Why confidence matters more than expertise

Why confidence matters more than expertise

For daily news stories, visit www.NewScientist.com/news Why cocky pundits prosper, and why we love them MARK LENNIHAN/AP PHOTO EVER wondered why th...

225KB Sizes 0 Downloads 67 Views

For daily news stories, visit www.NewScientist.com/news

Why cocky pundits prosper, and why we love them

MARK LENNIHAN/AP PHOTO

EVER wondered why the pundits who failed to predict the current economic crisis are still being paid for their opinions? It’s a consequence of the way human psychology works in a free market, according to a study of how people’s self-confidence affects the way others respond to their advice. The research, by Don Moore of Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, shows that we prefer advice from a confident source, even to the point that we are willing to forgive a poor track record. Moore argues that in competitive situations, this can drive those offering advice to increasingly exaggerate how sure they are. And it spells bad news for scientists who try to be honest about gaps in their knowledge. In Moore’s experiment, volunteers were given cash for correctly guessing the weight of people from their photographs. In each of the eight rounds of the study, the guessers bought advice from one of four other volunteers. The guessers could see in advance how confident each of these advisers was (see table), but not which weights they had opted for. From the start, the more confident advisers found more buyers for their advice, and this caused the advisers to give answers that were more and more precise as the game progressed. This escalation in precision

disappeared when guessers simply had to choose whether or not to buy the advice of a single adviser. In the later rounds, guessers tended to avoid advisers who had been wrong previously, but this effect was more than outweighed by the bias towards confidence. The findings add weight to the idea that if offering expert opinion is your stock-in-trade, it pays to appear confident. Describing his work at an Association for Psychological Science meeting in San Francisco last month, Moore said that following the advice of the most confident person often makes sense, as there is evidence that precision and expertise do tend to go hand in hand. For example, people give a narrower range of answers when asked about subjects with which they are more familiar (Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, vol 107, p 179). There are times, however, when this link breaks down. With complex but politicised subjects such as global warming, for example, scientific experts who stress uncertainties lose out to activists or lobbyists with a more emphatic message. So if honest advice risks being ignored, what is a responsible scientific adviser to do? “It’s an excellent question, and I’m not sure that I have a great answer,” says Moore. Peter Aldhous ■

Confidence pays Volunteers acting as “advisers” profited by selling their advice to other volunteers – who in turn won cash by guessing a person’s weight ADVISERS CAN SPREAD THEIR ESTIMATES OF A PARTICULAR PERSON’S WEIGHT DEPENDING ON HOW CONFIDENT THEY ARE Estimated weight (lbs)

ADVISER A

ADVISER B

ADVISER C

ADVISER D

5%

120-129 130-139

5%

140-149

20% 20%

50%

150-159 160-169

15%

10%

20%

170-179

70%

5%

40%

180-189

15%

5%

10%

100%

10%

190-199 200-209 210-219

INFORMATION GIVEN TO “GUESSERS” WHO THEN HAD TO BUY ADVICE FROM ONE OF THE ADVISERS ADVISER A

ADVISER B

ADVISER C 20%

15%

20%

70%

50%

20%

15%

10%

40%

ADVISER D

100%

ADVISER D’s OPINION WAS MOST LIKELY TO ATTRACT BUYERS

–No doubts allowed– 6 June 2009 | NewScientist | 15