferent projections within the brain. J Auton Nerv Syst 1979; 1: 183–202.
266.
Rowland NE, Caputo FA, Fregly MJ. Water intake induced in rats by serotonin and 5-hydroxytryptophan: Different mechanisms? Brain Res Bull 1987; 18: 501–508.
267.
Rowland NE, Fregly MJ. Comparison of the effects of the dipeptidyl peptidase inhibitors captopril, ramipril, and enalapril on water intake and sodium appetite of Sprague–Dawley rats. Behav Neurosci 1988; 102: 953–960.
268.
Rowland NE, Fregly MJ, Li B-H, Smith GC. Action of angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors in rat brain: Interaction with isoproterenol assessed by Fos immunocytochemistry. Brain Res 1994; 654: 34–40.
269.
Sakaguchi K, Chai SY, Jackson B, Johnston CI, Mendelsohn FA. Blockade of angiotensin converting enzyme in circumventricular organs of the brain after oral lisinopril administration demonstrated by quantitative in vitro autoradiography. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1987; 14: 155–158.
270.
Sakaguchi K, Chai SY, Jackson B, Johnston CI, Mendelsohn FA. Differential angiotensinconverting enzyme inhibition in brain after oral administration of perindopril demonstrated by quantitative in vitro autoradiography. Neuroendocrinology 1988; 48: 223–228.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
349
271.
Sakaguchi K, Chai SY, Jackson B, Johnston CI, Mendelsohn FA. Inhibition of tissue angiotensin converting enzyme. Quantitation by autoradiography. Hypertension 1988; 11: 230–238. 272. Sakai RR, Chow SY, Epstein AN. Peripheral angiotensin II is not the cause of sodium appetite in the rat. Appetite 1990; 15: 161–170. 273. 274. 275. 276. 277.
Sakai RR, Fine WB, Epstein AN, Frankmann SP. Salt appetite is enhanced by one prior episode of sodium depletion in the rat. Behav Neurosci 1987; 101(5): 724–731. Sakai RR, Nicolaidis S, Epstein AN. Salt appetite is suppressed by interference with angiotensin II and aldosterone. Am J Physiol 1986; 251: R762–R768. Saper CB, Reis DJ, Joh T. Medullary catecholamine inputs to the anteroventral third ventricular cardiovascular regulatory region in the rat. Neurosci Lett 1983; 42: 285–291. Saphir W. Chronic hypochloremia simulating psychoneurosis. JAMA 1945; 129: 510–512. Schadt JC. Experimental observations—animals. In: Secher NH, Pawelczyk JA, Ludbrook J, Eds. Blood Loss and Shock. Boston: Little, Brown, 1994: 11–23.
278.
Schelling P, Hutchinson JS, Ganten U, Sponer G, Ganten D. Impermeability of the blood– cerebrospinal fluid barrier for angiotensin II in rats. Clin Sci Mol Med 1976; 3: 399s–402s.
279.
Schiffrin EL, Genest J. Mechanism of captopril-induced drinking. Am J Physiol 1982; 242(11): R136–R140. Schiffrin EL, Gutkowska J, Genest J. Mechanism of captopril-induced renin release in conscious rats. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 1981; 167: 327–332. Schulkin J, Eng R, Miselis RR. The effects of disconnecting the subfornical organ on behavioral and physiological responses to alterations of body sodium. Brain Res 1983; 263: 351–355. Seeley RJ, Galaverna O, Schulkin J, Epstein AN, Grill HJ. Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. II. Effects on intraoral NaCl intake. Behav Brain Res 1993; 59: 19–25. Shapiro RE, Miselis RR. The central neural connections of the area postrema of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1985; 234: 344–364. Sibbald JR, Hubbard JI, Sirett NE. Responses from osmosensitive neurons of the rat subfornical organ in vitro. Brain Res 1988; 461: 205–214. Simpson JB, Epstein AN, Camardo JS Jr. Localization of receptors for the dipsogenic action of angiotensin II in the subfornical organ of the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1978; 92: 581–601. Simpson JB, Routtenberg A. Subfornical organ: Site of drinking elicitation by angiotensin II. Science 1973; 181: 1172–1175. Sladek CD, Johnson AK. Effect of anteroventral third ventricle lesions on vasopressin release by organ-cultured hypothalamo-neurohypophyseal explants. Neuroendocrinology 1983; 37: 78–84. Snyder RL Jr, Sutin J. Effect of lesions of the medulla oblongata on electrolyte and water metabolism in the rat. Exp Neurol 1961; 4: 424–435. Sofroniew MV. Direct reciprocal connections between the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and dorsomedial medulla oblongata: Evidence from immunohistochemical detection of tracer proteins. J Comp Neurol 1983; 213: 399–405.
280. 281.
282. 283. 284. 285.
286. 287.
288. 289.
290. Stella A, Zanchetti A. Functional role of renal afferents. Physiol Rev 1991; 71: 659–682. 291. Strauss MB. Body Water in Man. The Acquisition and Maintenance of the Body Fluids. Boston: Little, Brown, 1957. 292. Stricker EM. Osmoregulation and volume regulation in rats: Inhibition of hypovolemic thirst by water. Am J Physiol 1969; 217(1): 98–105. 293.
Stricker EM. The renin–angiotensin system and thirst: A reevaluation. II. Drinking elicited in rats by caval ligation and isoproterenol. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1977; 91: 1220–1231. 294. Stricker EM. Excessive drinking by rats with septal lesions during hypovolemia induced by subcutaneous colloid treatment. Physiol Behav 1978; 21(6): 905–907.
FIN 153
350
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST
295.
Stricker EM. The renin–angiotensin system and thirst: Some unanswered questions. Fed Proc 1978; 37: 2704–2710.
296.
Stricker EM. Thirst and sodium appetite after colloid treatment in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1981; 95: 1–25.
297.
Stricker EM. Thirst and sodium appetite after colloid treatment in rats: Role of the renin–angiotensin–aldosterone system. Behav Neurosci 1983; 97: 725–737.
298.
Stricker EM. Thirst and sodium appetite after colloid treatment in rats with septal lesions. Behav Neurosci 1984; 98(2): 356–360.
299.
Stricker EM. Central control of water and sodium chloride intake in rats during hypovolaemia. In: Ramsay D, Booth D, Eds. Thirst. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1991: 194–203.
300.
Stricker EM, Gannon KS, Smith JC. Thirst and salt appetite induced by hypovolemia in rats: analysis of drinking behavior. Physiol Behav 1992; 51: 27–37.
301.
Stricker EM, Hosutt JA, Verbalis JG. Neurohypophyseal secretion in hypovolemic rats: Inverse relation to sodium appetite. Am J Physiol 1987; 252: R889–R896.
302.
Stricker EM, Jalowiec JE. Restoration of intravascular fluid volume following acute hypovolemia in rats. Am J Physiol 1970; 218(1): 191–196.
303.
Stricker EM, Schreihofer AM, Verbalis JG. Sodium deprivation blunts hypovolemia-induced pituitary secretion of vasopressin and oxytocin in rats. Am J Physiol 1994; 267(5): R1336– R1341.
304.
Stricker EM, Vagnucci AH, McDonald Jr RH, Leenen FH. Renin and aldosterone secretions during hypovolemia in rats: Relation to NaCl intake. Am J Physiol 1979; 237: R45–R51.
305.
Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Central inhibitory control of sodium appetite in rats: Correlation with pituitary oxytocin secretion. Behav Neurosci 1987; 101: 560–567.
306.
Stricker EM, Verbalis JG. Sodium appetite. In: Stricker EM, Ed. Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 10, Neurobiology of Food and Fluid Intake. New York: Plenum Press, 1990: 387–419.
307.
Stricker EM, Wolf G. Blood volume and tonicity in relation to sodium appetite. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1966; 62: 275–279.
308.
Sullivan MJ, Cunningham JT, Nissen R, Allen AM, Coderre E, Renaud LP. Ibotenic acid lesions of the diagonal band of Broca result in exaggerated polyethylene glycol-induced drinking behavior. Soc Neurosci Abstr 1991; 17: 885.
309.
Summy-Long J, Severs WB. Angiotensin and thirst: Studies with a converting enzyme inhibitor and a receptor antagonist. Life Sci 1974; 15: 569–582.
310.
Swanson PP, Timson GH, Frazier E. Some observations of the physiological adjustment of the albino rat to a diet poor in salts when edestin is the source of dietary protein. J Biol Chem 1935; 109: 729–737.
311.
Takamata A, Mack GW, Gillen CM, Nadel ER. Sodium appetite, thirst, and body fluid regulation in humans during rehydration without sodium replacement. Am J Physiol 1994; 266(Regul Int Comp Physiol 35): R1493–R1502.
312.
Tarjan E, Denton DA, McBurnie MI, Weisinger RS. Water and sodium intake of wild and New Zealand rabbits following angiotensin. Peptides 1988; 9: 677–679.
313.
Tarjan E, Ferraro T, May C, Weisinger RS. Converting enzyme inhibition in rabbits: Effects on sodium and water intake/excretion and blood pressure. Physiol Behav 1993; 53: 291–299.
314.
Thoren P. Reflex effects of left ventricular mechanoreceptors with afferent fibres in the vagal nerves. In: Hainsworth R, Kidd C, Linden RJ, Eds. Cardiac Receptors. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1977: 259–277.
315.
Thorn GW, Forsham PH, Emerson K Jr. The Diagnosis and Treatment of Adrenal Insufficiency. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1951.
316.
Thornton SN, Nicolaidis S. Blood pressure effects of iontophoretically applied bioactive hormones in the anterior forebrain of the rat. Am J Physiol 1993; 265: R826–R833.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE
351
317.
Thrasher TN, Brown CJ, Keil LC, Ramsay DJ. Thirst and vasopressin release in the dog: An osmoreceptor or sodium receptor mechanism? Am J Physiol 1980; 238: R333–R339.
318.
Thrasher TN, Keil LC. Regulation of drinking and vasopressin secretion: Role of organum vasculosum laminae terminalis. Am J Physiol 1987; 253: R108–R120.
319.
Thrasher TN, Keil LC, Ramsay DJ. Hemodynamic, hormonal and drinking responses to reduced venous return in the dog. Am J Physiol 1982; 243: R354–R362.
320.
Thunhorst RL. Role of peripheral angiotensin in salt appetite of the sodium-deplete rat. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1996; 20: 101–106.
321.
Thunhorst RL, Ehrlich KJ, Simpson JB. Subfornical organ participates in salt appetite. Behav Neurosci 1990; 104: 637–642.
322.
Thunhorst RL, Fitts DA. Peripheral angiotensin causes salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1994; 267: R171–R177.
323.
Thunhorst RL, Fitts DA, Simpson JB. Separation of captopril effects on salt and water intake by subfornical organ lesions. Am J Physiol 1987; 252(21): R409–R418.
324.
Thunhorst RL, Fitts DA, Simpson JB. Angiotensin-converting enzyme in subfornical organ mediates captopril-induced drinking. Behav Neurosci 1989; 103: 1302–1310.
325.
Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. Angiotensin-dependent salt appetite and the lamina terminalis. XI International Conference on the Physiology of Food and Fluid Intake. Oxford, England, 1993: 94.
326.
Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. Effects of arterial pressure on drinking and urinary responses to intracerebroventricular angiotensin II. Am J Physiol 1993; 264: R211–R217. Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. Renin-angiotensin, arterial blood pressure and salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1994; 266: R458–R465. Thunhorst RL, Johnson AK. The role of arterial pressure and arterial baroreceptors in the modulation of the drinking response to centrally-administered angiotensin II. In: Pleschka K, Gerstberger R, Eds. Integrative and Cellular Aspects of Autonomic Functions: Temperature and Osmoregulation. Paris: John Libbey Eurotext, 1994: 397–405. Thunhorst RL, Kirby RF, Johnson AK. Role of renal nerves in sodium depletion-induced salt appetite. Am J Physiol 1996; 271: R806–R812. Thunhorst RL, Lewis SJ, Johnson AK. Role of arterial baroreceptor input on thirst and urinary responses to intracerebroventricular angiotensin II. Am J Physiol 1993; 265: R591–R595. Thunhorst RL, Lewis SJ, Johnson AK. Effects of sinoaortic baroreceptor denervation on depletion-induced salt appetite. Am J Physiol 1994; 267: R1043–R1049. Thunhorst RL, Morris M, Johnson AK. Endocrine changes associated with a rapidly developing sodium appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1994; 267: R1168–R1173. Tordoff MG, Schulkin J, Friedman MI. Hepatic contribution to satiation of salt appetite in rats. Am J Physiol 1986; 251: R1095–R1102.
327. 328.
329. 330.
331. 332. 333. 334.
Tordoff MG, Schulkin J, Friedman MI. Further evidence for hepatic control of salt intake in rats. Am J Physiol 1987; 253: R444–R449. 335. Toth E, Stelfox J, Kaufman S. Cardiac control of salt appetite. Am J Physiol 1987; 252: R925–R929. 336.
Travis KA, Johnson AK. In vitro sensitivity of median preoptic neurons to angiotensin II, osmotic pressure and temperature. Am J Physiol 1993; 264: R1200–R1205.
337.
Trippodo NC, McCaa RE, Guyton AC. Effect of prolonged angiotensin II infusion on thirst. Am J Physiol 1976; 230: 1063–1066. 338. Ueda H, Katayama S, Kato R. Area postrema—Angiotensin-sensitive site in brain. In: Assaykeen TA, Ed. Control of Renin Secretion: Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, Vol 17. New York: Plenum Press, 1972: 109–116. 339. Unger T, Ganten D, Lang RE, Scholkens BA. Is tissue converting enzyme inhibition a determinant of antihypertensive efficacy of converting enzyme inhibitors? Studies with the
FIN 153
352
JOHNSON AND THUNHORST two different compounds, Hoe498 and MK421, in spontaneously hypertensive rats. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 1984; 6: 872–880.
340.
van der Kooy D, Koda LY. Organization of the projections of a circumventricular organ: The area postrema in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1983; 219: 328–338.
341.
Van Eekelen JAM, Phillips MI. Plasma angiotensin II levels at moment of drinking during angiotensin II intravenous infusion. Am J Physiol 1988; 255: R500–R506.
342.
van Houten M, Schiffrin EL, Mann JF, Posner BI, Boucher R. Radioautographic localization of specific binding sites for blood-borne angiotensin II in the rat brain. Brain Res 1980; 186: 480–485.
343.
Veelken R, Sawin LL, DiBona GF. Epicardial serotonin receptors in circulatory control in conscious Sprague–Dawley rats. Am J Physiol 1990; 258: H466–H472.
344.
Velletri P, Bean BL. Comparison of the time course of action of captopril on angiotensin converting enzyme with the time course of its antihypertensive effect. J Cardiovasc Pharmacol 1981; 3: 1068–1081.
345.
Verney EB. The antidiuretic hormone and the factors which determine its release. Proc R Soc 1947; 135B: 25–106.
346.
Watson WE. The effect of removing the area postrema on the sodium and potassium balances and consumptions in the rat. Brain Res 1985; 359: 224–232.
347.
Weindl A. Neuroendocrine aspects of circumventricular organs. In: Ganong WF, Martini L, Eds. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. New York: Oxford Univ. Press, 1973: 3–33.
348.
Weisinger RS, Blair-West JR, Denton DA, McBurnie M, Osborne PG, Tarjan E, Williams RM. Angiotensin and salt appetite of BALB/c mice. Am J Physiol 1990; 259: R729–R735.
349.
Weisinger RS, Denton DA, Di Nicolantonio R, Hards DK, McKinley MJ, Oldfield B, Osborne PG. Subfornical organ lesion decreases sodium appetite in the sodium-depleted rat. Brain Res 1990; 526: 23–30.
350.
Weisinger RS, Denton DA, Di Nicolantonio R, McKinley MJ. The effect of captopril or enalaprilic acid on the Na appetite of Na-deplete rats. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 1988; 15: 55–65.
351.
Weisinger RS, Denton DA, Di Nicolantonio R, McKinley MJ, Muller AF, Tarjan E. Role of angiotensin in sodium appetite of sodium-deplete sheep. Am J Physiol 1987; 253: R482– R488.
352.
Weisinger RS, Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Muller AF, Mouw D. Cerebrospinal fluid [NA] and salt appetite. Brain Res 1985; 326: 95–105.
353.
Weisinger RS, Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Muller AF, Tarjan E. Angiotensin and Na appetite of sheep. Am J Physiol 1986; 251(20): R690–R699.
354.
Weiss ML, Moe KE, Epstein AN. Interference with central actions of angiotensin II suppresses sodium appetite. Am J Physiol 1986; 250: R250–R259.
355.
Wilkin LD, Patel KP, Schmid PG, Johnson AK. Increased norepinephrine turnover in the median preoptic nucleus following reduced extracellular fluid volume. Brain Res 1987; 423: 369–372.
356.
Wilkins L, Richter CP. A great craving for salt by a child with cortico-adrenal insufficiency. JAMA 1940; 114: 866–868.
357.
Wilson KM, Rowland N, Fregly MJ. Drinking: A final common pathway? Appetite 1984; 5: 31–38.
358.
Wise BL, Ganong WF. Effect of brain stem stimulation on renal function. Am J Physiol 1960; 198: 1291–1295.
359.
Wise BL, Ganong WF. The effect of ablation of the area postrema on water and electrolyte metabolism in dogs. Acta Neuroveg 1960; 22: 14–32.
360.
Wishart TB, Mogenson GJ. Reduction of water intake by electrical stimulation of the septal region of the rat brain. Physiol Behav 1970; 5: 1399–1404.
FIN 153
THIRST AND SALT APPETITE 361.
353
Woodward DJ, Moises HC, Waterhouse BD, Hoffer BJ, Freedman R. Modulatory actions of norepinephrine in the central nervous system. Fed Proc 1979; 38: 2109–2116.
362.
Yang ZF, Epstein AN. Blood-borne and cerebral angiotensin and the genesis of salt intake. Horm Behav 1991; 25: 461–476. 363. Zardetto-Smith AM, Beltz TG, Johnson AK. Role of the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in experimentally-induced salt appetite. Brain Res 1994; 645: 123–134. 364. Zardetto-Smith AM, Johnson AK. Chemical topography of efferent projections from the median preoptic nucleus to pontine monoaminergic cell groups in the rat. Neurosci Lett 1995; 199: 215–219. 365.
366.
367. 368. 369.
Zardetto-Smith AM, Magnuson DJ, Gray TS. Efferent projections from the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the nucleus of the solitary tract and the ventrolateral medulla in the rat. Soc Neurosci Abstr 1989; 15: 265. Zardetto-Smith AM, Thunhorst RL, Cicha MZ, Johnson AK. Afferent signaling and forebrain mechanisms in the behavioral control of extracellular fluid volume. In: North WG, Moses AM, Share L, Eds. The Neurohypophysis: A Window on Brain Function. Ann NY Acad Sci 1993; 689: 161–176. Zhang DM, Epstein AN, Schulkin J. Medial region of the amygdala: Involvement in adrenal–steroid-induced salt appetite. Brain Res 1993; 600: 20–26. Zimmerman MB, Blaine EH, Stricker EM. Water intake in hypovolemic sheep: Effects of crushing the left atrial appendage. Science 1981; 211: 489–491. Zucker IH. Left ventricular receptors: Physiological controllers or pathological curiosities? Basic Res Cardiol 1986; 81: 539–557.
FIN 153