C’ision Re.
Vol. 33. No. 7. pp. 707-712.
1983
0042-6959:83,67070’-~~3.ou:0
Prin!cd in Great Britain
PergamonPrw Ltd
TRANSIENT
PROCESSING
SUSAN
IN CHROMATIC
INDUCTION
E. D~vtfs*, IREXE A. FMVRE and JOHN S. WERNER
University of Colorado. Laboratory of Psychology. Boulder. CO 80309, U.S.A.
Abstract-The non-additivity of steady backgrounds and superimposed test flashes was investigated with 25 observers using a hue cancellation procedure. The subjects task was to vary the ratio of two lights comprising an incremental test flash (AR/AG) so as to maintain a unique yellow hue (i.e. red-green qui~ibrium) in the presence of a lon~wave~ength adapting background. Measurements were obtained for four different luminances of the test flash (for each background), covering a range of t.E-2.0 log units. The adaptive hue shifts generally support Walraven’s tinding that the background affects the hue of the test flash only by way of altering the spectral sensitivities of the receptors, but it does not add to the hue signals from th; test-flash. .
Color
induction
Hue
Chromatic adaptation
non-additivity
Walraven (1976) proposed that adaptation to steady chromatic backgrounds results in both a setective attenuation of receptor sensitivities in accordance with the von Kries’ (1905) Coefficient Rule and the separate processing of incremental stimuli and steady backgrounds. The latter process implies a non-additivity of backgrounds and superimposed test stimuli such that the background alters the receptor spectral sensitivities (thereby shifting the hue of the test) but does not directly add to the hue signals from the test stimulus, Rather, the visual system uses the spatial and temporal transients that define the test stimulus to process it separately from the background. Walraven called this non-additivity discounting the background. In Walraven’s original study temporlti transients were provided by free scanning. but in his subsequent studies (Walraven, 1981; Werner and Walraven. 1982) the stimuli were flashed. Shorter test stimuli might maximize tem~ral-transient informatian, but somewhat longer test flashes have often been used to increase the refiability of hue judgements (Werner and Walraven, 1982). The paradigm from which Walraven derived his two-process model involved the presentation of a test stimulus (composed of a 66Onm. AR and a 540nm, AG, mixture) that was superimposed on a larger steady background (6~nm, R}. The observer’s task was to adjust the test ratio, AR/AG, to maintain a unique yellow. For each background, the luminance of the test stimulus was varied over a wide range. When only the test portion of the stimulus was analyzed, it was discovered
that the ratio
AR/AG
was
independent of the test luminance. Thus, there was a non-additivity of the test and background fields, Other data indicate that there are conditions in whieh *Author to whom reprint requests should be addressed.
Adaptation
Background
the b~i~kground is not comptetely discounted, i.e. there is an additive effect of the background (Larimer. 1981; SheveIL 1978, 1980, 1982). However, it has been shown with Walraven’s (1981) paradigm that nonadditivity holds over a wide range of adapting-background luminances (OS-S.0 log td) and background chromaticities (Werner and Walraven. 1982). Drum (19Xi) has recently proposed that since most of these experiments were conducted with a small number of observers, the differences between the results of Shevell and of W&awn may axially be based upon ~~d~vidua1di&rences between observers, Drum tested what might be the limiting case in Watraven’s theory: whether or not the hue of a dim test Rash could be canceiled by the additive effect of a background (i.e. wbether the appearance of an otherwise “green” test Rash could shift at low luminances of the test flash). He found that this shift in hue did occur for three of his five observers. His other two observers reported that “the hue of the test flash was always either greenish or so weak as to be uninterpretabie” (p. 960). Shevell (1978) aiso reported the former effect, although he noted that “often the subject found the AR = 0 case to be impossible” (p. f651f l-his was indeed true for Rve of the six backgrounds in his spatial and temporal transient condition. In general, Shevell (1978, 1982) does not find an additive effect for backgrounds beiow about 50 td, although Drum did with backgrounds as low as t0 td. Again, given the possibility of individual differences in the additive effect, these different results may not be surprising. Since Drum’s experiment was based on only five subjects, the range of individual differences could not be completely assessed. Moreover, Drum’s method (nomjnal scale, color naming) did not allow for a ciear determination of the magnitude of the deviations from complete discounting of the background. For
708
SCSAY E. DAVIES ZI trl
these re;Lsons. u’e have measured the non-additivity of backgrounds using Walraven’s (19s I) paradigin and a larger number
of observers.
The range of background approximately
the
luminances
same
range
used covered
as used
by
Drum
(19511.
.\IETHODS
Obsrrwrs
Twenty-five adults (aged 18-34) served as subjects. They had normal
trichromatic
Farnsworth-Munsell Dvorine
and
American
plates. All observers psychophysics
vision
according
to the
I00 hue test, as well as by the Optical
pseudoisochromatic
had limited
or no experience
and were naive as to the purpose
in
of the
experiment.
The stimulus
is schematically
Fig. I. It was a 0.6-1.8’ presented
A three-channel the stimuli
background optical
of channels
I:! nm
(Channel
half-bandpass)
2: Corning
screen. The spectral
I and 1, which were brought
by a beam-splitter
to form the test flash, were (Channel or
(2412;
the chromatic
1: Instruments
a broad-band
j-d = 640nm).
channel
provided
C2403:
id = 652 nm) and was brought
background
filter
The third (Corning
together
beam
The sessions began with IO min of dark adaptation. The observer’s task was to adjust a neutral density wedge (increasing or decreasing the luminance of the 540 nm component of the test flash) until the AR,‘AG ratio was at the red-green (R-G) equilibrium point. Measurements were obtained for six trials in a state of neutral adaptation (i.e. no background). This provided the only practice trials for the observers. The neutral indeed
condition to
that
with adaptation the
background
condid
Followthe observers viewed the
shift the red-green
equilibrium
point.
ing these measurements, steady background for 4 min. Red-green ratios were then measured in randomly presented blocks of 3 trials at each luminance level of the test tlash. There was a total of 9 trials for each luminance contrast. Contrast
was
defined
by
the
luminance
ratio:
(AR + AG)/R.
by a pellicle.
LOG
was compared
demonstrate
with
The radiance was controlled by neutral density wedges and filters that were placed in focussed and collimated beams. respectively. The spatial configuration of the test flash the test-flash
Procedure
ditions
system was used to present
shaped by a monochromator S-A;
(2 set on. 4 set off)
(R).
on a rear-projection
composition
by the inset in
(AR + AG) that was
as a flash superimposed
on a 9.8” uniform
together
shown
annulus
was controlled by a photographic field stop placed in a collimated beam (common to channels I and 2) that was conjugate with the projection screen. The head position of the observers was maintained with a chin rest (30 cm from the screen). Luminance calibrations were made at selected points with an S.E.I. photometer. The luminance at other points was measured relative to these points with a P-I-N 10 photodiode and photometric filter that was coupled to a linear readout system (United Detector Technology, Corp.).
AR
RESULTS
AND
DISCUSSIOS
For fifteen observers, the AR/AG ratios were measured at 4 different luminance contrasts, with a
(1, =640)
Fig. 1: Log AG is plotted as a function of log AR for each observer. The data for each observer are plotted relative to a line with a slope of unity. The origin of each function along the axis of abscissae is arbitrary.
709
Transient processing in chromatic induction
?I
5 0.5
LOG
AR
(X,=640)
Fig. 2. Log AG is plotted as a function of log AR for each observer. The data for each observer are plotted relative to a line with a slope of unity. The origin of each function along the axis of abscissae is arbitrary. The filled circles are the original data taken from Fig. I ; the open circles show the replication measurements.
background a pupil
of 2.63 cdim’
diameter
LeGrand.
(approx.
4.5 mm.
Drum
This
background
a larger
The
reported
an additive contrasts
0.3. 7.0, 3.7 and 17.6. Comparable backgrounds
in his Fig. AR AG
effect of the approx.
the
average,
from
the
neutral
background
the R-G the
are 0.5
background
condition
shifted
Fig. I. where log AG AR. According
is plotted
to Walraven’s
esis, the ratio
AR/AG
the
in psychophysical
about
the
whether
purposes
different
described. original in Fig. For
the
to
the
similar.
of
with
five
are presented
in
normalized
Measurements
luminance
the
were made
contrasts
eleven-point
(spanning
the second
(range
the
the background
luminance.
should
evident
be most
there is an additive to deviate
the high contrast
Thus,
we would from
the unit
points.
with
the deviations
point
using
a Wilcoxon
test (Siegel,
for
the
1956). The computations
not affected
by the manner
normalized.
With
not. however.
reject the null
non-additivity
fit the unit
subjects
If
proportion contrast than
A large apparent argued
additive
that
an additive’
ments under
the data
were
test we could (P < 0.05) of
I were obtained
possibility
observers reported
often
poorly
became
reported that
difficult.
(Shevell,
from
contrast
used were
unlikely.
equilibrium that
the
point
was
hue judgments
unlikely
that
reliable
for substan-
of the test Hash. This conclu-
by reports
from subjects in other
1978; Walraven.
of our lowest
of
adjust-
At these low contrasts
R-G
seems
1982). Most
be not
Many
in making
have been obtained
luminances
is not
it could
effect to have been clearly seemed
and
It
could
sion is corroborated Walraven,
contrasts
difficulty
the
defined
measurements lower
I. However,
these conditions.
they
tially
of the background
for an additive
This
contrast
for this test are
effect
the luminance
low enough manifest. our
of the background.
in Fig.
three subjects
Thus,
in these data.
in the data of Fig.
contrast
01 the slope estinvrtiorrs
The data presented
fall below
(SZ) they are above
of
studies Rekrhility
syscon-
effect
signed-ranks
hypothesis
show at low
these points
slope at all contrasts.
effect is not evident
to
Shevell
contrasts.
highest
in which
slope
the remaining
lowest
this distribution-free
two observers
the unit
the line. The data from
intervals.
the deviations
matched-pairs
from
from
to the pre-
slopes by more
slope for each observer’s
the data
deviations
are sufficient
(TM)
the low
We compared
that
tematic
the data are
functions.
observer
an additive
expect
points
the line and for another
to
at the lowest
four
trasts. For one observer
proportion
effect for a significant
point
complete
According
effect is a constant
Note
inten-
and that
the slopes of the individual
mean
was 0.96.
in the four-
It seems that
for
for the individual
0.55-1.1 I).
(1952). the additive
reliable
data are are quite
subjects
were
bars show 95% confidence
The mean slope calculated
of the observers.
point
indeed
was I.04
a
that were pre-
condition
for the individual
condition
estimate
AG vs
be 1.00. The data are plotted
by pinning
a unit
with
plotting
with a line of unit slope; the points
dicted line. Error 0.99
of log hypoth-
be invariant
log coordinates.
AR. the slope should each subject
as a function
point
observers
data and the replication 2. These two sets of data
slope calculated
the
chosen from the fifteen observers
used in the first condition.
viously
ex-
of
the data were reliable,
measurements
who were randomly
in
by a factor
non-additivity
should
sity. Thus, on double
from
naive
The mean slope for these same subjects
The results for the fifteen observers
points
we repeated
The shown
4.66.
was
and
range of 0.3-l 7.6) using the procedures
equilibria
change
who were unpracticed
periments
at eleven
contrasts
of I.2 and 4.6 cd,‘m’
(based upon
observers
research. To determine
6 mm).
were
luminance
I). On
ratio
chromatic
by
range used by
luminance
and 0.4, respectively
tabled
pupil.
is in the luminance
(1981) who
for Drum’s
value
75 td for comparison
since he assumed
background.
42 td based upon
the
1957. p. 106: approx.
with Drum
of
I98 I
importantly, point
is already
; Werner
the
and
luminance
below
that
in
SLSASE. DAVIESet al
LOG AR
(&=6401
Fig. 3. Log AC is plotted as a function of log AR for each observer. The data for each observer are plotted relative to a line with a slope of unity. The origin of each function along the axis of abscissae is arbitrary. The data in the top panel are based on luminance contrasts of 0.2, 0.6, 2.0 and 7.6; the data in the bottom panel are based on luminance contrasts of 0.1, 0.5. 1.7 and 6.3, Drum‘s
e.uperimcnt
where
3n
additive
effect
was
observers. Using ten additional naive observers, we did howcvcr repeat the initial procedure using lower contrasts. The contrasts used for five of these observers were approx. 0.2. 0.6, 2.0 and 7.6. The contrasts used for the remaining live observers were approx. 0.1, 0.5, 1.7 and 6.3. The results from these two lumin~tnce-contr~~st conditions are shown in separate panels of Fig. 3. The mean slopes for the data in the top and bottom panels were 0.9 I and 0.99, respectively. One may now see a tendency for the lowest point to deviate from the slope of unity. For 7 of the IO observers in Fig. 3 the reported for three of five
lowest contrast point deviates in the direction of an additive et%% Statistical tests were not conducted because of the small number of observers in each condition, however, it seems cfear that an additive effect is present at these very low luminance contrasts. There is a close fit for all other points to the line of unit slope.
Replications with other bac~roitfi~~ To determine whether these observations generalize to different luminances of the background. five observers were randomly selected from the first condition for replications with three additional backgrounds. These backgrounds covered a range of
LOG AR (Ids6401 Fig. 1. Log AG is plotted as a function of log AR for each observer. The data for each observer are plotted relative to a line with a slope of unity. The origin of each function along the axis of abscissae is arbitrary. Different symbols denote the data for the different background luminances.
Transient processing in chromatic induction -0.64 to 0.82 log cd: m’. which was approximately the same range that was used by Drum (1981; -0.51 to 1.26 log cdim’). The procedure was the same as in the previous conditions except that the luminance contrast of the lowest point was determined separately for each observer in order to obtain the lowest intensity for which reliable judgments could be made. R-G equilibrium measurements were also obtained at two higher luminances of the test flash. separated by approx. 0.6 log unit. Each data point was based on at least 6 settings. The results for the three additional backgrounds and the original background of 0.42 log cd/m’ are shown in Fig. 4. These data were normalized by vertical translations, pinning the second point to the predicted function of slope 1.0. The data clearly show the same pattern of results for all backgrounds. The mean slopes were 0.98, 1.07, I.01 and 0.98 for the -0.64. -0.24, 0.42 and 0.U logcdim’ conditions, respectively. Systematic deviations from a slope of unity are not apparent for any of the luminance contrasts and backgrounds.* CONCLLSlON
According to Walraven (1976, 1981). the relation between AC and AR can be expressed by the following equation: AG = AR g(R)
(1)
where y(R), the adaptation factor, only depends on the intensity (R) of the background. Shevell (1978,
*Although our backgrounds covered approximately the same luminance range that was used by Drum (1981). our methods were different. Drum focussed on whether an incremental stimulus having a dominant wavelength of 498 nm could shift in hue (i.e. whether AG could be cancelled by R) when presented at intensities from 0.6 to I.5 log units above increment threshold. To address this issue with our data, we calculated the intercepts of the linear equations that related AR to AC3 when these increments were expressed in terms of the percentage of the background luminance. The data used for these CJIculations were based on the three background conditions (with five subjects) for which the luminance of the increments was mdde as low as possible for each observer. (This condition is favorable for demonstrating an additive effect.) It would be predicted from the additivity hypothesis that for AR% = 0, there must be a value for AC”/, (to cancel the additive effect of the background) whatever the luminance level of the background. Non-additivity (discounting the background) would imply that when AR:; = 0. then AGo% = 0. since there is no additive effect to be cancelled. Thus, regardless of the background luminance, Walraven’s hypothesis (equation I) predicts an intercept at On/ This extrapolation is based upon the linearity of R-G equilibria (hue invariance of unique yellow) under moderate chromatic adaptation, an assumption that is justified by the results of Cicerone er nl. (1975). The average intercept obtained for the three conditions and five observers was -0.60/, with a range across observers from -3.2% to + 1.3%. Therefore. evidence for an additive effect is not apparent for these observers under these conditions.
711
1982). using a number of different conditions. gested that the more general formulation is: AG = [AR +f(R)] g(R)
sug(2)
wheref(R) represents the additive effect of the background. It follows from equation (1) that the slope of the function relating log AG to log AR should be 1.00, while equation (2) predicts a slope that is less than 1.00. Walrnven (1979) reanalyzed Shevell’s data and concluded that (except for points near increment threshold) the non-additivity hypothesis (equation I) was supported by Shevell’s own data from conditions in which spatial and temporal transients were available to separate the background from the test flash. (See, however, Shevell, 1980). Walraven (1979) suggested that these transients were clearly provided in Shevell’s (1978) spatial and temporal transient condition which involved a 60-90’ annular test flash that was presented on a steady 4’ background. More recently, Shevell (1982) has reported large additive effects. but his steady condition did not involve temporal transients. Our stimulus conditions tttdsimized spatial and temporal transients as in Walraven’s (1981) paradigm and in Shevell’s (1975) spatial and temporal transient condition. The resultant slopes were close to 1.00. the value predicted by Walraven’s non-additivity hypothesis. Thus, the data from our 25 observers lead us to conclude that with Walraven’s paradigm (except for the lowest contrast conditions) there is strong support for his non-additivity hypothesis. The existence of individual differences in non-additivity is supported by the deviations from the unit slope of some observer’s lowest contrast point. However, the possibility must also be considered that some of these individual differences are related to criterion shifts since most observers found the task difficult at these low luminance contrasts. It might also be noted that in comparing the data for the different observers it should be considered that different stimulus conditions could produce an entirely different spectrum of individual differences from that observed under our conditions. It is true that the visual system may not always be able to parse out the steady-state signal (Shevell, 1978, 1980, 1982) but it is remarkable that it does so over most of its range (Walraven, 1979, 1981; Werner and Walraven,
1982). Walraven
(1976) demonstrated
that
subtracts out the effect of quanta from the adapting field; these quanta have a minimal (additive effect) or nil (discounting the background) effect on the hue of spatio-temporally distinct test fields. The nature of this neural mechanism is likely to be of fundamental importance for understanding chromatic induction. there is a (presumptively)
neural mechanism that
Ackno,vledyenle,,ts-This research was supported by funds from the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant I R03 MH3409-01) and from the University of Colorado Institute of Cognitive Science. We thank Bruce Drum. Kenneth
711
Sushs
E.
Knoblauch. Steven K. Shevell and Jan Walraven for their comments.
REFERESCES
Cicerone C. M.. Krantz D. H. and Larimer J. I 1975) Gpponent-process additivity-III. Effect of moderate chtomatic adaptation. Visioti Res. 15, IIZj-I IX. Drum B. (1981) Additive effect of backgrounds in chromatic induction. Visiufl Res. 21. 959-961. Kries J. v. (1905 1970) Influence of adaptation on the effects produced by luminous stimuli. In ~aft~~uc~ der Ph,ssiologie des Menschen, Vol 3. pp. 109-182. Vieweg. Brunswick. Republished (1970). Sources of Co/or Science. pp. jOl-512. MIT Press. Cambridge. Massa chusetts. Larimer J. (I98 I) Red/green opponent colors equilibria measured on chromatic adapting fields: Evidence for gain changes and restoring forces. Vision Res. 21. 501-512. LeGrand Y. (1957) Liykf. Colotrr and Vision (Translated by
DAVIES
rr cd.
2nd Ed. Hunt R. W. G.. Walsh J. W. T. and Hunt F. R. W.). Chapman & Hall. London. Shevell S. K. (1978) The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception. Vision Res. 18. 1649-1661. Shevell S. K. (1980) Unambiguous evidence for the additive effect in chromatic adaptation. Vision Res. LO. 637-639. Shevell S. K. (19SZ) Color perception under chromatic adaptation: Equilibrium yellow and long-wavelength adaptation. Visiotr Res. 22. 279-292. Siegel S. Nonparamenic Srarisrics for rhe Behaviorul Sciences. McGraw-Hill. New York. Walraven J. (1976) Discounting the background-The missing link in the explanation of chromatic induction. Vision Res. 16. 289-295. Walraven J. (1979) No additive effect of backgrounds in chromatic induction. Vision Res. 19. 1061-1063. Walraven J. (1981) Perceived colour under conditions of chromatic adaptation: Evidence for gain control by R mechanisms. V>sion Res. 21, 61 I-620. _ Werner J. S. and Walraven J. (I 982) Effect of chromatic adaptation on the achromatic locus: The role of contrast, luminance, and background color. t’isicla Ref. 22. 929-943.