Accepted Manuscript Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: A multidisciplinary synthesis
Ramón Spaaij, Hebe Schaillée PII: DOI: Reference:
S1359-1789(18)30117-4 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.11.007 AVB 1251
To appear in:
Aggression and Violent Behavior
Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:
27 April 2018 10 September 2018 26 November 2018
Please cite this article as: Ramón Spaaij, Hebe Schaillée , Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: A multidisciplinary synthesis. Avb (2018), https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.avb.2018.11.007
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: A multidisciplinary synthesis
PT
Ramón Spaaij Institute for Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia
SC
RI
Department of Sociology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Hebe Schaillée
NU
Research Unit Sport & Society, Faculty of Physical Education and Physiotherapy, Vrije Universiteit
D
MA
Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
PT E
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ramón Spaaij, College of Sport and Exercise Science, Victoria University, PO Box 14428, Melbourne, Victoria 8001, Australia. Email:
AC
CE
[email protected].
1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: A multidisciplinary synthesis
Abstract Physical and psychological harm as a result of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport continues to be a cause for concern. This article critically reviews and synthesizes
PT
contemporary scientific research on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport.
RI
The authors identify the need to understand violence in amateur sport within its social
SC
ecology. The proposed framework sensitizes researchers, policymakers, and practitioners to the multi-level web of interacting influences on unsanctioned aggression and violence in
NU
amateur sport, as well as to factors and issues to be considered in relation to the prevention and mitigation of violent behavior in amateur sport. The findings indicate that there is a
MA
dearth of studies that analyze the meanings and narratives of aggression and violence created by amateur sports participants themselves. The article proposes that situational approaches
D
and, in particular, the role of bystanders offer promising directions for future research, policy,
PT E
and practice concerning unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport.
CE
Keywords
Bystander intervention; Moral disengagement; Social ecology of violence; Sport; Third
AC
parties; Violence prevention
2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport: A multidisciplinary synthesis
1. Introduction
Sports provide an insightful window onto the complexity of aggressive and violent
PT
behavior. There has been considerable scientific and policy interest in the potentially
RI
pacifying or therapeutic role that sport can play in violence and crime prevention (Harwood,
SC
Lavidor, & Rassovsky, 2017; Mutz & Baur, 2009). At the same time, aggression and violence occur at all levels of sport, from professional and amateur adult competitions to collegiate
NU
and youth leagues. It affects athletes of all ages and in a variety of different sports (Fields, Collins, & Comstock, 2010). In combat and contact sports, certain forms of aggression and
MA
violence are sanctioned or tolerated, necessary for successful performance (Sheldon & Aimar, 2001), and accepted as a fairly normal part of a highly engaged athlete’s role identity
D
(Coakley, 2009; Curry, 1993). Both male and female athletes can enjoy, and find moral
PT E
validation through, the aggression of their sport and experience it as positive (Kerr, 2016; Matthews & Channon, 2016). Moreover, contact sports practices may reproduce or reinforce
CE
unsanctioned aggression and violence (i.e., acts that are outside the rules of the game and potentially harmful to participants) that can have spillover effects to other life domains
AC
(Klimczak, Podstawski, & Dobosz, 2014; Nixon, 1997). This article aims to provide new insight into the factors, processes, and contexts that shape unsanctioned aggression and violence (where violence is considered to be the most extreme expression of aggression) in amateur sport from a multidisciplinary perspective. Developing a robust understanding of violence in sport requires multi-level analyses that integrate work conducted within different academic disciplines and fields (Spaaij, 2014). Scholars in different disciplines have often found similar themes (e.g., the role of collective
3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT norms), while also offering complementary insights, concepts, and theories that, in conjunction, provide a fuller analysis of the issue (Fields, Collins, & Comstock, 2007). At least some of the scientific disagreement on aggression and violence in sport is semantic in nature, and there is also “a good deal of agreement” (Sacks, Petscher, Stanley, & Tenenbaum, 2003, p. 167). However, it seems that the development of a holistic, multi-level explanation
PT
for aggression and violence in sport has been hindered by the fragmented nature of the
SC
(Gee, 2010a,b; Kirker, Tenenbaum, & Mattson, 2000).
RI
academic literature and, in particular, a range of methodological and conceptual limitations
This knowledge gap may also hinder the development of effective prevention and
NU
remediation strategies. Preventative and curative action toward violence in sport is firmly on the agenda of authorities and sports governing bodies. Education campaigns and prevention
MA
programs targeting violence in sport operate across the world (Sáenz Ibáñez et al., 2012). For example, in 2011, the Dutch national government introduced the nationwide program
D
“Towards a Safer Climate for Sport,” funded at €7 million per year and implemented by the
PT E
Netherlands Olympic Committee*Netherlands Sports Federation (NOC*NSF) and national sports associations. Programs such as this have established specific interventions such as
CE
public education campaigns, codes of conduct, and stiffer penalties. Most of these interventions focus on the mitigation and remediation of the harmful effects of violent
AC
behavior, but pay relatively scant attention to the multi-level causes of this behavior. For example, an independent evaluation of the “Towards a Safer Climate for Sport” program stressed the need to “do more to [scientifically] underpin what is required to positively influence the social climate on and around Dutch sporting fields” (Romijn, van Kalmthout, & Breedveld, 2016, p. 44; our translation). This article critically synthesizes contemporary scientific research on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur, non-professional sports, which we define as sports in
4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT which participants engage recreationally and largely or entirely without remuneration. Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport have attracted limited systematic scientific research compared to its professional, elite counterparts. Yet, amateur sport is the level where the bulk of mass participation is concentrated, for instance through sports clubs, programs, schools, and colleges. Individuals engage in amateur sport primarily for fun, social,
PT
and health reasons rather than financial gain (although some athletes consider it a stepping
RI
stone towards a professional sports career). We recognize that violence in different sports
SC
settings (e.g., amateur versus professional) may be driven or fueled by similar social processes and conditions, and that spillover or modelling effects across these different spaces
NU
may exist. Relevant linkages and homologies will be identified in this article. This article is structured as follows. We will first clarify definitional issues and
MA
explore the spectrum of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. This is followed by a brief discussion of the review methods. The article proceeds by proposing a
D
socio-ecological model that brings together individual, contextual, sociocultural, and
PT E
situational influences. The proposed framework sensitizes researchers, policymakers, and practitioners to the multi-level web of interacting influences on unsanctioned aggression and
CE
violence in amateur sport, as well as to factors and issues to be considered in relation to the prevention and mitigation of unsanctioned aggression and violence. The article proposes that
AC
situational approaches and, in particular, bystander intervention offer promising directions for future research, policy, and practice concerning unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport.
5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2. Defining unsanctioned aggression and violence within the context of amateur sport
2.1.
Unsanctioned aggression and violence: definitional dilemmas
The task of defining aggression and violence is an important one because it
PT
determines what behaviors we render (in)visible, how we assess and respond to these
RI
behaviors, and where we look for explanations and remedies. Yet, it is also a notoriously difficult task because violence in sport and society is characterized by its breadth and
SC
diversity. De Haan (2008) notes that the concept of violence “is multifaceted, socially
NU
constructed and highly ambivalent,” and hence essentially contested (p. 28). Wieviorka (2009) argues that what makes violence so difficult to define is the need to adopt a double
MA
perspective that recognises both the “objectivity” of violence and “the way subjectivity influences how it is experienced, lived, observed, represented, desired or undergone by
D
individuals, groups and societies” (p. 2).
PT E
The problem of defining aggression and violence plagues sports research. The debate on the International Society of Sport Psychology (ISSP) Position Stand is a case in point
CE
(Kerr, 1999, 2002; Sacks et al., 2003; Tenenbaum et al., 1997, 2000). The ISSP Position Stand defines aggression as any behavior (physical, verbal, or gestural) directed toward
AC
another individual with the intent to injure (Tenenbaum et al., 1997). Kerr (2002) argues this definition does not readily apply to team contact sports. While aggressive acts can be harmful to participants, they may be within the rules or laws of the game. We should therefore distinguish between sanctioned and unsanctioned aggression in sport (Kerr, 2012, 2017). In light of this distinction, Terry and Jackson (1985) define violence as “harm-inducing behavior bearing no direct relationship to the competitive goals of sport, and relates, therefore, to incidents of uncontrolled aggression outside the rules of sport, rather than highly
6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT competitive behavior within the rule boundaries” (p. 27). In this article, we draw upon this definition to focus particularly on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. This can include both instrumental violence (i.e., a non-provoking situation) and reactive/hostile violence (i.e., a provoking situation) (Abrams, 2010; Sherrill & Bradel, 2017), as long as they occur outside of the rules of a sport.
PT
Additional taxonomic differentiation is important considering that the frequency and
RI
the nature of unsanctioned aggression and violence appear to vary across different forms and
SC
levels of sport (Guilbert, 2004; Levin et al., 1995; Pedersen, 2007). The multi-layered nature of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport is evident in, for example, Guilbert’s (2004,
NU
2006) distinction between three classes of sports practice in relation to the forms of violence that are represented and exerted. “Hard violence” sports require bodily contact, and winning
MA
may require deliberate physical harm to an opponent (e.g., karate, rugby). “Soft violence” sports allow other forms of violent behavior, especially psychological and verbal violence
D
(e.g., tennis, volleyball). Finally, there are sports where violence is hidden or absent (e.g.,
PT E
shooting) (Guilbert, 2004, 2006). We might consider these different types as part of a layered continuum of intensity rather than as discrete categories. Especially in “hard violence” sports
CE
practices there can be a combination of physical and psychological violence. It is primarily (but not exclusively) in “hard violence” sports practices that both sanctioned and
AC
unsanctioned aggression and violence seem to be relatively normalized (Guilbert, 2004, 2006). Yet, unsanctioned aggression in sport is itself multi-faceted; for example, in relation to the motivation underlying the act (Kerr, 2016, 2017).
7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2.2. Narrow versus extended definitions: persisting tensions
Some scholars have questioned the narrow conceptualization of violence as physically inflicted aggressive actions, as proposed in, for instance, the ISSP Position Stand. Young (2012, 2015) laments the narrow parameters of traditional definitions of sports-related
PT
violence. Young (2012) suggests that broadening these parameters to include a wider range of
RI
harmful or abusive behaviors would allow us to see that “the subject matter may be far more expansive and varied than commonly assumed” (p. 13), and to identify “the central ways in
SC
which violence related to sport is acted out and experienced by persons and groups involved”
NU
(Young, 2012, p. 59).
The relatively narrow focus of this article on unsanctioned aggression and violence in
MA
amateur sport does not resolve this definitional conundrum. Previous research and anecdotal evidence suggest that at least 15 of the 18 formations of sports-related violence identified by
D
Young (2012) are enacted and experienced in amateur sport, albeit to varying degrees. These
PT E
include: player violence; crowd violence; individualized fan-player violence; player violence away from the game; street crimes; violence against the self; athlete initiation/hazing;
CE
harassment, stalking, and threat; sexual assault; partner abuse/domestic violence; offences by coaches, administrators, or medical staff; parental abuse; sexism/racism; other identity
AC
violence; and offences against workers and the public. These variegated formations of sportsrelated violence can be contrasted with minimalist definitions of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport that recede from view the broader social structures and cultural processes within which sports-related violence occurs. This critique is reflected in studies of genderbased violence, child maltreatment, and other forms of physical and emotional abuse in sport. These studies have found that narrow interpretations of violence as physically aggressive acts overlook various modes of socio-cultural domination or “symbolic violence” (Bourdieu,
8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2002); that is, the subtle and seemingly unintentional actions exercised through everyday practices of social and institutional life that violate personhood and wellbeing and, in this way, exert control (Brackenridge, 2010; Brackenridge & Rhind, 2014; Messner, 2007). A distinctive advantage of this extended definition of sports-related violence is that it draws attention to a wider range of issues, social processes, and practices that can be
PT
considered violent based on their physical or psychological impact. But this is also a
RI
disadvantage: stretching the meaning of violence this far may cause it to lose both its
SC
distinguishing character and its value and focus as an analytical construct (Spaaij, 2015). Therein lies a major tension and analytical problem in contemporary debates on sports-related
NU
violence.
Another tension, exemplified in the discussion above, is that existing definitions focus
MA
on the objectivity rather than the subjectivity of violence, in Wieviorka’s (2009) aforementioned use of the terms. Objectivist definitions and taxonomies risk reifying
D
aggression and violence in sport, hence creating artefacts. As will be shown below, relatively
PT E
few studies of aggression and violence in amateur sport analyze the meanings and narratives of aggression and violence as participants themselves create them (notable exceptions include
CE
Claringbould, Spaaij, & Vermeulen, 2018; Gill, 2007). Considering these tensions, scholars of aggression and violence in amateur sport would do well to heed De Haan’s (2008)
AC
suggestion to accept that “a proper definition of ‘violence’ should not a priori be seen as a starting point for empirical research but as a temporary outcome, which may or may not prove to be useful in future research. Exploring a diversity of definitions is fruitful because by means of adjusting concepts scientific progress can made” (p. 38).
9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 3. Methods
The literature search focused on peer-reviewed research published between 1995 and 2017 in books, journals, and reports. To ensure the full breadth of international and multidisciplinary literature was covered in the review, four databases were searched:
PT
Sportdiscus, EBSCO, Web of Science, and Google Scholar. Search keywords included
RI
“sport,” “amateur sport,” “recreational sport,” “school sport,” and “college sport” in
SC
combination with “violence” and “aggression” to allow for a comprehensive search with a broad scope of publications. Search results included English-language publications as well as
NU
relevant literature published in Spanish, French, Portuguese, German, and Dutch. The foreign-language literature enabled us to look beyond North America and the United
MA
Kingdom, where the bulk of research has been conducted, to consider the permutations of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport across the western world. In a few
D
cases, we also referred to earlier, influential publications to highlight continuities and
PT E
disjunctures in the literature, especially with regard to elements of the socio-ecological model presented.
CE
Empirical studies and theoretical publications were included in the review. The identified literature captured not only the attitudes, behaviors, and experiences of athletes
AC
themselves, but also of other actors involved in unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport, especially coaches, referees, parents, and spectators. The literature covered both on-field behavior (during competition) and its relationship to off-field behaviors, which are also included in this review. All age groups were covered – children’s, youth, and adult sports. Only literature that examined amateur sports and unsanctioned aggression and violence (broadly defined, as noted above) was included in the review; within this literature, We specifically identified research that focused on “hard violence” sports practices (Guilbert,
10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2004), but also included research on other sports practices that provided insight into layers of the socio-ecological model in Table 1 (discussed below). The majority of literature examined in this article addressed a relatively small number of combat and team contact sports (e.g., ice hockey, American football, rugby, Australian rules football) that can be grouped in this category. Yet, the divergent terminologies used in previous research posed some challenges
PT
for our analytical focus on unsanctioned aggression and violence. Existing research varied in
RI
its definitions of aggression and violence, which ranged from narrow to broad. Many
SC
publications did not explicitly differentiate between sanctioned and unsanctioned aggression and violence, while several studies used additional terms such as anti-social,
NU
unsportspersonlike, or delinquent behavior to refer to behaviors that exhibited clear overlap with unsanctioned aggression and violence. In what follows, we use the latter terms
MA
consistently and reinterpret the terminology used by authors where necessary. Table 1
PT E
INSERT TABLE 1 HERE
D
summarizes the study contexts of the empirical studies discussed below.
CE
The synthesis of the literature was guided by a relatively broad definition that focused on unsanctioned, harm-inducing behavior (Terry & Jackson, 1985). In the identified
AC
literature, this definition translated mainly into the following behavioral categories within Young’s (2012) wider taxonomy: physical violence and verbal abuse by players towards other players or towards officials (especially referees); physical, psychological, and verbal violence by spectators or parents towards players, officials, or other spectators; physical and psychological violence by coaches towards players; and player violence away from the game. Our thematic analysis of this literature was informed by the socio-ecological model shown in Figure 1.
11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
INSERT FIGURE 1 HERE
4. A socio-ecological framework of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur
PT
sport
RI
The aforementioned definitional issues highlight the multi-faceted nature of
SC
unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. Scholars in the disciplines of psychology, sociology, criminology, law, education, anthropology, and history have all
NU
investigated particular pieces or, in socio-ecological terms, “layers” of the sports violence puzzle; yet, they typically fail to bring together and integrate the different approaches in an
MA
interdisciplinary or transdisciplinary way. Much of the contemporary literature on the subject reproduces rather than bridges philosophical, theoretical, and methodological divides (Spaaij,
D
2014), hence hindering the development of a more holistic understanding of the determinants
PT E
of violence in sport. A major challenge is to bring together and integrate insights and explanations emanating from different academic disciplines, research paradigms, and
CE
methodologies. A notable exception is Terry and Jackson’s (1985) integrative review of the determinants of violence in sport (broadly defined), which brings together distal and
AC
proximate causes. More than thirty years on, their study remains one of few attempts to encourage genuine interdisciplinary analysis in the study of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport. In order to contribute to an interdisciplinary synthesis of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport, this article proposes an integrated model, presented in Figure 1, that focuses attention on the multiple, inter-related influences on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. The socio-ecological model acknowledges that unsanctioned
12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT aggression and violence in sport amateur result from interaction between the individual, situation, and social environment. It further considers that the environment is made up of different subsystems or levels of analysis, and that processes or mechanisms that affect, and are affected by, behavior operate at each of these levels of analysis (McLeroy, Bibeau, Steckler, & Glanz, 1988). This framework assists not only with identifying the social
PT
determinants of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport, but also with crafting
RI
strategies to prevent and reduce unsanctioned aggression and violence at the individual and
SC
environmental levels. The model sensitizes us to the importance of strategies directed at the multi-level influences on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport.
NU
In this article, the research literature on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport will be discussed with consideration given to multiple dimensions suggested by
MA
the model. It is beyond the scope of this article to provide an exhaustive overview of all layers and factors shown in Figure 1. Instead, we will focus on recent and emergent issues in
D
contemporary scientific research that add to existing knowledge on violence in amateur sport,
PT E
and that contain directions for future research, policy, and practice in this area. We particularly foreground situational approaches that offer a promising terrain for further
CE
research.
AC
INSERT FIGURE 1 HERE
4.1. Individual influences
A number of cross-sectional and quasi-experimental studies have examined the relationship between sport participation and unsanctioned aggression and violence, with particular attention being given to the influence of individual and personality factors. Two
13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sets of influences are reflected in contemporary research: personal values and demographic factors.
4.1.1. Personal values
PT
Certain personal values and orientations are associated with unsanctioned aggression
RI
and violence in amateur sport. Danioni and Barni’s (2017) cross-sectional study of 172
SC
adolescent team sports participants (e.g., volleyball, soccer, basketball, and rugby) in Italy found that personal values of self-enhancement, focused on achievement, power, and self-
NU
interest, were positively related to unsanctioned aggression and violence towards both teammates (r=.29) and opposing players (r=.35). Values of openness to change, which
MA
promote the interests of the individual and of being a free spirit, were positively correlated with aggressive and violent behaviors towards opponents, but not towards teammates
D
(Danioni & Barni, 2017). Some of the associations between the adolescents’ personal values
PT E
and their sports behavior were moderated by parental pressure towards their children in sport. Specifically, the association between self-enhancement values and aggressive or violent
Barni, 2017).
CE
behavior was stronger when parental pressure to succeed in sport was higher (Danioni &
AC
Research has also examined the role of passion for one’s sport as a predictive factor in unsanctioned aggression and violence. This research has identified an association between having an obsessive passion for sport and aggressive behavior (e.g., injuring other players). A cross-sectional study of 60 high school and college basketball players found that, overall, obsessively-passionately athletes tended to be more aggressive than harmoniously-passionate athletes (Donahue, Rip, & Vallerand, 2009). The former were more likely to report the use of unsanctioned aggression (a large Cohen’s effect size of .71), especially under identity threat
14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT (a moderate Cohen’s effect size of .40). These athletes appeared to act aggressively toward others who threatened their social identity as a competent athlete. Harmoniously-passionate athletes, whose main goal was to master their skills instead of focusing on the end result (win or lose), were less likely to report the use of aggressive behaviors (Donahue et al., 2009). We should note that this study relied exclusively on self-report data, rather than data on athletes’
PT
actual behavior. Moreover, this research did not conceptualize identity as contextual, and thus
RI
did not elicit how identities can change per situation and in situations. We will return to this
SC
issue in section 4.4.
NU
4.1.2. Demographic factors
MA
Research consistently shows important gender differences in unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. Male participants are more likely to perceive unsanctioned
D
aggression and violence as legitimate (Conroy et al., 2001) and to exhibit violent behavior
PT E
than female participants, especially in team contact sports (Coulomb-Cabagno & Rascle, 2006; Messner, 2007; Pappas, McKenry, & Skilken Catlett, 2004; Sønderlund et al., 2014).
CE
Yet, research on male and female soccer players suggests that these differences may be substantially reduced when behavior scores are adjusted for empathy, perceived performance
AC
climate, or sports experience (Kavussanu, Stamp, Slade, & Ring, 2009). We will return to the influence of gender on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport in the discussion of sociocultural influences, in order to draw connections between individual and sociocultural influences. Moreover, research that focuses on the meanings that female participants in team contacts sports themselves create suggests that women are also potential aggressors. For example, Gill’s (2007) ethnographic study of women rugby players shows that in identifying themselves as both violent protagonists and victims of rape the women
15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT were able to articulate an independent and powerful position as women, and redefine what femininity means in a violent context. Age also appears to be a significant factor; yet, it may be moderated by the level of competition. Conroy et al.’s (2001) cross-sectional study of 1,018 sports participants aged 819 years found that increases in athletes’ perceptions of the legitimacy of aggressive behavior
PT
(e.g., physical violence, verbal abuse) were positively related to age and being male, and
RI
contact sport participation. Older participants perceived unsanctioned aggression and
SC
violence in sport as being more legitimate than did younger participants (Conroy et al., 2001). The level of competition seems to be a significant moderating factor. Conroy et al. (2001)
NU
found that aggression was perceived as being increasingly acceptable when the participant was competing at higher levels of sport. In a similar vein, Bloom and Smith’s (1996) study of
MA
the degree to which violence in ice hockey spills over into violent behavior in other social settings found that only players over the age of 17 playing in highly competitive
PT E
D
professionalized leagues were prone to a spillover effect.
CE
4.2. Contextual influences
AC
4.2.1. Type and level of sport
The type and level of sport and the setting in which sport is played are important contextual factors in unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. A crosssectional study of 2,756 high-school students found no significant differences in violent behavior (e.g., fighting, assault) between athletes and non-athletes (Levin, Smith, Caldwell, & Kimbrough, 1995). However, the study identified that the nature of the sport in which the students participated was related to behavior. While contact sport athletes (e.g., American
16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT football, wrestling, basketball) were overall quite comparable to non-athletes, the non-contact sport athletes (male and female) had a lower propensity to exhibit certain forms of violent behavior, such as assault. An explanation for this finding may be in the psychological characteristics of the non-contact sport athletes, and specifically their more sophisticated moral reasoning or their value orientation (Levin et al., 1995).
PT
Other studies have identified an association between contact sport participation and
RI
unsanctioned aggression and violence; yet, this relationship needs to be qualified. An
SC
experimental study on the link between aggression (e.g., instrumental physical force) and sports involvement in male high school students found that contact sports participants (e.g.,
NU
American football) behaved more aggressively than those in low- or non-contact sports (e.g. track and field) (Huang, Cherek, & Lane, 1999). Moesch, Birrer, and Seiler’s (2010) study of
MA
2,438 adolescents similarly concluded that adolescents who accepted violent cognitions and who exhibited violent behavior frequently were over-represented in contact sports. In
D
contrast, non-violent adolescents participated more in individual sports with a focus on
PT E
aesthetic dimensions. The authors interpreted this finding in light of the characteristics of the different types of sport. Team sports with body contact “include more aggressive moments
CE
than do aesthetic sports,” and “this could lead to a transfer of learned behavior in other domains” (Moesch et al., 2010, p. 326). This finding is consistent with other scientific studies
AC
(Conroy et al., 2001; Kreager, 2007), and points to the learning of aggression and violence as an embodied process (Wacquant, 2004). The observed association between contact sport participation and unsanctioned aggression and violence needs to be qualified. It is important to consider the aforementioned distinction between instrumental aggression (i.e., a non-provoking situation) and hostile/reactive aggression (i.e., a provoking situation). Contact sport participation (e.g., American football, soccer) may positively predict instrumental aggression, but not hostile
17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT aggression (Coulomb-Cabagno & Rascle, 2006; Sherrill & Bradel, 2017); although some research, such as Kreager (2007), suggests otherwise. In a study of 38 male university students, contact sport participation was only associated with aggression in a provocation-free competitive context, whereas “contact athletes appeared to increase aggressive responding to provocation to a lesser extent than those who did not participate in contact sports” (Sherrill &
PT
Bradel, 2017, p. 54). Coulomb-Cabagno and Rascle’s (2006) video analysis of 180 handball
RI
and soccer games found that instrumental aggression increased but hostile aggression
SC
decreased as the competition level rose.
These findings may be explained by the fact that the chances of success within
NU
competitive contact sports increase with instrumental aggression and decrease with hostile aggression (Abrams, 2010; Sherrill & Bradel, 2017). As the stakes of the competition
MA
increase, athletes may perceive that punishment and reward structures for unsanctioned aggression and violence are simultaneously changing, causing shifts in perceptions of
D
legitimacy. Participants are generally more likely to exhibit unsanctioned aggression when
PT E
the probability of punishment is low or the instrumental value of unsanctioned aggression is high (i.e., where it is perceived to increase the probability of a positive performance outcome)
CE
(Conroy et al., 2001). These findings can inform debates on aggression and violence in general by showing that the meanings participants attach to aggressive and violent behavior
AC
depend on the specific contexts of justification (De Haan, 2008); in this case, the level and type of sport.
This pattern may be culturally specific. Maxwell, Visek, and Moores’ (2009) study of the perceived legitimacy of aggression in 471 male Hong Kong Chinese athletes confirmed that contact sports (e.g., rugby) participants perceived aggression as more legitimate than did players of non-contact sports (η2=.17). However, it also suggested some cultural differences in this regard. The study identified the tendency among Hong Kong Chinese athletes to
18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT perceive all types of aggression as less legitimate with advancing competitive level (with the exception of rugby players). This finding may be explained by differences in socialization experience, which, in the Chinese context, can involve a tendency to disapprove of aggressive behavior (Maxwell et al., 2009). Unfortunately, Maxwell et al.’s (2009) study focused primarily on hostile aggression rather than instrumental aggression, and it is therefore not
PT
possible to verify whether some cross-cultural differences may also apply to the relationship
SC
RI
between instrumental aggression and the level of sport participation.
NU
4.2.2. Issues of selection and spuriousness
How can we explain the common finding in scientific research regarding the
MA
association between team contact sports participation and (instrumental) unsanctioned aggression and violence? A first factor to consider is selection effects. While the
D
aforementioned studies make an important contribution by distinguishing effects across
PT E
different types and levels of sports, their reliance on cross-sectional designs means that they are unable to distinguish selection from socialization effects (Kreager, 2007; Moesch et al.,
CE
2010). To what extent were athletes who exhibit more unsanctioned aggression and violence in contact sports already more aggressive and violent prior to sports participation?
AC
Participation in contact sports “may reflect the individuals’ preferences for such activities rather than the shaping of aggressive behavior through participation” (Huang et al., 1999, p. 1260). The effects of sport participation on unsanctioned aggression and violence may thus be spurious, and explained primarily by population heterogeneity in aggressive propensities or prior socialization experiences. Some studies specifically address issues of selection and spuriousness. Endresen and Olweus’s (2005) longitudinal study found elevated levels of violent behavior outside the
19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT sports setting among preadolescent and adolescent boys involved in “power sports” (boxing, wrestling, weightlifting, and martial arts) when compared with boys who did not participate in such sports. There were no indications of selection effects, leading the authors to conclude that “the negative effects of power sports participation are likely to apply to ‘normal or average boys’, and not only to boys with already elevated levels of antisocial involvement”
PT
(Endresen & Olweus, 2005, p. 476). This study’s methodology has been subject to critique,
RI
for example with regard to the validity of the questionnaire, the lack of differentiation
SC
between sports, and the study’s failure to account for the type of guidance or pedagogy used within the selected power sports (Vertonghen & Theeboom, 2010). The relationship between
NU
martial arts practice and unsanctioned aggression among children and adolescents appears to remain contested (Gubbels, van der Stouwe, Spruit, & Stams, 2016; Vertonghen &
MA
Theeboom, 2010).
In line with Endresen and Olweus’s (2005) findings, though instead focusing on team
D
contact sports, Kreager’s (2007) analysis of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
PT E
Health concluded that self-selection does not account for the entire relationship between playing team contact sports and subsequent male violence. Selection effects were identified,
CE
with prior fighting and delinquency attenuating a significant proportion of direct effects for American football and wrestling. In other words, “aggressive kids are likely to enter contact
AC
sports and the coaches of these activities are likely to choose aggressive kids to fill more competitive teams” (Kreager, 2007, p. 719). But, Kreager concludes, “net of prior fighting and delinquency, football remains a significant predictor of serious fighting and a spurious sports-violence relationship” (Kreager, 2007, 719-720). Here, again, we see the importance of embodied learning and “habitus” (Wacquant, 2004) on aggression and violence in sport.
20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4.3. Sociocultural influences
4.3.1. Social learning and socialization
Scientific evidence points to social learning as a robust explanation for unsanctioned
PT
aggression and violence among team contact sports participants, and especially young male
RI
athletes. Violent behavior patterns that are learned through, for example, peer socialization or
SC
particular coaching styles in contact sport settings, might generalize to non-sporting settings (Huang et al., 1999). Some studies have focused specifically on social learning and normative
NU
socialization, and indicate how this process is gendered. Kreager’s (2007) study of 6,397 participants and non-participants aged 10-19 found that it is the combination of heavy
MA
physical contact and a team setting (e.g., in American football), and not merely the physical contact associated with contact sports, that encourages violence. Kreager (2007) demonstrates
D
that the effect is mediated by (gendered) peer networks/contexts, where embeddedness in all-
PT E
football networks substantially increases the risk of male serious fighting. Kreager’s (2007) study presents strong evidence for both social learning and masculinity explanations (with the
CE
latter focusing on the intertwinement of on-field violence, success, prestige, and essentialist images of “maleness” in contact sports). In a similar vein, Conroy et al.’s (2001) findings
AC
suggest that normative socialization in contact sports leads participants to perceive aggressive behavior as more legitimate than peers who were not socialized through contact sports over the years. The length of an athlete’s participation in these sports was found to be positively related to their perceptions of the legitimacy of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport (Conroy et al., 2001). This finding needs to be viewed in light of age and competition level, as discussed earlier.
21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT These and other studies draw attention to social learning and social norms as a mediating factor in the relationship between contact sports participation and violent behavior. Sønderlund et al. (2014) and O’Brien et al. (2018) interpret unsanctioned aggression and violence in sportspeople as a consequence of particular social norms and hyper-masculine identities present in some team contact sports (e.g., rugby, Australian rules football), which
PT
are maintained and enacted through team bonding practices and demonstrations of
RI
masculinity that frequently involve verbal and physical violence. This conclusion is
NU
(Messner, 2007; Pappas et al., 2004; Spaaij, 2015).
SC
consistent with several sociological studies of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport
MA
4.3.2. Moral atmosphere
Whereas sociological research foregrounds the role that socialization and social norms
D
play in shaping unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport, psychological and
PT E
pedagogical studies have examined this issue in relation to the concept of moral atmosphere (Bortoli et al., 2012; Guivernau & Duda, 2002; Shields, LaVoi, Bredemeier, & Power, 2007;
CE
Stephens & Kavanagh, 2003). Moral atmosphere refers to a team’s, a group’s, or an organization’s moral climate or ethos, and is often studied in terms of collective norms. There
AC
is consistent evidence that moral atmosphere is an important determinant of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. For example, Bortoli, Messina, Zorba, and Robazza’s (2012) cross-sectional study of 382 young male soccer players found that a moral atmosphere that encourages aggressive behaviors (e.g. physical violence directed at opposing players) is positively linked to aggressive and violent conduct by players. The authors further identified a weak but significant relation between performance climate (which emphasizes competitiveness, social comparison, and normative-based evaluation) and a moral
22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT atmosphere that encourages aggressive behavior. This finding is in line with other research, which shows that performance climate and ego orientation are positive predictors of unsanctioned aggression among young male soccer players (Kavussanu, 2006). Moral atmosphere research has specifically examined the role of coaches, team members, parents, and spectators in shaping sports participants’ moral actions (Bortoli et al.,
PT
2012; Shields et al., 2007; Walters et al., 2016). Several studies highlight the influence of
RI
these actors in creating a climate within which sports participants perceive or learn that
SC
unsanctioned aggression and violence will (not) be tolerated, endorsed, or penalized; for example, with regard to the role coaches or peers play in fostering a prosocial ethics and
NU
atmosphere (Baar & Wubbels, 2013; Rutten et al., 2007, 2008; Vertonghen & Theeboom, 2010). Consistent with most research in this area, a study of self-reported “poor sport
MA
behavior” (including physical violence) among 676 male and female youth sport participants found that perceived coach and spectator behaviors were strong predictors of self-reported
D
poor sport behavior among youth sport participants, followed by team norms (Shields et al.,
PT E
2007). This finding suggests that poor coach, spectator, or parental behavior may provide a cultural script to youth sports participants that unsanctioned aggression and violence will be
CE
tolerated or endorsed. In this regard, Nicholson and Hoye’s (2005) focus group research among Australian amateur sports clubs concluded that poor spectator behavior exhibited by
AC
parents “can be adopted by players, leading to inappropriate player behavior at junior levels” (p. 98). Yet, these studies rarely distinguish between instrumental and hostile aggression, and hence we know relatively little about how moral atmosphere might predict these two types of aggression differently.
23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 4.3.3. Moral disengagement and neutralization techniques
Moral atmosphere and socialization can, in some instances, result in a normalization of transgressive behavior in amateur sport. According to a mixed methods study conducted in three amateur soccer clubs in the Netherlands, this normalization process can create a climate
PT
within which participants may perceive particular forms of unsanctioned aggression and
RI
violence as legitimate behaviors (Claringbould, Spaaij & Vermeulen, 2018). Such behaviors
SC
typically included the use of instrumental aggression towards opposing players and verbal abuse or intimidation of referees and assistants. This study identified two moral
NU
disengagement mechanisms that participants typically used when they engaged in unsanctioned aggression or violence (Claringbould et al., 2018). First, participants tended to
MA
displace responsibility for their behavior to others, particularly (assistant) referees, and to frame their aggressive or violent behavior as a legitimate response to unfair actions on the
D
part of (assistant) referees or opponents. Second, participants sought to morally justify their
PT E
personal and in-group (i.e. teammates’) behaviors as being “part of the game” and intended to positively influence the outcome of the contest (Claringbould et al., 2018; see also Veldboer
CE
et al., 2003).
These findings are consistent with both the neutralization techniques identified by
AC
Sykes and Matza (1957) and Bandura et al.’s (1996) moral disengagement mechanisms, especially aggressors’ minimization of personal agency through denial or displacement of responsibility, and their appeal to higher loyalties (Ribeaud & Eisner, 2010). Other studies lend further evidence to the importance of these mechanisms. For example, Traclet, Romand, Morlet, and Kavussanu’s (2011) study of amateur soccer players found a tendency among players to displace responsibility for their behavior on coaches and referees, and to justify their acts by portraying them as serving moral or socially worthy purposes, such as
24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT preserving the team’s success or reputation. Importantly, Traclet et al. (2011) further showed that some aggressive and violent acts elicited more justifications than others. Specifically, instrumental aggression elicited more displacement of responsibility than hostile behaviors (Traclet et al., 2011).
RI
PT
4.4. Situational influences
SC
It is important to distinguish sociocultural influences on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport, which encompass a longer-term perspective, from factors in a
NU
given situation, since different mechanisms are necessary for both (Moesch et al., 2010). What the aforementioned individual, sociocultural, and contextual factors can predict is that
MA
some individuals will engage in unsanctioned aggression and violence “at some time, in some place, against some people, in some manner” within amateur sport (Cooney, 2009, p. 586).
D
Recent theory and research in the social sciences of violence seeks to move beyond this by
PT E
focusing attention on the situational dynamics of violence (Collins, 2008, 2009; Weenink, 2014). Situational approaches aim to explain violence through event-level interactions
CE
between individuals. They focus on the specific processes within face-to-face interactions that give rise to, or prevent, violence, and, in doing so, allow us to grasp how unsanctioned
AC
aggression and violence in amateur sport emerge at specific points in time, in specific places, and in specific ways. In other words, even though individual factors and motivations for violent behavior (e.g., self-enhancement, obsessive passion) may be present, and environmental conditions (e.g., moral atmosphere; social learning) may be conducive to unsanctioned aggression and violence, they still need to go through the eye of the situational needle in order for unsanctioned aggression and violence to occur.
25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Systematic situational, micro-level analysis of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport is still in its infancy (an exception is Klimczak et al., 2016). Below we discuss two areas of situationally focused research that warrant attention: conflictual interactions and the role of bystanders.
RI
PT
4.4.1. Conflictual interactions
SC
Unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport often appear to arise from conflictual interactions between actors on and around the playing field. Face-to-face
NU
interactions between players and spectators on the one hand, and (assistant) referees on the other hand, have attracted particular attention in scientific research. It is often argued that
MA
poor officiating (e.g., inconsistent decisions, overlooking fouls) is one of the main sources of unsanctioned aggression and violence in sport (Shapcott, Bloom, & Loughead, 2007; Traclet
D
et al., 2011). Referees frequently report that they have been victims of unsanctioned
PT E
aggression and violence (Ackery, Tator, & Snider, 2012). Some research suggests that “grey areas” in the officiating of amateur sport can give rise to player or spectator violence; for
CE
example, where referees have a greater knowledge and understanding of the technicalities and nuances of the rules compared to the players or spectators (Nicholson & Hoye, 2005;
AC
Veldboer, Boonstra, & Duyvendak, 2003). A study of unsanctioned aggression and violence in Dutch amateur soccer found that contested decisions or perceived partiality on the part of referees were the most common trigger for violent behavior on the soccer pitch (in approximately half of all incidents) (Duijvestijn et al., 2013). Emerging evidence suggests that the issue here is not so much participants’ actual (lack of) knowledge and awareness of the rules and punishments, but rather the extent to which they are convinced of their own rightness (Rijnhout, Giesen, van Hoof, & van Aken,
26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2016). This sense of rightness, and participants’ willingness to confront officials, seems to be frequently fueled by the passive or active support that bystanders afford participants, particularly at home games. When participants and bystanders perceive the behavior of officials in game situations (e.g., an offside or penalty decision) as illegitimate, they are more inclined to consider it morally legitimate to challenge or even intimidate referees
PT
(Claringbould et al., 2018). Participants and spectators expect officials to be authoritative and
RI
impartial, but they often do not experience officials’ behavior as living up to this expectation
SC
(Claringbould et al., 2018).
NU
4.4.2. Bystanders
MA
Bystanders play an important role not just in the formation of moral atmosphere and social norms, but also in situational dynamics of unsanctioned aggression and violence in
D
amateur sport. For example, Baar and Wubbels’ (2013) study of coaches in amateur sports
PT E
clubs found that coaches attributed peer-directed aggression to group dynamics and situational factors. A noteworthy finding from recent research centers on how bystanders
CE
occupy physical space at amateur sports activities and venues. A group of actors dominating the situational space can mark the rise of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur
AC
sport. The assertion of situational dominance provides a potential pathway to violence by producing a stratification in the emotional balance of the situation (Collins, 2008). In amateur youth sport, one way in which parents and coaches create situational dominance is through physical proximity to the playing field, coupled with verbal and non-verbal communication (i.e., shouting, cursing, threatening) that referees and players may interpret as intimidating (Claringbould et al., 2018). Parents and spectators who are highly identified with their sports team are more likely to exhibit such behaviors (see also Wann, Carlson, & Schrader, 1999).
27
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Claringbould, Spaaij, and Vermeulen (2018) found that, in amateur soccer, the physical proximity between spectators (parents and coaches) and linespersons allowed intimidating behavior to be exhibited more freely. While some spectators demonstrated an awareness that their behavior was pushing the boundaries of the socially acceptable, they tended to downplay and morally justify their own behavior. In contrast, they typically disapproved of
PT
the same behaviors when exhibited by opposing spectators or players (Claringbould et al.,
RI
2018).
SC
An important lesson from research on bystanders is that their identities and actions are created and defined in interactions within situations. In contrast to studies that draw on
NU
essentialist notions of identity (see section 4.1.1), social psychological work on helping behavior and violence suggests that group identities are not fixed but can change per situation
MA
and in situations, and that individuals develop lines of action as they identify situationally with a group (Levine, Prosser, Evans, & Reicher, 2005). In socio-ecological terms, this work
PT E
D
helps to understand individual behavior in context.
CE
5. Discussion
The research herein indicates both the myriad ways unsanctioned aggression and
AC
violence can manifest themselves in amateur sport, and the multiple actors involved in these behaviors as perpetrators, victims, bystanders, or authority figures. A critical synthesis of the contemporary literature shows that no single factor on its own can provide a sufficient understanding or explanation of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. The literature reviewed in this article suggests that individual, contextual, sociocultural, and situational influences interact to either facilitate, augment, reduce, or prevent unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. This conclusion is consistent with previous
28
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT reviews, which found that the social environments and sports contexts within which athletes are embedded have an important influence on how athletes perceive unsanctioned aggression and violence, and on how they act in potentially violent situations (Fields et al., 2007; Kimble et al., 2010; Terry & Jackson, 1985). In other words, while unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport are most visible and most felt at the individual and interpersonal
PT
level, their causes are embedded within social networks and cultural norms within and
RI
beyond sports environments.
SC
A socio-ecological perspective encourages policymakers and practitioners to recognize that the prevention and mitigation of unsanctioned aggression and violence in
NU
amateur sport requires a multi-pronged approach that addresses not only individual factors, but also situational, contextual, and sociocultural influences. The literature suggests that all
MA
actors involved in amateur sport (i.e., coaches, players, parents, officials, spectators) have a role to play and can potentially use their positions to prevent or de-escalate unsanctioned
D
aggression and violence. Some studies particularly emphasize the need to foster a more
PT E
tolerant climate that de-emphasizes hyper-competitiveness and a win-at-all-cost mentality (Fiore, 2003; Kavussanu, 2006; Kreager, 2007), while others point to the need for further
CE
education of coaches, officials, and spectators to increase their awareness and skills in recognizing and handling violent behavior and situations (Baar & Wubbels, 2013; Nicholson
AC
& Hoye, 2005; Sáenz Ibáñez et al., 2012). When considering these potential strategies, it is essential that we take into account the nuances that scientific research has identified, notably with regard to the distinction between instrumental (i.e., a non-provoking situation) and hostile (i.e., a provoking situation) aggression, and how perceptions regarding hostile aggression differ across competitive levels, gender, and cultures. The emergent attention to situational influences in the academic literature on unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport is consistent with two promising
29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT strands of criminological theory and research. First, it aligns with situational opportunity theories of crime that move beyond offender-focused approaches. Rather than focusing on individuals’ motivations and biographical tendencies that shape criminal activity, situational opportunity theories explore the impact of environmental, physical, and social factors on criminal behavior (Wilcox & Cullen, 2018). Second, situational approaches to unsanctioned
PT
aggression and violence in amateur sport can draw on empirical and theoretical literature on
RI
bystander effects in the rise and de-escalation of violent behavior. From a situational
SC
perspective, bystanders play an unavoidably active role in victim/victimizer interaction and in creating or transforming situational asymmetry that can serve as a pathway to violence.
NU
Whereas Collins (2008) perceives third parties mainly as escalating forces, recent social psychological research emphasizes conciliatory behaviors in violent incidents (Levine et al.
MA
2011).
The situational influences examined in this article draw policy and practical attention
D
to situational prevention and mitigation approaches that are yet to be fully explored and tested
PT E
in amateur sports contexts. Findings from situationally focused studies seem to confirm the potential merits of insights from situational crime prevention and bystander intervention
CE
strategies that have been applied in other contexts, such as school bullying, sexual violence prevention programs, and anti-racism programs (Bennett, Banyard, & Garnhart, 2014;
AC
Nelson, Dunn, & Paradies, 2011; Twemlow, Fonagi, & Sacco, 2004). There may be a policy potential of bystander anti-violence in sport through the focus that situational approaches bring to what people do in the here and now. Insights gained in non-sport settings can be used to develop a specific understanding of what promotes and hinders bystander actions in amateur sport, and what is needed to enhance prevention education initiatives focused on increasing positive social norms and bystander intervention. Further research on situational
30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT dynamics of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport is needed to provide the necessary evidence base for the development of such interventions.
6. Conclusion
PT
Participation in amateur sport can contribute to the social fabric and health of
RI
communities. The potentially deleterious consequences of unsanctioned aggression and
SC
violence in amateur sport for personal and social development are well documented, especially in relation to children and young people (Fields, Collins, & Comstock, 2010).
NU
Amateur sport is the competition level where the bulk of mass participation is concentrated, for instance through sports clubs, programs, and schools. Unsanctioned aggression and
MA
violence in amateur sport have attracted comparatively limited systematic scientific research relative to its counterparts in particular high-profile, mediatized professional contact sports.
D
However, as we have shown in this article, the body of scientific literature offers important
PT E
insights into this social phenomenon. This article has identified the need for understanding unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport within its social ecology and, to this
CE
end, has examined factors and influences that operate at different levels of analysis. The proposed framework sensitizes researchers and practitioners to the multi-level web of
AC
interacting influences on unsanctioned aggression and violence, as well as to factors and issues to be considered in relation to the prevention and mitigation of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport. In addressing its objective, this article has two noteworthy limitations. First, its critical synthesis of existing scientific literature is not exhaustive. Our primary aim was to explore connections between different levels of analysis, rather than exhaustively review previous literature on each level of analysis independently. This article should therefore be
31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT viewed in conjunction with previous studies that have focused in depth on particular factors. Second, this article focuses more on substantive findings from scientific research than on methodological aspects of these studies, with the exception of brief methodological descriptions provided in this article and in Table 1. The quality of the reviewed work in terms of methodological approaches and theory varies (Kimble et al., 2010), and recent research
PT
offers significant critical appraisal of established methodologies in this field of study (Gee,
RI
2010b, 2011). We would add to these evaluations that, with noted exceptions, there is still a
SC
dearth of interpretivist studies that analyze the meanings and narratives of aggression and violence created by amateur sports participants themselves. In pursuing more objectivist
NU
definitions, there is a risk that scholars move research away from naturally occurring situations in which participants give meaning to aggressive or violent behavior. Herein lies an
MA
important challenge for future research: to enlarge the space for a diversity of, and dialogue between, definitions and approaches in the study of unsanctioned aggression and violence in
References
CE
PT E
D
amateur sport.
Abrams, M. (2010). Anger management in sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
AC
Ackery, A. D., Tator, C. H., & Snider, C. (2012). Violence in Canadian amateur hockey: The experience of referees in Ontario. Clinical Journal of Sports Medicine, 22, 86-90. Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 364-374. Bennett, S., Banyard, V., & Garnhart, L. (2014). To act or not to act, that is the question?
32
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Barriers and facilitators of bystander intervention. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29, 476-496. Bloom, G. A., & Smith, M. D. (1996). Hockey violence: A test of cultural spillover theory. Sociology of Sport Journal, 13, 65-77. Bortoli, L., Messina, G., Zorba, M., & Robazza, C. (2012). Contextual and individual
PT
influences on antisocial behavior and psychobiosocial states of youth soccer players.
RI
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 397-406.
SC
Bourdieu, P. (2002). Masculine domination. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Brackenridge, C. (2010). Violence and abuse prevention in sport. In K. Kaufman (Ed.), The
NU
prevention of sexual violence: A practitioner’s sourcebook (pp. 401-414). Holyoke, MA: NEARI Press.
MA
Brackenridge, C., & Rhind, D. (2014). Child protection in sport: Reflections on thirty years of science and activism. Social Sciences, 3, 326-340.
D
Claringbould, I., Spaaij, R., & Vermeulen, J. (2018). Grensoverschrijdend gedrag op het
PT E
voetbalveld. Vrijetijdsstudies, 35, 23-35. Coakley, J. (2009). Sports in society: Issues and controversies (10th ed.). Boston, MA:
CE
McGraw-Hill.
Collins, R. (2009). The micro-sociology of violence. The British Journal of Sociology, 60,
AC
566-576.
Collins, R. (2008). Violence: A micro-sociological theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Conroy, D. E., Silva, J. M., Newcomer, R. R., Walker, B. W., & Johnson, M. S. (2001). Personal and participatory socializers of the perceived legitimacy of aggressive behavior in sport. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 405-418. Cooney, M. (2009). The scientific significance of Collins’s Violence. The British Journal of
33
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Sociology, 60, 586-594. Coulomb-Cabagno, G., & Rascle, O. (2006). Team sports players’ observed aggression as a function of gender, competitive level, and sport type. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1980-2000. Curry, T. J. (1993). A little pain never hurt anyone: Athletic career socialization and the
PT
normalization of sports injury. Symbolic Interactionism, 16, 273-290.
RI
Danioni, F., & Barni, D. (2017). The relations between adolescents’ personal values and
SC
prosocial and antisocial behaviours in team sports. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2017.1367951.
NU
Donahue, E., Rip, B., & Vallerand, R. (2009). When winning is everything: On passion, identity, and aggression in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 526-534.
MA
Duijvestijn, P., van Dijk, B., van Egmond, P., de Groot, M., van Sommeren, D., & Verwest, A. (2013). Excessief geweld op en om de voetbalvelden: Praktijkonderzoek naar
D
omvang, ernst en aanpak van “voetbalgeweld”. Amsterdam: DSP-Groep.
PT E
Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2005). Participation in power sports and antisocial involvement in preadolescent and adolescent boys. The Journal of Child Psychology
CE
and Psychiatry, 46, 468-478.
Fields, S., Collins, C. L., & Comstock, R. D. (2010). Violence in youth sports: Hazing,
AC
brawling and foul play. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 44, 32-37. Fields, S. K., Collins, C. L., & Comstock, R. D. (2007). Conflict on the courts: A review of sports-related violence literature. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 8, 359-369. Fiore, D. K. (2003). Parental rage and violence in youth sports: How can we prevent soccer moms and hockey dads from interfering in youth sports and causing games to end in fistfights rather than handshakes. Villanova Sports & Entertainment Law Journal, 10, 103-129.
34
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Gee, C. J. (2011). Aggression in competitive sports: Using direct observation to evaluate incidence and prevention focused intervention. In J. K. Luiselli and D. D. Reed (Eds.), Behavioral Sport Psychology (pp. 199-210). New York: Springer. Gee, C. J. (2010a). Predicting the use of aggressive behaviour among Canadian amateur hockey players: A psychosocial examination. Unpublished PhD Thesis. Toronto:
PT
University of Toronto.
RI
Gee, C. J. (2010b). Using a direct observation methodology to study aggressive behavior in
SC
ice hockey: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Behavioral Health and Medicine, 1, 79-90.
NU
Gill, F. (2007). “Violent” femininity: Women rugby players and gender negotiation. Women’s Studies International Forum, 30, 416-426.
MA
Gubbels, J., van der Stouwe, T., Spruit, A., & Stams, G. J. (2016). Martial arts participation and externalizing behavior in juveniles: A meta-analytic review. Aggression and
D
Violent Behavior, 28, 73-81.
PT E
Guilbert, S. (2006). Violence in sports and among sportsmen: A single or two‐ track issue? Aggressive Behavior, 32, 231-240.
CE
Guilbert, S. (2004). Sport and violence: A typological analysis. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 39, 45-55.
AC
Guivernau, M., & Duda, J. (2002). Moral atmosphere and athletic aggressive tendencies in young soccer players. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 67-85. de Haan, W. (2008). Violence as an essentially contested concept. In S. Body-Gendrot & P. Spierenburg (Eds.), Violence in Europe: Historical and contemporary perspectives (pp. 27-40). Heidelberg: Springer. Harwood, , A., Lavidor, M., & Rassovsky, Y. (2017). Reducing aggression with martial arts:
35
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT A meta-analysis of child and youth studies. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 34, 96101. Huang, D. B., Cherek, D. R., & Lane, S. D. (1999). Laboratory measurement of aggression in high school age athletes: Provocation in a nonsporting context. Psychological Reports, 85, 1251-1262.
PT
Kavussanu, M. (2006). Motivational predictors of prosocial and antisocial behaviour in
RI
football. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 575-588.
SC
Kavussanu, M., Stamp, R., Slade, G., & Ring, C. (2009). Observed prosocial and antisocial behaviors in male and female soccer players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
NU
21, S62-S76.
Kerr, J. H. (2017). The motivation behind unsanctioned violence in international rugby: A
MA
case study of a former elite player. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 15(1), 80-91.
D
Kerr, J. H. (2016). Physical aggression and violence in women’s sport: A review of existing
PT E
research. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 47, 43-66. Kerr, J. H. (2012). Violence in rugby. Smashwords (ebook). Available at
CE
http://www.smashwords.com/books/view/257426 (accessed June 14, 2016). Kerr, J. H. (2002). Issues in aggression and violence in sport: The ISSP position stand
AC
revisited. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 68-78. Kerr, J. H. (1999). The role of aggression and violence in sport. A rejoinder to the ISSP position stand. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 83-88. Kimble, N. B., Russo, S. A., Bergman, B. G., & Galindo, V. H. (2010). Revealing an empirical understanding of aggression and violent behavior in athletics. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15, 446-462. Kirker, B., Tenenbaum, G., & Mattson, J. (2000). An investigation of the dynamics of
36
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT aggression: Direct observation in ice hockey and basketball. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 373-386. Klimczak, J., Barczyński, B. J., Podstawski, R., & Kalina, R. M. (2016). The level of bravery and aggressiveness of the sports activity organisers for the youth: Simulation research. Archives of Budo, 12, 345-354.
PT
Klimczak, J., Podstawski, R., & Dobosz, D. (2014). The association of sport and violence,
RI
aggression and aggressiveness: Prospects for education about non-aggression and
SC
reduction of aggressiveness. Archives of Budo, 10, 273-286.
Kreager, D. (2007). Unnecessary roughness? School sports, peer networks, and male
NU
adolescent violence. American Sociological Review, 72, 705-724. Levin, D. S., Smith, E. A., Caldwell, L. L., & Kimbrough, J. (1995). Violence and high
MA
school sports participation. Pediatric Exercise Science, 7, 379-388. Levine, M., Prosser, A., Evans, D., & Reicher, S. (2005). Identity and emergency
D
intervention: How social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries
PT E
shape helping behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 443-453. Levine, M., Taylor, P., & Best, L. (2011). Third parties, violence, and conflict resolution: The
CE
role of group size and collective action in the microregulation of violence. Psychological Science, 22, 406-412.
AC
Matthews, C. R., & Channon, A. (2016). “It’s only sport”: The symbolic neutralization of “violence”. Symbolic Interaction, 39, 557-576. Maxwell, J. P., Visek, A. J., & Moores, E. (2009). Anger and perceived legitimacy of aggression in male Hong Kong Chinese athletes: Effects of type of sport and level of competition. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 289-296. McLeroy, K. R., Bibeau, D., Steckler, A., & Glanz, K. (1988). An ecological perspective on health promotion programs. Health Education Quarterly, 15, 351-377.
37
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Messner, M. A. (2007). Out of play: Critical essays on gender and sport. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Moesch, K., Birrer, D., & Seiler, R. (2010). Differences between violent and non-violent adolescents in terms of sport background and sport-related psychological variables. European Journal of Sport Science, 10, 319-328.
PT
Mutz, M., & Baur, G. (2009). The role of sports for violence prevention: Sport club
RI
participation and violent behaviour among adolescents. International Journal of Sport
SC
Policy, 1, 305-321.
Nelson, J. K., Dunn, K., & Paradies, Y. (2011). Bystander anti-racism: A review of the
NU
literature. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 11, 263-284.
and Social Issues, 21, 379-391.
MA
Nixon, H. L. (1997). Gender, sport, and aggressive behaviour outside sport. Journal of Sport
O’Brien, K. S., Forrest, W., Greenlees, I., Rhind, D., Jowett, S., Pinsky, I., Espelt, A.,
D
Bosque-Prous, M., Sønderlund, A., Vergani, M., & Iqbal, M. (2018). Alcohol
PT E
consumption, masculinity, and alcohol-related violence and anti-social behaviour in sportspeople. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 21, 335-341.
CE
Pappas, N., McKenry, P., & Skilken Catlett, B. (2004). Athlete aggression on the rink and off the ice: Athlete violence and aggression in hockey and interpersonal relationships.
AC
Men and Masculinities, 6, 291-312. Pedersen, D. (2007). Perceived aggression in sports and its relation to willingness to participate and perceived risk of injury. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104, 201-211. Ribeaud, D., & Eisner, M. (2010). Are moral disengagement, neutralization techniques, and self-serving cognitive distortions the same? Developing a unified scale of moral neutralization of aggression. International Journal of Conflict and Violence, 4, 298315.
38
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Rijnhout, R., Giesen, I., van Hoof, A., & van Aken, M. (2016). Agressie van jeugdige voetballers en de rol van het recht: kennis en (on)begrip van de (spel)regels empirisch getoetst. Aansprakelijkheid Verzekering en Schade, 34, 171-179. Romijn, D., van Kalmthout, J., & Breedveld, K. (2016). VSK monitor 2016: Voortgangsrapportage Actieplan “Naar een veiliger sportklimaat”. Utrecht,
PT
Netherlands: Mulier Institute.
RI
Rutten, E., Deković, M., Stams, G. J., Schuengel C., Hoeksma, J., & Biesta, G. (2008).
SC
On- and off-field antisocial and prosocial behavior in adolescent soccer players: A multilevel study. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 371-387.
NU
Rutten, E., Stams, G. J., Biesta, G., Schuengel, C., Dirks, E., & Hoeksma, J. (2007). The contribution of organized youth sport to antisocial and prosocial behavior in
MA
adolescent athletes. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 255-264. Sacks, D. N., Petscher, Y., Stanley, C. T., & Tenenbaum, G. (2003). Aggression and violence
PT E
Psychology, 1, 167-179.
D
in sport: Moving beyond the debate. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
Sáenz Ibáñez, A., Gimeno Marco, F., Gutiérrez Pablo, H., & Garay Ibáñez de Elejalde, B.
CE
(2012). Prevención de la agresividad y la violencia en el deporte en edad escolar: Un estudio de revision. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte, 12, 57-72.
AC
Sheldon, J. P., & Aimar, C. M. (2001). The role aggression plays in successful and unsuccessful ice hockey behaviors. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 304-309. Sherrill, A. M., & Bradel, L. T. (2017). Contact sport participation predicts instrumental aggression, not hostile aggression, within competition: quasi-experimental evidence. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 9, 50-57. Shields, D. L., LaVoi, N. M., Bredemeier, B. L., & Power, F. C. (2007). Journal of Sport and
39
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Exercise Psychology, 29, 747-762. Sønderlund, A. L., O’Brien, K., Kremer, P., Rowland, B., De Groot, F., Staiger, P., Zinkiewicz, L., & Miller, P. G. (2014). The association between sports participation, alcohol use and aggression and violence: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 17, 2-7.
RI
Sociology of Sport (pp. 324-334). London: Routledge.
PT
Spaaij, R. (2015). Sport and violence. In R. Giulianotti (Ed.), Routledge Handbook of
SC
Spaaij, R. (2014). Sports crowd violence: An interdisciplinary synthesis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19, 146-155.
NU
Stephens, D., & Kavanagh, B. (2003). Aggression in Canadian youth ice hockey: The role of moral atmosphere. International Sports Journal, 7, 109-119.
MA
Sykes, G., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664-670.
D
Tenenbaum, G., Sacks, D. N., Miller, J. W., Golden, A. S., & Doolin, N. (2000). Aggression
PT E
and violence in sport: A reply to Kerr’s rejoinder. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 315326.
CE
Tenenbaum G., Stewart, E., Singer, R. N., & Duda J. (1997). Aggression and violence in sport: An ISSP position stand. The Sport Psychologist, 11, 1-7.
AC
Terry, P. C., & Jackson, J. J. (1985). The determinants and control of violence in sport. Quest, 37, 27-37. Twemlow, S., Fonagy, P., & Sacco, F. (2004). The role of the bystander in the social architecture of bullying and violence in schools and communities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036, 215-232. Veldboer, L., Boonstra, N., & Duyvendak, J. W. (2003). Agressie in de sport: Fysieke en
40
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT verbale agressie in de Rotterdamse amateursport: ervaringen en verklaringen. Utrecht, Netherlands: Verwey-Jonker Instituut. Vertonghen, J., & Theeboom, M. (2010). The social-psychological outcomes of martial arts practise among youth: A review. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 9, 528-537. Wacquant, L. (2004). Body and soul: Notebooks of an apprentice boxer. Chicago, IL:
PT
University of Chicago Press.
RI
Walters, S., Schluter, P., Stamp, D., Thomson, R., & Payne, D. (2016). Coaches and referees'
SC
perspectives of sideline behaviour in children's team sports: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Sport Management, Recreation & Tourism, 23, 51-74.
NU
Wann, D. L., Carlson, J. D., & Schrader, M. P. (1999). The impact of team identification on the hostile and instrumental verbal aggression of sport spectators. Journal of Social
MA
Behavior and Personality, 14, 279-286.
Weenink, D. (2014). Frenzied attacks: A micro-sociological analysis of the emotional
D
dynamics of extreme youth violence. The British Journal of Sociology, 65, 411-33.
PT E
Wieviorka, M. (2009). Violence: A new approach. London: Sage. Wilcox, P., & Cullen, F. T. (2018). Situational opportunity theories of crime. Annual Review
CE
of Criminology, 1, 123-148.
Young, K. (2015). Assessing the sociology of sport: On sports violence and ways of seeing.
AC
International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 50, 640-644. Young, K. (2012). Sport, violence and society. New York: Routledge.
41
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Figure 1
AC
CE
PT E
D
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
A socio-ecological model of unsanctioned aggression and violence in amateur sport
42
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Table 1 Characteristics of the empirical studies Year
Author
Country
Methods
Sample
Sport
Age group
Sport
T P
participation
I R
Behavioral focus/ terminology
type
1995
Levin et al.
United
Questionnaire
2,756
States
Contact sports (e.g., American
High school
football, wrestling, basketball);
SC
Noncontact sports (e.g., tennis,
U N
swimming, track) 1996
Bloom &
Canada
Interview
753
Ice hockey
Smith 1999
Huang et al.
United
Behavioral
States
testing,
16
D E
Conroy et al.
United
Questionnaire
C C
A
Delinquent behavior (e.g.,
Players
Violence (e.g., fighting, assault)
15-18 years
Players
Aggression (e.g., instrumental physical force)
basketball);
T P E
States
12-21 years
Violence (e.g., assault);
damaging property)
American football and
questionnaire
2001
A M
High contact sports (e.g.,
students
Players
1,018
Low contact sports (e.g., track and baseball) Collision sports (e.g., ice
8-19 years
Players
Aggressive behaviour (e.g.,
hockey, American football,
verbal aggression, physical
lacrosse);
aggression)
Contact sports (e.g., basketball, field hockey, soccer); Noncontact sports (e.g., baseball, bowling, golf, tennis)
43
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2003
Veldboer et
Netherlands
Questionnaire
787
al.
Several sports including soccer,
18 years and
field hockey, and water polo
above
Players
Aggression (e.g., intimidating, kicking, or hitting an opposing player)
2004
Pappas et al.
Canada and
Interview
5
Ice hockey
25-30 years
United
Endresen &
Norway
Longitudinal
477
Olweus
2005
Nicholson &
Australia
Focus group
62
Coulomb-
E C
Cabagno &
observation
Rascle
2006
Kavussanu
United Kingdom
2006
Walters et al.
Players and
Violent and antisocial
oriental martial arts
SC
non-
behaviour (e.g., fighting,
participants
attacking other people)
Not
Players,
Poor sport spectator behaviour
specified
parents,
(e.g., verbal abuse, vandalism,
coaches,
physical violence)
New Zealand
C A
Questionnaire
Questionnaire
D E
PT
Structured
180
fighting, sexual assault)
11-13 years (at Time 1)
U N
soccer, and basketball
France
I R
Violence and aggression (e.g.,
Boxing, wrestling, weightlifting,
Australian rules football, netball,
Hoye
2006
T P
players
States 2005
Former
A M
administrators, venue managers, and officials
Soccer and handball
games
Not
Players
Aggression
Players
Antisocial behaviour (e.g.,
specified; primarily adults
325
Soccer
12-17
elbowing an opposing player) 287
Rugby union, touch rugby,
16 years and
Referees and
Sideline behavior (e.g., verbal
netball, and soccer
above
coaches
abuse)
44
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2007
Pedersen
United
Questionnaire
285
States
Several sports including ice
Not
Players and
Aggression (e.g., intent to
hockey, boxing, rugby,
specified;
non-
harm)
American football, wrestling
mean: 23.7
participants
(men) and
T P
21.3 (women)
I R
years 2007
Kreager
United
Secondary
States
analysis
6,397
Gill
United
Ethnography
One club
Shields et al.
United
Players and
specified
club members
9-15 years
Players
U N
Rugby
Kingdom 2007
Not
American football, basketball, baseball, wrestling, and tennis
2007
SC
Players and
Several sports including
Questionnaire
676
A M
States
Basketball, soccer, American
10-19 years
nonparticipants
football, ice hockey,
D E
Violence (e.g., fighting)
Violence (e.g., sexual assault)
Poor sportspersonship (e.g., hitting or kicking others)
baseball/softball, and lacrosse
2007
2008
2009
Rutten et al.
Rutten et al.
Kavussanu et
Netherlands
Netherlands
England
al.
2009
Ackery et al.
PT
Questionnaire
E C
Questionnaire
C A
Questionnaire,
260
331
Soccer and swimming
12-18 years
Players
vandalism)
Soccer
9-19 years
Players
Questionnaire
Antisocial behavior (e.g., verbal abuse)
464
Soccer
15-47 years
Players
observation
Canada
Antisocial behavior (e.g.,
Antisocial behaviors (e.g., pushing from behind, kicking, aggressive to referee)
632
Ice hockey
12-67 years
Referees
Violence (e.g., verbal and physical abuse)
45
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2009
Maxwell et
Hong Kong
Questionnaire
471
al.
Basketball, rugby union, soccer,
2009
Donahue et
14-56 years
Players
and squash Canada
Questionnaire
60
verbal)
Basketball
14-17 years
Players
al. 2010
Gee
Canada
Mixed
678
Ice hockey
13-18 years
Aggressive behaviour (e.g.,
coaches, and
fighting, high sticking,
parents
kneeing)
SC
Players
Violence (e.g., physical,
16-22 years
Players
I R
and archival) Switzerland
Questionnaire
2,438
Contact sports;
12-18 years
U N
Noncontact sports; Team sports;
A M
Individual sports 2011
Traclet et al.
France
Stimulated
30
recall
Soccer
D E
interview 2012
2013
Bortoli et al.
Baar & Wubbels
2016
Rijnhout et
Italy
T P E
Questionnaire
Netherlands
C C
Interview
A
Netherlands
Questionnaire
382
194
Soccer
T P
Players,
(questionnaire
Moesch et al.
Aggression (e.g., harming an opponent)
methods
2010
Aggression (e.g., physical,
psychological)
Antisocial behaviour (e.g., hitting, illegal tackling)
14-16 years
Players
Antisocial behaviour (e.g., pushing, intimidating opposing player)
17 sports
Not
Coaches,
Peer aggression (e.g.,
specified
school
bullying)
teachers 114
Soccer
15-18 years
al.
Players
Aggression (e.g. physical violence, verbal abuse)
46
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 2017
Danioni &
Italy
Questionnaire
172
Barni
Team sports including
2017
2018
Sherrill &
United
Bradel
States
Claringbould
Netherlands
et al.
2018
O’Brien et
Questionnaire
Interview and
38
300
13-19 years
physical violence, verbal
and rugby
abuse)
Contact sports (e.g., American
18 and
football)
above
Soccer
al.
Several sports including soccer,
A M
rugby (union/league), hockey, cricket, netball, basketball, athletics, lacrosse, swimming,
D E
and tennis
T P E
C C
A
47
T P
I R
Aggression (e.g., physical force beyond sanctioned limits)
Players,
Transgressive behavior (e.g.,
SC
coaches,
assault, verbal abuse,
officials,
intimidation)
18 years and
Players
U N
1,720
Players
15-18 years (players)
Questionnaire
Antisocial behavior (e.g.,
volleyball, soccer, basketball,
observation
England
Players
above
spectators Violence and antisocial behavior (e.g., assault, vandalism, sexual assault).
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights We examine aggression and violence in amateur sport in its social ecology
Individual, situational, contextual, and sociocultural influences are analyzed
Emergent evidence shows the importance of situational factors
Bystander intervention can inform future research and practice
AC
CE
PT E
D
MA
NU
SC
RI
PT
48
Figure 1