Accepted Manuscript The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism in cats (Felis catus) is associated with “Roughness” assessed by owners Minori Arahori, Yusuke Hori, Atsuko Saito, Hitomi Chijiiwa, Saho Takagi, Yuki Ito, Arii Watanabe, Miho Inoue-Murayama, Kazuo Fujita PII:
S1558-7878(15)00139-2
DOI:
10.1016/j.jveb.2015.07.039
Reference:
JVEB 904
To appear in:
Journal of Veterinary Behavior
Received Date: 28 March 2015 Revised Date:
14 June 2015
Accepted Date: 30 July 2015
Please cite this article as: Arahori, M., Hori, Y., Saito, A., Chijiiwa, H., Takagi, S., Ito, Y., Watanabe, A., Inoue-Murayama, M., Fujita, K., The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism in cats (Felis catus) is associated with “Roughness” assessed by owners, Journal of Veterinary Behavior (2015), doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.07.039. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism in cats (Felis catus) is associated
2
with “Roughness” assessed by owners.
5
Yusuke Hori1, 2
6
Atsuko Saito3
7
Hitomi Chijiiwa1
8
Saho Takagi1
9
Yuki Ito3
SC
Minori Arahori1
M AN U
4
RI PT
3
Arii Watanabe3
11
Miho Inoue-Murayama4, 5
12
Kazuo Fujita1
13
AC C
EP
TE D
10
14
1
15
606-8501, Japan.
16
2
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Chiyoda, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan.
17
3
Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences,
18
The University of Tokyo, 3-8-1 Komaba, Meguro, Tokyo 153-8902, Japan.
Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
4
Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8203, Japan.
20
5
Wildlife Genome Collaborative Research Group, National Institute for Environmental
21
Studies, Onogawa, Tsukuba 305-8506, Japan.
22
RI PT
19
*Address for reprint requests and correspondence: Minori Arahori, Department of
24
Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8501,
25
Japan.
26
Tel: +81757532759.
27
E-mail address:
[email protected] (M Arahori).
M AN U
TE D EP
29
AC C
28
SC
23
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Abstract:
31
Polymorphisms of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) have been hypothesized to
32
correlate with social behavior in humans and other mammals. We examined the
33
relationships between owner-assessed personality and OXTR polymorphisms in cats,
34
which have long a history of domestication by humans. We analyzed the exon1 region of
35
OXTR in 94 DNA samples (57 males and 37 females), and found 3 SNPs (single
36
nucleotide polymorphisms). We focused on the G738A polymorphism. Questionnaires
37
containing 30 questions with 6-point scales were completed by the cats’ owners. Factor
38
analysis for the scores identified four factors, which we named “Openness”,
39
“Friendliness”, “Roughness” and “Neuroticism”. A GLMM analysis revealed that cats
40
with the A allele in the SNP G738A showed significantly higher “Roughness” scores than
41
cats without the A allele. After dividing cats into four sex x neutering groups, only
42
neutered females showed the significant difference between the A allele carriers and non
43
A allele carriers in “Roughness” scores. Age also affected personality: younger cats
44
showed much higher “Openness” scores whereas older cats showed higher “Roughness”
45
scores. This study shows that genetic variation in cats may be linked to their personality
46
traits, a finding that may be useful from the viewpoint of companion animal welfare; for
47
example, to match potential good owners and cats using genetic information.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
30
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
48
Key words:
50
domestic cats; genetic polymorphism; oxytocin receptor gene; personality
RI PT
49
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
51
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
52
INTRODUCTION In recent years, researchers have become increasingly interested in oxytocin, the
54
peptide hormone, which is believed to be related to various social behavior in humans
55
such as pair bonding, nursing for children, and coping with stressors (for review; Carter,
56
2014). Many studies have also focused on oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR)
57
polymorphisms in humans, which underlie individual variation in behavioral tendencies
58
including empathy (e.g. Wu et al., 2012), cooperation (e.g. Haas et al., 2013), psychiatric
59
disorder such as autism (e.g. Lerer et al., 2008), and prosociality (e.g. Israel et al., 2009;
60
Kogan et al., 2011). OXTR polymorphisms are also associated with individual differences
61
in nonhuman primates and other mammals (e.g. Kis et al., 2014; Staes et al., 2014). In
62
particular, recent studies suggest that the oxytocin system of dogs (Canis familiaris),
63
which show human-like communicative skills due to their long history of domestication,
64
correlates with their behavior toward humans. For example, dogs’ behavior (e.g.
65
proximity seeking) to both strangers and owners in unfamiliar situations were different
66
based on their genotype of OXTR (Kis et al., 2014). Dogs taking oxytocin spray showed
67
more affiliation behavior to their owners (Romero et al., 2014), and showed positive
68
expectation bias (Kis et al., 2015) than dogs with placebo spray.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
53
69
By contrast, little research has been done on cats (Felis catus), another companion
70
animal with a long history of domestication (Driscoll et al., 2007; Hu et al., 2014). Like 5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
dogs, cats show several human-like cognitive skills, including social referencing (Merola
72
et al., 2015), following human pointing (Kraus et al., 2014), and vocal recognition (Saito
73
and Shinozuka, 2013). To reveal genetic influences on cat behavior could further reveal
74
the effects of domestication on social behavior as a result of genetic change. However, as
75
yet it is unknown whether genetic polymorphisms in cats influence their social behavior
76
or personality.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
71
We examined whether OXTR polymorphisms in domestic cats was linked to their
78
owner-assessed personality. We conducted a preliminary study to investigate how genetic
79
variation in the oxytocin system affects cats’ personality, as assessed by cat owners.
80
TE D
77
MATERIAL AND METHODS
82
Subjects and DNA collection
EP
81
We collected buccal cell samples from 94 cats in Japan (57 males, 37 females;
84
mean age = 4 years 6.99 months. SD = 47.90 months) after obtaining permission from
85
the owners. Seventy-two cats were neutered (46 males and 26 females). Thirty-eight cats
86
belonged to six “cat cafés,” where customers can freely interact with cats, and fifty-six
87
cats were pets in their owner’s house. The numbers of cats in each cat café were 13, 6, 6,
88
5, 5 and 4, respectively. In Japan, more than 80% of cats are reported as mixed breed
AC C
83
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
(mongrel/moggy) by the owners or as of unidentified breed (Japan Pet Food Association,
90
2014). Sixty-six cats (70.20%) in the present were described as mixed breed. The samples
91
included one mother-offspring pair, one pair of sisters, and a trio of brothers.
92
Genetic analysis
RI PT
89
DNA was extracted from buccal cells using QIAamp blood and tissue kit
94
(QIAGEN, Valencia, CA, USA). We amplified exon1 region in OXTR by polymerase
95
chain reaction (PCR) with 10 μl mixture for each sample, containing LA taq, dNTPs, GC
96
buffer I (TaKaRa, Otsu, Shiga, Japan), template DNA, and the forward (OXTR_cat1F:
97
5’-GCAGCGCCATTCCTTAAA-3’,
98
5’-CATGTTCGCCTCCACCTACT-3’)
99
5’-CGTGATCCACGTGATGTAGG
M AN U
SC
93
TE D
and
the -3’,
reverse
OXTR_cat2F: (OXTR_cat1R: OXTR_cat2R:
5’-ATTCCAAGGCCCACCTCT-3’) primers. PCR condition consisted of 95 ℃
101
preheating for 2 min, 35 cycling of 95℃ for 30s, 60℃ for 30s, 74℃ for 1 min, and 74℃
102
for 10 min as last extension. After PCR, we purified for PCR products using High Pure
103
PCR Product Purification Kit (Roche Diagnostics K.K., Minato, Tokyo, Japan). After
104
checking the purification status by electrophoresing on 1.5% agarose gel, we conducted
105
the sequence reaction with the condition of preheating of 96℃ for 1 min, 25 cycling of
106
96℃ for 10s, 50℃ for 5s, and 60℃ for 1 min. After that, we conducted ethanol
AC C
EP
100
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
precipitation, after which we sequenced the genome using the Applied Biosystems
108
3130/3130xl Genetic Analyzer (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA).
109
Rating of cat personality
RI PT
107
We collected questionnaires that was used to identify personality in Japanese Akita
111
dogs (Konno et al., 2011). The questionnaire contained 30 questions with 6-point scales
112
asking about the personality of the cat, and owners answered it about their cats. We
113
performed parallel analysis and factor analysis to reveal the personality structure of cats,
114
and used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) to examine the effects of the target
115
genotype, age, breed, sex x neutering (because neutering effect was predicted to be
116
different
117
neutered/non-neutered) and environment (cat café or home) on scale-scores on each
118
factor. Because 70.20% of the sample was mixed, we included breed as a random factor.
119
We used R (ver. 3.0.3) for all statistical analysis (R Development Core Team: R, 2009),
120
and also used the libraries such as “psych (Revelle, 2015)”, “GPArotation (Bernaards and
121
Jennrich, 2005)”, “lme4 (Bates et al., 2014)”, “car (Fox and Weisberg, 2011)” and
122
“genetics (Warnes et al., 2013).”
M AN U
SC
110
sex,
we
used
TE D
on
AC C
123 124
four
categories;
male/female
x
EP
depending
RESULTS
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
125
Genotyping Among sequenced 1056bp, dog and human sequences were different from those of
127
cats in 127bp (12.03%) and 132bp (12.50%), respectively. Among 307 amino acids, dogs
128
and humans differed from cats in 19 (6.19%) and 20 (6.51%) amino acids, respectively
129
(Figure S1). We found 3 synonymous SNPs (single nucleotide polymorphisms, C474T,
130
G723A, and G738A) in exon1. Allele frequencies of the SNP G723A were extremely
131
high, and C474T and G738A were strongly linked (Pairwise Linkage Disequilibrium:
132
= 59.87, p < 0.001), so we focused on G738A (Table 1). Allele frequencies did not
133
overstep in G738A as a result of Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (Fisher’s exact test; p =
134
1.00). The OXTR sequence in cats in our study was deposited in the DDBJ database
135
(Accession No. LC029887). We divided genotypes into 2 groups for analysis; A allele
136
carriers and non A allele carriers.
137
Personality structure of cats
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
138
RI PT
126
The number of factors was determined via parallel analysis and the scree plot.
139
Factor analysis with maximum likelihood estimation and a varimax rotation detected four
140
factors (Table S1), which we named “Openness” (highest positive loadings: playful,
141
inquisitive, and curious), “Friendliness” (adaptable, calm, and friendly), “Roughness”
142
(irritable, ‘dominant’, forceful, and moody) and “Neuroticism” (vigilant, nervous, and
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
fearful). We defined factor loadings ≥|0.5| as salient items. No item had salient loading on
144
more than one factor. We calculated scale-score of each factor for each cat. The internal
145
consistencies of the four factors assessed by Cronbach’s α were 0.85 for “Neuroticism”,
146
0.87 for “Roughness”, 0.92 for “Openness”, and 0.90 for “Friendliness”; hence the
147
reliability of the questionnaire was sufficiently high. To evaluate test-retest reliability and
148
consistency of individual personality scores across time, we collected a second
149
questionnaire randomly from the same owners for almost 25% of the sample (12 males
150
and 12 females). The lag between the first and second questionnaire varied from 1 to 19
151
months. Pearson correlation coefficients (r) were 0.63 for “Openness”, 0.65 for
152
“Friendliness”, 0.70 for “Neuroticism”, and 0.76 for “Roughness”, and were all
153
significant at p < 0.01, indicating that personality scores of each cat were stable according
154
to Konno et al. (2011).
155
Association between the genotype and scale-scores
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
156
RI PT
143
A GLMM analysis and type II Wald chi-square tests revealed no significant effect
157
for “Friendliness” and “Neuroticism”, but there was an effect of age on “Openness”:
158
younger cats showed higher “Openness” scores ( = 0.06, = 15.36, standard error
159
= 0.003, 95% Cl=0.99, 1.00, p < 0.001). GLMM showed an effect of OXTR genotype
160
( = 0.07, = 5.10, standard error = 0.24, 95% Cl=0.38, 0.97, p = 0.02) and age
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
( = 0.06, = 4.89, standard error = 0.002, 95% Cl=1.00, 1.01, p = 0.03) on
162
“Roughness”, with older cats and those with the A allele showing higher “Roughness”
163
scores. When dividing samples into four groups (neutered male, neutered female, intact
164
male and intact female) and conducting GLMM for each group, neutered cats of both
165
sexes showed the same tendency in “Roughness” scores, but only neutered females
166
showed a statistically significant effect of genotype (male: = 0.58, =
167
1.16, standard error = 0.33, 95% Cl = 0.36, 1.34, p = 0.28; female: = 0.40,
168
= 8.35, standard error = 0.47, 95% Cl = 0.10, 0.64, p < 0.01; Figure 1). In
169
intact cats of both sexes the tendency was reversed, although the sample size was small
170
and
171
(male: = 0.02, = 0.51, standard error = 0.57, 95% Cl = 0.22, 2.04, p = 0.47;
172
female: = 0.58, = 2.35, standard error = 0.72, 95% Cl = 0.08, 1.36, p =
173
0.13; Figure 1). To test whether differences between subgroups statistically significant,
174
we included sex x genotype, neutering x genotype and sex x neutering x genotype
175
interaction terms for “Roughness” score, but there was no significant effect.
M AN U
SC
RI PT
161
was
TE D
effect
not
statistically
significant
AC C
EP
the
176 177 178
DISCUSSION We found three SNPs in OXTR in cats, and showed that one of these SNPs had an
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
effect on owner-assessed “Roughness” scores. To our knowledge, this is the first
180
demonstration of a link between genetic polymorphisms and personality variation in cats.
181
The present study also gives new information on personality structure in cats. In a
182
previous study that included an analysis of domestic cat personality (Gartner et al., 2014),
183
three factors emerged “Neuroticism” (highest positive loadings: anxious, insecure, and
184
tense), “Impulsiveness” (excitable, active, and playful), “Dominance” (aggressive to
185
conspecifics, bullying, and not ‘submissive’). These factors are similar to those that we
186
found, but we also found a factor that was not identified in the previous study (Gartner et
187
al., 2014). It was possibly because of procedural differences between these two studies
188
(e.g. the number of raters, items of the questionnaires, living environment such as home,
189
cat cafés, or shelters, and whether cats are pets).
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
179
In recent years, felids other than domestic cats (e.g. Gartner and Powell, 2012;
191
Gartner and Weiss, 2013; Gartner et al., 2014) have been being examined from the
192
viewpoint of personality. Our finding about OXTR in cats may be an indicator for
193
personality evolution of felids and its genetic basis. For example, several studies have
194
compared genetic polymorphisms in dopamine D4 receptor gene between dogs and
195
wolves (Canis lupus), and reveal that wolves often have polymorphisms which show high
196
activity-impulsivity in dogs, suggesting natural selection through domestication (e.g.
AC C
EP
190
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Hejjas et al., 2007; Inoue-Murayama, 2012). Comparing our results to those from wild
198
cats and other felids may also reveal whether the mutation in these SNPs took place in
199
cats during domestication, as an adaptation to living in close contact with people.
RI PT
197
There were limitations on this study. First, the G738A SNP is a synonymous SNP
201
coding the same amino acid, but the role of synonymous SNPs are unknown. Several
202
studies reported that synonymous SNPs (e.g. rs2228485, rs237902) have an effect on
203
social behavior in humans (Stankova et al., 2012; Lucht et al., 2013). Silent SNPs may
204
sometimes change the process of protein folding (Komar, 2007), but the mechanism
205
underlying this phenomenon reported in our study is a remaining issue. Second, whether
206
the oxytocin system in cats is generally sex-dependent remains unknown although the
207
significant differences in “Roughness” depending on genotypes in neutered female cats in
208
this study are noteworthy. There is evidence of a significant sex x genotype of OXTR in
209
“Harm Avoidance” scores in humans with the effect of the genotype being stringer in
210
women than in men (Stankova et al., 2012). No significant interaction among sex,
211
neutering and genotype was found in this study, and sex and neutering effect are also
212
unclear, so we need to add sample especially to females to sufficiently conduct subgroup
213
analysis. Third, we found age effect, but we cannot discuss it because of the small sample
214
size and several very old cats (more than 10 years old) in subjects. One possibility for the
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
200
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
215
relationship of age and “Roughness” is that a few items (e.g. “moody”) were strongly
216
related to old cats. In previous studies showing a link between oxytocin system and dogs’ social
218
behavior (Kis et al., 2014; Romero et al., 2014; Kis et al., 2015), the main dependent
219
variable was the dogs’ responses toward humans. In the questionnaire used here, owners
220
were asked about their cats’ everyday activities. Therefore the object of the cats’
221
interactions varied across questions (e.g. stranger, owner’s family, other cats). Future
222
researches may wish to conduct a test battery measuring cats’ behavior toward owners
223
will be needed to focus on human-cat relationships, to allow comparison of results with
224
those reported in dogs.
225
CONCLUSION
EP
226
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
217
This study that cats have four personality factors: “Openness”, “Friendliness”,
228
“Roughness”, and “Neuroticism”. This study also demonstrated the relationships
229
between genetic polymorphisms and personality traits in cats. G738A, one of three SNPs
230
in exon1 of OXTR found in this study, was associated with “Roughness” scores assessed
231
by owners. This finding may have implications for animal welfare, for example, in
232
predicting compatibility in cat-cat or cat-owner relationships, developing data that shows
AC C
227
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
233
what genetic types of cats would match potential owners.
234
Acknowledgements We thank all cats and their owners who participated in our study. We also thank
236
Akitsugu Konno (Teikyo University of Science) for the questionnaire, Mana Tsuzuki and
237
Ayami Hyuga (Kyoto University) for data collection, and James R. Anderson for English
238
correction. This work was supported by the Cooperation Research Program of the
239
Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University. This work was supported by the Japan
240
Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research No.
241
25118003 to AS, Nos. 25118005 and 25290082 to MI-M, Nos. 25118002 and 25240020
242
to KF.
243
Authorship statement
EP
244
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
235
MA designed this study, collected DNA sample and questionnaires, conducted
246
genotyping, analyzed data, and drafted the manuscript. AS, HC, ST, YI, and AW
247
contributed to data collection. YH and MI-M conducted data analysis, and MI-M and KF
248
provided critical discussion regarding the analyses and the manuscript. All authors have
249
approved the final article.
AC C
245
250
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
251 252
Conflict of Interest We have no competing interests.
254
RI PT
253
Ethics statement
This study adhered to the ethical guidelines of Kyoto University, and was approved
256
by the Animal Experiments Committee of the Graduate School of Letters of Kyoto
257
University.
M AN U
SC
255
258
References
260
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S. 2014. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models
TE D
259
261
using
262
http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
264 265 266 267 268
S4.
R
package
version
1.1-7,
Available
at:
EP
and
Bernaards, C.A., Jennrich, R.I., 2005. Gradient Projection Algorithms and Software for
AC C
263
Eigen
ArbitraryRotation Criteria in Factor Analysis, Educational and Psychological Measurement: 65, 676-696. Available at: http://www.stat.ucla.edu/research/gpa.
Carter, C.S., 2014. Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annu Rev Psycholl. 65, 17-39. Driscoll, C.A., Menotti-Raymond, M., Roca, A.L., Hupe, K., Johnson, W.E., Geffen, E.,
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Harley, E.H., Delibes, M., Pontier, D., Kitchener, A.C., Yamaguchi, N., O'Brien
270
S.J., Macdonald, D.W., 2007. The Near Eastern origin of cat domestication.
271
Science. 317, 519-523. (doi: 10.1126/science.1139518)
272
RI PT
269
Fox, J., and Weisberg, S., (2011). An {R} Companion to Applied Regression, Second
273
Edition.
274
http://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion
276
CA:
Sage.
Available
at:
SC
Oaks
M AN U
275
Thousand
Gartner, M.C., Powell D.M., 2012. Personality assessment in snow leopards (Uncia uncia). Zoo Biol. 31(2), 151-165.
Gartner, M.C., Weiss, A., 2013. Scottish wildcat (Felis silvestris grampia) personality
278
and subjective well-being: Implications for captive management. Appl. Anim.
279
Behav. Sci. 147(3), 261-267.
TE D
277
Gartner, M.C., Powell, D.M., Weiss, A., 2014. Personality structure in the domestic cat
281
(Felis silvestris catus), Scottish wildcat (Felis silvestris grampia), clouded leopard
283 284
AC C
282
EP
280
(Neofelis nebulosa), snow leopard (Panthera uncia), and African lion (Panthera leo):
A comparative
study.
J.
Comp.
Psychol.
128,
414-426.
(doi:
10.1037/a0037104)
285
Haas, B.W., Anderson, I.W., Smith, J.M., 2013. Navigating the complex path between the
286
oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and cooperation: an endophenotype approach.
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
287
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7. (doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00801) Hejjas, K., Vas, J., Kubinyi, E., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Miklosi, A., Ronai, Z., 2007. Novel
289
repeat polymorphisms of the dopaminergic neurotransmitter genes among dogs
290
and wolves. Mamm. Genome 18, 871-879. (doi: 10.1007/s00335-007-9070-0)
RI PT
288
Hu, Y., Hu, S., Wang, W., Wu, X., Marshall, F.B., Chen, X., Hou, L., Wang, C., 2014.
292
Earliest evidence for commensal processes of cat domestication. Proc. Natl. Acad.
293
Sci. USA. 111, 116-120. (doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311439110)
295
M AN U
294
SC
291
Inoue-Murayama, M., 2012. Genetic polymorphism as a background of dog behavior. Jpn. J. Anim. Psychol. 62, 91-99. (doi: 10.2502/janip.62.1.2)
Israel, S., Lerer, E., Shalev, I., Uzefovsky, F., Riebold, M., Laiba, E., Bachner-Melman,
297
R., Maril, A., Bornstein, G., Knafo, A., Ebstein, R.P., 2009. The oxytocin receptor
298
(OXTR) contributes to prosocial fund allocations in the dictator game and the social
299
value orientations task. PLoS One 4, e5535. (doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005535)
AC C
EP
TE D
296
300
Japan Pet Food Association., 2014. Factual survey of pet dogs and cats. Available at:
301
http://www.petfood.or.jp/data/chart2014/index.html Accessed at February 6, 2015.
302
Kis, A., Bence, M, Lakatos, G., Pergel, E., Turcsán, B., Pluijmakers, J., Vas, J., Elek, Z.,
303
Brúder, I., Földi, L., Sasvári-Székely, M., Miklósi, Á., Rónai, Z., Kubinyi, E., 2014.
304
Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
305
behavior
306
10.1371/journal.pone.0083993)
in
dogs
(Canis
familiaris).
PLoS
One
9,
e83993.
(doi:
Kis, A., Hernádi, A., Kanizsár, O., Gácsi, M., Topál, J., 2015. Oxytocin induces positive
308
expectations about ambivalent stimuli (cognitive bias) in dogs. Horm. Behav. 69,
309
1-7. (doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.12.004)
SC
RI PT
307
Kogan, A., Saslow, L.R., Impett, E.A., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., Saturn, S.R., 2011.
311
Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and
312
expression of the prosocial disposition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 108,
313
19189-19192. (doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112658108)
315
Komar, A.A., 2007. Genetics. SNPs, silent but not invisible. Science 315, 466-467. (doi:
TE D
314
M AN U
310
10.1126/science.1138239)
Konno, A., Inoue-Murayama, M., Hasegawa, T., 2011. Androgen receptor gene
317
polymorphisms are associated with aggression in Japanese Akita Inu. Biol. Lett. 7,
AC C
318
EP
316
658-660. (doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0087)
319
Kraus, C., van Waveren, C., Huebner, F., 2014. Distractible dogs, constant cats? A test of
320
the distraction hypothesis in two domestic species. Anim. Behav. 93, 173-181.
321
(doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.026)
322
Lerer, E., Levi, S., Salomon, S., Darvasi, A., Yirmiya, N., Ebstein, R.P., 2008.
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship
324
to Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and cognition. Mol. Psychiatry. 13, 980-988.
325
(doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087)
RI PT
323
Lucht, M.J., Barnow, S., Sonnenfeld, C., Ulrich, I., Grabe, H.J., Schroeder, W., Völzke,
327
H., Freyberger, H.J., John, U., Herrmann, F.H., Kroemer, H., Rosskopf, D., 2013.
328
Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and “mind-reading” in
329
humans-an
330
10.3109/08039488.2012.700731)
exploratory
study.
referencing
333
10.1007/s10071-014-0832-2)
336
J.
Psychiat.
67,
15-21.
(doi:
cat–human
communication.
TE D
and
Anim.
Cogn.
1-10.
(doi:
EP
Revelle, W., 2015. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research, Northwestern
University,
Evanston,
Illinois,
USA,
Available
at:
AC C
335
Nord.
Merola, I., Lazzaroni, M., Marshall-Pescini, S., Prato-Previde, E., 2015. Social
332
334
M AN U
331
SC
326
http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych Version = 1.5.1.
337
Romero, T., Nagasawa, M., Mogi, K., Hasegawa, T., Kikusui, T., 2014. Oxytocin
338
promotes social bonding in dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 111, 9085-9090.
339
(doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322868111)
340
R Core Team, 2014. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
341
Foundation
342
http://www.R-project.org/.
344
Statistical
Computing,
Vienna,
Austria.
Available
at:
Saito, A., Shinozuka, K., 2013. Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis
RI PT
343
for
catus). Anim. Cogn. 16, 685-690. (doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.026)
Staes, N., Stevens, J.M.G., Helsen, P., Hillyer, M., Korody, M., Eens, M., 2014. Oxytocin
346
and vasopressin receptor gene variation as a proximate base for inter- and
347
intraspecific behavioral differences in bonobos and chimpanzees. PLoS One 9,
348
e113364. (doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113364)
M AN U
SC
345
Stankova, T., Eichhammer, P., Langguth, B., Sand, P.G., 2012. Sexually dimorphic effects
350
of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variants on Harm Avoidance. Biol. Sex Differ. 3,
351
17. (doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-17)
354
package
EP
353
Warnes, G., Gorjanc, G., Leisch, F., Man, M., 2013. genetics: Population Genetics. R version
1.3.8.1.
Avialable
at:
AC C
352
TE D
349
http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=genetics.
355
Wu, N., Li, Z., Su, Y., 2012. The association between oxytocin receptor gene
356
polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. J Affect. Disord. 138, 468-472. (doi:
357
10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009)
358
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
359
Figure and table caption
361
Figure 1. “Roughness” score for each group of sex x neutering x the SNP G738A
362
genotype (±s.e.).
363
Table 1. Genotype frequencies of 3 SNPs in exon1 of OXTR in cats.
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
360
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1.
G723A
G738A
C/C
C/T
0.28 (26)
0.38 (36)
G/G
G/A
0.87 (82)
0.09 (8)
G/G 0.53 (50)
0.34 (32) A/A
0.04 (4)
G/A
A/A
0.40 (38)
0.06 (6)
AC C
EP
TE D
2
T/T
RI PT
C474T
Frequency (n)
SC
SNP
M AN U
1
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure
M AN U
SC
RI PT
1
2 3
Figure 1.
AC C
EP
TE D
4
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights
2
Arahori et al.: The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism in cats (Felis
3
catus) is associated with dominance assessed by owners.
4
RI PT
1
・Three synonymous SNPs were found in exon1 of oxytocin receptor gene in cats.
6
・Questionnaires completed by cat owners showed four factors in cat personality.
7
・Cats with A allele in G738A showed higher “Roughness” than non A allele carriers.
M AN U
SC
5
AC C
EP
TE D
8
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1
Electronic Supplementary Materials
2
The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism in cats (Felis catus) is associated
4
with “Roughness” assessed by owners.
RI PT
3
Minori Arahori1
7
Yusuke Hori1, 2
8
Atsuko Saito3
9
Hitomi Chijiiwa1
M AN U
6
SC
5
Saho Takagi1
11
Yuki Ito3
12
Arii Watanabe3
13
Miho Inoue-Murayama4, 5
14
Kazuo Fujita1
AC C
EP
TE D
10
15 16
1
17
606-8501, Japan.
18
2
Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Chiyoda, Tokyo 102-0083, Japan. 1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
19
3
20
Sciences, The University of Tokyo, 3-8-1 Komaba, Meguro, Tokyo 153-8902, Japan.
21
4
Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8203, Japan.
22
5
Wildlife Genome Collaborative Research Group, National Institute for Environmental
23
Studies, Onogawa, Tsukuba 305-8506, Japan.
SC
RI PT
Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, Graduate School of Arts and
24
*Address for reprint requests and correspondence: Minori Arahori, Department of
26
Psychology, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, Sakyo, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan.
27
Tel: +81757532759.
28
E-mail address:
[email protected] (M Arahori).
TE D
EP AC C
29
M AN U
25
2
30 31
Figure S1. (a)
AC C
EP
TE D
M AN U
SC
RI PT
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
M AN U
SC
RI PT
32
33
TE D
36
EP
35
Figure S1. (b)
AC C
34
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure legend
38
Figure S1. (a) Sequence alignment of exon1 region of OXTR of cat described in this study,
39
and dog and human. The sequence in cat was deposited in the DDBJ database (Accession
40
No. LC029887). Accession numbers of dog and human are NM_001198659 and
41
NM_000916, respectively. The intron region is shadowed. Locations of primers are
42
surrounded by squares. Square brackets indicate protein coding regions. The numbers
43
above sequence alignment begin from the translation start codon. Dots in dog and human
44
sequences mean that the bases are identical to those of cat. Gaps indicate sequence is
45
deleted. In this study, three new SNPs were found in cat sequence; C474T, G723A,
46
G738A, indicated by squares and bold letters. The human SNPs related to social behavior
47
(Wu et al., 2012; Stankova et al., 2012; Lucht et al., 2013) are indicated by squares and
48
bold letters. (b) Deduced amino acid alignment of oxytocin receptor of cat described in
49
this study, and dog and human. In human, one amino acid change happens because of the
50
SNP rs4686302, indicated by a square.
SC
M AN U
TE D
EP
AC C
51
RI PT
37
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table S1. Factor loadings of items for each of four factors. Factor
Item
“Friendliness”
Playful
0.87
0.15
-0.17
Curious
0.84
0.28
0.01
Inquisitive
0.83
0.11
0.01
Active
0.79
0.23
0.00
Inventive
0.73
0.15
-0.10
Focused
0.60
0.11
-0.26
Mischievous
0.41
-0.03
0.20
0.01
0.21
Adaptable
0.14
0.82
0.03
-0.44
0.89
Friendly
0.17
0.75
-0.17
-0.40
0.78
Calm
0.08
0.74
-0.34
-0.29
0.75
Sociable
0.20
0.70
-0.17
-0.29
0.64
Gentle
0.06
0.57
-0.49
0.05
0.57
Affectionate
0.16
0.35
0.04
-0.03
0.15
-0.11
0.87
0.13
0.80
-0.07
0.75
0.12
0.60
0.13
“Neuroticism”
communalities
-0.12
0.82
-0.17
0.81
-0.26
0.77
-0.11
0.69
0.05
0.57
-0.04
0.44
SC
M AN U
Dominant
-0.10
TE D
Irritable
“Roughness”
RI PT
“Openness”
-0.15
-0.09
0.73
0.20
0.60
Defiant
0.09
-0.21
0.71
0.09
0.56
-0.22
-0.08
0.67
0.09
0.51
0.21
-0.01
0.54
0.15
0.36
-0.07
-0.03
0.39
-0.12
0.17
-0.02
-0.01
0.29
0.16
0.11
0.28
-0.06
0.29
-0.04
0.17
Aggressive Excitable Distractible Restless Impulsive
EP
Moody
AC C
52
Quitting
-0.03
0.01
0.27
-0.11
0.09
Vigilant
-0.23
-0.15
0.14
0.78
0.70
Fearful
-0.11
-0.33
0.12
0.69
0.61
Timid
-0.16
-0.33
0.04
0.67
0.59
Nervous
0.01
-0.15
0.04
0.66
0.46
Cautious
-0.29
-0.35
-0.05
0.55
0.51
Anxious
-0.04
-0.16
0.30
0.44
0.31
Attentive
0.12
0.06
-0.04
0.15
0.04
0.15
0.14
0.11
0.11
Variance explained (%)
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Note: The questionnaire was the same as in a previous study with Japanese Akita dogs
54
(Konno et al., 2011). We rounded off to two decimal places of loadings. Salient loadings
55
(|>.5|) are in boldface. The right-hand side refers to communalities (h2). The left-hand
56
side refers to items.
RI PT
53
SC
57 58
M AN U
59 60
64 65 66 67
EP
63
AC C
62
TE D
61
68 69 70
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
References
72
Konno, A., Inoue-Murayama, M., Hasegawa, T., 2011. Androgen receptor gene
73
polymorphisms are associated with aggression in Japanese Akita Inu. Biol Lett. 7,
74
658-660. (doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0087)
RI PT
71
Lucht, M.J., Barnow, S., Sonnenfeld, C., Ulrich, I., Grabe, H.J., Schroeder, W., Volzke,
76
H., Freyberger, H.J., John, U., Herrmann, F.H., Kroemer, H., Rosskopf, D., 2013.
77
Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and “mind-reading” in
78
humans-an
79
10.3109/08039488.2012.700731)
exploratory
M AN U
SC
75
study.
Nord
J
Psychiat.
67,
15-21.
(doi:
Stankova, T., Eichhammer, P., Langguth, B., Sand, P.G., 2012. Sexually dimorphic effects
81
of oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) variants on Harm Avoidance. Biol. Sex Differ.
82
3, 17. (doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-17)
84 85
EP
Wu, N., Li, Z., Su, Y., 2012. The association between oxytocin receptor gene
AC C
83
TE D
80
polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. J Affect Disord. 138, 468-472. (doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009)
86
8