Withdrawal emotional-regulation in infant rats from genetic animal models of depression

Withdrawal emotional-regulation in infant rats from genetic animal models of depression

Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com...

279KB Sizes 5 Downloads 26 Views

Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Withdrawal emotional-regulation in infant rats from genetic animal models of depression Y. Braw a , O. Malkesman b , A. Merenlender c , A. Bercovich b , M. Dagan b , D.H. Overstreet d , A. Weller a,∗ a

Department of Psychology and the Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan 52900, Israel Interdisciplinary Program in the Brain Sciences and the Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel c Faculty of Life-Sciences and the Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel d Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA b

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Received 21 February 2008 Received in revised form 24 April 2008 Accepted 28 April 2008 Available online 4 May 2008 Keywords: Animal models Depression WKY FSL Attachment Chronic-stress

a b s t r a c t Children of depressed parents exhibit high rates of emotion-dysregulation, characterized by excessive withdrawal or approach strategies toward the mother in infancy. The understanding of factors affecting the establishment of these behavioral deficits is limited. The current study utilized two genetic animal models of depression, the Wistar–Kyoto (WKY) and Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) rat strains. In addition, in order to assess the interactive effects of depressive vulnerability and exposure to early life stress, the subjects were raised either in a standard rearing condition or exposed to mild chronic-stress on postnatal days (PND) 2–9. On PND 10–11, an isolation test examined the pups’ emotion-regulation. WKY pups produced less separation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) and proximity-seeking behaviors, compared to controls. In addition, WKY pups did not show the expected potentiation effect that was evident in control pups (an increase in USV and pivoting behavior after a short reunion with the dam). FSL pups exhibited less proximity-seeking behaviors compared to controls while showing levels of USV, potentiation of USV, and change in proximity-seeking behaviors that were similar to controls. No differences between the strains were found in self-grooming. The early life chronic-stress paradigm had no effect on the behaviors of the pups, indicating either stress-resilience or a limited effect of the paradigm. Overall, the results tentatively imply a tendency of the WKY and FSL pups towards withdrawal behavior instead of approachbehavior when regulating emotion, with a more pronounced pattern in WKY pups. This behavioral profile is reminiscent of avoidant attachment, a characteristic of many children of depressed parents. © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Emotional-regulation, “extrinsic and intrinsic processes involved in monitoring, facilitating and inhibiting heightened levels of positive and negative affect” [1,2], received in the past years increasing research attention [3–8]. Emotional-regulation is comprised of a multitude of physiological, cognitive, and behavioral overlapping processes, and follows a developmental trajectory of increasing reliance on self-regulation by the infant [2,9–11]. The maturation of these emotion-regulation capacities is considered central to the organism’s growing abilities to handle stress, develop coping skills, mediate attentional and learning processes and achieve optimal functioning [10,12,13]. Parental depression has been shown to adversely affect the development of the offspring’s emotion-regulation, along with

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +972 3 5318548; fax: +972 3 7384105. E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Weller). 0166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.026

effects on their social and cognitive development [14–17]. Children of depressed parents exhibit emotional, social, and cognitive deficits and are at risk for psychiatric disorders [15,18–20]. These infants are typically described as withdrawn and exhibit “depressed-like” behavior [21]. Animal models of depression are used in order to understand the neurobiological basis of the disorder and to predict successful treatment strategies [22,23]. Genetic animal models of depression are a relatively new type of models, exhibiting hereditary vulnerability similar to that found in depressed humans. The present research utilized two well-studied genetic animal models, the Flinder’s Sensitive Line (FSL) and the Wistar–Kyoto (WKY) rats [24–26]. The FSL model was created by selectively breeding Sprague–Dawley (SD) rats to exhibit hyper-sensitivity to cholinergic agonists, as found in depressed humans [27,28]. FSL rats exhibit abnormalities in central neurochemical systems associated with depression (e.g., dopaminergic, serotoninergic and HPA axis) and behavioral deficits resembling those of depressed humans [24,29–32]. The second animal model, the WKY strain, was inbred from the Wistar strain

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

and exhibits characteristic depressive-like behavior and sensitivity to stress [25,33,34]. In addition, WKY rats exhibit neurochemical abnormalities in several systems (e.g., dopaminergic and noradrenergic), as well as in peripheral hormones (e.g., HPA axis and TSH) [35–37]. Earlier studies indicated physiological and behavioral abnormalities of FSL pups at PND 1 and reduced approach-behavior toward the dam in both FSL and WKY strains, at PND 17–19 [38,39]. In addition, research in our laboratory of older (pre-pubertal) WKY and FSL rats indicated that these rats exhibit heightened depressive-like behaviors, such as greater immobility in the swimtest compared to controls [35,40]. The two strains presented opposite profiles of social play and of basal corticosterone and ACTH hormonal levels [35]. WKY rats further exhibited anhedonic and anxious-like profiles too [40,41]. In the current study we aimed to elaborate upon these previous findings, focusing on emotionregulation in these animal models. This is important in light of the high rates of comorbid emotion-dysregulation and depression; in clinical samples the rate of comorbid anxiety disorders in depressed youth can run as high as 70%, and in community samples the average is 25%–50%[42–45]. Comorbidity is also associated with increased severity of depression [46–48], and may even exert a potentiating effect [45,49]. In the current study, emotion-regulation was analyzed in a series of two separations from the dam, an extended version of the isolation-test [50]. Both separation-protest and proximity-seekingbehaviors were assessed. Ultrasonic-vocalization (USV), whistle like sounds in frequencies of 40–70 kHz produced by rat pups, were used as indicators of separation-protest [51–53]. USV production plays an ethologically important role in mother-offspring communication, promptly stimulating maternal behavior [54–56]. In addition, ‘Potentiation of USV’ was assessed during a subsequent separation from the dam (after a previous separation) [57]. Finally, proximity-seeking behaviors, active behaviors through which an infant achieves proximity or maintains contact with the dam, were assessed [58]. We hypothesized that WKY and FSL pups will exhibit abnormal USV production and potentiation in light of the susceptibility of these behaviors to earlier parental behavior [59,60]. More broadly, we hypothesized that WKY/FSL pups will exhibit emotiondysregulation compared to their control strains. The second aim of the study was the investigation of the effect of early life stress on the establishment of emotion-regulation in the two animal models. In humans, early life stress constitutes a major risk-factor for the development and persistence of mental disorders [61,62]. According to diathesis-stress theories, stress interacts with hereditary vulnerability in the establishment of these mental disorders [63]. In order to assess these interactive effects, the subjects were tested either in a standard rearing condition or after exposure to mild chronic-stress (PND 2–9) [64]. The stress paradigm chosen is based on the finding that limiting the amount of available bedding material in the cage constitutes a persistent stressor for dam and pups [65]. This paradigm is unique in that it constitutes a handling manipulation with long-term effects, while avoiding the use of stress-arousing manipulations alien to the world of the rat. We hypothesized that differences between the ‘depressed-like’ and control strains will be more pronounced after exposure to the early life mild chronic-stress paradigm. 2. Method 2.1. Subjects WKY, Wister, FSL and SD pups at post-natal day 10/11 participated in the study (N = 196). There were a total of 8 experimental groups (4 strains × 2 growing conditions), with N = 24–25 pups per group. Two siblings per litter were tested in order to achieve litter representation while controlling for litter effects [66]. Their results

95

were averaged and entered as single datum to the analyses, resulting in N = 12–13 per group. The animals were reared in the ‘Developmental Psychobiological Laboratory’ in Bar-Ilan University. Cages of pregnant dams were checked daily before noon and parturition day was noted as post-natal day 0 (PND 0). Dam and pups were maintained in individual polycarbonate cages (26 cm × 42 cm × 30 cm), in our colony with temperatures of 22 ± 2 ◦ C and a light–dark cycle of 14:10 (lights on between 5:00 and 19:00). The animals had food and water ad lib. On PND 2/3 litter size was culled to 6 (with equal sex distribution when possible) and the litters were divided randomly to 2 groups, as described in the procedure section [in accordance to; 64]. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee in Bar-Ilan University and adhered to the guidelines of the Society for Neuroscience and the American Psychological Association. 2.2. Materials 2.2.1. Pup observation cage A transparent plastic cage (12.5 cm diameter, 13.5 cm high), used for recording USV and other relevant behaviors of the pup. 2.2.2. Dam-pups observation cage Identical to the home cage (26 cm × 42 cm × 30 cm). The dam was kept in the cage and it was used for dam-pup reunions. 2.2.3. USV measurement and data analysis system USV measurement was conducted using a QMC mini bat detector set to frequencies of 40–45 KHz [67]. USV’s were recorded using a mini-disc recorder and counted later by pressing a button that activated a silent electronic counter. Reliability tests showed an inter-observer correlation of 0.97 with this method [68] and 0.86 in our laboratory on the current data set (using the ‘Ultravox’ automatic acoustic monitoring device, designed for USV detection; Noldus Information Technology b.v., Wageninpen, The Netherlands). Pup behavior and dam-pup interactions were analyzed using the Observer 5.0, a software package for the collection, analysis and presentation of observational data (Noldus Information Technology). 2.3. Procedure Pups from each strain were divided into two groups on PND 2/3: (2) standard growing conditions: litters and dams were transferred to standard growing cages (with bedding from wood shavings). Bedding was replaced at PND 4/5: (2) ‘limited-bedding’ condition (simulating mild chronic-stress): litters and dams were transferred to standard cages equipped with a steel grid (2.5 cm above the cages floor); the grid served as the cage floor while allowing the passage of excrements. The only bedding materials available were paper towels (total: 112.5 cm × 22.5 cm), towels that the dam shreds and uses to make a nest [64]. The paper towels were replaced at PND 4/5. Overall, the procedure created 8 groups (4 rat strains, 2 growingconditions), with two independent variables; strain (WKY pups compared to Wistar or FSL compared to SD) and growing-condition (chronic-stress compared to the control, standard housing-condition). At PND 8/9 all litters and dams were returned to standard housing-conditions for two days until the start of the experiment. At PND 10/11 between 7:00 and 9:00 a litter was taken out of the home cage and placed in a temporary cage (20 cm × 26 cm × 22 cm; a small amount of cage bedding was transferred to this cage to maintain original cage scent) in a humid incubator at 33 ◦ C. At the same time, the dam was transferred, in the home cage, to the next room. Next, the isolation-test procedure was conducted as follows: (a) first separation from dam: a pup was taken out of the incubator 2 min after being placed there with its litter, and put in the ‘pup observation cage’ for 5 min. USV production was detected using a QMC mini bat detector and the pup’s behavior was video taped for later analysis. (b) Reunion with dam: the pup was moved to the dams’ cage for 5 min. (c) Second separation: the pup was put back into the ‘pup observation cage’. USV and relevant behaviors were recorded for 5 min. (d) Afterwards a second pup underwent an identical procedure, using a new clean ‘pup observation cage’ (as described in a–c, above). The onset of this second pups’ test was about 17 min from placement in the incubator.

3. Data analysis All analyses were performed separately for the two different progenitor strains. Thus, one set of analyses compared pups from the WKY ‘depressed-like’ strain to their control strain, Wistar. Another set of analyses compared FSL rats to SD controls. USV and pup behavior analyses: USV data were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA with strain and growing-condition as between-subject measures and potentiation (difference between USV in the first isolation and the second, after a 5 min reunion with dam) as a within-subject repeated-measure. Pups’ behaviors [pivoting, supported rearing [wall-climbing] and self-grooming,

96

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

Fig. 1. (A) USV production of WKY pups (PND 10/11) compared to Wistar controls (combining results from two sibling pups): Mean ± S.E.M. of frequency in separation (first separation from dam) and in potentiation (second separation after a reunion with dam). (B) Number of proximity-seeking behaviors and self-grooming of WKY compared to Wistar controls (combining results from two sibling pups): Mean ± S.E.M. of pivoting, supported rearing and self-grooming. Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

modified from 69] were analyzed using two separate MANOVAs and one ANOVA. In each MANOVA data on number and duration of behavior were entered; the ANOVA for self-grooming only included duration, because of a large correlation with number; r = 0.851). Significant MANOVAs were followed by one-way ANOVA’s to ascertain source of significance. Since no significant growing-condition effects were found, these results are not presented. 4. Results 4.1. WKY pups’ behavior compared to Wistar controls 4.1.1. Ultra-Sonic Vocalizations (USV) There was a significant strain main-effect with WKY pups emitting less USV compared to controls [F(1,45) = 64.84, p < .001]. The effect was accompanied by a significant potentiation main-effect and a strain × potentiation interaction [F(1,45) = 44.25, p < .001; F(1,45) = 40.90, p < .001; respectively]; WKY pups showed similar USV levels before and after the reunion with the dam, while control pups showed higher USV in the second separation (a potentiation effect). There were no additional significant potentiation interactions. See Fig. 1A.

the first separation and second separation from the dam, while Wistar pups showed a decrease both in number and duration [F(1,44) = 24.65, p < .001; F(1,44) = 27.34, p < .001; for number and duration of pivoting, respectively, Table 1]. There were no other significant potentiation interactions. 4.1.3. Supported rearing There was a significant strain main-effect [F(2,43) = 3.34, p < .05; Fig. 1B]; WKY pups performed less rearings and had shorter duration of rearing, compared to controls [F(1,44) = 4.44, p < .05; F(1,44) = 5.21, p < .05; respectively]. There was a significant potentiation main-effect without an accompanying potentiation × strain interaction [F(2,43) = 46.07, p < .001, Table 1]. There were no other significant potentiation interactions. 4.1.4. Self-grooming There was no strain effect [F(1,44) = 0.28, n.s.; not presented]. There was a significant potentiation main-effect with pups performing fewer self-groomings after reunion with the dam [F(1,44) = 4.60, p < .05, Table 1]. There were no significant potentiation interactions. 4.2. FSL pups’ behavior compared to SD controls

4.1.2. Pivoting There was a significant strain main-effect [F(2,43) = 6.18, p < .01; Fig. 1B]; post hoc one-way ANOVA’s showed that WKY pups made less pivots compared to controls [F(1,44) = 10.39, p < .01]. There was also a significant potentiation main-effect and potentiation × strain interaction [F(2,43) = 33.90, p < .001; F(2,43) = 15.41, p < .001; respectively]; WKY pups showed similar pivoting in

4.2.1. Ultra-Sonic Vocalizations (USV) There was no significant main-effect for strain F(1,45) = 1.12, n.s.; Fig. 2A]. A significant potentiation main-effect was found, indicating that more USV were produced after a short reunion with the dam, compared to USV in the initial separation from dam [F(1,45) = 36.71, p < .001]. There were no significant potentiation interactions.

Fig. 2. (A) USV production of FSL pups (PND 10/11) compared to SD controls (combining results from two sibling pups): Mean ± S.E.M. of frequency in separation (first separation from dam) and in potentiation (second separation after a reunion with dam). (B) Number of proximity-seeking behaviors and self-grooming of FSL compared to SD controls (combining results from two sibling pups): Mean ± S.E.M. of pivoting, supported rearing and self-grooming. Note: ***p < .001.

97

4.2.2. Pivoting There was a significant strain main-effect [F(2,44) = 19.13, p < .001; Fig. 2B]; FSL pups made less pivots and had shorter duration of pivoting [F(1,45) = 30.67, p < .001; F(1,45) = 4.43, p < .05; respectively]. There was a significant potentiation main-effect [F(2,44) = 13.51, p < .001]; pups made fewer pivots and had a shorter duration of pivoting after reunion with the dam, [F(1,45) = 13.96, p < .001; F(1,45) = 27.50, p < .001; respectively, Table 1]. There were no significant potentiation interactions.

4.39 (±0.68), N = 24 4.79 (±1.15), N = 24 4.37 (±1.17), N = 24 4.44 (±1.04), N = 25

4.2.3. Supported rearing There was a significant strain main-effect [F(2,44) = 19.77, p < .001 Fig. 2B]; FSL pups performed less rearings and had a shorter duration of rearing compared to controls [F(1,45) = 34.07, p < .001; F(1,45) = 17.45, p < .001; respectively]. There was also a significant potentiation main-effect [F(2,44) = 24.90, p < .001]; pups made fewer rearings and had a shorter duration of rearing after a reunion with the dam [F(1,45) = 24.31, p < .001; F(1,45) = 41.38, p < .001; respectively, Table 1]. There were no significant potentiation interactions. 4.2.4. Self-grooming There was no significant strain main-effect [F(1,45) = 1.05, n.s; not presented], as well as no significant potentiation main-effect or interactions (Table 1). In summary, WKY pups produced less distress-vocalization and proximity-seeking behaviors, compared to controls. In addition, WKY pups did not show the expected potentiation effect that was evident in control pups (an increase in USV and proximity-seeking behaviors after a short reunion with the dam). FSL pups exhibited less proximity-seeking behaviors compared to controls indicating a similar although less pronounced behavioral profile to that of the WKY pups. No differences between the strains was found in selfgrooming, a self-comforting behavior that was used as a behavioral control measure. Finally, the early life chronic-stress paradigm had no effect on the behaviors of the pups. Note: see also Figs. 1B and 2B for overall frequency data (averaging both separation periods).

6.93 (±1.00), N = 24 5.21 (±1.03), N = 24 8.04 (±1.76), N = 24 4.90 (±1.25), N = 25 54.20 (±8.19), N = 24 70.67 (±9.07), N = 24 40.50 (±7.28), N = 24 80.82 (±8.36), N = 25 33.44 (±4.86), N = 24 55.56 (±5.87), N = 24 52.69 (±6.33), N = 24 57.16 (±4.50), N = 25 WKY Wistar FSL SD Duration (s)

27.14 (±5.15), N = 24 19.88 (±2.95), N = 24 22.31 (±3.87), N = 24 39.88 (±4.41), N = 25

80.97 (±8.21), N = 24 115.44 (±10.94), N = 24 85.61 (±9.80), N = 24 126.94 (±8.37), N = 25

2nd separation (potentiation) 1st separation

3.22 (±0.47), N = 24 1.85 (±0.31), N = 24 2.13 (±0.44), N = 24 1.27 (±0.22), N = 25 2.71 (±0.35), N = 24 3.64 (±0.42), N = 24 2.91 (±0.43), N = 24 6.38 (±0.70), N = 25 5.91 (±0.56), N = 24 7.02 (±0.40), N = 24 5.10 (±0.48), N = 24 9.20 (±0.66), N = 25 4.77 (±0.50), N = 24 8.64 (±0.60), N = 24 7.41 (±0.73), N = 24 10.58 (±0.45), N = 25 WKY Wistar FSL SD Frequency (number)

3.72 (±0.47), N = 24 3.77 (±0.42), N = 24 4.41 (±0.62), N = 24 8.82 (±0.79), N = 25

Self-grooming

2nd separation (potentiation) Supported rearing

1st separation strain

2nd separation (potentiation) Pivoting

1st separation

Behavior

Table 1 Mean ± S.E.M. of frequency and duration (in seconds) of pups’ behaviors in the isolation test (beyond growing conditions), during the 1st and 2nd periods of maternal separation

2.34 (±0.33), N = 24 1.75 (±0.38), N = 24 1.28 (±0.31), N = 24 1.22 (±0.31), N = 25

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

5. Discussion The current study utilized genetic animal models of depression in order to increase our understanding of emotion-regulation in children with a hereditary tendency towards depressive-like behavior. The study utilized a mild chronic-stress paradigm (‘limited-bedding’) to test the possible interaction between depressive vulnerability and exposure to early life stress [65]. The study’s overall findings indicate that emotion-regulation deficits were evident in both animal models when compared to their respective control strains. Compared to controls, WKY pups reacted to separation from their dam with markedly low rates of USV. This abnormal separation-protest behavior was accompanied by low rates of proximity-seeking behaviors (active behaviors that could, in principle, increase proximity to the dam if she was in the testing arena). The pups’ behavior was additionally assessed after a short exposure to the dam, conditions in which the pups tend to increase their USV compared to their vocalization in the first separation [57]. This phenomenon, potentiation of USV, was found to be affected by earlier experiences of the pup [50,60,69]. In the current study, WKY pups did not exhibit the expected potentiation effect. Similarly, WKY pups did not alter their pivoting (an additional proximity-seeking behavior) after an exposure to the dam. Pups belonging to the second animal model (FSL) exhibited certain similarities to the WKY strain, with a less pronounced overall behavioral profile. These pups exhibited less proximity-seeking behaviors compared to controls.

98

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

At the same time, FSL pups did not differ from controls in their separation-protest and exhibited potentiation of USV. Earlier studies [38,39] indicated physiological and behavioral abnormalities of FSL pups (PND 1) and reduced approach-behavior toward the dam in both FSL and WKY strains (PND 17–19). The current study elaborates upon these earlier findings and points toward behavioral alterations in WKY and FSL pups, alterations that are already evident at PND 10–11. WKY and FSL pups exhibit a tendency toward excessive withdrawal behaviors from the dam, with WKY pups exhibiting a more pronounced profile. This observed profile is reminiscent of avoidant attachment common in children to depressed parents [70], with subsequent increased developmental risk [71,72]. Thus, these behaviors can be speculatively conceptualized as components of an abnormally developing attachment system, a motivational system that develops at an early age [73]. Future studies may address this possibility. The use of USV measurements for this aim presents a valid paradigm for the assessment of attachment bonds. In this regard, the markedly low rates of USV evident in WKY pups warrant special attention in order to elucidate their underpinnings. One pathway through which the pup can ultimately affect its own development is by alterations in its interactions with the dam. Reductions in proximity-seeking behaviors are likely to lead to reduced treatment and contact by the dam [for example, USV stimulates maternal responses such as pup retrieval, maternal licking and inhibition of biting; 54,55,74]. A vicious cycle can thus be created, in which altered dam-pup interactions impair the physiology and behavior of the pup later in life [75–77]. Beside the contribution of the pups’ genotype, its behavioral phenotype may also be influenced by the prenatal environment and pattern of maternal care received from their similar-genotype dam and from subject-siblings-dam interactions. In fact, our research team found differences in maternal behavior patterns between WKY/FSL and control strains [78–80]. Thus, the role of these environmental sources needs to be elucidated by future studies. Since the WKY and FSL strains are considered genetic animal models of depression, this allows the investigation of environmental factors hypothesized to affect the establishment of emotion-regulation [22,81]. The current study addressed this possibility by exposing the pups to different early life stress levels. The overall findings indicate that the behavior of the pups was not affected by the stress paradigm. Most of the earlier studies focused on the physiological effects of the mild chronic-stress paradigm used in the study (’limited bedding’) and did not assess its effect on behavior [see review; 82]. An exception in this regard is a study by Brunson et al. [83] who found that this stress paradigm causes delayed, progressive impairments of synaptic and behavioral measures of hippocampal function (with deficits evident in middle-age, but not young adult rats). This presents an interesting possibility that is in line with findings in our laboratory [indicating that this chronic-stress paradigm has limited behavioral effects in youngage rats; 41]. Overall, it is possible that either the stress paradigm is too mild to have a clear behavioral impact, or that deficits will be evident only in later stages of rats’ life. Two possible lines of future research follow: (1) manipulation of variables in the stress paradigm (e.g., the amount of towel available for the dam). (2) Conducting behavioral tests at later stages of the rats’ life-cycle. In addition, two factors limit our ability to strongly conclude from our measurements of USV that we exclusively assessed separation-protest. First, the USV separation manipulation was performed in room temperature. Although ambient temperature in the test arena was monitored and was similar in all experimental groups, it is still very likely that a portion of the USV were elicited by the change in temperature (from the incubator to the testing arena). We note that this situation has ecological validity –

when separated from the nest and dam, a pup would naturally be concomitantly exposed to cooler, external ambient temperatures. Second, the analysis of ultrasonic vocalization was performed solely on PND 10/11. Though the differences found between the experimental groups are clear, our analysis does not allow us to ascertain whether the differences are due only to deviations from the normal range of responding or also to changes in the time of “emergence” of normal response patterns. Since USV emission follows a specific ontogenetic profile, analysis at several time-points would have been more informative. The lack of effect of the mild chronic-stress condition and the fact that isolation-induced patterns were already altered at an early age, point toward a strong hereditary influence on the strains’ behavioral phenotype. Such a conclusion fits findings of strong hereditary transmission of emotional-regulation patterns to children of depressed parents [84,85]. More studies are clearly needed to untangle emotion-regulation transmission pathways. For this aim, future research can also use additional methods such as crossfostering. It should be noted, though, that cross-fostering has an inherent limitation, as some studies demonstrate that fostering itself has an impact on maternal behavior. The use of dams belonging to genetic animal models of depression presents a additional complication; the fostering itself can be viewed as stress-arousing and it may alter the dams’ behavior [in accordance with diathesisstress theories; 23,63,86]. FSL and WKY dams may therefore react differentially to the adoption compared to controls, adding a major confound to such a study. An additional issue to note regards the possible testing of HPA axis activity; measurement of corticosterone levels at this age could potentially have strengthened the pattern of results. However, the neuroendocrine stress response of the immature rat, specifically on days 4–14 of life, is characterized by attenuated hormonal responses and altered gene regulation in response to stress as compared to the adult situation [87–90]. Thus, at the age of the current study, it was not preferable to assess HPA hormones, in our opinion. Instead, we have assessed ACTH and corticosterone following the current manipulations, in older, preweanling rats beyond the hyporesponsive age range, and published the pattern of results [39]. Gotlib and Goodman [91] describe a shift from examination of the deficits of children to depressed parents to the identification of factors that are associated with this risk. However, despite this shift in focus we do not have an adequate understanding of the mechanisms that underlie the risk for these adverse outcomes. Animal models offer the opportunity increase our knowledge on the intergenerational transmission of emotion-regulation disturbances in children of depressed parents. Future studies in the current models may further unravel the underlying physiological mechanisms. Acknowledgements The research reported in this paper was completed as part of the first and second author’s Ph.D. dissertations (YB and OM), Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel. YB and OM were supported by President’s fellowships, Bar-Ilan University. This work was supported by a grant from the Israel Science Foundation to AW. References [1] Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Neural correlates of emotion regulation in psychopathology. Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11:413–8. [2] Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition, in the development of emotion regulation. In: Fox NA, editor. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994. p. 25–52. [3] Ressler KJ, Mayberg HS. Targeting abnormal neural circuits in mood and anxiety disorders: from the laboratory to the clinic. Nat Neurosci 2007;10:1116–24. [4] Campos JJ, Frankel CB, Camras L. On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev 2004;75:377–94.

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100 [5] John OP, Gross JJ. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. J Pers 2004;72:1301–33. [6] Raver CC. Placing emotional self-regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Dev 2004;75:346–53. [7] Southam-Gerow MA, Kendall PC. Emotion regulation and understanding: implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clin Psychol Rev 2002;22:189–222. [8] Weller A, Feldman R. Emotion regulation and touch in infants: the role of cholecystokinin and opioids. Peptides 2003;24:779–88. [9] Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, Stegall S. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2006;27:155–68. [10] Gianino A, Tronick EZ. In: Field T, McCabe P, Schneiderman N, editors. The mutual regulation model: the infant’s self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive capacities in Stress and coping across development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. p. 47–68. [11] Hofer MA. Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994;59:192–207. [12] Field T. The effects of mother’s physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994:208–27. [13] Schore AN. The effects of secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J 2000;22:6–66. [14] Maughan A, Cicchetti D, Toth SL, Rogosch FA. Early-occurring maternal depression and maternal negativity in predicting young children’s emotion regulation and socioemotional difficulties. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2007;35:685– 703. [15] Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Risk for psychopathology in children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev 1999;106:458–90. [16] Gelfand DM, Teti DM. The effects of maternal depression on children. Clin Psychol Rev 1990;10:329–53. [17] APA. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [18] Goodman SH. Depression in mothers. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2007;3:107–35. [19] Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Dev Psychopathol 1992;4:49–66. [20] Downey G, Coyne JC. Children of depressed parents: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 1990;108:50–76. [21] Field T. Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav Dev 1995;18:1–13. [22] Jesberger JA, Richardson JS. Animal models of depression: parallels and correlates to severe depression in humans. Biol Psychiatry 1985;20:764–84. [23] Nestler EJ, Barrot M, DiLeone RJ, Eisch AJ, Gold SJ, Monteggia LM. Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 2002;34:13–25. [24] Overstreet DH, Friedman E, Mathe AA, Yadid G. The Flinders Sensitive Line rat: a selectively bred putative animal model of depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:739–59. [25] Pare WP. Stress ulcer susceptibility and depression in Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. Physiol Behav 1989;46:993–8. [26] Pare WP. Investigatory behavior of a novel conspecific by Wister Kyoto, Wister and Sprague–Dawley rats. Brain Res Bull 2000;53:759–65. [27] Daws LC, Overstreet DH. Ontogeny of muscarinic cholinergic supersensitivity in the flinders sensitive line rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1999;62:367–80. [28] Janowsky DS, Risch C, Parker D, Huey L, Judd L. Increased vulnerability to cholinergic stimulation in affective disorder patients. Psychopharmacol Bull 1980;16:29–31. [29] Lavi-Avnon Y, Weller A, Finberg JP, Gispan-Herman I, Kinor N, Stern Y, et al. The reward system and maternal behavior in an animal model of depression: a microdialysis study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;196:281–91. [30] Yadid G, Nakash R, Deri I, Grin T, Kinor N, Gispan I, et al. Elucidation of the neurobiology of depression: Insights from a novel genetic animal model. Prog Neurobiol 2000;62:353–78. [31] Malkesman O, Braw Y, Ram E, Maayan R, Weizman A, Kinor N, et al. Dehydroepiandrosterone and monoamines in the limbic system of a genetic animal model of childhood depression. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;18:255–61. [32] Malkesman O, Braw Y, Weller A. Assessment of antidepressant and anxiolytic properties of NK1 antagonists and Substance P in Wistar Kyoto rats. Physiol Behav 2007;90:619–25. [33] Lopez-Rubalcava C, Lucki I. Strain differences in the behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs in the rat forced swimming test. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;22:191–9. [34] Lahmame A, del Arco C, Pazos A, Yritia M, Armario A. Are Wistar–Kyoto rats a genetic animal model of depression resistant to antidepressants? Eur J Pharmacol 1997;337:115–23. [35] Malkesman O, Braw Y, Maayan R, Weizman A, Overstreet DH, Shabat-Simon M, et al. Two different putative genetic animal models of childhood depression. Biol Psychiatry 2006;59:17–23. [36] Jiao X, Pare WP, Tejani-Butt S. Strain differences in the distribution of dopamine transporter sites in rat brain. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biological Psychiatry 2003;27:913–9. [37] Solberg LC, Olson SL, Turek FW, Redei E. Altered hormone levels and circadian rhythm of activity in the WKY rat, a putative animal model of depression. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2001;281:R786–94. [38] Shayit M, Yadid G, Overstreet DH, Weller A. 5-HT(1A) receptor subsensitivity in infancy and supersensitivity in adulthood in an animal model of depression. Brain Res 2003;980:100–8.

99

[39] Braw Y, Malkesman O, Merlender A, Bercovich A, Dagan M, Overstreet DH, et al. Stress hormones and emotion-regulation in two genetic animal models of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006;31:1105–16. [40] Malkesman O, Braw Y, Zagoory-Sharon O, Golan O, Lavi-Avnon Y, Schroeder M, et al. Reward and anxiety in genetic animal models of childhood depression. Behav Brain Res 2005;164:1–10. [41] Braw Y, Malkesman O, Bercovich A, Dagan M, Lavi-Avnon Y, Schroeder M, et al. Anxiety-like behaviors in pre-pubertal rats of the Flinder’s Sensitive Line (FSL) and Wistar–Kyoto (WKY) animal models of depression. Behav Brain Res 2006;167:261–9. [42] Zalsman G, Brent DA, Weersing VR. Depressive disorders in childhood and adolescence: an overview: epidemiology, clinical manifestation and risk factors. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2006;15:827–41, vii. [43] Hammen C, Compas B. Unmasking unmasked depression: The problem of comorbidty in childh and adolescent depression. Clin Psychol Rev 1994;14:585–603. [44] Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, et al. Childhood and adolescent depression: a review of the past 10 years. Part I J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35:1427–39. [45] Axelson DA, Birmaher B. Relation between anxiety and depressive disorders in childhood and adolescence. Depress Anxiety 2001;14:67–78. [46] Barbee JG. Mixed symptoms and syndromes of anxiety and depression: diagnostic, prognostic, and etiologic issues. Ann Clin Psychiatry 1998;10:15–29. [47] Lydiard RB. Coexisting depression and anxiety: special diagnostic and treatment issues. J Clin Psychiatry 1991;52(Suppl.):48–54. [48] Roy-Byrne PP, Stang P, Wittchen HU, Ustun B, Walters EE, Kessler RC. Lifetime panic-depression comorbidity in the National Comorbidity Survey. Association with symptoms, impairment, course and help-seeking. Br J Psychiatry 2000;176:229–35. [49] Bernstein GA. Comorbidity and severity of anxiety and depressive disorders in a clinic sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1991;30:43–50. [50] Hofer MA, Brunelli SA, Masmela J, Shair HN. Maternal interactions prior to separation potentiate isolation-induced calling in rat pups. Behav Neurosci 1996;110:1158–67. [51] Brunelli SA, Hofer MA. In: Blass E, editor. Selective breeding for an infantile phenotype (isolation calling): a window on developmental processes in Dev Psychobiol. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2001. p. 433–82. [52] Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. p. 206–22. [53] Hofer MA. Multiple regulators of ultrasonic vocalization in the infant rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996;21:203–17. [54] Noirot E. Ultrasounds and maternal behavior in small rodents. Dev Psychobiol 1972;5:371–87. [55] Brouette-Lahlou I, Vernet-Maury E, Vigouroux M. Role of pups’ ultrasonic calls in a particular maternal behavior in Wister rat: pups’ anogenital licking. Behav. Brain Res 1992;50:147–54. [56] Zippelius H-M, Schleidt WM. Ultraschall-Laute bei jungen Mausen (ultrasonic vocalization in infant mice). Naturwissenschaften 1956; 43:502. [57] Hofer MA, Masmela JR, Brunelli SA, Shair HN. The ontogeny of maternal potentiation of the infant rats’ isolation call. Dev Psychobiol 1998;33:189–201. [58] Weiss RS. In: Parkes CM, Stevenson-Hinde J, editors. The attachment bond in childhood and adulthood in attachment across the life cycle. London/New-York: Tavistock/Routledge; 1991. [59] Brunelli SA, Masmela JR, Shair HN, Hofer MA. Effects of biparental rearing on ultrasonic vocalization (USV) responses in rat pups. J Comp Psychol 1998;112:331–43. [60] Amsel A, Radek CC, Graham M, Letz R. Ultrasound emission in infant rats as an indicant of arousal during appetitive learning and extinction. Science 1977;197:786–8. [61] Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2006;256:174–86. [62] Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: preclinical and clinical studies. Biol Psychiatry 2001;49:1023–39. [63] Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 1991;110:406–25. [64] Gilles EE, Schultz L, Baram TZ. Abnormal corticosterone regulation in an immature rat model of continuous chronic stress. Pediatr Neurol 1996;15:114–9. [65] Avishai-Eliner S, Gilles EE, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Bar-El Y, Baram TZ. Altered regulation of gene and protein expression of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis components in an immature rat model of chronic stress. J Neuroendocrinol 2001;13:799–807. [66] Abbey H, Howard E. Statistical procedure in developmental studies on species with multiple offspring. Dev Psychobiol 1973;6:329–35. [67] Blass EM, Fillion TJ, Weller A, Brunson L. Separation of opioid from nonopioid mediation of affect in neonatal rats: nonopioid mechanisms mediate maternal contact influences. Behav Neurosci 1990;104:625–36. [68] Hofer MA, Shair H. Ultrasonic vocalization during social interaction and isolation in 2-week-old rats. Dev Psychobiol 1978;11:495–504. [69] Hofer MA, Masmela JR, Brunelli SA, Shair HN. Behavioral mechanisms for active maternal potentiation of isolation calling in rat pups. Behav Neurosci 1999;113:51–61.

100

Y. Braw et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 193 (2008) 94–100

[70] Martins C, Gaffan EA. Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infantmother attachment: a meta-analytic investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41:737–46. [71] Atkinson L, Zucker K. Attachment and psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 1997. [72] Goldberg S. Attachment and development. London: Arnold; 2000. [73] Polan MJ, Hofer MA. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Psychobiological origins of infant attachment and separation responses, in Handbook of attachment: theory research and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [74] Smotherman WP, Bell RW, Starzec J, Elias J, Zachman TA. Maternal responses to infant vocalizations and olfactory cues in rats and mice. Behav Biol 1974;12:55–66. [75] Kehoe P, Shomaker WJ, Triano L, Hoffman J, Arons C. Repeated isolation in the neonatal rat produces alterations in behavior and ventral striatal dopamine release in the juvenile after amphetamine challenge. Behav Neurosci 1996;110:1435–44. [76] Matthews K, Hall FS, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. Retarded acquisition and reduced expression of conditioned locomotor activity in adult rats following repeated early maternal separation: Effects of prefeeding, d-amphetamine, dopamine antagonists and clonidine. Psychopharmacol Bull 1996;126:75–84. [77] Denenberg VH. Commentary: is maternal stimulation the mediator of the handling effect in infancy? Dev Psychobiol 1999;34:1–3. [78] Lavi-Avnon Y, Shayit M, Yadid G, Overstreet HD, Weller A. Immobility in the swim test and observations of maternal behavior in lactating Flinders Sensitive Line rats. Behav Brain Res 2005;161:155–63. [79] Lavi-Avnon Y, Yadid G, Overstreet DH, Weller A. Abnormal patterns of maternal behavior in a genetic animal model of depression. Physiol Behav 2005;84:607–15. [80] Braw Y, Malkesman O, Dagan M, Bercovich A, Lavi-Avnon Y, Weller A. Maternal behavior in dams belonging to the Wistar–Kyoto (WKY) and Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) animal models of depression; unpublished data.

[81] Willner P. Animal models as simulations of depression, Trends Pharmacol Sci 1991;12:131-6. [82] Fenoglio KA, Brunson KL, Baram TZ. Hippocampal neuroplasticity induced by early-life stress: functional and molecular aspects. Front Neuroendocrinol 2006;27:180–92. [83] Brunson KL, Kramar E, Lin B, Chen Y, Colgin LL, Yanagihara TK, et al. Mechanisms of late-onset cognitive decline after early-life stress. J Neurosci 2005;25:9328–38. [84] Silberg J, Rutter M. Nature-nurture interplay in the risks associated with parental depression in Children of depressed parents. In: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. Washington, DC: APA; 2002. [85] Harris JR. The nurture assumption: why children turn out the way they do. New-York: Free press; 1998. [86] Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Nemeroff CB. Parental depression: animal models of an adverse life event. Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:1265–83. [87] Levine S. The pituitary-adrenal system and the developing brain. Prog. Brain Res 1970;32:79–85. [88] Levine S. The ontogeny of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. The influence of maternal factors. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1994;746:275–88 [discussion 289–93]. [89] Baram TZ, Yi S, Avishai-Eliner S, Schultz L. Development neurobiology of the stress response: multilevel regulation of corticotropin-releasing hormone function. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1997;814:252–65. [90] Vazquez DM. Stress and the developing limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998;23:663–700. [91] Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Transmission of risk to children of depressed parents: integration and conclusions. In: Goodman SH, Gotlib IH, editors. Children of depressed parents: mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. Washington, DC: APA; 2002. p. 307–26.