Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction

Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction

Journal Pre-proof Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction Jillian E. Hardee, Camille Phaneuf, Lora Cope, Robe...

596KB Sizes 0 Downloads 53 Views

Journal Pre-proof Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction Jillian E. Hardee, Camille Phaneuf, Lora Cope, Robert Zucker, Ashley Gearhardt, Mary Heitzeg PII:

S0195-6663(19)31333-9

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.104578

Reference:

APPET 104578

To appear in:

Appetite

Received Date: 16 October 2019 Revised Date:

2 December 2019

Accepted Date: 30 December 2019

Please cite this article as: Hardee J.E., Phaneuf C., Cope L., Zucker R., Gearhardt A. & Heitzeg M., Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction, Appetite (2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.104578. This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. © 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

Title: Neural correlates of inhibitory control in youth with symptoms of food addiction Jillian E. Hardeea*, Camille Phaneufb, Lora Copea, Robert Zuckera, Ashley Gearhardtb, Mary Heitzega a

University of Michigan Department of Psychiatry and Addiction Center 4250 Plymouth Rd Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA b

University of Michigan Department of Psychology 530 Church St Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA

*Corresponding Author: Jillian E. Hardee, PhD University of Michigan Department of Psychiatry and Addiction Center 4250 Plymouth Rd Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA [email protected]

1

Abstract Prior research has found that food addiction is associated with reward-related neural differences, but research has yet to examine whether there are also neural differences in inhibitory control. This may be particularly relevant during adolescence as it is a key developmental period where difficulties in inhibitory control are more prevalent. The Yale Food Addiction Scale is a selfreport questionnaire that applies substance use disorder diagnostic criteria to certain foods that has also been adapted for children. Here we investigate the association between addictive-like eating and brain functioning during inhibitory control in youth. Seventy-six right-handed participants 8.2–17.8 years (44 male) were recruited. Participants performed a go/no-go task during functional magnetic resonance imaging and completed the Yale Food Addiction Scale for Children, after which they were categorized into two groups according to their scores (No Symptom Group = 0; YFAS-C Group: score ≥ 1). Inhibitory control was probed with a contrast of correct no-go versus go trials. An independent-samples t-test comparing groups revealed a significant difference in three primary clusters, all exclusively in the left hemisphere (No Symptoms Group > YFAS-C Group): middle temporal gyrus/occipital gyrus, precuneus/calcarine sulcus, and inferior frontal gyrus. Specifically, the YFAS-C Group showed deactivation in all three clusters. Adolescents who endorse food addiction appear to show hypoactivation in response to the inhibitory control portion of a go/no-go task, which suggests possible inhibitory control difficulties. Keywords: fMRI, inhibitory control, food addiction, neuroimaging, adolescent

2

1. Introduction The term food addiction refers to a process involving physical and psychological dependence on certain foods, particularly those high in fat and refined carbohydrates (Davis & Cater, 2009; Davis et al., 2011; Gearhardt, Corbin, & Brownell, 2009; Gearhardt, Davis, Kuschner, & Brownell, 2011a; Schulte, Grilo, & Gearhardt, 2016). Similar to drug and alcohol addiction, addiction to these highly palatable foods can involve tolerance, withdrawal, and loss of control (Gearhardt et al., 2009). Food addiction has been shown to be strongly associated with obesity, body mass index (BMI)1, and body fat percentage (Davis & Carter, 2009; Gearhardt et al., 2012; Pedram et al., 2013). Thus, food addiction may be a contributing factor in problem eating behaviors, including binge eating disorders and obesity (Schulte et al., 2013, Flint et al., 2014). Just as inhibitory control problems can contribute to problematic substance use and substance use disorders (for review, see Mitchell & Potenza, [2014]), poor inhibitory control is thought to be one contributing factor to the “loss of control” aspect of food addiction, leading to excessive food intake (Chen et al., 2018). Indeed, there is growing evidence suggesting that decreased inhibitory control is associated with increases in unhealthy eating (Jasinka et al., 2012) and overeating (Guerrieri et al., 2007), including overeating in response to negative emotional states (Bekker, van de Meerekdonk, & Mollerus, 2004; Racine et al., 2009). Previous studies have also shown that individuals who are more impulsive and have poor inhibitory control are more likely to be overweight or obese than those who have greater inhibitory control (Guerrieri, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2008) or are of a healthy weight (Nederkoorn, Braet, Van Eijs, Tanghe, & Jansen, 2006). Furthermore, individuals with weaker versus those with stronger inhibitory control have been shown to have a higher BMI (Batterink, Yokum, & Stice, 2010; Cohen, Yates, Duong, & Convit, 2011). There is also neural evidence suggesting that poor inhibitory control may contribute to risk for being overweight or obese. Several neuroimaging studies report decreased activation in prefrontal inhibitory control regions, such as the ventrolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices, for obese compared to lean participants in response to high-calorie food images

1

Abbreviations: AUD, alcohol use disorder; BMI, body mass index; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; NFAS, no food addiction symptoms; YFAS-C, Yale Food Addiction Scale for Children.

3

(Silvers et al., 2014) or when trying to inhibit responses to high-calorie foods (Batterink et al., 2010). Additionally, decreased recruitment of the inferior, middle, and superior frontal gyri during an executive control task was found to predict a greater rate of weight gain in subsequent years (Kishinevsky et al., 2012). Thus, hypo-activation of the prefrontal cortex in obese individuals has been proposed as an explanation for poor control over food intake (AlonsoAlonso & Pascual-Leone, 2007). Reduced activation has been found in inhibitory control and performance monitoring regions in women with food addiction (Gearhardt et al., 2011b; Franken, Nijs, Toes, & van der Veen, 2016), where higher food addiction scores were associated with less activation in lateral orbitofrontal cortex when receiving highly palatable food (i.e. chocolate milkshake) (Gearhardt et al., 2011b). The authors measured food addiction with the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS), a self-report questionnaire that applies the diagnostic criteria of substance use disorders to food (Gearhardt et al., 2009). The patterns of neural responses reviewed above have been implicated in other consumptive addictions, such as alcohol or tobacco use (Sutherland & Stein, 2018; Zahr, Pfefferbaum, & Sullivan, 2017), further extending the similarities between substance use and addictive-like eating. However, as two of these studies did not use fMRI tasks that directly measure inhibitory control (Gearhardt et al., 2011b; Silvers et al., 2014), it is possible that other neural processes are at work; thus, further research is needed to directly link food addiction with inhibitory control problems. The YFAS is the most commonly used measure to assess food addiction (Meule & Gearhardt, 2014). It was initially developed for the assessment of food addiction in adults and has since been adapted for children (YFAS-C; Gearhardt, Roberto, Seamans, Corbin, & Brownell, 2013). The downward extension of the scale was important because addictive eating in children has been shown to be related to elevated BMI and reduced satiety. Although research on how addictive processes contribute to problematic food consumption in adults is growing, research in children and adolescents is still limited. Childhood-onset obesity typically persists into adulthood (Epstein, Wing, & Valoski, 1985), similar to the way that substance use disorders that emerge during early adolescence often last throughout an individual’s life (Chen, Storr, & Anthony, 2009). This risk is thought to stem from variations in an already vulnerable neural system (i.e. reward dysfunction, increased impulsivity) (Tapert, Caldwell, & Burke, 2004), in addition to an increased likelihood of using substances to cope (Clark, Thatcher, & Tapert, 2008). Highly palatable foods, if they have an addictive potential, may have more of an impact 4

on children as opposed to adults due to neural and psychological vulnerabilities. To date, however, no studies have investigated the link between food addiction symptoms and patterns of neural activation in children and adolescents. Here we investigated inhibitory control neural circuitry in adolescents with symptoms of food addiction versus those who exhibit no symptoms. Previous fMRI and electrophysiology studies examining addictive-like eating have used reward and/or a salience-based tasks (Imperatori et al., 2015; Kishinevsky et al., 2012; Gearhardt et al., 2011b; Schulte, Yokum, Jahn, & Gearhardt, 2019), thus the investigation of inhibitory control is innovative. Inhibitory control was assessed using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and the go/no-go paradigm, and food addiction symptomology was measured with the YFAS-C. Due to the novelty of this study, we used a whole-brain analysis approach in order to avoid making biased assumptions about the data. We predicted that adolescents scoring higher on the YFAS-C would show less activation in key prefrontal inhibitory control regions in the brain compared to adolescents who show no food addiction symptomology.

2. Methods 2.1 Participants Seventy-six right-handed individuals (44 males) aged 8.2–17.8 years participated in this study. Participants were recruited from the Michigan Longitudinal Study, a prospective, multiwave study of families with high levels of parental alcohol use disorder (AUD) and a contrast sample of nonalcoholic families from mid-Michigan that began in 1987 (Zucker, 1996). Since 2005, neuroimaging data has been collected every 1–2 years on a subset of these participants, starting between the ages of 7–12 years. The YFAS-C (Gearhardt et al., 2013), described in Section 2.2, was incorporated into the neuroimaging protocol in 2014 and is collected after each fMRI scan. All participants with useable fMRI scans and complete YFAS-C data were eligible for the current study. Participants were split into two groups based on their YFAS-C scores (see Section 2.2). The study was conducted according to the guidelines provided by the Declaration of Helsinki and the ethical requirements of the University of Michigan (HUM00084126). Parental AUD diagnosis for the larger Michigan Longitudinal Study was based on DSMIV criteria, and assessed by way of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule–Version 4 (Robins, Cottler, Bucholz, Compton, North, & Rourke, 2000; Robins, Helzer, Croughan, & Ratcliff, 5

1981), supplemented with the Drinking and Drug History (Zucker, Ellis, Fitzgerald, Bingham, & Sanford, 1996; Zucker et al., 2000). For details on parental AUD diagnosis, see Supplementary Material. Participants in the current neuroimaging study came from families with and without a family history of AUD (there were no significant differences between groups for family history; see Table 1). Exclusionary criteria for this study included: neurological, acute, uncorrected, or chronic medical illness; current or recent (within 6 months) treatment with centrally active medications; and history of psychosis or schizophrenia in first-degree relatives. The presence of Axis I psychiatric or developmental disorders that would interfere with the interpretation of the data was also exclusionary; this did not include past history of mood disorder or current unmedicated mood disorder, or current or past history of conduct or attention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Diagnosis was determined using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule– Child (Costello, Edelbrock, Kalas, Kessler, & Klaric, 1982). Families in which the target child displayed evidence of fetal alcohol effects were excluded from the original ascertainment. As part of the Michigan Longitudinal Study, all offspring were assessed annually on substance use and related problems (Zucker et al., 1996). For details on annual substance use assessments, please see Supplementary Material. All participants were told to abstain from alcohol and illicit substances for 48 hours prior to the fMRI scan. For participants age 15 years and older, urine drug screens were conducted immediately prior to the fMRI scan; positive results were exclusionary. In participants age 14 years and younger, we relied on verbal confirmation of drug and alcohol abstinence on the day of the scan. No participants had to be excluded from the current study due to a positive drug screen or affirmative self-report of alcohol or drug use. All participants gave written consent/assent after explanation of the experimental protocol and at least one parent gave written informed consent, as approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board. Height (in) and weight (lbs) for each participant were collected at the time of the scan. Table 1 contains participant information. (Table 1 approximately here) 2.2 Yale Food Addiction Scale for Children Participants were categorized into one of two groups based on their symptom count scores on the YFAS-C; the No Food Addiction Symptom Group (NFAS Group) contained participants who scored 0 on the YFAS-C (n = 41), and the YFAS-C Group contained

6

participants who endorsed ≥1 food addiction symptoms on the YFAS-C (n = 35). Groups were matched on age at fMRI scan. See Table 1. The Yale Food Addiction Scale is the most commonly used measure to assess the construct of food addiction (Meule & Gearhardt, 2014). The full YFAS-C is a 25-item self-report questionnaire that applies the diagnostic criteria for substance use disorders to consumption of certain foods, namely highly palatable foods such as ice cream, pizza, and chocolate. Because of participant burden concerns, we used a modified YFAS-C with nine questions; one question representing each symptom relating to the seven diagnostic criteria for substance dependence, as well as two questions for clinically significant impairment/distress, was used. This same approach has been used to abbreviate the adult version of the YFAS; tests for internal consistency reveal that the YFAS remains psychometrically sound in this form (Flint et al., 2014; Schulte & Gearhardt, 2017). The question representative of each symptom was based on the highest factor loading from the original validation (Gearhardt et al., 2013). Each question is scored 0 (symptom absent) or 1 (symptom present). For example, answering “always” to Question 1 (“I eat foods even when I am not hungry”) gives a score of 1, but answering “never”, “rarely”, “sometimes”, and “very often” leads to a score of 0. Scores for all nine questions are totaled based on these answers. Table 2 outlines each question and scoring information. Table S2 details how each participant in the YFAS-C Group answered each of the nine questions. (Table 2 approximately here) 2.3 fMRI task A go/no-go task (Durston, Thomas, Yang, Ulug, Zimmerman, & Casey, 2002) was used to probe response inhibition. Participants were instructed to respond to target stimuli (letters other than X) by pressing a button (go trials) but make no response to infrequent non-target stimuli (letter X; no-go trials). Letters were white and presented on a black screen. Stimulus duration was 500 ms, followed by 3500 ms of fixation. Each no-go trial could be preceded by 1, 3, or 5 go trials; order of the trials was pseudorandomized. There were 5 runs of 49 trials, each run lasting 3 minutes and 2 sec and containing 11, 12, or 13 no-go trials for a total of 60 no-go trials out of 245 trials. Reaction times for correct go responses (Hit RT), accuracy for correct go trials (Hit), accuracy for false alarms (False Alarms), and reaction times for false alarms (False Alarm RT) were recorded. Before the fMRI scan, all participants had a practice session of 49 trials on a desktop computer. 7

2.4 MRI data acquisition Whole-brain blood oxygenated level-dependent images were acquired on a 3.0 Tesla GE Signa scanner (Milwaukee, WI) using a T2*-weighted single-shot combined spiral in-out sequence (Glover & Law, 2001) with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90º; FOV = 200 mm; 64 x 64 matrix; in-plane resolution = 3.12 x 3.12 mm; slice thickness = 4 mm; 29 slices. A high-resolution anatomical T1 scan was obtained for spatial normalization (three-dimensional spoiled gradient-recalled echo; TR = 25 ms; min TE; FOV= 25 cm; 256 x 256 matrix; slice thickness = 1.4 mm). Participant head motion was minimized using foam pads placed around the head along with a forehead strap. In addition, the importance of keeping still was emphasized. 2.5 Data Analysis Hypotheses were specified before data was collected. Additionally, the analytic plan was pre-specified before any analyses were performed. 2.5.1 Demographics, performance, substance use variables: Independent-samples t-tests were used to look for group differences in age, IQ (as assessed by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, or WISC-III), body mass index (BMI; measured at the time of the fMRI scan via height and weight measurements), and each of the performance variables: Hit, Hit RT, False Alarms, and False Alarm RT. Fisher’s Exact Test was used to look for group differences in sex and family history of AUD. The number of participants for each group who reported substance use prior to their fMRI scan (having used alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, other drugs, or any combination of these) is reported in Table 1, and Fisher’s Exact Test was used to look for group differences between these. For diagnoses, Fisher’s Exact Test was used for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as no other diagnoses (conduct disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, or depressive disorder) were present in this sample. 2.5.2 fMRI data: Functional images were reconstructed using an iterative algorithm (Noll, Fessler, & Sutton, 2005; Sutton, Noll, & Fessler, 2003). Subject head motion was corrected using FSL 5.0.2.2. (Analysis Group, FMRIB, Oxford, United Kingdom) (Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady, & Smith, 2002). Analysis of estimation of motion parameters confirmed that overall head motion within each run did not exceed 3 mm translation or 3º rotation in any direction. All remaining image processing (including slice timing correction) and statistical analysis were completed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM8; Wellcome Institute of Cognitive Neurology, 8

London, United Kingdom). Functional images were spatially normalized to a standard stereotaxic space as defined by the Montreal Neurological Institute. A 6 mm full-width halfmaximum Gaussian spatial smoothing kernel was applied to improve signal-to-noise ratio and to account for differences in anatomy. Individual-level analysis was completed using a general linear model. Three regressors of interest (correct no-go, failed no-go, and correct go) were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function, with event durations of 4 seconds from stimulus presentation. Motion parameters were modeled as nuisance regressors to remove residual motion artifacts. The main contrast of interest was correct no-go (correct reject) > go trials (CR > GO). This was calculated for group-level analysis by linearly combining parameter estimates over all five runs of the task. The pre-specified significance level was set at a primary threshold of p < 0.001, cluster-level of p < .05 (family-wise error rate [FWE]-corrected for multiple comparisons). To confirm that the GNG task elicited the expected activation, a one-sample t-test was run using the entire sample (n = 76) for the contrast of interest (CR > GO). The main hypothesis of interest (group differences for the CR > GO contrast) was tested on a whole-brain basis using an independent-samples t-test. Values from significant clusters were extracted using MarsBaR Region of Interest toolbox (Brett, Anton, Valabregue, & Poline, 2002). Extracted values were then imported into SPSS (Version 24, IBM Corp, Armonk, NY) for graphical purposes and for post-hoc analyses. Associations among scan age, BMI, YFAS-C group, and mean activation in significant clusters were investigated using independent-samples ttests or Pearson’s correlations.

3. Results 3.1 Participant Characteristics There were no significant differences (i.e., all ps > .05) between groups on sex, age at fMRI scan, IQ, family history of AUD, BMI, or ADHD diagnosis. There were also no significant differences between the groups for substance use initiation prior to fMRI scan. There was a significant difference between groups for YFAS-C scores. Age was significantly positively correlated with BMI across the whole group, r = .34, p = .002. See Table 1 for statistics. 3.2 Task Performance

9

There were no significant differences between groups on task performance measures— Hits, Hit RT, False Alarms, and False Alarm RT. See Table 3 for statistics. (Table 3 approximately here) 3.3 fMRI Data 3.3.1 Task effect: Activation for the task effect passed correction for multiple comparisons in six regions: right middle frontal gyrus, right inferior temporal gyrus, right posterior cingulate gyrus, left angular gyrus, left middle frontal gyrus, and left inferior frontal gyrus. See Table S2 (Supplementary Material) for task effect coordinates and statistics. These regions are agreement with previous studies that have investigated the neural correlates of response inhibition (see Zhang, Geng, & Lee, 2017 for meta-analysis) and confirm that our task elicited the expected activation. 3.3.2 Group differences: An independent-samples t-test revealed a significant group difference for CR > GO at primary threshold of p < .001, cluster-level FWE-corrected p < .05, when comparing the NFAS to the YFAS-C Group, in three clusters: 1) left middle temporal gyrus, extending into the middle occipital gyrus; 2) left precuneus/left calcarine sulcus, extending into the posterior cingulate, and 3) left inferior frontal triangularis/opercularis region (Figure 1). See Table 4 for coordinates and statistics. In all three clusters, the YFAS-C Group showed less activation during successful inhibitory control compared to the NFAS Group (Figure 2). Posthoc correlations were run to demonstrate that results were not due to associations with age or BMI; activation in the three clusters was not significantly correlated with age (all ps > .437) or BMI (all ps > .329) across all subjects. The YFAS Group did not demonstrate significantly greater activation in any brain regions compared to the NFAS Group for CR > GO. (Figure 1 approximately here) (Table 4 approximately here) (Figure 2 approximately here) As one participant had a substantially higher YFAS-C score compared to the rest of the YFAS-C group (participant #35, see Table S1), the group difference analyses for CR > GO were rerun with this participant removed. All three clusters were still significant for at p < .001, cluster-level FWE-corrected p < .05, when comparing the NFAS to the YFAS-C Group. No other significant clusters were found.

10

4. Discussion In the current study, adolescents who had experienced food addiction symptoms demonstrated less neural activation during successful inhibitory control compared to individuals who had not experienced any symptoms. Although studies have explored the association between brain activity and BMI/obesity (Bauer & Houston, 2017; Gearhardt, Yokum, Stice, Harris, & Brownell, 2014; Val-Laillet et al., 2015; Yokum, Gearhardt, Harris, Brownell, & Stice, 2014) as well as disordered eating in adults and adolescents (for review, see Donnelly, Touyz, Hay, Burton, Russell, & Caterson, [2018]), to our knowledge this is the first study to examine the neural circuitry of food addiction symptomology in adolescents using the YFAS-C. Here we found that three clusters, encompassing the left middle temporal/occipital gyrus, the left precuneus/occipital gyrus, and left inferior frontal gyrus, showed less activation in the YFAS-C group compared to the NFAS group. Activation in both the left and right inferior frontal gyrus have been implicated in inhibitory control during the response inhibition portion of a go/no-go task (for left inferior frontal gyrus specifically, see Swick, Ashley, & Turk [2008]; for review, see Zhang et al. [2017]), suggesting that the inferior frontal gyrus is linked to suppressing prepotent responses. The inferior frontal gyrus is part of a larger network, including subcortical and cortical regions known to be involved in executive and motor control, that supports response inhibition during the go/no-go task. While we do interpret decreased activation here as less efficient processing, it is important to note that this could also be explained as more efficient processing—as task activation differences without performance differences can often be difficult to interpret. However, prior studies have linked a weakness, interpreted as less efficient processing, in this network to risk for substance use problems (for review, see Heitzeg, Cope, Martz, & Hardee [2015]); similarly, weaknesses in inhibitory control may be a relevant mechanism underlying risk for food addiction. In the only other neuroimaging study to use the YFAS, young adults with elevated symptomology exhibited decreased activation in inhibitory control systems when consuming a highly processed food reward, compared to young adults with one or fewer YFAS symptoms (Gearhardt et al., 2011b; Hsu, Wang, Ko, Hsieh, Chen, & Yen, 2017). Here, we used non-food cues to examine inhibitory control in a group of children and adolescents; this enabled us to examine the impact of non-food related inhibitory control at an earlier point in the developmental process. 11

Despite the fact that adolescence is a high-risk period for the emergence of addictive behaviors, little is known regarding how addictive-like eating develops during this time. In the dual systems model of adolescent brain development, the earlier maturation of subcortical reward networks relative to prefrontal cognitive (inhibitory) control networks creates a maturational mismatch between the two systems (Bourgeois & Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Casey, Jones & Hare, 2008; Steinberg, 2008). Prefrontal control matures linearly across development from late childhood through early adulthood (Galvan et al., 2006; Gogtay et al., 2004; Hare, Tottenham, Galvan, Voss, Glover, & Casey, 2008; Huttenlocher, 1979; Sowell, Peterson, Thompson, Welcome, Henkenius, & Toga, 2003), while the reward network follows a curvilinear trajectory that increases during late childhood, peaks during adolescence, and then declines during the late teens and early twenties (for review, see Shulman et al. [2016]). This imbalance is thought to contribute to higher rates of problematic substance use, such as binge drinking, as well as the increased risk for obesity (Dietz, 1994; Eaton et al., 2010). The YFAS has been associated with factors related to addiction, such as craving and impulsivity, and the negative consequences of overeating in both clinical (e.g. obese and overweight) and non-clinical adolescents (Gearhardt et al., 2013; Chen, Tang, Guo, Liu, & Xiao, 2015; Meule, Muller, Gearhardt, & Blechert, 2017; Richmond, Roberto, & Gearhardt, 2017; Schulte, Jacques-Tiura, Gearhardt, & Naar, 2018). Inhibition of a prepotent response contributes to self-control over excessive behaviors, such as continuing to take drugs or eat unhealthy foods. Less activation in the YFAS-C group during inhibitory control may signify a general weakness in the mechanism supporting response inhibition during adolescence. Weak inhibitory control circuitry in adolescents paired with the experience of addictive eating behaviors may be useful indicators for investigating the later emergence of clinical levels of food addiction. The role of the other two activation clusters—the middle temporal/occipital gyrus and the precuneus/occipital gyrus—is more speculative. It has been argued that, in addition to motor response inhibition, the go/no-go task is a measure of sustained attention (Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997). Thus, lower activation in both the middle temporal/occipital gyrus and the precuneus/occipital gyrus in the YFAS-C group could reflect disengagement of attention away from the task. Research has shown that brain regions known to mediate attention selectively to external stimuli (i.e. task stimuli) are also important for actively directing attention internally (i.e. to focus on the visceral or body state), otherwise known as 12

interoception (Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004; Nebel, Wiese, Stude, de Greiff, Diener, & Keidel, 2005; Pollatos, Kirsch, & Schandry, 2005; Tracy et al., 2007). Volkow and colleagues (Volkow, Wang, Tomasi, & Baler, 2013) suggest that impairments in self-control and interoceptive circuitry is a functional model of addiction that can be used to understand why some obese individuals have difficulty in regulating caloric intake and maintaining energy homeostasis. The ability to inhibit a prepotent response, such as in the go/no-go task, and exert self-control is a likely contributor to one’s ability to avoid engaging in excessive behaviors (e.g. taking drugs, overeating); thus, this would increase vulnerability to substance-related addictive behaviors or obesity (Volkow & Fowler, 2000; Volkow et al., 2008). These activation differences were found despite the fact that there were no significant differences between the groups with respect to go/no-go task performance. This is similar to results found in adolescents at risk for AUD, whose performance is analogous to controls for simple measures of response inhibition, such as the go/no-go task (Hardee et al., 2014; Heitzeg, Nigg, Yau, Zucker, & Zubieta, 2010; Norman, Pulido, Squeglia, Spadoni, Paulus, & Tapert, 2011). It is possible that performance differences may accompany aberrant neural responses with a more challenging task in the YFAS-C group. This study has limitations. First, the study was not powered to investigate sex differences. Given that males and females may show different risk factors for substance use (Foster, Hicks, Iacono, & McGue, 2015; Heitzeg, Hardee, & Beltz, 2018; Kendler, Edwards, & Gardner, 2015; Kuhn, 2015), it is possible their risk profiles for problem eating also differ. Future studies should investigate sex differences in the association between neural mechanisms involved in inhibitory control and vulnerability to food addiction. Second, this study was cross-sectional, which did not allow for the evaluation of whether deactivation in inhibitory control networks precede or follow elevated food addiction symptomology. Future work using a longitudinal design will allow for a better understanding of the developmental antecedents to problem eating. Additionally, nearly 75% of participants are considered at-risk as they have a family history of AUD, which precludes these results from generalizing to individuals without a family history of substance use. However, when controlling for a family history of AUD in supplementary analyses, we found it did not affect our results. Third, we did not have data on eating disorder psychopathology (e.g. binge eating severity), which is known to be strongly correlated with food addiction symptoms; thus, we could not look for differences between the groups. Additionally, 13

general psychopathologies, such as depression and anxiety, are known to be significantly correlated with food addiction symptoms (Burrows, Kay-Lambkin, Pursey, Skinner, & Dayas, 2018). While we did have measures of general psychopathology for this sample, it was missing for 13 out of the 76 participants; therefore, we did not include this data. In summary, the current study is the first to investigate neural differences based on food addiction symptoms in adolescents. Adolescents who endorse food addiction appear to show hypo-activation in response to the inhibitory control portion of a go/no-go task, which suggests possible inhibitory control difficulties. These findings converge with research in adults where food addiction is associated with behavioral inhibition difficulties; they also indicate that at least some part of the process is in place earlier. However, it is important to note that the implications of adolescents endorsing food addiction symptoms are currently unknown, as this research is still relatively nascent. It is essential to investigate whether this predicts the development of clinical levels of food addiction and related problems (e.g. obesity, binge eating). This work suggests that interventions looking to increase inhibitory control in adolescents who endorse food addiction, such as neurofeedback and/or transcranial direct current stimulation, may be a promising future direction.

Funding: This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA024804, AA025790, AA024433, and AA007065), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA027261), and the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (DK102532).

Acknowledgments: The authors would like to thank Mary Soules and Bailey Ferris for their contributions to data collection, analysis, and management.

14

References Alonso-Alonso, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). The right brain hypothesis for obesity. JAMA, 297(16), 1819-1822. doi:10.1001/jama.297.16.1819 Batterink, L., Yokum, S., & Stice, E. (2010). Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. Neuroimage, 52(4), 16961703. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059 Bauer, L. O., & Houston, R. J. (2017). The value of instability: an investigation of intrasubject variability in brain activity among obese adolescent girls. Int J Obes (Lond), 41(10), 1489-1495. doi:10.1038/ijo.2017.144 Bekker, M. H., van de Meerendonk, C., & Mollerus, J. (2004). Effects of negative mood induction and impulsivity on self-perceived emotional eating. Int J Eat Disord, 36(4), 461469. doi:10.1002/eat.20041 Bourgeois, J. P., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rakic, P. (1994). Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cereb Cortex, 4(1), 78-96 Brett, M., Anton, J. L., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J. B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Sendai, Japan Burrows, T., Kay-Lambkin, F., Pursey, K., Skinner, J., & Dayas, C. (2018). Food addiction and associations with mental health symptoms: a systematic review with meta-analysis. J Hum Nutr Diet, 31(4), 544-572. doi:10.1111/jhn.12532 Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Ann NY Acad Sci, 1124, 111-126. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.010 Chen, R., Li, D. P., Turel, O., Sorensen, T. A., Bechara, A., Li, Y., & He, Q. (2018). Decision making deficits in relation to food cues influence obesity: a triadic neural model of problematic eating. Front Psychiatry, 9, 264. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00264 Chen, C. Y., Storr, C. L., & Anthony, J. C. (2009). Early-onset drug use and risk for drug dependence problems. Addict Behav, 34(3), 319-322. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.10.021 Chen, G., Tang, Z., Guo, G., Liu, X., & Xiao, S. (2015). The Chinese version of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: An examination of its validation in a sample of female adolescents. Eat Behav, 18, 97-102. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.05.002 Clark, D. B., Thatcher, D. L., & Tapert, S. F. (2008). Alcohol, psychological dysregulation, and adolescent brain development. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 32(3), 375-385. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00601.x

15

Cohen, J. I., Yates, K. F., Duong, M., & Convit, A. (2011). Obesity, orbitofrontal structure and function are associated with food choice: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open, 1(2), e000175. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000175 Costello, A., Edelbrock, C., Kalas, R., Kessler, M., & Klaric, S. (1982). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children: child version. Rockville, MD Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci, 7(2), 189-195. doi:10.1038/nn1176 Davis, C., & Carter, J. C. (2009). Compulsive overeating as an addiction disorder. A review of theory and evidence. Appetite, 53(1), 1-8. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.018 Davis, C., Curtis, C., Levitan, R. D., Carter, J. C., Kaplan, A. S., & Kennedy, J. L. (2011). Evidence that 'food addiction' is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite, 57(3), 711-717. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017 Decaluwe, V., & Braet, C. (2003). Prevalence of binge-eating disorder in obese children and adolescents seeking weight-loss treatment. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord, 27(3), 404-409. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0802233 Dietz, W. H. (1994). Critical periods in childhood for the development of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr, 59(5), 955-959. doi:10.1093/ajcn/59.5.955 Donnelly, B., Touyz, S., Hay, P., Burton, A., Russell, J., & Caterson, I. (2018). Neuroimaging in bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder: a systematic review. J Eat Disord, 6, 3. doi:10.1186/s40337-018-0187-1 Durston, S., Thomas, K. M., Yang, Y., Ulug, A. M., Zimmerman, R. D., & Casey, B. J. (2002). A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Dev Sci, 2002(5), F9-F16 Eaton, D. K., Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S., Ross, J., Hawkins, J., et al. (2010). Youth risk behavior surveillance - United States, 2009. MMWR Surveill Summ, 59(5), 1-142. Epstein, L. H., Wing, R. R., & Valoski, A. (1985). Childhood obesity. Pediatr Clin North Am, 32(2), 363-379 Flint, A. J., Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., Brownell, K. D., Field, A. E., & Rimm, E. B. (2014). Food-addiction scale measurement in 2 cohorts of middle-aged and older women. Am J Clin Nutr, 99(3), 578-586. doi:10.3945/ajcn.113.068965 Foster, K. T., Hicks, B. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2015). Gender differences in the structure of risk for alcohol use disorder in adolescence and young adulthood. Psychol Med, 45(14), 3047-3058. doi:10.1017/S0033291715001014

16

Franken, I. H. A., Nijs, I. M. T., Toes, A., & van der Veen, F. M. (2016). Food addiction is associated with impaired performance monitoring. Biol Psychol, 131, 49-53. doi:10.1016/jbiopsycho.2016.07.005 Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., et al. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J Neurosci, 26(25), 6885-6892. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1062-06.2006 Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite, 52(2), 430-436 Gearhardt, A. N., Davis, C., Kuschner, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2011a). The addiction potential of hyperpalatable foods. Curr Drug Abuse Rev, 4(3), 140-145 Gearhardt, A. N., Roberto, C. A., Seamans, M. J., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2013). Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale for children. Eat Behav, 14(4), 508512. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.07.002 Gearhardt, A. N., White, M. A., Masheb, R. M., Morgan, P. T., Crosby, R. D., & Grilo, C. M. (2012). An examination of the food addiction construct in obese patients with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord, 45(5), 657-663. doi:10.1002/eat.20957 Gearhardt, A. N., Yokum, S., Orr, P. T., Stice, E., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2011b). Neural correlates of food addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 68(8), 808-816. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.32 Gearhardt, A. N., Yokum, S., Stice, E., Harris, J. L., & Brownell, K. D. (2014). Relation of obesity to neural activation in response to food commercials. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci, 9(7), 932-938. doi:10.1093/scan/nst059 Glover, G.H., & Law, (2001). C.S. Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts. Magn. Reson. Med, 46, 515–522 Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 101(21), 8174-8179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101 Guerrieri, R., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2008). The interaction between impulsivity and a varied food environment: its influence on food intake and overweight. Int J Obes (Lond), 32(4), 708-714. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803770 Guerrieri, R., Nederkoorn, C., Stankiewicz, K., Alberts, H., Geschwind, N., Martijn, C., & Jansen, A. (2007). The influence of trait and induced state impulsivity on food intake in normal-weight healthy women. Appetite, 49(1), 66-73. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.11.008

17

Hardee, J. E., Weiland, B. J., Nichols, T. E., Welsh, R. C., Soules, M. E., Steinberg, D. B., Zubieta, J. K., Zucker, R. A., & Heitzeg, M. M. (2014). Development of impulse control in children of alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry, 76(9), 708-716. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.005 Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., & Casey, B. J. (2008). Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol Psychiatry, 63(10), 927-934. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015 Heitzeg, M. M., Cope, L. M., Martz, M. E., & Hardee, J. E. (2015). Neuroimaging risk markers for substance abuse: recent findings on inhibitory control and reward system functioning. Curr Addict Rep, 2(2), 91-103. doi:10.1007/s40429-015-0048-9 Heitzeg, M. M., Hardee, J. E., & Beltz, A. M. (2018). Sex differences in the developmental neuroscience of adolescent substance use risk. Curr Opin Behav Sci, 23, 21-26. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.020 Heitzeg, M. M., Nigg, J. T., Yau, W. W., Zucker, R. A. & Zubieta, J. K. (2010) Striatal dysfunction marks preexisting risk and medial prefrontal dysfunction is related to problem drinking in children of alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry, 68, 287-295. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.020 Hsu, J. S., Wang, P. W., Ko, C. H., Hsieh, T. J., Chen, C. Y., & Yen, J. Y. (2017). Altered brain correlates of response inhibition and error processing in females with obesity and sweet food addiction: A functional magnetic imaging study. Obes Res Clin Pract, 11(6), 677-686. doi:10.1016/j.orcp.2017.04.011 Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res, 163(2), 195-205 Imperatori, C., Fabbricatore, M., Innamorati, M., Farina, B., Quintiliani, M. I., Lamis, D. A., et al. (2015). Modification of EEG functional connectivity and EEF power spectra in overweight and obese patients with food addiction: An eLORETA study. Brain Imaging Behav, 9(4): 703-716. doi:10.1007/s11682-014-9324-x Jasinska, A. J., Yasuda, M., Burant, C. F., Gregor, N., Khatri, S., Sweet, M., et al. (2012). Impulsivity and inhibitory control deficits are associated with unhealthy eating in young adults. Appetite, 59(3), 738-747. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.001 Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage, 17(2), 825-841 Kendler, K. S., Edwards, A. C., & Gardner, C. O. (2015). Sex differences in the pathways to symptoms of alcohol use disorder: a study of opposite-sex twin pairs. Alcohol Clin and Exp Res, 39(6), 998-1007. doi:10.1111/acer.12694

18

Kishinevsky, F. I., Cox, J. E., Murdaugh, D. L., Stoeckel, L. E., Cook, E. W., 3rd, & Weller, R. E. (2012). fMRI reactivity on a delay discounting task predicts weight gain in obese women. Appetite, 58(2), 582-592. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.029 Kuhn, C. (2015). Emergence of sex differences in the development of substance use and abuse during adolescence. Pharmacol Ther, 153, 55-78. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2015.06.003 Meule, A., & Gearhardt A. N. (2014). Five years of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: Taking stock and moving forward. Curr Addict Rep, 1(3), 193-205.doi:10.1007/s40429-014-0021-z Meule, A., Muller, A., Gearhardt, A. N., & Blechert, J. (2017). German version of the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0: Prevalence and correlates of 'food addiction' in students and obese individuals. Appetite, 115, 54-61. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.003 Mitchell, M. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Addictions and Personality Traits: Impulsivity and Related Constructs. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep, 1(1), 1-12. doi:10.1007/s40473-013-0001-y Nebel, K., Wiese, H., Stude, P., de Greiff, A., Diener, H. C., & Keidel, M. (2005). On the neural basis of focused and divided attention. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res, 25(3), 760-776. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.011 Nederkoorn, C., Braet, C., Van Eijs, Y., Tanghe, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Why obese children cannot resist food: the role of impulsivity. Eat Behav, 7(4), 315-322. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005 Noll, D. C., Fessler, J. A., & Sutton, B. P. (2005). Conjugate phase MRI reconstruction with spatially variant sample density correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging, 24(3), 325-336 Norman, A. L., Pulido, C., Squeglia, L. M., Spadoni, A. D., Paulus, M. P., & Tapert, S. F. (2011). Neural activation during inhibition predicts initiation of substance abuse in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend, 119, 216-223. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.06.019 Pedram, P., Wadden, D., Amini, P., Gulliver, W., Randell, E., Cahill, F., et al. (2013). Food addiction: its prevalence and significant association with obesity in the general population. PLoS One, 8(9), e74832. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074832 Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., & Schandry, R. (2005). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum Brain Mapp. 26(1), 54-64. doi:10.1002/hbm.20121 Racine, S. E., Culbert, K. M., Larson, C. L., & Klump, K. L. (2009). The possible influence of impulsivity and dietary restraint on associations between serotonin genes and binge eating. J Psychiatr Res, 43(16), 1278-1286. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.05.002

19

Richmond, R. L., Roberto, C. A., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2017). The association of addictivelike eating with food intake in children. Appetite, 117, 82-90. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2017.06.002 Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). 'Oops!': performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747-758 Robins, L. N., Cottler, L. B., Bucholz, K. K., Compton, W. M., North, C. S., & Rourke, K. M. (2000). Diagnostic Interview Schedule for the DSM-IV (DIS-IV). St. Louis, Missouri: Washington University School of Medicine Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Ratcliff, K. S. (1981). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 38(4), 381-389 Schulte, E. M., Grilo, C. M., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2016). Shared and unique mechanisms underlying binge eating disorder and addictive disorders. Clin Psychol Rev, 44, 125-139. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.02.001 Schulte, E. M., & Gearthardt, A. N. (2017). Development of the modified Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0. Eur Eat Disord Rev, 25(4), 302-308. doi:10.1002/erv.2515 Schulte, E. M., Jacques-Tiura, A. J., Gearhardt, A. N., & Naar, S. (2018). Food addiction prevalence and concurrent validity in African American adolescents with obesity. Psychol Addict Behav, 32(2), 187-196. doi:10.1037/adb0000325 Schulte, E. M., Yokum, S., Jahn, A., & Gearhardt, A. N., (2019). Food cue reactivity in food addiction: A functinal magnetic resonanace imaging study. Physiol Behav, 208: 112574. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112574 Shulman, E. P., Smith, A. R., Silva, K., Icenogle, G., Duell, N., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Dev Cogn Neurosci, 17, 103-117. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.010 Silvers, J. A., Insel, C., Powers, A., Franz, P., Weber, J., Mischel, W., et al. (2014). Curbing craving: behavioral and brain evidence that children regulate craving when instructed to do so but have higher baseline craving than adults. Psychol Sci, 25(10), 1932-1942. doi:10.1177/0956797614546001 Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., & Toga, A. W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci, 6(3), 309315. doi:10.1038/nn1008 Steinberg, L. (2008). Neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk taking. Dev Rev, 28, 1-27. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002.A

20

Sutherland, M. T., & Stein, E. A. (2018). Functional neurocircuits and neuroimaging biomarkers of tobacco use disorder. Trends Mol Med, 24(2), 129-143. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2017.12.002 Sutton, B. P., Noll, D. C., & Fessler, J. A. (2003). Fast, iterative image reconstruction for MRI in the presence of field inhomogeneities. IEEE Trans Med Imaging, 22(2), 178-188 Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci, 9, 102. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-102 Tapert, S. F., Caldwell, L., & Burke, C. T. (2004). Alcohol and the adolescent brain: Human studies. Alcohol Research & Health, 28(4), 205-212 Tracy, J., Goyal, N., Flanders, A., Weening, R., Laskas, J., Natale, P., et al. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging analysis of attention to one's heartbeat. Psychosom Med, 69(9), 952-960. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b60cf Val-Laillet, D., Aarts, E., Weber, B., Ferrari, M., Quaresima, V., Stoeckel, L. E., et al. (2015). Neuroimaging and neuromodulation approaches to study eating behavior and prevent and treat eating disorders and obesity. Neuroimage Clin, 8, 1-31. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.016 Volkow, N. D., & Fowler, J. S. (2000). Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex, 10(3), 318-325 Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Telang, F., Fowler, J. S., Thanos, P. K., Logan, J., et al. (2008). Low dopamine striatal D2 receptors are associated with prefrontal metabolism in obese subjects: possible contributing factors. Neuroimage, 42(4), 1537-1543. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.002 Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Tomasi, D., & Baler, R. D. (2013). Obesity and addiction: neurobiological overlaps. Obes Rev, 14(1), 2-18. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01031.x Yokum, S., Gearhardt, A. N., Harris, J. L., Brownell, K. D., & Stice, E. (2014). Individual differences in striatum activity to food commercials predict weight gain in adolescents. Obesity, 22(12), 2544-2551. doi:10.1002/oby.20882 Zahr, N. M., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2017). Perspectives on fronto-fugal circuitry from human imaging of alcohol use disorders. Neuropharmacology, 122, 189-200. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.01.018 Zhang, R., Geng, X., & Lee, T. M. C. (2017). Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: an fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct, 222(9), 39733990. doi:10.1007/s00429-017-1443-x

21

Zucker, R. A. (1996). Longitudinal research on alcohol problems: the flow of risk, problems, and disorder over time. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 20(8 Suppl), 93A-95A Zucker, R. A., Ellis, D. A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Bingham, C. R., & Sanford, K. P. (1996). Other evidence for at least two alcoholisms II: life course variation in antisociality and heterogeneity of alcoholic outcome. Dev Psychopathol, 8, 831-848 Zucker, R. A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Refior, S. K., Puttler, L. I., Pallas, D. M., & Ellis, D. A. (2000). The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholic: description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. Fitzgerald, H. E., Lester, B. M., Zucker, R. A. (Eds.), Children of Addiction: Research, Health, and Policy Issues, Routledge Falmer, New York; pp. 109-141

22

Table 1. Participant Characteristics

Males/Female (n) Age at fMRI scan Full-scale IQ* Family history of AUD (n)

All Participants (n = 76) 44/32 14.3 (2.8) 101.8 (12.6) 13 FH-/63 FH+ 21.8 (4.1) 0.7 (1.0)

NFAS Group (n = 41) 26/15 14.6 (2.9) 102.5 (12.1)

YFAS-C Group (n = 35) 18/17 14.0 (2.8) 100.9 (13.3)

Statistic

Significance

— t(74) = 0.89 t(72) = 0.78

p = .35a p = .38b p = .57b*

7 FH-/34 FH+

6 FH-/29 FH+



p = 1.00a

22.1 (4.7) 21.5 (3.4) t(72) = 0.62 p = .54c BMI 0.0 (0.0) 1.5 (1.0) t(74) = -9.0 p = 0.001c YFAS-C Substance Use Prior to fMRI Scan (n; present) Used alcohol 17 9 8 — p > .99a Used cannabis 12 7 5 — p > .99a Used nicotine 6 4 2 — p = .68a Used other drugs 4 3 1 — p = .62a Used multiple substances 10 6 4 — p > .99a Lifetime Diagnosis (n; present) Conduct disorder 0 — — — — ADHD 4 1 3 — p = .31a Generalized anxiety disorder 0 — — — — Depressive disorder 0 — — — — Note. NFAS: No Food Addiction Symptom; YFAS-C: Yale Food Addiction Scale for Children; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; AUD, alcohol use disorder; FH-/+, family history negative/positive for alcohol use disorder; BMI, body mass index; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Numbers represent means, with standard deviations in parentheses, unless otherwise noted. a Two-tailed Fisher’s exact test. b Two-tailed independent samples t-test, equal variances assumed c Two-tailed independent samples t-test, equal variances not assumed *One participant in each group was missing an IQ score

23

Table 2. Modified YFAS-C Scoring Dichotomy and Scale In the last year (past 12 months):

Never

Rarely

Sometimes

Very Often

Always

0

0

0

0

1

0

0

0

1

1

0

0

1

1

1

0

0

0

1

1

0

0

0

1

1

0

0

0

1

1

No

Yes

Q7. I eat in the same way even though it is causing problems

0

1

Q8. I need to eat more to get the food feelings I want (for example, for happy, calm, relaxed)

0

1

Q9. I am unable to cut down on certain foods

1

0

Q1. I eat foods even when I am not hungry Q2. I feel tired a lot because I eat too much Q3. I avoid places where I cannot eat the food I want Q4. I eat certain foods to stop from feeling upset or sick Q5. The way I eat makes me really unhappy Q6. The way I eat causes me problems (for example, problems at school, with my parents, with my friends) In the last years (past 12 months):

Symptom count scoring dichotomy is represented by 0 or 1. Number in bold is number that contributes to symptom count for each particular question.

24

Table 3. Go/No-Go Task Performance by Group NFAS Group

YFAS-C Group

Statistic

Significance

96.8 (3.2) 96.3 (4.0) t(74) = 0.52 p = .60 Hits (%) 429.7 (59.2) 438.2 (58.4) t(72) = -0.63 p = .53a Hit RT (ms) 41.1 (20.7) 44.5 (19.8) t(74) = -0.72 p = .47 False Alarms (%) False Alarm RT 388.2 (58.3) 392.3 (49.8) t(74) = -0.34 p = .75 (ms) Note. ms, milliseconds. For NFAS and YFAS-C columns, the numbers given are means, with standard devotion in parentheses. Statistic and significance columns refer to two-tailed independent-samples t-tests. a Degrees of freedom reduction reflects equal variances not assumed.

25

Table 4. CR > GO contrast from independent-samples t-test of NFAS vs YFAS-C Groups % ROI Cluster 1 Left Middle Temporal Gyrus

53.7%

Left Occipital Gyrus

26.1%

Outside (not identified)

20.2%

Cluster 2 Left Precuneus

40.3%

Left Calcarine Sulcus

30.5%

Outside (not identified)

13.6%

Lingual Gyrus

9.8%

Vermis 4/5

5.9%

Cluster 3

x

y

z

k

t-value

FWE p-value

-40

-66

16

257

4.42

.034

-14

-52

12

236

4.10

.046

-56

24

20

312

3.86

.016

Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus 68.3% Pars Triangularis Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus – 31.7% Pars Opercularis Note. ROI = region of interest; x, y, z are peak voxel coordinates for clusters of activation in Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates; k = cluster size; threshold p < .05, FWE cluster corrected.

26

Figure Captions: Figure 1. Regions displaying a significant difference for the CR > GO contrast at initial threshold of p < .001 uncorrected with a cluster-wise threshold of p < .05 FWE. Three clusters passed criteria: Cluster 1 encompassed the left middle temporal and occipital gyrus (red arrow); Cluster 2 encompassed the left precuneus and calcarine sulcus (green arrow); Cluster 3 encompassed the left inferior frontal gyrus – pars triangularis and pars opercularis (white arrow). See Table 4 for full coordinates.

Figure 2. Mean activations extracted from each cluster from the CR > GO contrast in Figure 1 for the NFAS and YFAS-C groups. See Table 4 for cluster labels and coordinates. Error bars represent one standard error of the mean.

27

The study was conducted according to the guidelines provided by the Declaration of Helsinki and the ethical requirements of the University of Michigan (HUM00084126). All participants gave written consent/assent after explanation of the experimental protocol and at least one parent gave written informed consent, as approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board.