The power of language: Functional brain network topology of deaf and hearing in relation to sign language experience

The power of language: Functional brain network topology of deaf and hearing in relation to sign language experience

Accepted Manuscript The power of language: functional brain network topology of deaf and hearing in relation to sign language experience Michel R.T. S...

4MB Sizes 0 Downloads 51 Views

Accepted Manuscript The power of language: functional brain network topology of deaf and hearing in relation to sign language experience Michel R.T. Sinke, Jan W. Buitenhuis, Frank van der Maas, Job Nwiboko, Rick M. Dijkhuizen, Eric van Diessen, Willem M. Otte PII:

S0378-5955(18)30235-1

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2018.12.006

Reference:

HEARES 7660

To appear in:

Hearing Research

Received Date: 31 May 2018 Revised Date:

8 December 2018

Accepted Date: 12 December 2018

Please cite this article as: Sinke, M.R.T., Buitenhuis, J.W., van der Maas, F., Nwiboko, J., Dijkhuizen, R.M., van Diessen, E., Otte, W.M., The power of language: functional brain network topology of deaf and hearing in relation to sign language experience, Hearing Research, https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.heares.2018.12.006. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1

The power of language: functional brain network topology of deaf and

2

hearing in relation to sign language experience

3

Michel R.T. Sinke 1 *, Jan W. Buitenhuis 1, Frank van der Maas 3,4, Job Nwiboko 4, Rick M.

5

Dijkhuizen 1, Eric van Diessen 2 § and Willem M. Otte 1,2 §

6

§ Shared last author

7

Biomedical MR Imaging and Spectroscopy Group, Center for Image Sciences, University Medical Center Utrecht and Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

9 10

SC

1

2

M AN U

8

Department of Pediatric Neurology, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands

11 12 13

3

Reabilitação Baseadana Comunidade (RBC) Effata, Bissorã, Oio, Guinea-Bissau 4

CBR Effata, Omorodu Iseke Ebonyi LGA, Ebonyi State, Nigeria

TE D

14 15

AC C

18

EP

16 17

RI PT

4

19

* Corresponding author

20

Michel R.T. Sinke, MSc

21

Biomedical MR Imaging and Spectroscopy Group, Center for Image Sciences, University

22

Medical Center Utrecht, Yalelaan 2, 3584 CM Utrecht, The Netherlands.

23

Email: m.r.t.sinke @umcutrecht.nl. Phone: +31 30 253 5569; Fax: +31 30 253 5561

24

ORCID ID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8185-9209

25 26 1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Abstract

28

Prolonged auditory sensory deprivation leads to brain reorganization. This is indicated by

29

functional enhancement in remaining sensory systems and known as cross-modal plasticity. In

30

this study we investigated differences in functional brain network topology between deaf and

31

hearing individuals. We also studied altered functional network responses between deaf and

32

hearing individuals with a recording paradigm containing an eyes-closed and eyes-open

33

condition.

34

Electroencephalography activity was recorded in a group of sign language-trained deaf (N =

35

71) and hearing people (N = 122) living in rural Africa. Functional brain networks were

36

constructed from the functional connectivity between fourteen electrodes distributed over the

37

scalp. Functional connectivity was quantified with the phase lag index based on bandpass

38

filtered epochs of brain signal. We studied the functional connectivity between the auditory,

39

somatosensory and visual cortex and performed whole-brain minimum spanning tree analysis

40

to capture network backbone characteristics.

41

Functional connectivity between different regions involved in sensory information processing

42

tended to be stronger in deaf people during the eyes-closed condition in both the alpha and

43

beta frequency band. Furthermore, we found differences in functional backbone topology

44

between deaf and hearing individuals. The backbone topology altered during transition from

45

the eyes-closed to eyes-open condition irrespective of deafness, but was more pronounced in

46

deaf individuals. The transition of backbone strength was different between individuals with

47

congenital, pre-lingual or post-lingual deafness. Functional backbone characteristics

48

correlated with the experience of sign language. Overall, our study revealed more insights in

49

functional network reorganization caused by auditory deprivation and cross-modal plasticity.

50

It further supports the idea of a brain plasticity potential in deaf and hearing people. The

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

27

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 51

association between network organization and acquired sign language experience reflects the

52

ability of ongoing brain adaptation in people with hearing disabilities.

53

Keywords

55

Cross-modal plasticity; Deafness; ASL; EEG; Functional networks; Minimum spanning tree

RI PT

54

56 57

Abbreviations

59 60

ASL = American Sign Language; CBR = Community-Based Rehabilitation; EEG = Electroencephalography; MRI = Magnetic Resonance Imaging; MST = Minimum Spanning Tree

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

58

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1. Introduction

62

Impairment or loss of hearing interferes with many activities in daily life, specifically limiting

63

communication with others. This could easily lead to social isolation. The prevalence of this

64

serious disability is greatest in middle- and low-income countries (Durkin, 2002; Stevens et

65

al., 2013; WHO, 2014). While in the United States about two out of every 1,000 children are

66

born with disabling hearing loss (Vohr, 2003), this number is considerably higher in Sub-

67

Saharan Africa where about two percent of the children is born with disabling hearing loss

68

(WHO, 2012). Many of these children have profound hearing loss resulting in absolute

69

deafness. Infectious diseases are a major cause of deafness in these regions (Mulwafu et al.,

70

2016).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

61

Prolonged periods of sensory deprivation often leads to extensive reorganization in the

72

brain. This reorganization is caused by compensatory and cross-modal plasticity (Bavelier and

73

Neville, 2002; Merabet and Pascual-Leone, 2010; Ptito et al., 2001). Brain reorganization

74

after auditory deprivation has been mapped by different functional neuroimaging modalities,

75

such as positron emission tomography, functional near-infrared spectroscopy, functional

76

magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) (Buckley and Tobey,

77

2011; Dewey and Hartley, 2015; Doucet et al., 2006; Finney et al., 2001; Yoshida et al.,

78

2011). Consequently, deaf are better than hearing people at detecting visual stimuli (e.g.

79

Almeida et al., 2015; Bavelier et al., 2006, 2000; Bosworth and Dobkins, 2002; Brozinsky

80

and Bavelier, 2004; Dye et al., 2007; Finney and Dobkins, 2001; Hauser et al., 2007; Neville

81

and Lawson, 1987a, 1987b) and show increased tactile sensitivity as well (Auer et al., 2010;

82

Levänen and Hamdorf, 2001; Meredith and Lomber, 2011). Accordingly, the auditory cortex

83

of deaf is found to be responsive to non-auditory stimuli (e.g. Almeida et al., 2015; Auer et

84

al., 2010; Buckley and Tobey, 2011; Doucet et al., 2006; Finney et al., 2001; Karns et al.,

85

2012; Meredith and Lomber, 2011; Neville and Lawson, 1987b; Scott et al., 2014). For

86

example fMRI and positron emission tomography studies showed that the cortical auditory

AC C

EP

TE D

71

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT and association areas of deaf people are responsive to visual motion stimuli. These regions

88

include the planum temporale (Petitto, 2000; Sadato et al., 2005; Shiell et al., 2016) and

89

primary auditory cortices, like posterior superior temporal gyrus (Almeida et al., 2015; Ding

90

et al., 2015; Karns et al., 2012; Li et al., 2015) and Heschl’s gyrus (Karns et al., 2012; Meyer

91

et al., 2007; Scott et al., 2014; Smith et al., 2011). Cortical reorganization (Campbell and

92

Sharma, 2014, 2013) and responsive auditory cortex to visual stimuli in deaf have also been

93

described with functional brain data recorded with EEG (e.g. Buckley and Tobey, 2011;

94

Doucet et al., 2006; Neville and Lawson, 1987b, 1987a). While this reorganization occurs

95

inevitably as a result of profound deafness, cross-modal plasticity also strongly relates to the

96

acquisition and use of sign language (Meyer et al., 2007; Pénicaud et al., 2013). Furthermore,

97

the extent of cross-modal plasticity is dependent on the age of onset and the duration of

98

deafness (Brotherton et al., 2016; Li et al., 2013). Although stimuli- and task-based

99

approaches have provided valuable insights in cross-modal plasticity, they do not capture the

100

mutual dependency of different functional brain regions as well as the integrative nature of

101

the human brain (Hackett, 2012; Stam and van Straaten, 2012).

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

87

The human brain forms a complex integrative network, which consists of spatially

103

distributed, but functionally connected regions that continuously interact with each other

104

(Bassett et al., 2018; Bassett and Sporns, 2017; Bullmore et al., 2009; Bullmore and Sporns,

105

2009; van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol, 2010). As such, functional brain connectivity and

106

reorganization can be better understood when these processes are not studied in isolation.

107

Brain network analysis explicitly takes the interdependencies between functionally connected

108

regions into account as it shifts emphasis from specific locational changes to global

109

topological alternations. The functional network topology, and potential reorganization

110

therein, can be effectively mapped with several network metrics (Bassett et al., 2018; Bassett

111

and Sporns, 2017; Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). Classical graph analysis describes the human

AC C

EP

102

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT brain as a collection of nodes (i.e. functional brain regions such as the auditory or visual

113

cortex) and edges (i.e. the functional connections between regions), and provides quantitative

114

information on the topological properties of these networks (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009;

115

Heuvel et al., 2012; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). The healthy human brain has been

116

characterized as a complex network that effectively combines global and efficient integration

117

with segregation of functionally specialized brain regions (Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). This

118

unique topology with high integration and segregation is defined as a small-world network

119

organization (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009; Watts and Strogatz, 1998). Deviation from a small-

120

world organization has been related to many neurological and psychiatric disorders (Bassett

121

and Bullmore, 2009; Stam, 2014). Surprisingly few studies have used network analysis to

122

examine reorganization of brain networks in deaf individuals. Pre-lingual deaf adults showed

123

increased network clustering and nodal efficiency compared to controls, whereas brain

124

networks from post-lingual deaf adults did not differ from controls (Kim et al., 2014). This

125

indicates that auditory experience might affect the morphology of brain networks in deaf

126

adults. In another study increased functional connectivity was found between regions within

127

the limbic system, a system involved in sensory information processing (Li et al., 2016).

128

Functional network hubs shifted in the deaf subjects. The small-worldness did not differ

129

between pre-lingual deaf as compared to hearing controls (Li et al., 2016). Yet another study

130

found increased functional connectivity in brain networks in deaf during rest, which was also

131

shown to be related to sign language experience (Malaia et al., 2014).

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

132

RI PT

112

Resting-state functional connectivity measurements can be performed in an eyes-open

133

or eyes-closed condition. Opening and closing the eyes are very basic attention-directing

134

behaviors. Eyes-open is related to ‘exteroceptive’ awareness, characterized by more

135

specialized overt attention and oculomotor activity, whereas eyes-closed is related to

136

‘interoceptive’ awareness, characterized by more integrative multisensory activity and

6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT imagination (Xu et al., 2014). Eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions relate to different brain

138

states (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Zhang et al., 2015) and topological organizations of functional

139

networks (Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Tan et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2014). This is even so in

140

darkness (Hüfner et al., 2009, 2008). In EEG-studies functional networks showed increased

141

global efficiency and decreased clustering during the eyes-open state, specifically in the alpha

142

band, which might be due to alpha desynchronization, i.e. a reduction in the number of

143

functional connections in the eyes-open state (Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Miraglia et al.,

144

2016; Tan et al., 2013).

SC

RI PT

137

Despite the usefulness of classical network analysis in capturing brain network

146

reorganization, it has some intrinsic limitations. The classical network analysis is particularly

147

limited in comparing inter-subject networks with different network densities (van Wijk et al.,

148

2010). The network density is defined as the number of connections relative to the potential

149

number of connections. Commonly used network metrics, such as the clustering coefficient –

150

used to measure network segregation – and average path length – used to measure network

151

integration – are highly affected by the number of connections within a network (Stam et al.,

152

2014; van Wijk et al., 2010). Therefore, comparing healthy and affected (or reorganized)

153

brain networks, in a situation of cross-modal plasticity or alpha desynchronization, might

154

yield biased results (Tewarie et al., 2015; van Wijk et al., 2010; Zalesky et al., 2010).

155

Solutions have been provided. A promising alternative network characterization approach not

156

limited by the network density is the assessment of the network backbone. Network

157

backbones are robustly and efficiently operationalized by the minimum spanning tree (MST)

158

(Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al., 2015). An increasing number of studies have shown the

159

usefulness of MSTs in capturing subtle network changes in brain development and ageing

160

(Boersma et al., 2013; Otte et al., 2015; Smit et al., 2016; Vourkas et al., 2014). MSTs have

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

145

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 161

also been useful in characterizing multiple sclerosis, Alzheimer’s disease and epilepsy

162

(Engels et al., 2015; Tewarie et al., 2014; van Diessen et al., 2016, 2014). The present study therefore investigated the effects of prolonged periods of deafness

164

on the functional brain network backbone topology. To that aim we acquired resting-state

165

EEGs in deaf and hearing individuals. Data were recorded within a unique homogeneous

166

population living in a representative rural region in sub-Saharan Africa. In this region

167

deafness is a common disability and cochlear implants are not available. We hypothesized

168

stronger functional connectivity (i.e. more integration) between auditory cortex and other

169

sensory (i.e. visual and somatosensory) cortices in deaf people, due to cross-modal plasticity.

170

Hence, we also expected differences in functional network backbone topology between

171

controls and deaf. Given the expected cross-modal plasticity as well as auditory sensitivity to

172

visual stimuli, we further expected larger shifts in functional network topology between eyes-

173

open (i.e. ‘exteroceptive’ awareness) and eyes-closed (i.e. ‘interoceptive’ awareness) in deaf.

174

We also explored whether (shifts in) functional backbone topologies were different between

175

congenital, pre-lingual and post-lingual deaf. Lastly, we anticipated a relationship between

176

functional backbone characteristics and years of American Sign Language (ASL) experience.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

178

AC C

177

RI PT

163

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2. Methods

179

2.1 Study setting and ethics

181

The study pipeline as described below is schematically visualized in Figure 1. Our study was

182

conducted at two inclusive primary schools and one inclusive secondary school, located in

183

two separate rural villages in Ebonyi State, southeast Nigeria. The schools are part of a

184

Community-Based Rehabilitation (CBR) program, which implies that all students has to live

185

in surrounding villages and communities, aiming for a full integration within the community.

186

Inclusive education means that both students with and without disabilities are allowed to

187

participate in regular classes, and are supported to learn, contribute and participate in all

188

aspects of the educational program. This inclusive educational approach is a potential strategy

189

to reduce the individual as well as shared burden of disability (Eleweke and Rodda, 2002;

190

Pförtner, 2014). The number of students with and without disabilities enrolled in the schools

191

is approximately equal. Since the integration of deaf people forms one of the main focuses of

192

this CBR program, the majority of students with disability are deaf. Standard ASL forms an

193

integral part of the educational program for more than twenty years. This sign language has to

194

be learned by all teachers and students, both the hearing and the deaf. Besides, deaf students

195

also receive speech therapy. All lessons are taught in English and if a teacher does not yet

196

sufficiently master sign language there will be an interpreter who translates spoken language

197

into sign language for deaf students and vice versa. The CBR program leads to an increasing

198

amount of people within the community that speak sign language. This implies a more regular

199

use, development and integration of sign language by deaf students in their daily lives.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

AC C

200

RI PT

180

Our study was approved by the organizational boards (RBC/CBR Effata), the local

201

health ministry (Izzi, Local Government Area) and the federal government (Ebonyi State

202

House of Assembly, Abakaliki [7-11-2016]) in Nigeria. The study protocol was clearly

203

explained to all students in class before they were asked to participate in the EEG recordings.

204

Written informed consent was obtained from adult participants and caretakers of students

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 205

below eighteen years. In addition, we also obtained assent from the students below eighteen

206

years.

207

2.2 Participants

209

Table 1 shows the demographic information of all participants. We included 193 participants

210

between ten and 43 years old (mean age of 18.5 (standard deviation 6.0); gender: 103 male,

211

90 female), both students and teachers. Sign language experience of participants varied

212

between zero and seventeen years at the time of recording. We selected both hearing (n=122)

213

and deaf (n=71) participants. The pre-lingual deaf participants were all capable of lip reading.

SC

RI PT

208

M AN U

214

2.3 Data acquisition

216

We used a sixteen sensor / fourteen channel EEG monitor configured to sample at 128 Hertz

217

with a 16-bit resolution (EMOTIV Inc, San Francisco, USA), which has been validated and

218

successfully applied in several studies (Aspinall et al., 2013; Badcock et al., 2015, 2013;

219

McMahan et al., 2015; Prause et al., 2016; Yu and Sim, 2016). This wireless headset can be

220

connected to a computer via Bluetooth and is an invaluable tool to collect EEG signals from

221

participants in rural or resource-limited areas, where access to a standard EEG system is often

222

impossible or burdensome. Two sensors were preserved for reference and grounding: the

223

‘common mode sense’ (CMS; located at P3) sensor was used as the active reference for

224

absolute referencing. The ‘driven right leg’ (DRL; located at P4) sensor was used for

225

feedback noise cancelation. The electrodes were located at anterofrontal (AF3, AF4, F3, F4,

226

F7, F8), frontocentral (FC5, FC6), occipital (O1, O2), parietal (P7, P8) and temporal sites (T7,

227

T8), according to the International 10–20 system. Signal quality scores are recorded for each

228

electrode with a range from one to five (no units), with five as best quality score.

AC C

EP

TE D

215

229

Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a sound-attenuated room where the

230

Emotiv headset was placed. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes closed for the first

10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT three minutes and open in the next two minutes, or vice versa. The order of the condition

232

sequence was assigned alternatingly, so that half of the participants started in the eyes-open

233

condition whereas the other half of the participants started in the eyes-closed condition. The

234

researcher kept a log on deviations from the protocol, or unusual events in the environment,

235

that may affect the experiment. Example recordings are shown in Figure 2. As an EEG

236

contains a multitude of overlapping signal waves with distinct amplitudes and frequencies we

237

separated the signal into the five most common frequency ranges. The EEG signals were

238

band-pass filtered into the delta (0.5–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–16 Hz), beta (16–32 Hz)

239

and gamma (32–64 Hz) frequency bands. Examples are shown in Suppl. Figure 1 and 2.

SC

RI PT

231

M AN U

240

2.4 Data cleaning and window selection

242

Time segments were removed from the recordings if i) the research log indicated a deviation

243

from the protocol, ii) the EEG signal quality score was below four for any of the channels,

244

and iii) if the absolute deviation of the gyroscope signals relative to the gyroscope signal

245

median exceeded five times the standard deviation. This threshold was based on visual data

246

inspection (See example in Suppl. Figure 3). Subsequently, the cleaned and filtered time-

247

series were cut into ten-second epochs. Functional connectivity measurements as well as

248

multiple network backbone metrics has been shown to stabilize within recordings if the epoch

249

length is six seconds or longer (Fraschini et al., 2016). In addition, multiple epochs per subject

250

further increase the stability of network backbone metrics (van Diessen et al., 2015).

251

Therefore we used multiple epochs combined with this conservative ten-second length.

EP

AC C

252

TE D

241

253

2.5 Functional connectivity

254

For each epoch a functional network was constructed, which can either be visualized as a

255

connectivity matrix or a network graph (Figure 3). Recorded time-series within each epoch

256

were used to determine functional connectivity (i.e. forming the ‘edges’ in a network)

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT between different electrodes capturing neuronal signals from underlying brain areas (i.e.

258

forming the ‘nodes’ in a network). Functional connectivity was computed and quantified with

259

the phase lag index. This is a measure of the asymmetry of the distribution of instantaneous

260

phase differences between two time series and scales between zero and one (Pillai and

261

Sperling, 2006). It is relative resistant to the influence of common sources, including volume

262

conduction and active reference electrodes. An index of zero indicates no phase coupling (i.e.

263

no functional connectivity) between time series, or coupling with a phase difference centered

264

on zero ± p radians. A non-zero index indicates the presence of phase coupling (i.e. functional

265

connectivity), where higher values indicate stronger functional connections. A more

266

mathematical description of computing the phase lag index can be found elsewhere (Stam et

267

al., 2007).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

257

268

2.6 Functional connectivity strength between auditory, visual and somatosensory cortices

270

Since we expected remodeling of the auditory cortex in deaf people, reflected as enhanced

271

functional connectivity between the auditory and sensory cortices, we characterized in both

272

groups the average phase lag index between sets of electrodes. These sets were the temporal

273

electrodes T7/T8, covering the auditory cortex, the parietal electrodes P7/P8, covering the

274

somatosensory cortex, and the occipital electrodes O1/O2, covering the visual cortex, in eyes-

275

open and eyes-closed conditions.

EP

AC C

276

TE D

269

277

2.7 Minimum spanning tree analysis

278

For each functional network a minimum spanning tree was calculated from the connectivity

279

graph G by applying Kruskal’s algorithm (Kruskal, 1956) (Figure 3B). This tree captures the

280

network’s backbone and is defined as a subset of the network nodes (forming the original

281

weighted graph G) that connects all the nodes and does not contain cycles or loops (Jackson

12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 282

and Read, 2010). Mathematically, a minimum spanning tree T minimizes the sum of the costs

283

of its edges, l(T ) = ∑ l w over the set of all possible spanning trees on G (Hidalgo et al., w∈T

2007). Since we are interested in the strongest functional connections (i.e. the network

285

backbone), we first inverted the edge weights of the functional network before determining

286

the MSTs. Subsequently, several MST metrics were calculated at the nodal or network level.

287

Although some metrics are determined at the nodal level (e.g. degree or betweenness

288

centrality), they can still be used to summarize – or indicate – specific properties for the

289

backbone as a whole. For example with the ‘maximum degree’ or the ‘average strength’,

290

where higher values may indicate higher overall connectivity. The following MST metrics

291

were calculated at nodal or network level:

292

i)

Maximum node degree (nodal): every tree was summarized by taking the maximum node degree: Smax, the node with the maximum number of connections.

ii)

Leaf number (Nleaf) (network): the number of nodes of the tree with exactly one

TE D

293 294

M AN U

SC

RI PT

284

connection to any other node (with maximum degree = 1). A higher leaf number is

296

related to increased global efficiency and integration (Stam et al., 2014; Tewarie et al.,

297

2015). iii)

has a lower bound of two and an upper bound of m = N – 1. The largest possible

299

diameter will decrease with increasing leaf number (Boersma et al., 2013; Stam et al.,

300

2014; Tewarie et al., 2015).

301 302

Diameter (d) (network): the largest distance between any two nodes in a tree, which

AC C

298

EP

295

iv)

Eccentricity (network): the shortest path length between a tree node I and any other

303

node from the tree. Eccentricity decreases when nodes become more central in the

304

tree.

305 306

v)

Radius (nodal): the smallest node eccentricity in the tree. The lower the eccentricity, the more central a node in a tree.

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 307

vi)

weights (Hagmann et al., 2010; Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

308 309

Strength (nodal): the tree node strength is a summation of all nodal connection

vii)

Maximum betweenness centrality (BCmax): a network hub metric which relies on the identification of the number of shortest paths that pass through a node (Rubinov and

311

Sporns, 2010). The more the passages, the higher the betweenness-centrality (i.e.

312

hubness), which is defined by bc i =

1 ( n − 1)( n − 2)

n

RI PT

310



j ≠ k ,k ≠ i , j ≠ i

g jk (i ) g jk

where gjk is the

shortest path between two nodes and gjk(i) is the number of node paths that actually

314

pass through i. We summarized the tree by taking the maximum betweenness

315

centrality.

317

viii)

M AN U

316

SC

313

Closeness centrality (nodal): the inverse of the sum of all distances to other nodes (Sabidussi, 1966).

318

2.8 Statistical analyses

320

All statistical analyses were performed within a Bayesian framework. Differences between

321

groups, eye conditions and interactions between groups and eye conditions were evaluated

322

with Bayes factors. Bayes factors were extracted from Bayesian model comparisons. This was

323

done for all frequency bands separately. We determined the model likelihood of a null model

324

without an interaction effect of group and condition as well as the likelihood of an alternative

325

model with an interaction effect of group and condition for functional connectivity strength.

326

This was done for all MST metrics and for their potential relationship with sign language

327

experience. Bayes factors give the ratio of model likelihoods, indicating which model is

328

supported by the data. For example, if – given the data – a null model (M0) without an effect

329

of condition (i.e. eyes-open versus eyes-closed) on functional connectivity strength has a very

330

low probability, whereas an alternative model (M1) with an effect of condition on functional

331

connectivity strength has a high probability, this would yield a high Bayes factor (e.g. 50).

AC C

EP

TE D

319

14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT This may be interpreted as very strong evidence for M1 as M1 is fifty times more likely than

333

M0 in explaining the data. Table 2 gives an overview of Bayes factors and their interpretation

334

(Raftery, 1995). Since sex (Boersma et al., 2011) and age (Smit et al., 2012) influence

335

functional network topologies, we tested whether models with strong evidence were affected

336

by sex- and age.

RI PT

332

All network analyses, statistical modeling and visualizations were performed in R

338

(http://www.r-project.org/) using the packages igraph, BayesFactor, reshape2, dplyr and

339

ggplot2. Epoch data and scripts are freely available at the Open Science Framework in

340

anonymized form (Otte et al., 2018b) and the GitHub repository (Otte et al., 2018a).

SC

337

M AN U

341

AC C

EP

TE D

342

15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3. Results

343 344

3.1 Functional connectivity strength between auditory, visual and somatosensory cortices

346

Figure 4 and Suppl. Figure 6 show the functional connectivity strength between the occipital

347

cortex and the parietal cortex for the different frequency bands. Occipital-parietal functional

348

connectivity strength was lower in the eyes-open condition compared to the eyes-closed

349

condition in the theta, alpha and beta frequency bands. After transition from eyes-closed to

350

eyes-open, differences in functional connectivity strength were most pronounced in the alpha

351

frequency band with a significant reduction of 54.9% (95% confidence interval (CI): -68.2%

352

to -41.6%) in controls and an even larger reduction of 88.0% (95% CI: -112% to -63.6% in

353

deaf (Figure 4B). A decrease in functional connectivity strength from eyes-closed to eyes-

354

open was also present in the beta frequency band, with a reduction of 27.7% (95% CI: -37.3

355

to -18.0) in controls and 36.1% (95% CI: -49.2 to -23.0) in deaf. These reductions in

356

functional connectivity strength in the alpha and beta band were supported by Bayes factors

357

of respectively >100 (i.e. labeled as ‘extreme evidence’) and 2.6 (‘moderate evidence’)

358

(Table 3).

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

345

Similar trends were seen for functional connectivity strength between the parietal

360

cortex and temporal cortex (Suppl. Figure 4 and Suppl. Figure 7) as well as between the

361

occipital cortex and temporal cortex (Suppl. Figure 5 and Suppl. Figure 8). However, for

362

those connections no significant differences were found between controls and deaf in

363

functional connectivity reduction from eyes-open to eyes-closed, as indicated by Bayes

364

factors of <1 (Suppl. Table 1 and Suppl. Table 2). The parietal-temporal delta-band

365

connectivity strength was significantly more reduced from eyes-closed to eyes-open in deaf,

366

while no reduction was found in controls (Suppl. Figure 5), as indicated by the high Bayes

AC C

359

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 367

factor (Suppl. Table 2). All evidence was still present if age and sex were added to the

368

models.

369

3.2 Functional backbone differences between eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions

371

Transition from eyes-closed to eyes-open initiated visible changes in functional network

372

topology, as indicated by changes in backbone metrics. The most notable effects were found

373

in the alpha and beta bands (Table 4), which are shown in Figure 5. Overall the backbone

374

leaf number, the average and maximum strength as well as the kappa were lower in the eyes-

375

open condition as compared to the eyes-closed condition. In contrast, the diameter,

376

eccentricity, radius as well as the median and maximum closeness centrality were higher in

377

the eyes-open condition compared to the eyes-closed condition. Interestingly, in contrast to

378

deaf, controls did not show a transition effect on some network metrics in the alpha band (i.e.

379

leaf number, eccentricity, radius and diameter).

M AN U

SC

RI PT

370

TE D

380

3.3 Larger functional backbone modifications in deaf

382

Some functional backbone characteristics were different between deaf and hearing controls

383

(Figure 5). In both the alpha and beta band, functional backbone strength was stronger in the

384

eyes-closed condition and weaker in the eyes-open condition in deaf as compared to hearing

385

controls. In the alpha band the leaf number was lower in deaf, whereas the diameter and

386

radius were larger in the eyes-open condition.

AC C

387

EP

381

Several functional backbone characteristics showed larger shifts in deaf than controls

388

when comparing eyes-closed to eyes-open (Figure 5). For both the alpha and beta band,

389

evidence was found for a larger decrease in the average and maximum backbone connectivity

390

strength in deaf (as indicated by the Bayes factors in Table 4). Furthermore, for the alpha

391

band there was moderate to strong evidence for a larger increase in both betweenness and

392

closeness centrality in deaf. Anecdotal evidence was found for larger shifts in leaf number and 17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 393

diameter – in the alpha band – as well as in closeness centrality – in the beta band – in deaf

394

(Table 4). All evidence was still present if age and sex were added to the models. We found that the decrease in average connectivity strength (Suppl. Figure 9) and

396

increase in closeness centrality (Suppl. Figure 10) from eyes-closed to eyes-open was

397

different across the congenital, pre-lingual and post-lingual deafness types. This is supported

398

by the Bayes factors in Suppl. Table 3.

RI PT

395

399

3.4 Relation between backbone characteristics and American Sign Language

401

We investigated the relationship between sign language experience and functional backbone

402

characteristics. Initially, with no distinction made between deaf and controls. Sign language

403

experience was related to altered backbone characteristics in the delta, theta and alpha band.

404

The most pronounced effects were found in the theta band (Table 5), which are visualized in

405

Figures 6 and 7. More specifically, for the theta band an increase in ASL experience was

406

related to a higher average backbone connectivity strength and a lower closeness centrality for

407

the eyes-closed condition (Figure 6). Furthermore, strong evidence was found for a positive

408

relationship between sign language experience and higher backbone connectivity strength for

409

the delta band (Table 5). Figure 7 shows the relationship between sign language experience

410

and functional backbone characteristics for deaf subjects only. Again most pronounced effects

411

were found for the theta band (Table 6) with a positive relation between sign language

412

experience and higher average backbone connectivity strength. All associations were still

413

present if age and sex were added to the models.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

400

414

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4. Discussion

416

The present study used resting-state EEG to map functional network backbone differences

417

between deaf and hearing people. We showed that transition from eyes-closed to eyes-open

418

was associated with changes in functional connectivity strength between occipital and parietal

419

lobes as well as changes in functional backbone topology. In both deaf and hearing, these

420

changes occurred especially in the alpha and beta frequency bands. Moreover, the difference

421

in functional connectivity strength as well as in functional backbone characteristics between

422

eyes-closed to eyes-open tended to be larger in deaf as compared to hearing controls.

SC

RI PT

415

423

4.1 Functional cortical remapping

425

We examined functional connectivity strength between sensory regions involved in auditory,

426

visual and somatosensory information processing. With some evidence we can state that

427

functional connectivity appears stronger in deaf, specifically in the alpha and beta bands. This

428

might indicate increased integration between the different sensory cortical regions in deaf.

429

Our results are in agreement with previous fMRI studies that also found increased audiovisual

430

connectivity in deaf (Bola et al., 2017; Li et al., 2016, 2013; Shiell et al., 2014). With the

431

shifts from an eyes-closed to an eyes-open condition we found a reduction in functional

432

connectivity strength. This is expected given the shift from more ‘interoceptive’ awareness,

433

characterized by integrative multisensory activity, towards more ‘exteroceptive’ awareness

434

focused on attention and oculomotor activity (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Xu et al., 2014).

435

Specifically for the alpha band, this change may be related to ‘alpha desynchronization’

436

during the eyes-open state (Barry et al., 2009; Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2014).

437

Moreover, we found this reduction in functional connectivity strength from eyes-closed to

438

eyes-open to be larger in deaf than hearing controls. This may be explained by long-term

439

auditory deprivation and related cross-modal plasticity mechanisms (Bavelier and Neville,

440

2002; Merabet and Pascual-Leone, 2010). It may be explained more specifically by the

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

424

19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT enhanced sensitivity of the auditory cortex to non-auditory stimuli (e.g. Almeida et al., 2015;

442

Auer et al., 2010; Brozinsky and Bavelier, 2004; Dye et al., 2007; Finney and Dobkins, 2001;

443

Levänen and Hamdorf, 2001; Neville and Lawson, 1987a, 1987b) and increased audiovisual

444

connectivity in deaf (Bola et al., 2017; Li et al., 2016, 2013; Shiell et al., 2014) together with

445

the reduced integrative multi-sensory activity in the eyes-open state (Gómez-Ramírez et al.,

446

2017; Xu et al., 2014).

RI PT

441

447

4.2 Functional backbone differences between eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions

449

We found functional backbone differences between the eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions

450

in both deaf and hearing subjects, mainly in the alpha and beta bands. During the eyes-open

451

condition the functional network backbone showed an increased diameter and eccentricity

452

combined with a decreased leaf number and connectivity strength, although hearing controls

453

did not show differences in diameter and leaf number in the alpha band. These network

454

metrics indicate that the functional backbone topology in the eyes-open condition was more

455

chainlike (i.e. less integrated and with reduced global efficiency), whereas during the eyes-

456

closed condition the topology was more star-like (i.e. more functional integration and

457

increased global efficiency) (Stam et al., 2014). These results also nicely fit with the reduced

458

connectivity and integration in the ‘exteroceptive’ state, as well as the alpha

459

desynchronization, during the eyes-open condition (Barry et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2008;

460

Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Marx et al., 2004; Xu et al., 2014). Other studies reported

461

increased global efficiency and decreased clustering of functional networks in the alpha band

462

(Miraglia et al., 2016; Tan et al., 2013) and beta-band (Gaál et al., 2010; Knyazev et al., 2015)

463

in the eyes-open condition as compared to the eyes-closed condition, which seem to partly

464

contradict our findings. This could potentially be explained by the distinct age groups. One

465

study showed similar effects as our study in participants between 18-35 years, but the

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

448

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT opposite effects in participants between 51-80 years (Knyazev et al., 2015). This pattern is in

467

line with (Miraglia et al., 2016). However, two other studies also showed contrasting results

468

in younger participants (Gaál et al., 2010; Tan et al., 2013). These discrepancies might be due

469

to methodological differences as two studies (i.e. Miraglia et al., 2016; Tan et al., 2013) used

470

a large number of EEG electrodes or regions of interest (i.e. respectively 128 and 84)

471

compared to our study. Increased network sizes, with potential higher densities, may arguably

472

affect alteration of topological network characteristics if analyzed with classical network

473

metrics (van Wijk et al., 2010). Network size also affects backbone metrics (Tewarie et al.,

474

2014), which may explain the discrepancy between our study and Gaál et al. (2010).

SC

RI PT

466

M AN U

475

4.3 Larger functional backbone modifications in deaf

477

We found some backbone differences between deaf and hearing subjects. Functional

478

backbones in deaf showed higher strength in the eyes-closed, while lower strength during the

479

eyes-open condition, in both the alpha and beta band. Also in the eyes-open condition, the leaf

480

number was lower, while the radius and diameter were larger, which means that the functional

481

network topology in this state is different between deaf and hearing. More specifically, the

482

functional backbone of deaf shows less integration (i.e. reduced global efficiency) and

483

increased clustering. Previous network studies in deaf also showed increased clustering and

484

local efficiency, although the small-worldness was preserved (Kim et al., 2014; Li et al.,

485

2016).

EP

AC C

486

TE D

476

Furthermore, we found larger functional backbone shifts in these functional backbone

487

characteristics, from the eyes-open to the eyes-closed condition in deaf people. The patterns in

488

functional backbone strength correspond with changes we found in functional connectivity

489

strength between the different sensory cortical regions. They additionally indicate large-scale

490

functional connectivity changes, i.e. beyond the sensory cortices, since the functional

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT backbone topology is more altered in deaf when going from eyes-closed to eyes-open.

492

Accordingly, these findings may also be explained by transition from ‘exteroceptive’

493

awareness to ‘interoceptive’ awareness (Marx et al., 2004, 2003; Xu et al., 2014) together

494

with alterations in inter-regional connectivity – partly due to alpha desynchronization (Barry

495

et al., 2009; Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2014) – combined with auditory

496

deprivation and cross-modal plasticity (Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Merabet and Pascual-

497

Leone, 2010).

RI PT

491

We also found that the functional backbone connectivity strength differed between the

499

different forms of deafness. Functional backbone connectivity might depend on whether

500

people are born deaf or acquired deafness later in life. Both congenital and pre-lingual

501

deafness showed similar patterns as hearing controls, whereas post-lingual deaf showed the

502

largest deviation, which is in agreement with previous findings. It has been shown that cross-

503

modal plasticity is dependent on the age of onset and duration of deafness (Brotherton et al.,

504

2016; Li et al., 2016, 2013; Sadato et al., 2004). However, given the limited number of post-

505

lingual deaf in our study as well as the complex relationship of cross-modal plasticity with

506

other factors, such as age of onset, duration of deafness and sign-language experience, it is

507

impossible to be conclusive.

M AN U

TE D

EP

508

SC

498

4.4 Relation between backbone characteristics and sign language experience

510

Sign language comprehension have shown to be related to activation of brain regions which

511

are normally considered to be involved in unimodal (e.g. speech or sound) auditory

512

processing, such as frontal and temporal regions (Li et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2017; Malaia et

513

al., 2014; Meyer et al., 2007; Neville et al., 1998; Nishimura et al., 1999; Petitto, 2000; Sadato

514

et al., 2005). Language comprehension requires higher cognitive functions. Increased use and

515

experience in sign language may therefore arguably enhance functional integration between

AC C

509

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT different brain regions as well as cross-modal plasticity (Nishimura et al., 1999; Petitto, 2000;

517

Sadato et al., 2005). Accordingly, we found a relation between years of sign language

518

experience and functional backbone characteristics. This was found most prominently in the

519

theta band, but also in the delta and alpha bands. In both deaf and hearing, increasing

520

experience in sign language was related to a higher average backbone strength, suggesting

521

increased global efficiency and integration. The decreased closeness centrality suggests more

522

segregation and local clustering. Our reported relations with sign language experience are in

523

line with previous examinations of functional network differences between hearing signers

524

and non-signers. That study found sign-language comprehension to be related to increased

525

local efficiency, small-worldness and modularity (i.e. segregation) (Liu et al., 2017). To our

526

knowledge no network studies were performed on sign-language comprehension in deaf.

527

However, other connectivity studies in deaf showed enhanced functional connectivity

528

between brain regions which are specifically recruited for higher cognitive functions, such as

529

comprehension of sign language (Li et al., 2016; Malaia et al., 2014). Further research is

530

needed to elucidate the mechanisms of reorganization in functional brain networks in relation

531

to sign language acquisition and experience.

SC

M AN U

TE D

EP

532

RI PT

516

4.5. Differences between frequency bands

534

Our study shows that the effects of deafness, eyes-condition or sign-language experience are

535

limited to specific frequency bands, which suggests that auditory deprivation does not alter

536

functional networks as much in all frequency bands. Different brain networks and behavioral

537

functionalities are related to distinct frequency bands (Başar et al., 2000; Buschman and

538

Miller, 2007; Klimesch, 1999; Wróbel, 2000; Wrobel et al., 2007). Cognitive tasks that

539

involve working memory, such as sign language, are mostly related to alpha and theta activity

540

(Klimesch, 1999; Stam and van Straaten, 2012). This may explain the relationship we found

AC C

533

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT between sign language experience and functional backbone topology in those frequency

542

bands. Furthermore, both the alpha and beta band are related to attention, including visual

543

attention (Stam and van Straaten, 2012; Wróbel, 2000; Wrobel et al., 2007). These bands alter

544

by transition from eyes-closed (i.e. ‘interoceptive’ awareness) to eyes-open (i.e.

545

‘exteroceptive’ awareness) (Barry et al., 2009, 2007; Gómez-Ramírez et al., 2017). Overall, it

546

seems therefore that reorganization in topology of functional brain networks in deaf is

547

strongly associated with cognitive functioning as well as with attentional state. Further

548

research is nonetheless needed to investigate this into more detail.

SC

549

RI PT

541

4.6 Advantages of study design and tools

551

Our study used the unbiased MST approach (Tewarie et al., 2015) for network analyses. The

552

methodology illustrates that acquired backbone metrics are highly suitable in exploring the

553

topology and connectivity of brain networks and cross modal neuroplasticity (Engels et al.,

554

2015; Tewarie et al., 2014; van Diessen et al., 2016). Our study yielded a unique dataset of

555

subjects with intermediate to long periods of auditory deprivation combined with different

556

levels of sign language experience. Such a dataset would be difficult to acquire in Western

557

countries with well-established health-care systems and many people with hearing disabilities

558

equipped with a cochlear implant. Cochlear implants have shown to be related to functional

559

cortical reorganization (Strelnikov et al., 2010) and almost normal developing auditory

560

language processing (Hammes et al., 2002). In our study population, none of the participants

561

had a cochlear implant. Lastly, our study shows the usefulness of a portable EEG device.

562

These mobile devices are invaluable tools for use in rural or resource-limited settings. It

563

enabled us to acquire recordings from participants in a country where auditory deprivation –

564

and neurological disorders in general – are more prevalent than in Western countries, but

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

550

24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 565

where neuroimaging research is often impossible or burdensome due to geographic

566

conditions, poor health-care infrastructure and high levels of poverty.

567

4.7 Study limitations and future directions

569

Our study has limitations. The neural brain activity was measured at a limited amount of scalp

570

locations. EEG also lacks information from deeper brain structures. The EEG signals are

571

linear combinations of the neural generators they project to the scalp location of the

572

electrodes. The precision of functional network mapping is consequently reduced. Other

573

neuroimaging techniques, such as fMRI, might therefore be more capable to capture different

574

activation patterns of the whole brain at both cortical and sub-cortical level. The use of

575

wireless headsets for EEG recording may have increased the noise in the EEG signal, and

576

affected the backbone computations. However, recent studies have shown similar

577

performance of wireless headsets compared to standard EEG hardware (Badcock et al., 2013;

578

David Hairston et al., 2014; Schiatti et al., 2016).

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

568

History taking in our study might be affected by a recall bias. Patient records (i.e.

580

dates of births) in Nigeria are not stored like they are in modern Western countries. Many

581

people living in rural Nigeria do not exactly know their birthday. This bias may have

582

increased the noise-level in the regression analysis. Apart from backbone analysis future studies may try alternative promising network

AC C

583

EP

579

584

analysis techniques such as dynamic functional connectivity modeling (Avena-Koenigsberger

585

et al., 2017; Breakspear, 2017), Bayesian exponential random graph models (Caimo and Friel,

586

2011; Sinke et al., 2016), mixed-effect models (Simpson and Laurienti, 2015) and Gibbs

587

distribution models (La Rosa et al., 2016). These techniques also enable unbiased comparison

588

of networks differing in size and density but may capture more subtle differences between

589

groups. Combining techniques might further elucidate the role of specific brain areas in

25

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 590

functional network alterations in normal and sensory lacking conditions. This may ultimately

591

improve our understanding of neuroplasticity occurring after auditory and other types of

592

sensory deprivation.

593

5. Conclusion

595

In conclusion, we were able to detect functional backbone differences between eyes-closed

596

and eyes-open conditions as well as larger shifts in functional backbone characteristics in deaf

597

as compared to controls. Effects that are presumably a consequence of auditory deprivation

598

and cross-modal plasticity. Subtle differences were seen between different forms of deafness.

599

Our study demonstrated functional network backbone characteristics to be related with

600

increasing experience of sign language. Our study provide original insights into the

601

organization and reorganization of functional brain networks derived from EEG data, both in

602

deaf and healthy people. Our results further underpin the notion of brain-wide neuroplasticity

603

mechanisms and global network reorganization in the cortex of deaf, which emphasize the

604

importance to study the brain – as well as cross-modal plasticity – from a network

605

perspective. The link between the functional network backbone characteristics and acquired

606

sign language experience reflects ongoing brain adaptation in both hearing and deaf people.

607

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

594

Contributors

609

MRTS, JWB, EvD and WMO were involved in the study design. MRTS, JWB, FvdM, JN and

610

WMO were involved in the data acquisition. MRTS, JWB, and WMO performed the data

611

analysis. MRTS, JWB, FvdM, JN, RMD, EvD and WMO interpreted the data and wrote the

612

manuscript.

AC C

608

613 614

Funding

26

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 615

This work was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO-

616

VENI 016.168.038), and the Dutch Brain Foundation [F2014(1)-06].

617

Disclosure/Conflict of Interest

619

None of the authors has any conflict of interest to disclose in relation to this work.

RI PT

618

620

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

621

27

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Tables

622 623 Table 1. Participant demographics. Deaf pre-lingual

post-lingual

sign language

no sign

RI PT

congenital

Hearing controls

experience

language

experience

7

65

57

13 males / 29

16 males / 6

7 males

31 males / 34

36 males / 21

females

females

females

females

18 ± 5

19.6 ± 4.4

16.6 ± 4.1

17.3 ± 5.6

20 ± 7.6

(range 10-30)

(range 10-26)

(range 10-22)

(range 12-39)

(range 12-43)

First eyes

25 open /

11 open /

2 open / 5

30 open / 35

28 open / 29

condition

17 closed

11 closed

closed

closed

closed

Sign

8.9 ± 3.5

8.9 ± 3.4

8.1 ± 3.7

2.3 ± 2.6

-

(range 3-15)

(range 1-17)

Sex

Age (years)

language

624

625

AC C

(years)

(range 1-16)

EP

experience

(range 2-16)

SC

22

M AN U

42

TE D

N total

28

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 2. Bayes factors and their interpretations, based on (Raftery, 1995). M0: the baseline model without interaction term, M1: the alternative model with interaction term. In the subsequent tables the Bayes factors for M1 are reported only. Interpretation 100

Extreme evidence for M1

30



100

Very strong evidence for M1

10



30

Strong evidence for M1

3



10

Moderate evidence for M1

1



3

Anecdotal evidence for M1

M AN U

SC

>

1

628

629

630



1

Anecdotal evidence for M0

1/10



1/3

Moderate evidence for M0

1/30



1/10

Strong evidence for M0

1/100



1/30

Very strong evidence for M0

<

1/100

TE D

1/3

Extreme evidence for M0

EP

627

No evidence

AC C

626

RI PT

Bayes factor

631

632

633

29

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 3. Bayes factors of the interaction effect between condition (i.e. eyes-closed and eyes-open)

635

and group (i.e. deaf and controls) in relation to occipital-parietal functional connectivity

636

strength (per frequency band).

Bayes factor

Delta (0.5 – 4 Hz)

0.04

Theta (4 – 8 Hz)

0.07

Alpha (8 – 16 Hz)

>100 V

Beta (16 – 32 Hz)

2.39 I

637

I

0.04

M AN U

Gamma (32 – 64 Hz) Anecdotal evidence, V Extreme evidence for interaction

638

643 644 645 646 647 648 649

EP

642

AC C

641

TE D

639 640

SC

Frequency

RI PT

634

650 Table 4. Bayes factors of the interaction effect between condition (i.e. eyes-closed and eyesopen) and group (i.e. deaf and controls) in relation to the network backbone metrics (shown per frequency band).

30

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Gamma Alpha band

Beta band

band (32-

(0.5-4 Hz)

(4-8 Hz)

(8-16 Hz)

(16-32 Hz)

64 Hz)

Strength (max)

0.09

0.05

>100 V

1.37 I

0.06

Strength (mean)

0.03

0.19

>100 V

57.82 IV

0.07

Degree (max)

0.04

0.06

0.27

BC (max)

0.02

0.06

0.08

BC (median)

0.06

0.06

4.63 II

CC (max)

0.04

0.27

26.64 III

CC (median)

0.04

0.26

93.37 IV

Leaf

0.07

0.06

Diameter

0.08

0.06

Eccentricity

0.07

0.06

Radius

0.08

0.05

Tree-hierarchy

0.06

0.05

Kappa

0.10

0.10

0.06

0.09

0.06

0.06

0.06

0.14

1.59 I

0.09

2.61 I

0.10

2.31 I

0.08

0.09

2.42 I

0.08

0.10

0.39

0.07

0.08

0.16

0.06

0.06

0.73

0.14

0.11

0.62

0.06

0.12

SC

MST-metric

RI PT

band

TE D

Delta band

M AN U

Theta

BC = betweenness centrality, CC = closeness centrality, I Anecdotal evidence, II Moderate

EP

evidence, III Strong evidence, IV Very strong evidence V Extreme evidence for interaction effect

651 652

AC C

between group and condition.

31

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 5. Bayes factors of the relationship between the network backbone metrics and sign language experience in all participants. Gamma Theta band

Alpha band

Beta band

band (32-64

(0.5-4 Hz)

(4-8 Hz)

(8-16 Hz)

(16-32 Hz)

Hz)

Strength (max)

20.92 III

0.73

0.07

Strength (mean)

>100 V

>100 V

66.84 IV

0.22

0.06

0.37

3.94 II

0.05

0.06

BC (median)

0.07

0.05

0.09

CC (max)

0.91

2.37 I

11.80 III

Leaf

Degree (max)

0.04

0.05

0.09

0.09

0.08

0.04

SC

MST-metric

RI PT

Delta band

0.05

0.04

0.05

62.60 IV

0.05

0.18

52.06 IV

46.38 IV

0.04

0.25

0.04

0.04

0.21,

0.05

0.05

Diameter

0.04

0.04

0.21

0.04

0.05

Eccentricity

0.84

0.06

0.13

0.05

0.05

Radius

2.40 I

0.05

0.09

0.05

0.05

Tree-hierarchy

0.04

0.04

0.11

0.04

0.04

Kappa

0.17

0.05

0.86

0.08

0.04

TE D

EP

CC (median)

M AN U

0.06

BC (max)

BC = betweenness centrality, CC = closeness centrality, I Anecdotal evidence, II Moderate

AC C

evidence, III Strong evidence, IV Very strong evidence, V Extreme evidence for relation of MST metric with sign language experience.

653 654 655 656 657

32

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 6. Bayes factors of the relationship between the network backbone metric and ASLexperience in deaf people only. MST-metric

Delta band

Theta band

Alpha band

Beta band

Gamma

(0.5-4 Hz)

(4-8 Hz)

(8-16 Hz)

(16-32 Hz)

band (32-64

1.56 I

3.37 II

0.05

Strength (mean)

0.18

43.35 IV

0.71

Degree (max)

8.14 II

0.22

0.51

BC (max)

1.97 I

0.06

0.07

BC (median)

0.61

0.45

0.09

CC (max)

0.04

0.22

CC (median)

0.07

Leaf

0.12

0.10

0.11

0.09

0.10

0.16

SC

Strength (max)

RI PT

Hz)

0.07

0.06

0.07

2.08 I

0.06

0.07

0.99

1.75 I

0.07

0.07

0.36

0.46

0.15

0.10

0.08

Diameter

0.38

0.46

0.16

0.10

0.08

Eccentricity

0.69

0.10

0.10

0.06

0.05

Radius

2.44 I

0.10

0.08

0.05

0.07

Tree-hierarchy

0.04

0.38

0.11

0.09

0.06

5.00 II

0.44

0.50

0.21

0.09

TE D

EP

Kappa

M AN U

0.06

BC = betweenness centrality, CC = closeness centrality, I Anecdotal evidence, II Moderate

658 659 660

AC C

evidence, IV Very strong evidence for relation of MST metric with sign language experience.

661 662

33

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT References

663 664

Almeida, J., He, D., Chen, Q., Mahon, B.Z., Zhang, F., Gonçalves, Ó.F., Fang, F., Bi, Y., 2015.

665

Decoding visual location from neural patterns in the auditory cortex of the congenitally deaf.

666

Psychol. Sci. 26, 1771–1782. doi:10.1177/0956797615598970

668 669

Aspinall, P., Mavros, P., Coyne, R., Roe, J., 2013. The urban brain: analysing outdoor physical

activity with mobile EEG. Br. J. Sports Med. online, 1–6. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2012-091877 Auer, E., Bernstein, L., Sungkarat, W., Singh, M., 2010. Vibrotactile Activation of the Auditory Cortices in Deaf versus Hearing Adults. Changes 38, 319–335.

671

doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627.Brain

674

Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Misic, B., Sporns, O., 2017. Communication dynamics in complex brain

M AN U

673

SC

670

672

RI PT

667

networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 17–33. doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.149 Badcock, N.A., Mousikou, P., Mahajan, Y., de Lissa, P., Thie, J., McArthur, G., 2013. Validation of

675

the Emotiv EPOC ® EEG gaming system for measuring research quality auditory ERPs. PeerJ 1,

676

e38. doi:10.7717/peerj.38

Badcock, N.A., Preece, K.A., de Wit, B., Glenn, K., Fieder, N., Thie, J., McArthur, G., 2015.

678

Validation of the Emotiv EPOC EEG system for research quality auditory event-related

679

potentials in children. PeerJ 3, e907. doi:10.7717/peerj.907 Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J., Brown, C.R., 2009. EEG differences in children between

EP

680

TE D

677

eyes-closed and eyes-open resting conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1806–1811.

682

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.006

683

AC C

681

Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J., Magee, C.A., Rushby, J.A., 2007. EEG differences between

684

eyes-closed and eyes-open resting conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2765–2773.

685

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.07.028

686

Başar, E., Başar-Eroglu, C., Karakaş, S., Schürmann, M., 2000. Gamma, alpha, delta, and theta

687

oscillations govern cognitive processes. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 39, 241–248. doi:10.1016/S0167-

688

8760(00)00145-8

689

Bassett, D.S., Bullmore, E.T., 2009. Human brain networks in health and disease. Curr Opin Neurol.

34

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699

Bassett, D.S., Sporns, O., 2017. Network Neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 353–364. doi:10.1038/nn.4502.Network Bassett, D.S., Zurn, P., Gold, J.I., 2018. On the nature and use of models in network neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 566–578. doi:10.1038/s41583-018-0038-8

RI PT

691

22, 340–347. doi:10.1097/WCO.0b013e32832d93dd.Human

Bavelier, D., Dye, M.W.G., Hauser, P.C., 2006. Do deaf individuals see better? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 512–518. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.006

Bavelier, D., Neville, H.J., 2002. Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 443– 52. doi:10.1038/nrn848

SC

690

Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Corina, D., Liu, G., Neville, H., 2000. Visual attention to the periphery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals. J. Neurosci. 20, RC93--

701

RC93.

702

M AN U

700

Boersma, M., Smit, D.J.A., Boomsma, D.I., De Geus, E.J.C., Delemarre-van de Waal, H.A., Stam, C.J., 2013. Growing trees in child brains: graph theoretical analysis of electroencephalography-

704

derived minimum spanning tree in 5- and 7-year-old children reflects brain maturation. Brain

705

Connect. 3, 50–60. doi:10.1089/brain.2012.0106

706

TE D

703

Boersma, M., Smit, D.J.A., De Bie, H.M.A., Van Baal, G.C.M., Boomsma, D.I., De Geus, E.J.C., Delemarre-Van De Waal, H.A., Stam, C.J., 2011. Network analysis of resting state EEG in the

708

developing young brain: Structure comes with maturation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 413–425.

709

doi:10.1002/hbm.21030

AC C

EP

707

710

Bola, Ł., Zimmermann, M., Mostowski, P., Jednoróg, K., Marchewka, A., Rutkowski, P., Szwed, M.,

711

2017. Task-specific reorganization of the auditory cortex in deaf humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.

712

114, E600–E609. doi:10.1073/pnas.1609000114

713

Bosworth, R.G., Dobkins, K.R., 2002. The effects of spatial attention on motion processing in deaf

714

signers, hearing signers, and hearing nonsigners. Brain Cogn. 49, 152–69.

715

doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1497

716 717

Breakspear, M., 2017. Dynamic models of large-scale brain activity. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 340–352. doi:10.1038/nn.4497

35

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 718

Brotherton, H., Plack, C.J., Schaette, R., Munro, K.J., 2016. Time course and frequency specificity of

719

sub-cortical plasticity in adults following acute unilateral deprivation. Hear. Res. 341, 210–219.

720

doi:10.1016/j.heares.2016.09.003

721

Brozinsky, C.J., Bavelier, D., 2004. Motion velocity thresholds in deaf signers: Changes in lateralization but not in overall sensitivity. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 1–10.

723

doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.05.002

724

RI PT

722

Buckley, K.A., Tobey, E.A., 2011. Cross-modal plasticity and speech perception in pre- and postlingually deaf cochlear implant users. Ear Hear. 32, 2–15.

726

doi:10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e8534c

SC

725

Bullmore, E., Barnes, A., Bassett, D.S., Fornito, A., Kitzbichler, M., Meunier, D., Suckling, J., 2009.

728

Generic aspects of complexity in brain imaging data and other biological systems. Neuroimage

729

47, 1125–1134. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.032

731 732 733 734

Bullmore, E., Sporns, O., 2012. The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336–349. doi:10.1038/nrn3214

Bullmore, E., Sporns, O., 2009. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and

TE D

730

M AN U

727

functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 10, 186–198. doi:10.1038/nrn2575 Buschman, T.J., Miller, E.K., 2007. Top-Down Versus Bottom-Up Control of Attention in the Prefrontal and Posterior Parietal Cortices. Science (80-. ). 315, 1860–1862.

736

doi:10.1126/science.1138071

738 739 740 741 742 743

Caimo, A., Friel, N., 2011. Bayesian inference for exponential random graph models. Soc. Networks

AC C

737

EP

735

33, 41–55. doi:10.1016/j.socnet.2010.09.004 Campbell, J., Sharma, A., 2014. Cross-modal re-organization in adults with early stage hearing loss. PLoS One 9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0090594 Campbell, J., Sharma, A., 2013. Compensatory changes in cortical resource allocation in adults with hearing loss. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 1–9. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00071 Chen, A.C.N., Feng, W., Zhao, H., Yin, Y., Wang, P., 2008. EEG default mode network in the human

744

brain: Spectral regional field powers. Neuroimage 41, 561–574.

745

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.064

36

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 746

David Hairston, W., Whitaker, K.W., Ries, A.J., Vettel, J.M., Cortney Bradford, J., Kerick, S.E.,

747

McDowell, K., 2014. Usability of four commercially-oriented EEG systems. J. Neural Eng. 11,

748

046018. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/11/4/046018

749

Dewey, R.S., Hartley, D.E.H., 2015. Cortical cross-modal plasticity following deafness measured using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Hear. Res. 325, 55–63.

751

doi:10.1016/j.heares.2015.03.007

752

RI PT

750

Ding, H., Qin, W., Liang, M., Ming, D., Wan, B., Li, Q., Yu, C., 2015. Cross-modal activation of auditory regions during visuo-spatial working memory in early deafness. Brain 138, 2750–2765.

754

doi:10.1093/brain/awv165

755

SC

753

Doucet, M.E., Bergeron, F., Lassonde, M., Ferron, P., Lepore, F., 2006. Cross-modal reorganization and speech perception in cochlear implant users. Brain 129, 3376–3383.

757

doi:10.1093/brain/awl264

759 760

Durkin, M., 2002. The epidemiology of developmental disabilities in low-income countries. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 8, 206–211. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10039 Dye, M.W.G., Baril, D.E., Bavelier, D., 2007. Which aspects of visual attention are changed by

TE D

758

M AN U

756

761

deafness? The case of the Attentional Network Test. Neuropsychologia 45, 1801–1811.

762

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.019

765

EP

764

Eleweke, C.J., Rodda, M., 2002. The challenge of enhancing inclusive education in developing countries. Int. J. Incl. Educ. 6, 113–126. doi:10.1080/13603110110067190 Engels, M.M.A., Stam, C.J., van der Flier, W.M., Scheltens, P., de Waal, H., van Straaten, E.C.W.,

AC C

763

766

2015. Declining functional connectivity and changing hub locations in Alzheimer’s disease: an

767

EEG study. BMC Neurol. 15, 145. doi:10.1186/s12883-015-0400-7

768

Finney, E.M., Dobkins, K.R., 2001. Visual contrast sensitivity in deaf versus hearing populations:

769

Exploring the perceptual consequences of auditory deprivation and experience with a visual

770

language. Cogn. Brain Res. 11, 171–183. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00082-3

771 772 773

Finney, E.M., Fine, I., Dobkins, K.R., 2001. Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1171–1173. doi:10.1038/nn763 Fraschini, M., Demuru, M., Crobe, A., Marrosu, F., Stam, C.J., Hillebrand, A., 2016. The effect of

37

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 774

epoch length on estimated EEG functional connectivity and brain network organisation. J. Neural

775

Eng. 13, 036015. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/13/3/036015

776

Gaál, Z.A., Boha, R., Stam, C.J., Molnár, M., 2010. Age-dependent features of EEG-reactivitySpectral, complexity, and network characteristics. Neurosci. Lett. 479, 79–84.

778

doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.037

RI PT

777

779

Gómez-Ramírez, J., Freedman, S., Mateos, D., Pérez-Velázquez, J.L., Valiante, T., 2017. Eyes closed

780

or Eyes open? Exploring the alpha desynchronization hypothesis in resting state functional

781

connectivity networks with intracranial EEG 1–26. doi:10.1101/118174

783

Hackett, T.A., 2012. Information flow in the auditory cortical network. Hear. Res. 271, 133–146. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2010.01.011.Information

SC

782

Hagmann, P., Sporns, O., Madan, N., Cammoun, L., Pienaar, R., Wedeen, V.J., Meuli, R., Thiran, J.-

785

P., Grant, P.E., 2010. White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in the late

786

developing human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107, 19067–19072.

787

doi:10.1073/pnas.1009073107

Hammes, D.M., Novak, M.A., Rotz, L.A., Willis, M., Edmondson, D.M., Thomas, J.F., 2002. Early

TE D

788

M AN U

784

789

identification and cochlear implantation: Critical factors for spoken language development. Ann.

790

Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. Suppl. 189, 74–78.

Hauser, P.C., Dye, M.W.G., Boutla, M., Green, C.S., Bavelier, D., 2007. Deafness and visual

EP

791

enumeration: Not all aspects of attention are modified by deafness. Brain Res. 1153, 178–187.

793

doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.065

794

AC C

792

Heuvel, M.P. Van Den, Kahn, R.S., Goñi, J., Sporns, O., 2012. Brain Communication. Proc. Natl.

795

Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 11372–77. doi:10.1073/pnas.1203593109/-

796

/DCSupplemental.www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1203593109

797 798 799

Hidalgo, C.A., Klinger, B., Barabasi, A.-L., Hausmann, R., 2007. The product space conditions the development of nations. Science (80-. ). 317, 482–487. doi:10.1126/science.1144581 Hüfner, K., Stephan, T., Flanagin, V.L., Deutschländer, A., Stein, A., Kalla, R., Dera, T., Fesl, G.,

800

Jahn, K., Strupp, M., Brandt, T., 2009. Differential effects of eyes open or closed in darkness on

801

brain activation patterns in blind subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 466, 30–34.

38

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 802

doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2009.09.010 Hüfner, K., Stephan, T., Glasauer, S., Kalla, R., Riedel, E., Deutschländer, A., Dera, T., Wiesmann,

804

M., Strupp, M., Brandt, T., 2008. Differences in saccade-evoked brain activation patterns with

805

eyes open or eyes closed in complete darkness. Exp. Brain Res. 186, 419–430.

806

doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1247-y

RI PT

803

807

Jackson, T.S., Read, N., 2010. Theory of minimum spanning trees. I. Mean-field theory and strongly

808

disordered spin-glass model. Phys. Rev. E - Stat. Nonlinear, Soft Matter Phys. 81, 1–18.

Karns, C.M., Dow, M.W., Neville, H.J., 2012. Altered cross-modal processing in the primary auditory

810

cortex of congenitally deaf adults: A visual-somatosensory fMRI study with a double-flash

811

illusion. J. Neurosci. 32, 9626–9638. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6488-11.2012 Kim, E., Kang, H., Lee, H., Lee, H.-J., Suh, M.-W., Song, J.-J., Oh, S.-H., Lee, D.S., 2014.

M AN U

812

SC

809

813

Morphological brain network assessed using graph theory and network filtration in deaf adults.

814

Hear. Res. 315, 88–98. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2014.06.007

Klimesch, W., 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a

816

review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3

817

TE D

815

Knyazev, G.G., Volf, N. V., Belousova, L. V., 2015. Age-related differences in electroencephalogram connectivity and network topology. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 1849–1859.

819

doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.02.007

821 822

Kruskal, J.B., 1956. On the shortest spanning subtree of a graph and the traveling salesman problem. Proc. Am. Math. Soc. 7, 48–50. doi:10.2307/2033241

AC C

820

EP

818

La Rosa, P.S., Brooks, T.L., Deych, E., Shands, B., Prior, F., Larson-Prior, L.J., Shannon, W.D., 2016.

823

Gibbs distribution for statistical analysis of graphical data with a sample application to fcMRI

824

brain images. Stat. Med. 35, 566–580. doi:10.1002/sim.6757

825 826 827 828 829

Levänen, S., Hamdorf, D., 2001. Feeling vibrations: Enhanced tactile sensitivity in congenitally deaf humans. Neurosci. Lett. 301, 75–77. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01597-X Li, W., Li, J., Wang, J., Zhou, P., Wang, Z., Xian, J., He, H., 2016. Functional reorganizations of brain network in prelingually deaf adolescents. Neural Plast. 2016, 1–10. doi:10.1155/2016/9849087 Li, W., Li, J., Wang, Z., Li, Y., Liu, Z., Yan, F., Xian, J., He, H., 2015. Grey matter connectivity

39

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 830

within and between auditory, language and visual systems in prelingually deaf adolescents.

831

Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 33, 279–290. doi:10.3233/RNN-140437

832

Li, Y., Booth, J.R., Peng, D., Zang, Y., Li, J., Yan, C., Ding, G., 2013. Altered intra- and interregional synchronization of superior temporal cortex in deaf people. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1988–

834

1996. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs185

835

RI PT

833

Liu, L., Yan, X., Liu, J., Xia, M., Lu, C., Emmorey, K., Chu, M., Ding, G., 2017. Graph theoretical

836

analysis of functional network for comprehension of sign language. Brain Res. 1671, 55–66.

837

doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2017.06.031

840

SC

839

Malaia, E., Talavage, T.M., Wilbur, R.B., 2014. Functional connectivity in task-negative network of the Deaf: effects of sign language experience. PeerJ 2, e446. doi:10.7717/peerj.446 Marx, E., Deutschländer, A., Stephan, T., Dieterich, M., Wiesmann, M., Brandt, T., 2004. Eyes open

M AN U

838

841

and eyes closed as rest conditions: Impact on brain activation patterns. Neuroimage 21, 1818–

842

1824. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.026

843

Marx, E., Stephan, T., Nolte, A., Deutschländer, A., Seelos, K.C., Dieterich, M., Brandt, T., 2003. Eye closure in darkness animates sensory systems. Neuroimage 19, 924–934. doi:10.1016/S1053-

845

8119(03)00150-2

TE D

844

McMahan, T., Parberry, I., Parsons, T.D., 2015. Modality specific assessment of video game player’s

847

experience using the Emotiv. Entertain. Comput. 7, 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.entcom.2015.03.001

848

Merabet, L.B., Pascual-Leone, A., 2010. Neural reorganization following sensory loss: the opportunity of change. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 44–52. doi:10.1038/nrn2758

AC C

849

EP

846

850

Meredith, M.A., Lomber, S.G., 2011. Somatosensory and visual crossmodal plasticity in the anterior

851

auditory field of early-deaf cats. Hear. Res. 280, 38–47. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2011.02.004

852

Meyer, M., Toepel, U., Keller, J., Nussbaumer, D., Zysset, S., Friederici, A.D., 2007. Neuroplasticity

853

of sign language: implications from structural and functional brain imaging. Restor. Neurol.

854

Neurosci. 25, 335–351.

855

Miraglia, F., Vecchio, F., Bramanti, P., Rossini, P.M., 2016. EEG characteristics in “eyes-open”

856

versus “eyes-closed” conditions: Small-world network architecture in healthy aging and age-

857

related brain degeneration. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1261–1268.

40

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 858 859 860 861

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2015.07.040 Mulwafu, W., Kuper, H., Ensink, R.J.H., 2016. Prevalence and causes of hearing impairment in Africa. Trop. Med. Int. Heal. 21, 158–165. doi:10.1111/tmi.12640 Neville, H.J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., Braun, A., Clark, V., Jezzard, P., Turner, R., 1998. Cerebral organization for language in deaf and hearing subjects:

863

biological constraints and effects of experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 922–9.

864

doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.922

867 868 869

detection task: II. Brain Res. 405, 268–283.

SC

866

Neville, H.J., Lawson, D., 1987a. Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a movement

Neville, H.J., Lawson, D., 1987b. Attention to central and peripheral visual space in a movement detection task: III. Brain Res. 405, 284–294.

M AN U

865

RI PT

862

Nishimura, H., Hashikawa, K., Doi, K., Iwaki, T., Watanabe, Y., Kusuoka, H., Nishimura, T., Kubo,

870

T., 1999. Sign language “heard” in the auditory cortex. Nature 397, 116. doi:10.1038/16376

871

Otte, W.M., Sinke, M.R.T., Van Diessen, E., 2018a. Functional brain network analysis with minimum

873 874

spanning trees. https://zenodo.org/record/2066281#.XAudsRNKhDU.

TE D

872

Otte, W.M., Sinke, M.R.T., van Diessen, E., Dijkhuizen, R.M., 2018b. Functional Connectivity and Brain Networks in Deaf and Hearing. Open Sci. Framew. Otte, W.M., van Diessen, E., Paul, S., Ramaswamy, R., Subramanyam Rallabandi, V.P., Stam, C.J.,

876

Roy, P.K., 2015. Aging alterations in whole-brain networks during adulthood mapped with the

877

minimum spanning tree indices: The interplay of density, connectivity cost and life-time

878

trajectory. Neuroimage 109, 171–189. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.011

AC C

EP

875

879

Pénicaud, S., Klein, D., Zatorre, R.J., Chen, J.K., Witcher, P., Hyde, K., Mayberry, R.I., 2013.

880

Structural brain changes linked to delayed first language acquisition in congenitally deaf

881

individuals. Neuroimage 66, 42–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.076

882

Petitto, L.A., 2000. Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed

883

languages: implications for the neural basis of human language. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97,

884

13961–13966.

885

Pförtner, K., 2014. Community-based inclusive education: Best practices from Nicaragua, El

41

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 886

Salvador, Guatemala and Honduras. Disabil. CBR Incl. Dev. 25, 72–81.

887

doi:10.5463/DCID.v25i1.321

889 890

Pillai, J., Sperling, M.R., 2006. Interictal EEG and the diagnosis of epilepsy. Epilepsia 47, 14–22. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00654.x Prause, N., Siegle, G.J., Deblieck, C., Wu, A., Iacoboni, M., 2016. EEG to primary rewards: Predictive

RI PT

888

891

utility and malleability by brain stimulation. PLoS One 11. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0165646

892

Ptito, M., Giguere, J.F., Boire, D., Frost, D.O., Casanova, C., 2001. When the auditory cortex turns

895 896 897

Raftery, A.E., 1995. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol. Methodol.

SC

894

visual. Prog. Brain Res. 134, 447–458.

doi:10.2307/271063

Rubinov, M., Sporns, O., 2010. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and

M AN U

893

interpretations. Neuroimage 52, 1059–1069. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

898

Sabidussi, G., 1966. The centrality index of a graph. Psychometrika 31, 581–603.

899

Sadato, N., Okada, T., Honda, M., Matsuki, K.-I., Yoshida, M., Kashikura, K.-I., Takei, W., Sato, T., Kochiyama, T., Yonekura, Y., 2005. Cross-modal integration and plastic changes revealed by lip

901

movement, random-dot motion and sign languages in the hearing and deaf. Cereb. Cortex 15,

902

1113–1122. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh210

Sadato, N., Yamada, H., Okada, T., Yoshida, M., Hasegawa, T., Matsuki, K.-I., Yonekura, Y., Itoh,

EP

903

TE D

900

H., 2004. Age-dependent plasticity in the superior temporal sulcus in deaf humans: a functional

905

MRI study. BMC Neurosci. 5, 1–6. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-5-56

AC C

904

906

Schiatti, L., Faes, L., Tessadori, J., Barresi, G., Mattos, L., 2016. Mutual information-based feature

907

selection for low-cost BCIs based on motor imagery. Proc. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med.

908

Biol. Soc. EMBS 2016–Octob, 2772–2775. doi:10.1109/EMBC.2016.7591305

909

Scott, G.D., Karns, C.M., Dow, M.W., Stevens, C., Neville, H.J., 2014. Enhanced peripheral visual

910

processing in congenitally deaf humans is supported by multiple brain regions, including primary

911

auditory cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–9. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00177

912 913

Shiell, M.M., Champoux, F., Zatorre, R.J., 2016. The right hemisphere planum temporale supports enhanced visual motion detection ability in deaf people: Evidence from cortical thickness. Neural

42

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 914 915

Plast. 1–10. doi:10.1155/2016/7217630 Shiell, M.M., Champoux, F., Zatorre, R.J., 2014. Reorganization of auditory cortex in early-deaf

916

people: Functional connectivity and relationship to hearing aid use. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 150–

917

163. doi:10.1162/jocn

919 920

Simpson, S.L., Laurienti, P.J., 2015. A two-part mixed-effects modeling framework for analyzing

RI PT

918

whole- brain network data. Neuroimage 113, 310–319.

Sinke, M.R.T., Dijkhuizen, R.M., Caimo, A., Stam, C.J., Otte, W.M., 2016. Bayesian exponential random graph modeling of whole-brain structural networks across lifespan. Neuroimage 135,

922

79–91. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.066

SC

921

Smit, D.J.A., Boersma, M., Schnack, H.G., Micheloyannis, S., Boomsma, D.I., Hulshoff Pol, H.E.,

924

Stam, C.J., de Geus, E.J.., 2012. The brain matures with stronger functional connectivity and

925

decreased randomness of its network. PLoS One 7, e36896. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036896

M AN U

923

Smit, D.J.A., Geus, E.J.C. de, Boersma, M., Boomsma, D.I., Stam, C.J., 2016. Life-span development

927

of brain network integration assessed with Phase Lag Index connectivity and minimum spanning

928

tree graphs. Brain Connect. 8, 1–38. doi:10.1089/brain.2015.0359

TE D

926

Smith, K.M., Mecoli, M.D., Altaye, M., Komlos, M., Maitra, R., Eaton, K.P., Egelhoff, J.C., Holland,

930

S.K., 2011. Morphometric differences in the heschl’s gyrus of hearing impaired and normal

931

hearing infants. Cereb. Cortex 21, 991–998. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq164

933

Stam, C.J., 2014. Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 683–695. doi:10.1038/nrn3801

AC C

932

EP

929

934

Stam, C.J., Nolte, G., Daffertshofer, A., 2007. Phase lag index: Assessment of functional connectivity

935

from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum. Brain

936

Mapp. 28, 1178–1193. doi:10.1002/hbm.20346

937

Stam, C.J., Tewarie, P., Van Dellen, E., van Straaten, E.C.W., Hillebrand, a, Van Mieghem, P., 2014.

938

The trees and the forest: Characterization of complex brain networks with minimum spanning

939

trees. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 92, 129–138. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.001

940 941

Stam, C.J., van Straaten, E.C.W., 2012. The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1067–1087. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011

43

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 942

Stevens, G., Flaxman, S., Brunskill, E., Mascarenhas, M., Mathers, C.D., Finucane, M., 2013. Global

943

and regional hearing impairment prevalence: An analysis of 42 studies in 29 countries. Eur. J.

944

Public Health 23, 146–152. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckr176

945

Strelnikov, K., Rouger, J., Demonet, J.F., Lagleyre, S., Fraysse, B., Deguine, O., Barone, P., 2010. Does brain activity at rest reflect adaptive strategies? evidence from speech processing after

947

cochlear implantation. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1217–1222. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp183

948

RI PT

946

Striem-Amit, E., Almeida, J., Belledonne, M., Chen, Q., Fang, Y., Han, Z., Caramazza, A., Bi, Y., 2016. Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf.

950

Sci. Rep. 6, 29375. doi:10.1038/srep29375

951

SC

949

Tan, B., Kong, X., Yang, P., Jin, Z., Li, L., 2013. The difference of brain functional connectivity between eyes-closed and eyes-open using graph theoretical analysis. Comput Math Methods Med

953

1–15. doi:10.1155/2013/976365

M AN U

952

Tewarie, P., Hillebrand, A., Schoonheim, M.M., van Dijk, B.W., Geurts, J.J.G., Barkhof, F., Polman,

955

C.H., Stam, C.J., 2014. Functional brain network analysis using minimum spanning trees in

956

Multiple Sclerosis: An MEG source-space study. Neuroimage 88, 308–318.

957

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.022

958

TE D

954

Tewarie, P., van Dellen, E., Hillebrand, A., Stam, C.J., 2015. The minimum spanning tree: An unbiased method for brain network analysis. Neuroimage 104, 177–188.

960

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.015 van den Heuvel, M.P., Hulshoff Pol, H.E., 2010. Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-

AC C

961

EP

959

962

state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 519–534.

963

doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008

964

van Diessen, E., Numan, T., van Dellen, E., van der Kooi, A.W., Boersma, M., Hofman, D., van

965

Lutterveld, R., van Dijk, B.W., van Straaten, E.C.W., Hillebrand, A., Stam, C.J., 2015.

966

Opportunities and methodological challenges in EEG and MEG resting state functional brain

967

network research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1468–1481. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.11.018

968 969

van Diessen, E., Otte, W.M., Braun, K.P.J., Stam, C.J., Jansen, F.E., 2014. Does sleep deprivation alter functional EEG networks in children with focal epilepsy? Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8, 67.

44

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 970

doi:10.3389/fnsys.2014.00067

971

van Diessen, E., Otte, W.M., Stam, C.J., Braun, K.P.J., Jansen, F.E., 2016. Electroencephalography

972

based functional networks in newly diagnosed childhood epilepsies. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127,

973

2325–2332. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2016.03.015 van Wijk, B.C.M., Stam, C.J., Daffertshofer, A., 2010. Comparing brain networks of different size and

975

connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One 5, e13701. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013701

976 977

RI PT

974

Vohr, B., 2003. Infants and children with hearing loss - part 1. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 9, 218–9. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10082

Vourkas, M., Karakonstantaki, E., Simos, P.G., Tsirka, V., Antonakakis, M., Vamvoukas, M., Stam,

979

C.J., Dimitriadis, S., Micheloyannis, S., 2014. Simple and difficult mathematics in children: A

980

minimum spanning tree EEG network analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 576, 28–33.

981

doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2014.05.048

985 986 987 988 989 990

M AN U

WHO, 2014. WHO | Deafness and hearing loss. World Heal. Organ.

TE D

984

442.

doi:/entity/mediacentre/factsheets/fs300/en/index.html WHO, 2012. WHO global estimates on prevalence of hearing loss. World Heal. Organ. 1–15. doi:10.1002/2014GB005021

EP

983

Watts, D.J., Strogatz, S.H., 1998. Collective dynamics of `small-world’ networks. Nature 393, 440–

Wróbel, A., 2000. Beta activity: a carrier for visual attention Andrzej. Acta Neurobiol Exp 60, 247–60. doi:10.1016/j.procs.2015.07.351

AC C

982

SC

978

Wrobel, A., Ghazaryan, A., Bekisz, M., Bogdan, W., Kaminski, J., 2007. Two Streams ofAttention-

991

Dependent B Activity in the Striate Recipient Zone of Cat’s Lateral Posterior–Pulvinar Complex.

992

J. Neurosci. 27, 2230–2240. doi:10.1080/00268976.2013.805848

993

Xu, P., Huang, R., Wang, J., Van Dam, N.T., Xie, T., Dong, Z., Chen, C., Gu, R., Zang, Y.F., He, Y.,

994

Fan, J., Luo, Y. jia, 2014. Different topological organization of human brain functional networks

995

with eyes open versus eyes closed. Neuroimage 90, 246–255.

996

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.060

997

Yoshida, H., Kanda, Y., Takahashi, H., Miyamoto, I., Chiba, K., 2011. Observation of cortical activity

45

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 998

during speech stimulation in prelingually deafened adults with cochlear implantation by positron

999

emission tomography-computed tomography. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 120, 499–504.

1000 1001

doi:10.1177/000348941112000802 Yu, J.-H., Sim, K.-B., 2016. Classification of color imagination using Emotiv EPOC and event-related potential in electroencephalogram. Opt. - Int. J. Light Electron Opt. 127, 9711–9718.

1003

doi:10.1016/j.ijleo.2016.07.074

1004

RI PT

1002

Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Bullmore, E.T., 2010. Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 53, 1197–1207. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041

1006

Zhang, D., Liang, B., Wu, X., Wang, Z., Xu, P., Chang, S., Liu, B., Liu, M., Huang, R., 2015.

SC

1005

Directionality of large-scale resting-state brain networks during eyes open and eyes closed

1008

conditions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 1–10. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00081

M AN U

1007

AC C

EP

TE D

1009

46

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 1. Schematic overview of the study pipeline. Resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) was acquired for five minutes with wireless headsets in deaf and controls. The sensor locations corresponding to the fourteen channels are shown in orange; the reference sensors are shown in blue. The first minute of data acquisition, required for acclimatization, was

RI PT

discarded, yielding two blocks of recordings – two minutes with eyes-closed and two minutes with eyes-open. The order of the eyes condition (‘open’ and ‘closed’) was alternated. Subsequently, functional networks were constructed from ten-second epochs from distinct

SC

frequency bands (i.e. delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma), and functional network backbone metrics were determined for all bands. These network backbone metrics were related to years

M AN U

of sign language experience. Main effects between groups (i.e. deaf and control) and conditions (i.e. eyes-open and eyes-closed) as well as interaction effects between group and condition were assessed using model selection within a Bayesian framework.

TE D

Figure 2. Raw EEG time-series of one participant. The visualized time-series were acquired from a deaf male (22 years old). Vertical grey lines indicate the initial acclimatization period, the first two minutes (i.e. eyes-closed in this subject) and the second

EP

two minutes (i.e. eyes-open). The y-scaling is arbitrary. Labels of the fourteen channels are

AC C

shown on top of the time-series. Abbreviations of these channel labels are explained in the main text.

Figure 3. Schematic overview of functional network and minimum spanning tree (MST) construction. (A) EEG epochs were measured from 14 electrodes, represented as network nodes. The electrodes were placed across the left (L, yellow) and right (R, orange) hemisphere (top), two electrodes served as references (blue). The recorded time-series (bottom) were used to determine functional connectivity between brain regions. (B) The determined functional

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT connections can either be depicted as a weighted functional network graph (top) or an adjacency matrix (bottom) where columns and rows represent nodes and colored squares indicate functional connectivity between nodes. The color indicate functional connectivity strength (i.e. darker = stronger). (C) The weighted functional network can be used to

RI PT

determine the minimal spanning tree (MST), which only includes the strongest connections, forming a functional network backbone. This determined MST can also be depicted as a network graph (top) or an adjacency matrix (bottom). Subsequently, MST metrics, such as

SC

leaf fraction and diameter, can be determined. Abbreviations of channel labels are explained

M AN U

in the main text.

Figure 4. Functional connectivity strength between the occipital cortex and the parietal cortex. (A) The average functional connectivity, quantified with the phase lag index, between O1/O2 and P7/P8 (y-axis) is shown for eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions (x-axis) and all

TE D

frequency bands (top) in controls and deaf people. (B) The delta functional connectivity between eyes-open en eyes-closed is plotted as percentage change relative to the eyes-open functional connectivity values for each frequency band, based on the data shown in the left

AC C

EP

panel: ∆ in % = 100 × ( closed – open ) / open.

Figure 5. Network backbone comparisons between eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions for both deaf and controls. Functional network backbone characteristics in the alpha band (8-16Hz) and beta band (16-32 Hz) (top), are shown for deaf (yellow) and controls (blue) for both the eyes-open and eyes-closed condition (x-axis) and indicated by the following minimum spanning tree metrics (y-axis), (from top-left to bottom-right): diameter, maximum closeness centrality, median closeness centrality, leaf number, maximum strength, mean strength, eccentricity, radius and kappa. Error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 6. The relation between American Sign Language (ASL) and functional backbone characteristics. The sign language experience in years (x-axis) is related to functional backbone characteristics (y-axis) in the theta band (4-8 Hz) for both the eyes-open

RI PT

(yellow) and eyes-closed (blue) condition, as indicated by the following minimum spanning tree metrics (from top-left to bottom-right): mean strength, maximum degree, median betweenness centrality (BC), median closeness centrality (CC), leaf number, diameter, radius

M AN U

SC

and kappa. Shaded areas: 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 7. The relation between American Sign Language (ASL) and functional backbone characteristics in deaf subject only. The sign language experience in years (xaxis) related to functional backbone characteristics (y-axis) in the theta band (4–8 Hz) for both the eyes-open (yellow) and eyes-closed (blue) condition, as indicated by the following

TE D

minimum spanning tree metrics (from top-left to bottom-right): mean strength, maximum degree, median betweenness centrality (BC), median closeness centrality (CC), leaf number,

AC C

EP

diameter, radius and kappa. Shaded areas: 95% confidence intervals.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Highlights

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

Functional brain mapping in homogeneous population in rural Nigeria. Increased functional synchronization in deaf people in alpha en beta frequency. Synchronization effects are present in brain-wide network backbone structures. Topological network reorganization is associated with sign language acquisition.

AC C

• • • •