The relative stigmatization of eating disorders and obesity in males and females

The relative stigmatization of eating disorders and obesity in males and females

Accepted Manuscript The Relative Stigmatization of Eating Disorders and Obesity in Males and Females Jessica M. Murakami, Jamal H. Essayli, Janet D. L...

487KB Sizes 0 Downloads 6 Views

Accepted Manuscript The Relative Stigmatization of Eating Disorders and Obesity in Males and Females Jessica M. Murakami, Jamal H. Essayli, Janet D. Latner PII:

S0195-6663(16)30060-5

DOI:

10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.027

Reference:

APPET 2879

To appear in:

Appetite

Received Date: 11 September 2015 Revised Date:

10 February 2016

Accepted Date: 11 February 2016

Please cite this article as: Murakami J.M., Essayli J.H. & Latner J.D., The Relative Stigmatization of Eating Disorders and Obesity in Males and Females, Appetite (2016), doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.027. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Jessica M. Murakamia Jamal H. Essaylia Janet D. Latnera a

RI PT

The Relative Stigmatization of Eating Disorders and Obesity in Males and Females

University of Hawaiʻi at Manoa, Department of Psychology, 2530 Dole Street, Sakamaki C400 Honolulu, HI, 96822, USA

M AN U

SC

[email protected] [email protected] [email protected]

EP

TE D

Correspondence: Jessica M. Murakami, 2530 Dole Street, Sakamaki C400, Honolulu, HI 96822, ([email protected])

AC C

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

1

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Abstract Objective: Anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa (BN), binge-eating disorder (BED), and

22

obesity are stigmatized conditions known to affect both men and women. However, little

23

research has examined differences in stigmatization of individuals with these diagnoses or the

24

impact of gender on stigmatization. Such perceptions may play an important role in

25

understanding and reducing the stigma associated with weight and dysfunctional eating

26

behaviors. This study investigated stigmatizing attitudes toward eating disorders and obesity in

27

men and women.

28

Method: Participants were university undergraduates (N = 318; 73.6% female; mean age =

29

21.58 years, SD = 3.97) who were randomly assigned to read one vignette describing a male or

30

female target diagnosed with AN, BN, BED, or obesity. Participants then completed measures of

31

stigma and perceived psychopathology. Measures were analyzed using a 4 (target diagnosis) x 2

32

(target gender) MANOVA and subsequent ANOVAs.

33

Results: Measures of stigma and perceived psychopathology revealed significant main effects

34

for diagnosis (p < .001), but not for target gender. There were no interactions between target

35

diagnosis and gender. Although all diagnostic conditions were stigmatized, more biased

36

attitudes and perceptions of impairment were associated with targets with AN and BN compared

37

to targets with BED and obesity. Additionally, individuals with AN, BN, and BED were

38

perceived as having significantly more psychological problems and impairment than individuals

39

with obesity.

40

Conclusion: Although individuals with eating disorders and obesity both face stigmatizing

41

attitudes, bias against individuals with AN, BN, and BED may exceed stigma toward obesity in

42

the absence of binge eating. Future research is necessary to address stigmatizing beliefs to

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

19 20 21

2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

reduce and prevent discrimination against both men and women with eating disorders and

44

obesity.

45 46 47

Keywords: Stigma; weight bias; eating disorders; obesity; gender

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

43

3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

48 49 50

Introduction Widespread stigma has been documented against individuals with weight- and eatingrelated disorders including obesity (Puhl & Heuer, 2009), anorexia nervosa (AN; Stewart, Keel

52

& Schiavo, 2006), bulimia nervosa (BN; Roehrig & McLean, 2010), and binge-eating disorder

53

(BED; Bannon, Hunter‐Reel, Wilson, & Karlin, 2009). This stigma is associated with a number

54

of consequences, including social isolation, poor self-esteem, and psychological distress

55

(Corrigan & Rusch, 2002; Holmes & River, 1998; Puhl & Heuer, 2009). Moreover, there may

56

be a relationship between stigma and disordered eating, as research indicates that greater weight

57

stigma increases the risk for disordered eating, and vice versa (Puhl & Suh, 2015). Given the

58

profound effects of stigma, a more thorough understanding of stigmatizing attitudes toward

59

individuals across the spectrum of weight- and eating-related conditions is warranted.

SC

M AN U

Although research has demonstrated bias against individuals on both ends of the weight

TE D

60

RI PT

51

continuum (Swami, Pietschnig, Steiger, Tovee, & Voracek, 2010), few studies have compared

62

stigmatizing attitudes across obesity and eating disorders (EDs). It is important to note that

63

obesity is not an eating disorder. At the same time, research has demonstrated high rates of

64

eating disordered symptoms in individuals with obesity (Darby et al., 2009), indicating an

65

intimate relationship between weight status and eating disorders. Therefore, comparing the

66

stigma associated with obesity and different eating disorders may improve our understanding of

67

the roles of specific weight statuses (e.g., overweight, underweight) and behavioral eating

68

disturbances (e.g., restriction, binge eating, compensatory behaviors) in stigma. The

69

stigmatization of BED has received particularly little attention, despite the fact that many

70

individuals with BED exhibit obesity coupled with eating disturbances (e.g., Zachrisson, Vedul-

71

Kjelsas, Gotestam, & Mykletun, 2008; Kessler et al., 2013). Wingfield and colleagues (2011)

AC C

EP

61

4

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

compared stigma toward AN and BN, and found that women with BN were considered to be

73

more responsible for their condition, more self-destructive, and having less self-control. Only

74

one published study has examined stigmatizing attitudes across EDs and obesity (Ebneter &

75

Latner, 2013); findings suggested that women with AN and BN are seen as similarly impaired

76

and to blame for their condition, but more impaired and to blame for their disorder than women

77

with BED or obesity. It may be the case that the general public conceptualizes BED as more

78

similar to obesity than either AN or BN. Given the sparse comparative literature and mixed

79

findings, more research using multiple validated measures of stigma is necessary to gain a better

80

understanding of stigmatization across different EDs and obesity.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

72

Research exploring stigma toward obese men and women suggests that obese women

82

experience significantly more stigmatization than obese men (Puhl, Andreyeva, & Brownell,

83

2008). In contrast, very little is known about how stigmatizing attitudes toward men and women

84

with eating disorders differ. Although men have historically been thought to comprise a very

85

small minority of ED cases, more recent findings suggest that there may only be a small to

86

moderate difference in the frequency of EDs in men and women (Striegel-Moore et al, 2009).

87

Further, the clinical significance and level of impairment associated with EDs is similar between

88

genders (Striegel, Bedrosian, Wang, & Schwartz, 2012). Despite these findings, significantly

89

fewer males enter treatment for EDs compared to females (Striegel-Moore, Leslie, Petrill,

90

Garvin, & Rosenheck, 2000), leading to speculation that men face alienation and stigma for

91

having a “women’s disease” (Carlat, Camargo, & Herzog, 1997; Robinson, Mountford, &

92

Sperlinger, 2013). To our knowledge, only two studies (Wingfield et al., 2011; Griffiths, Mond,

93

Murray, & Youyz, 2014) have compared stigmatization of males and females with eating

94

disorders, revealing few differences in stigmatization between genders. In their study of

AC C

EP

TE D

81

5

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

attitudes toward individuals with AN and BN, Wingfield and colleagues (2011) found only one

96

difference between male and female targets in that women with these EDs were considered less

97

likely to recover compared to men. Similarly, in an examination of gender differences in

98

stigmatization of AN and muscle dysmorphia, Griffiths and colleagues (2014) found small

99

effects indicating that women were more stigmatized than men on four of twenty-seven items

RI PT

95

assessing attitudes and beliefs toward vignette characters. Given these findings, further work is

101

needed to examine potential differences in stigmatizing attitudes between men and women with

102

AN, BN, and BED.

The present study compared stigmatizing attitudes across obesity and EDs. On the basis

M AN U

103

SC

100

of past research (Ebneter & Latner, 2013), it was hypothesized that participants would endorse

105

more general stigma against EDs compared to obesity, with those with EDs being seen as more

106

psychologically impaired than those with obesity. Further, it was predicted that individuals with

107

AN and BN would be perceived as more psychologically impaired than those with BED and

108

obesity. This study also compared stigmatizing attitudes toward men and women with AN, BN,

109

BED, and obesity to explore the potential effect of gender on stigma. Given the sparse literature

110

in this area, no specific hypotheses related to gender were proposed. Materials and Methods

111

113

Participants

AC C

112

EP

TE D

104

The study sample consisted of 318 university participants (73.6% female; mean age =

114

21.58 years, SD = 3.97) recruited in April and May of 2014 from the University of Hawaiʻi at

115

Mānoa (UHM), a public university located in Honolulu, Hawaiʻi with over 14,000

116

undergraduates. Approximately 6% of UHM students are psychology majors. All participants

117

were enrolled in psychology courses and offered course credit for participation. The ethnic

6

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

background of this sample was 48.7% Asian, 25.8% Caucasian, 10.7% Pacific Islander, 3.5%

119

Hispanic, 3.5% African or African American, and 6.6% other ethnicity. Their mean body mass

120

index (BMI, kg/m2), based on self-reported height and weight, was 23.20 kg/m2 (SD = 5.09);

121

9.7% were underweight (BMI <18.5 kg/m2), 63.8% were normal weight (18.5 kg/m2 ≤ BMI <

122

25.0 kg/m2), 13.8% were overweight (25.0 kg/m2 ≤ BMI < 30.0 kg/m2), and 9.1% were obese

123

(BMI ≥ 30.0 kg/m2). 6.9% of participants endorsed a past diagnosis of AN, BN, and/or BED.

124

Procedures

M AN U

SC

RI PT

118

Data for this Institutional Review Board-approved study were collected using the online

126

survey software, Surveymonkey.com. Participants took the survey using this software for free.

127

At recruitment, participants were informed that the study was examining attitudes about different

128

groups of people. Participants were randomly assigned to read one vignette describing a male or

129

female target who met criteria for AN, BN, BED, or obesity, in a 2 (male vs. female) x 4 (AN vs.

130

BN vs. BED vs. obesity) between-subjects design. The number of participants assigned to rate

131

each of the eight vignettes ranged from 37 to 45. The six eating disorder vignettes (see

132

Appendix A) were based on those used by Mond and colleagues (2004; 2006), Ebneter, Latner,

133

and O’Brien (2011), and Ebneter and Latner (2013), and adapted to include gender. The obesity

134

vignettes were based on those used by Murakami and Latner (2015) and modified to include the

135

target’s gender and similar details to the eating disorder vignettes (e.g., the target’s overall diet).

136

The behaviors and symptoms of each target in the eating disorder vignettes were described such

137

that they met DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria for AN, BN, or BED.

138

To ensure target’s condition was unambiguous, all vignettes explicitly noted the target’s

AC C

EP

TE D

125

7

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

139

diagnosis by stating that the target had been recently diagnosed with the condition (e.g., “bulimia

140

nervosa”).

141

The AN vignettes described a male or female target who exhibits body dissatisfaction and a fear of becoming fat, subsequently becoming severely underweight through a combination of

143

frequent exercise and caloric restriction. The BN vignettes described a male or female target

144

who exhibits body dissatisfaction accompanied by recurrent episodes of binge eating and

145

compensatory vomiting. The BED vignettes described a male or female target who experiences

146

distress over recurrent episodes of binge eating that are associated with eating when not

147

physically hungry, solitary eating, and subsequent guilt. As past literature indicates that differing

148

etiological explanations may play a role in the stigmatization of eating disorders (Crisafulli,

149

Thompson-Brenner, Frank, Eddy, &Herzog, 2010), no biological or sociocultural influences

150

were described as contributing to the development of the target’s ED. Further, in order to avoid

151

potentially confounding weight stigma with the description of these eating disorders and because

152

the weight of individuals with eating disorders can widely vary, the weight of the targets with

153

AN, BN, and BED was deliberately not quantified. Finally, to ensure that participants

154

understood that patients met the medical definition of obesity, the obesity vignettes described a

155

male or female target with a height and weight that equaled a BMI of 35.1, with no symptoms of

156

BED. As research indicates that target weight history may influence stigma (Latner, Ebneter, &

157

O’Brien, 2012), it was noted that the obese target’s weight has been stable for several years. In

158

all four vignettes, the target was described as 21 years old, matching the mean age of respondents

159

typically recruited from the University of Hawaiʻi system campuses. All vignettes were made

160

uniform such that details unrelated to diagnosis and symptoms (e.g., names, occupation), and text

161

length (105-114 words), were matched across conditions. Each participant read one vignette and

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

142

8

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

162

then completed several measures that asked participants for their opinions about the specific

163

target described.

164

Measures Stigmatizing Attitudes. The 20-item Universal Measure of Bias (UMB; Latner, O’Brien,

RI PT

165

Durso, Brinkman, & MacDonald, 2008) assesses bias against different targets. This scale is

167

comprised of 4 subscales: Negative Judgment, Distance, Attraction, and Equal Rights. Items are

168

scored on a 7-point Likert Scale from 1 = strongly agree to 7 = strongly disagree; higher scores

169

correspond to stronger bias against the target. Items were designed to retain identical item

170

phrasing while allowing the insertion of a name or group into scale items (e.g., “People like

171

[Name] are sloppy”). The UMB has demonstrated good convergent validity and internal

172

consistency (Latner et al., 2008), and the measure has been used in previous research using

173

vignettes describing individuals of different weights (e.g., Latner et al., 2012). Cronbach’s alpha

174

for the overall scale in the present sample was 0.82, with subscale scores ranging from .73 to .87.

TE D

M AN U

SC

166

In addition to the UMB, the 11-item Universal Stigma Scale (USS; Ebneter et al., 2011;

176

Ebneter & Latner, 2013) was administered to assess participants’ stigmatizing attitudes toward

177

vignette targets. Items on this self-report questionnaire are scored on a 5-point Likert Scale from

178

1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree such that lower scores indicate more stigmatizing

179

attitudes (e.g., “A problem like [Name]’s is a sign of personal weakness”). This composite

180

measure yields two subscales—blame/personal responsibility and impairment/distrust—and has

181

been employed in previous research using vignettes to describe individuals with eating disorders

182

and obesity (Ebneter et al., 2011). In the present sample, Cronbach’s alphas for the

183

blame/personal responsibility and impairment/distrust subscales were .73 to .84, respectively.

AC C

EP

175

9

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Perceived Psychopathology. A brief scale assessed participants’ perceptions of the

184

target’s psychological symptoms (e.g., “[Name] is probably depressed,” “[Name] should seek

186

psychological treatment”). This four-item scale has been previously used to assess perceptions

187

of psychopathology for vignette-based research on weight stigma (Latner, Puhl, Murakami, &

188

O’Brien, 2014). Items were scored on a Likert scale from 1 = strongly agree to 4= strongly

189

disagree, such that lower scores indicate greater participant perceptions of target

190

psychopathology. In the present sample, Cronbach’s alpha was .85.

191

Statistical Analyses

SC

RI PT

185

Initial one-way univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were run between groups on

M AN U

192

demographic variables to confirm adequate sample randomization. No differences were found

194

between randomized experimental groups for BMI, reported eating disorder history, ethnicity,

195

gender, or age of participants. Mean scores were then computed for the UMB subscales, total

196

UMB, the USS subscales, and perceived psychopathology.

TE D

193

Dummy variables were assigned to each vignette condition using a 4 × 2 classification;

197

independent variables were target condition (AN vs. BN vs. BED vs. obesity) and target gender

199

(male vs. female). Subsequently, a two-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was

200

conducted to compare variables across target condition and target gender. The MANOVA

201

included the following dependent variables: UMB Negative Judgment, UMB Distance, UMB

202

Attraction, UMB Equal Rights, UMB Total, USS blame/responsibility, USS impairment/distrust,

203

and Perceived Psychopathology.1 Subsequent two-way ANOVAs were then conducted for each

204

variable. The alpha level was set at .05 for all analyses. Effect sizes were calculated using

AC C

EP

198

1

Additional analyses including participant BMI, participant’s history of eating pathology, and participant gender as covariates were also conducted, but yielded no significant difference in the overall pattern of results. 10

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

205

partial eta squared and interpreted using Cohen's (1988) guidelines for small (η2p = .01), medium

206

(η2p = .06), and large effect sizes (η2p = .14).

Results Two-way MANOVA revealed a significant multivariate main effect for target diagnosis

210

Wilks’ λ = .548, F(21, 709) = 7.88, p < .001, η2 p = .181, but not target gender, Wilks’ λ = .954,

211

F(7, 247) = 1.68, p = .112, η2 = .046. There were no multivariate interactions between target

212

diagnosis and target gender, Wilks’ λ = .949, F(21, 709) = 0.62, p = .904, η2 p = .017). Table 1

213

shows the means, standard deviations, pairwise comparisons, and Cohen’s d effect sizes for both

214

target genders across diagnostic conditions based on results of a follow up two-way ANOVA for

215

the entire sample.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

207 208 209

3.16 (0.13)ab 3.67 (0.12)ab 4.82 (0.15)a 2.81 (0.16) 3.62 (0.69)a

3.55 (0.13)a 3.74 (0.12)a 5.09 (0.14)a 2.55 (0.16) 3.76 (0.78)a

2.96 (0.15)b 3.32 (0.15)b 4.23 (0.17)b 2.58 (0.19) 3.23 (0.63)b

2.90 (0.14)b 2.80 (0.13)c 4.19 (0.15)b 2.75 (0.16) 3.03 (0.89)b

.026 .040 .036 .010 .044

3.61 (0.09)a 3.45 (0.09)a 2.01 (0.06)a

3.52 (0.09)ab 3.49 (0.09)a 1.90 (0.06)a

3.41 (0.10)ab 3.73 (0.11)b 2.12 (0.07)a

3.17 (0.09)b 3.96 (0.10)c 2.81 (0.06)b

.026 .030 .065

AC C

EP

Universal Measure of Bias* Negative Judgment Distance Attraction Equal Rights Total Universal Stigma Scale** Blame/Personal Responsibility Impairment/Distrust Perceived Psychopathology**

TE D

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of Universal Measure of Bias subscales, Universal Stigma Scale subscales, Perceived Psychology scale, and pairwise comparisons of stigmatizing attitudes across target diagnostic conditions AN BN BED Obesity Cohen’s Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) d (n = 82) (n = 84) (n = 78) (n = 74)

* Higher scores indicate greater bias **Lower scores indicate greater blame, impairment, or perceived psychopathology Means with different superscripts differ significantly from each other (e.g., Negative Judgment for AN was not significantly different from BN, BED, or obesity, but BN significantly differed from BED and obesity). 216 217 218

Significant effects for the vignette target’s diagnostic condition were observed on the UMB Negative Judgment subscale, F(3, 253) = 6.42, p < .001, η2 p = .071, the UMB Distance

11

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

subscale, F(3, 253) = 15.07, p < .001, η2 p = .152, the UMB Attraction subscale, F(3, 253) =

220

14.28, p < .001, η2 p = .145, and the overall UMB scale, F(3, 253) = 12.63, p < .001, η2p = .130.

221

No differences between diagnostic conditions were observed on the UMB Equal Rights

222

Subscale, F(3, 253) = 0.80, p = .491, η2p =.009. Post hoc tests using the Bonferroni’s correction

223

revealed that, for overall stigma, targets with BED or obesity were less stigmatized than targets

224

with AN or BN. Similarly, compared to targets diagnosed with BED or obesity, targets

225

diagnosed with AN and BN were considered less attractive. Although targets with BN were

226

significantly more negatively judged and elicited more discomfort with proximity compared to

227

targets with BED, on these two dimensions, targets with BN did not differ from targets with AN.

228

Finally, statistically significant differences emerged such that BN was more stigmatized than

229

obesity on dimensions of negative judgment, proximity discomfort, and attraction.

M AN U

SC

RI PT

219

Significant effects for vignette condition were also observed on the USS blame/personal

231

responsibility subscale, F(3, 253) = 3.88, p = .010, η2 p = .044, and the USS impairment/distrust

232

subscale, F(3, 253) = 10.22, p < .001, η2 p = .108. Post hoc tests revealed that targets diagnosed

233

with obesity were blamed significantly more for their condition compared to targets with AN.

234

No significant differences emerged between obesity and BN or BED on the USS blame/personal

235

responsibility scale. Further, targets with AN, BN, and BED were rated as significantly more

236

impaired than individuals with obesity, and targets with AN and BN were rated as more impaired

237

than individuals with BED.

EP

AC C

238

TE D

230

Finally, a significant effect emerged on the perceived psychopathology scale, F(3, 253) =

239

41.46, p < .001, η2 p = .330, such that individuals with eating disorders were perceived as

240

significantly more disturbed than individuals with obesity.

241

Discussion

12

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

242

The present study compared stigmatizing attitudes toward and perceived psychopathology associated with AN, BN, BED, and obesity in male and female targets. To

244

increase confidence in our findings, three different measures of stigma were administered and

245

compared. Results converged on all three measures and suggest that stigmatizing attitudes and

246

perceptions of psychopathology vary across obesity and ED diagnosis, typically yielding

247

medium to large effect sizes.

Consistent with prior findings (Ebneter & Latner, 2013) and our hypothesis, post-hoc

SC

248

RI PT

243

tests indicated that targets with AN and BN were more stigmatized than non-ED obese targets on

250

most measures of stigma. Further, statistically significant differences emerged between BN and

251

obesity targets on most measures of stigma, including negative judgment, proximity discomfort,

252

low attraction, impairment, and perceived psychopathology. One notable exception emerged on

253

the blame subscale of the USS: obese targets were blamed more for their condition than targets

254

with AN. These findings might indicate that the perception of disturbed eating behaviors (e.g.,

255

binge eating, compensatory behaviors, and dietary restraint) is a potent source of stigma, perhaps

256

even more so than perceived personal responsibility.

TE D

M AN U

249

Although AN was more stigmatized than obesity, they were blamed less for their

258

condition than obese individuals. This finding appears to contradict controllability theory, which

259

suggests that weight stigma is rooted in blame and can be thought of as the last socially

260

acceptable form of prejudice (Crandall, 1994). This finding also appears to conflict with

261

attribution theory (Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson, 1988), which posits that negative reactions are

262

the result of one’s perceived responsibility for their condition. Nevertheless, the negative

263

attitudes toward EDs observed in the present study may also be related to body weight. For

264

example, respondents might associate dysfunctional eating with weight fluctuations, weight gain,

AC C

EP

257

13

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

or a history of being overweight. Research has demonstrated that a history of obesity, even

266

among presently normal weight targets, can be as stigmatized as current obese status (Latner et

267

al., 2012). It is also possible that dysfunctional eating and compensatory behaviors (e.g.,

268

vomiting) might elicit disgust reactions, as disgust is associated with weight-related stigma

269

(Lieberman, Tybur, & Latner, 2012).

270

RI PT

265

Also consistent with our hypothesis and converging with prior research (Ebneter &

Latner, 2013) was the finding that targets with BED were seen as more attractive, less impaired,

272

and less stigmatized overall compared to targets with AN or BN. However, targets with BED

273

did not differ from targets with AN on dimensions of negative judgment and distance.

274

Nevertheless, targets with BED were significantly more stigmatized than targets with obesity on

275

measures of proximal discomfort, impairment, and perceived psychopathology. These results

276

suggest that although EDs are significantly more stigmatized than obesity, BED is nonetheless

277

viewed as a less severe condition than AN or BN. Alternatively, participants may have assumed

278

that the BED target was psychologically impaired because he or she was given a label of a

279

mental illness in the vignette description, rather than because of the nature of the BED symptoms

280

described. Additional research is necessary to determine whether the label of “binge-eating

281

disorder” contributes to stigma. Similarly, there were no significant differences in perceptions of

282

psychopathology between targets with AN, BN, and BED. Perceptions of mental illness and/or

283

the use of diagnostic labels in all three ED conditions may have led to their increased stigma, as

284

more global negative attitudes are associated with having any type of mental illness or disability

285

(Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer, & Rowlands, 2000; Corrigan, 2014). It is also worth exploring

286

whether the egosyntonic nature of eating disorders may render them more stigmatizing than

287

obesity (Crisp et al., 2000).

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

271

14

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

288

Additionally, stigmatization when a target’s diagnosis is provided may differ from stigmatization in the absence of this knowledge. In real world circumstances, the appearance of

290

the target may be more likely to elicit stigmatization. This may result in greater stigmatization of

291

individuals with visible conditions (such as obesity and severe AN resulting in emaciation)

292

relative to less apparent pathology (such as BN, BED, and less severe AN which may present as

293

normal weight or overweight).

RI PT

289

This study found no effect for target gender in the stigmatization of AN, BN, BED, or

295

obesity. Although null findings must be interpreted with caution, the absence of differences in

296

bias against male and female targets with obesity is surprising as past research has indicated that

297

obese women report experiencing significantly more stigmatization compared to obese men

298

(Puhl et al., 2008). Nevertheless, it is noteworthy that the eating disorder results supplement the

299

mixed findings of the two existing studies that investigated the relationship between gender and

300

stigmatization of eating disorders (Wingfield et al., 2011; Griffiths et al., 2014). In both studies,

301

few differences were observed in the stigmatization of men and women with eating disorders.

302

For example, no differences emerged in participant ratings of male and female targets with AN

303

or BN on dimensions of likeability, responsibility for their ED, sexual orientation, self-control,

304

or self-destructiveness (Wingfield et al., 2011). Further, an investigation of stigmatizing

305

attitudes against male and female targets with AN and muscle dysmorphia, yielded no gender

306

differences on 23 of 27 items assessing negative attitudes (Griffiths et al., 2014). In contrast to

307

the present study, this past research relied primarily on single-items rather than scaled scores

308

from validated measures of stigma. Taken together, the past and current findings may suggest

309

that men and women with eating disorders face similar levels of stigma. If this is indeed the

310

case, the low number of men who seek treatment for an eating disorder (Striegel-Moore et al.,

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

294

15

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

2000) may be related to barriers other than stigma from others. Barriers affecting men may

312

include poorer knowledge about eating disorders, reduced screening and referrals for disordered

313

eating, less access to eating disorder treatment, and/or self-stigmatization and shame related to

314

traditional gender roles and/or their condition. Nevertheless, given the limited research in this

315

area, further work is needed to clarify whether or not differences exist in the expression and

316

interpretation of stigma against males and females with eating disorders.

RI PT

311

The present study had several strengths and limitations. This study benefited from the

318

inclusion and interpretation of scaled scores from validated measures of stigma. Additionally,

319

this study utilized a large sample with considerable ethnic diversity. As such, the attitudes of

320

previously understudied populations, including Asians and Pacific Islanders, were assessed.

321

Nevertheless, although the study’s ethnically diverse sample is consistent with the demographics

322

of Honolulu, the ethnic distribution of the sample differs from other parts of the United States

323

and may not generalize to populations from other geographical regions. Further, this study’s

324

sample consisted of predominantly female students recruited from psychology courses at a

325

university. As such, the lack of participant gender differences could possibly reflect the sample

326

composition of this study rather than the absence of true differences between male and female

327

participants. Similarly, participants had attained higher levels of education compared to the

328

general population. Given the sample characteristics, the generalizability of this study’s findings

329

is limited. Future studies should strive to recruit more men as well as a wider range of age

330

groups, education levels, and ethnic groups. Further, although participants’ past diagnoses of an

331

eating disorder were assessed, no measure of current eating disordered behavior was

332

administered. Therefore, it is difficult to determine the extent to which participants' current

333

eating disorder psychopathology and experiences of stigmatization may have influenced their

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

317

16

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

responses. Additionally, the present study used explicitly-worded questions on the self-report

335

measures of stigma, which may have introduced social desirability as a potential confound;

336

future studies might include implicit measures of stigma. Finally, while the perceived

337

psychopathology scale has been used in previous research, additional research is recommended

338

to establish the validity of this scale.

339 340 341

Conclusion

342

stigmatization of targets at both ends of the weight spectrum. Further, these results suggest that

343

although individuals with eating disorders and obesity both face stigmatizing attitudes, bias

344

against individuals with AN, BN, and BED may exceed stigma toward obesity in the absence of

345

binge eating. Additionally, this study highlights the stigma associated with eating disorders and

346

obesity. Therefore, it is necessary for clinicians to work with patients suffering from eating

347

disorders and obesity to address and manage stigmatization as a result of their diagnoses. As

348

conditions associated with shame and guilt (e.g. Frank, 1991; Burney & Irwin, 2000; Jambekar,

349

Masheb, & Grilo, 2003), anti-stigma interventions may also aim to erode stigma as a barrier to

350

treatment seeking by individuals with these conditions. These findings underscore the

351

importance of a more detailed understanding of the different aspects of stigma related to these

352

conditions.

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

This study adds to the existing body of literature that documents the presence of

AC C

353

RI PT

334

Given the prevalence of obesity (Flegal, Carroll, Kit, & Ogden, 2012), eating disorders

354

Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2007), and weight stigmatization (Puhl & Heuer, 2009), further

355

research should continue to address these important questions regarding the nature and extent of

356

stigma associated with different eating- and weight-related conditions in men and women.

357

Additional research could also examine bias directed toward other variants of eating disorders

17

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

(e.g., AN purging subtype, targets of different weights), as well as toward isolated eating

359

behaviors (e.g., compensatory vomiting), to help identify the specific variants and symptoms of

360

eating disorders that might be most harshly stigmatized. Such research is necessary for the

361

development of effective stigma-reduction interventions to improve the treatment and quality of

362

life for individuals with these behaviors and conditions.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

363

RI PT

358

18

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

367 368

References American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

RI PT

364 365 366

Bannon, K. L., Hunter‐Reel, D., Wilson, G. T., & Karlin, R. A. (2009). The effects of causal beliefs and binge eating on the stigmatization of obesity. International journal of eating

370

disorders, 42(2), 118-124.

373 374 375 376 377 378

symptomatology. Journal of clinical psychology, 56(1), 51-61.

M AN U

372

Burney, J., & Irwin, H. J. (2000). Shame and guilt in women with eating‐disorder

Carlat, D. J., Camargo, C. A., & Herzog, D. B. (1997). Eating disorders in males: a report on 135 patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(8), 1127-1132. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

TE D

371

SC

369

Corrigan, P. W. (2014). The stigma of disease and disability: Understanding causes and overcoming injustices. American Psychological Association. Corrigan, P. W., & Rüsch, N. (2002). Mental illness stereotypes and clinical care: do people

380

avoid treatment because of stigma?. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills, 6(3), 312-334.

382 383 384 385

Crandall, C. S. (1994). Prejudice against fat people: ideology and self-interest. Journal of

AC C

381

EP

379

personality and social psychology, 66(5), 882.

Crisafulli, M. A., Thompson-Brenner, H., Franko, D. L., Eddy, K. T., & Herzog, D. B. (2010). Stigmatization of anorexia nervosa: Characteristics and response to intervention. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(7), 756-770.

19

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

386 387 388

Crisp, A. H., Gelder, M. G., Rix, S., Meltzer, H. I., & Rowlands, O. J. (2000). Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(1), 4-7. Darby, A., Hay, P., Mond, J., Quirk, F., Buttner, P., & Kennedy, L. (2009). The rising prevalence of comorbid obesity and eating disorder behaviors from 1995 to 2005. International

390

Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(2), 104-108.

391

RI PT

389

Ebneter, D. S., Latner, J. D., & O’Brien, K. S. (2011). Just world beliefs, causal beliefs, and acquaintance: Associations with stigma toward eating disorders and obesity. Personality

393

and Individual Differences, 51(5), 618-622.

Ebneter, D. S., & Latner, J. D. (2013). Stigmatizing attitudes differ across mental health

M AN U

394

SC

392

395

disorders: a comparison of stigma across eating disorders, obesity, and major depressive

396

disorder. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 201(4), 281-285.

397

Flegal, K. M., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Ogden, C. L. (2012). Prevalence of obesity and trends in the distribution of body mass index among US adults, 1999-2010. JAMA: the

399

journal of the American Medical Association, 307(5), 491-497.

402 403 404 405 406

61(2), 303-306.

EP

401

Frank, E. S. (1991). Shame and guilt in eating disorders. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,

Griffiths, S., Mond, J. M., Murray, S. B., & Touyz, S. (2014). Young peoples' stigmatizing attitudes and beliefs about anorexia nervosa and muscle dysmorphia. International

AC C

400

TE D

398

Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(2), 189-195.

Holmes, E. P., & River, L. P. (1999). Individual strategies for coping with the stigma of severe mental illness. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 5(2), 231-239.

20

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

407

Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G., & Kessler, R. C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of

408

eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological psychiatry,

409

61(3), 348-358.

412

RI PT

411

Jambekar, S. A., Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2003). Gender differences in shame in patients with binge‐eating disorder. Obesity research, 11(4), 571-577.

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P. A., Chiu, W. T., Deitz, A. C., Hudson, J. I., Shahly, V., ... & Xavier,

SC

410

M. (2013). The prevalence and correlates of binge eating disorder in the World Health

414

Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Biological psychiatry, 73(9), 904-914.

M AN U

413

415

Latner, J. D., Ebneter, D. S., & O’Brien, K. S. (2012). Residual obesity stigma: An experimental

416

investigation of bias against obese and lean targets differing in weight-loss history.

417

Obesity, 20, 2035-2038.

418

Latner, J. D., O'Brien, K. S., Durso, L. E., Brinkman, L. A., & MacDonald, T. (2008). Weighing obesity stigma: the relative strength of different forms of bias. International Journal of

420

Obesity, 32(7), 1145-1152.

Latner, J. D., Puhl, R. M., Murakami, J. M., & O'Brien, K. S. (2014). Food addiction as a causal

EP

421

TE D

419

model of obesity. Effects on stigma, blame, and perceived psychopathology. Appetite, 77,

423

79-84.

AC C

422

424

Lieberman, D., Tybur, J., & Latner, J. D. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender:

425

Contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity, 20, 1803-

426

1814.

427

Mond, J. M., Hay, P. J., Rodgers, B., Owen, C., & Beumont, P. J. V. (2004). Beliefs of women

428

concerning the severity and prevalence of bulimia nervosa. Social psychiatry and

429

psychiatric epidemiology, 39(4), 299-304.

21

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

430

Mond, J. M., Robertson-Smith, G., & Vetere, A. (2006). Stigma and eating disorders: Is there

431

evidence of negative attitudes toward anorexia nervosa among women in the

432

community?. Journal of Mental Health, 15(5), 519-532. Murakami, J. M., & Latner, J. D. (2015). Weight acceptance versus body dissatisfaction: Effects

434

on stigma, perceived self-esteem, and perceived psychopathology. Eating behaviors, 19,

435

163-167.

RI PT

433

O'Brien, K. S., Latner, J. D., Ebneter, D., & Hunter, J. A. (2013). Obesity discrimination: the role

437

of physical appearance, personal ideology, and anti-fat prejudice. International journal of

438

obesity, 37(3), 455-460.

M AN U

439

SC

436

Puhl, R. M., Andreyeva, T., & Brownell, K. D. (2008). Perceptions of weight discrimination:

440

prevalence and comparison to race and gender discrimination in America. International

441

journal of obesity, 32(6), 992-1000.

445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452

TE D

444

941-964.

Puhl, R., & Suh, Y. (2015). Stigma and Eating and Weight Disorders. Current psychiatry reports, 17(3), 1-10.

EP

443

Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity, 17(5),

Robinson, K. J., Mountford, V. A., & Sperlinger, D. J. (2013). Being men with eating disorders: perspectives of male eating disorder service-users. Journal of health psychology, 18(2),

AC C

442

176-186.

Roehrig, J. P., & McLean, C. P. (2010). A comparison of stigma toward eating disorders versus depression. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43(7), 671-674.

Stewart, M. C., Keel, P. K., & Schiavo, R. S. (2006). Stigmatization of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(4), 320-325.

22

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Striegel-Moore, R. H., Leslie, D., Petrill, S. A., Garvin, V., & Rosenheck, R. A. (2000). One-

454

year use and cost of inpatient and outpatient services among female and male patients

455

with an eating disorder: evidence from a national database of health insurance claims.

456

International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27(4), 381-9.

457

RI PT

453

Striegel‐Moore, R. H., Rosselli, F., Perrin, N., DeBar, L., Wilson, G. T., May, A., & Kraemer, H. C. (2009). Gender difference in the prevalence of eating disorder symptoms.

459

International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(5), 471-474.

Striegel, R. H., Bedrosian, R., Wang, C., & Schwartz, S. (2012). Why men should be included in

M AN U

460

SC

458

461

research on binge eating: Results from a comparison of psychosocial impairment in men

462

and women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45(2), 233-240. Swami, V., Pietschnig, J., Stieger, S., Tovee, M. J., & Voracek, M. (2010). An investigation of

464

weight bias against women and its associations with individual difference factors. Body

465

Image, 7(3), 194-199.

468 469 470 471 472

stigmas. Journal of personality and social psychology, 55(5), 738.

EP

467

Weiner, B., Perry, R. P., & Magnusson, J. (1988). An attributional analysis of reactions to

Wingfield, N., Kelly, N., Serdar, K., Shivy, V. A., & Mazzeo, S. E. (2011). College students' perceptions of individuals with anorexia and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of

AC C

466

TE D

463

Eating Disorders, 44(4), 369-375.

Zachrisson, H. D., Vedul‐Kjelsås, E., Götestam, K. G., & Mykletun, A. (2008). Time trends in obesity and eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41(8), 673-680.

473

23

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Appendix A

474

Anorexia Nervosa:

476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488

[Amanda/Matthew] is a 21-year-old second-year university student. Upon starting college, [Amanda/Matthew] was unhappy with the size and shape of [her/his] body so (s)he joined a fitness program at the gym and started running daily. Through this effort, (s)he gradually began to lose weight. At the same time [Amanda/Matthew] started to “diet”, avoiding all fatty foods, not eating between meals, and trying to eat set portions of “healthy foods” each day. On some days (s)he doesn’t eat anything at all. Through this combination of dieting and exercise, [Amanda/Matthew] has been able to further reduce [her/his] weight to the point that (s)he is now severely underweight. Despite [her/his] increasingly thin and gaunt appearance, [Amanda/Matthew] denies that (s)he is underweight. (S)he is terrified of becoming “fat” and refuses to make any effort to put weight back on. As a result, the relationship between [Amanda/Matthew] and [her/his] friends and parents has become strained and [her/his] grades at school have started to slip. Recently, [Amanda/Matthew] has been diagnosed with Anorexia Nervosa.

489

Bulimia Nervosa:

490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502

[Amanda/Matthew] is a 21-year-old second-year university student. Upon starting college, [Amanda/Matthew] was unhappy with the size and shape of [her/his] body so (s)he joined a fitness program at the gym and started running regularly. Through these efforts, (s)he gradually began to lose weight. [Amanda/Matthew] then started to “diet”, avoiding all fatty foods, not eating between meals, and trying to eat set portions of “healthy foods” each day. However, [Amanda/Matthew] has found it difficult to control [her/his] eating. While able to restrict [her/his] dietary intake during the day, later on (s)he is often unable to stop eating. (s)he may binge eat, for example, on an apple, two slices of cheesecake, a bag of cookies, a jam sandwich and three glasses of milk, all in one sitting. To prevent gaining weight, [Amanda/Matthew] makes [her/himself] vomit after these episodes of binge eating. As a result of [her/his] disorder, the relationship between [Amanda/Matthew] and [her/his] friends and parents has become strained and [her/his] grades at school have started to slip. Recently, [Amanda/Matthew] has been diagnosed with Bulimia Nervosa.

503

Binge Eating Disorder:

504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515

[Amanda/Matthew] is a 21-year-old second-year university student. Over the years [Amanda/Matthew], has been unhappy with the size and shape of [her/his] body so (s)he has tried a number of diet and healthy eating plans. [Amanda/Matthew] lives by himself and often feels lonely. To counteract these feelings, [Amanda/Matthew] eats foods that are high in calories such as chocolate and cheesecake. [Amanda/Matthew]’s overall diet is generally regular, with three meals a day that consist of a wide variety of foods. When [Amanda/Matthew] gets home from school, (s)he often goes to the fridge for a small snack; however, [Amanda/Matthew] finds that after eating the snack (s)he is unable to stop eating and continues to eat a large amount of food. (S)he may binge eat, for example, on an apple, two slices of cheesecake, a bag of cookies, a jam sandwich, and three glasses of milk, all in one sitting. [Amanda/Matthew] feels sad and guilty after these binge eating episodes and hates the shape of [her/his] body. [Amanda/Matthew] has never told anyone about how (s)he feels or the way (s)he loses control of [her/his] eating.

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

475

24

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Recently, [Amanda/Matthew] has been diagnosed with Binge Eating Disorder.

517

Obesity:

518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525

[Amanda/Matthew] is a 21-year-old second-year university student. (S)he reached [her/his] current height of [5 feet and 4 inches (1.62m)/5 feet and 10 inches (1.78m) at about 16 years of age and since then (s)he has always weighed around [205 pounds(93 kg)/245 pounds (111kg)]. [Amanda/Matthew]’s adult weight has remained stable over the past several years. [Amanda/Matthew]’s overall diet is generally regular, with three meals a day that consist of a wide variety of foods and snacks. [Amanda/Matthew] sometimes eats foods that are high in calories such as chocolate and cheesecake. Recently, [Amanda/Matthew]’s doctor has told [her/him] that, based on [her/his] height and weight, (s)he is Obese.

RI PT

516

SC

526

AC C

EP

TE D

M AN U

527

25